Jump to content

Nawat language (typological overview): Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m dash fix (via WP:JWB)
 
(43 intermediate revisions by 26 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Multiple issues|
{{Refimprove|date=January 2008}}
{{Technical|date=May 2022}}
}}
This rather technical article provides a '''typological sketch of the [[Pipil language]]''' (also known as Nawat). Another related article outlines [[Pipil grammar]] in fuller detail. The distinctive purpose of the present article is to single out those specific features of Nawat linguistic structure that are relevant to this language's general typological classification and characterization, answering the question: What major features make this language similar to or different from other languages? Most of the assertions in this article are generalizations from information found in the Pipil grammar article.

This article is likely to be of most interest and use to readers interested in general [[linguistics]], [[language typology]], and related areas such as [[Areal feature|areal typology]], and especially (though not exclusively) to professional linguists. Another possible use of this article is as a thumbnail sketch of the language, a checklist of its most salient points, serving as a brief guide to the fuller language description in the Pipil grammar article.

==Phonology==
==Phonology==
===Phoneme inventory===


===Phoneme inventory===
The Nawat [[phoneme]] inventory is smaller than that of most languages languages in the area. Phonemically relevant [[phonation|voice]] distinctions are generally absent: [[stop consonant|stop]]s are normally voiceless (though there exist some voiced [[allophones]]), as are [[fricative]]s and [[affricate]]s; [[liquid consonant|liquid]]s, [[nasal consonant|nasal]]s and [[semivowel]]s are normally voiced (though there exist voiceless allophones).
The Nawat [[phoneme]] inventory is smaller than that of most languages in the area. Phonemically relevant [[phonation|voice]] distinctions are generally absent: [[plosive]]s are normally voiceless (though there exist some voiced [[allophones]]), as are [[fricative]]s and [[affricate]]s; [[liquid consonant|liquid]]s, [[nasal stop|nasal]]s and [[semivowel]]s are normally voiced (though there exist voiceless allophones).


===Other aspects===
===Other aspects===
Line 8: Line 16:


==Morphology==
==Morphology==
Inflectional and derivational [[morphology (linguistics)|morphology]] are of moderate complexity, with a fairly balanced mix of [[prefix]]ing and [[suffix]]ing mechanisms.

Inflectional and derivational [[morphology]] are of moderate complexity, with a fairly balanced mix of [[prefix]]ing and [[suffix]]ing mechanisms.


===Nominal morphology===
===Nominal morphology===
In the [[nominal]] morphology there is no [[inflection]] for [[grammatical case|case]] or [[definiteness]], the morphological categories being [[grammatical number|number]], [[construct state|state]] (absolute vs. construct) and [[grammatical person|person]] (of the [[possession (linguistics)|possessor]], with construct state).
In the [[Nominal (linguistics)|nominal]] morphology there is no [[inflection]] for [[grammatical case|case]] or [[definiteness]], the morphological categories being [[grammatical number|number]], [[construct state|state]] (absolute vs. construct) and [[grammatical person|person]] (of the [[possession (linguistics)|possessor]], with construct state).


===Verbal morphology===
===Verbal morphology===
{| border="1" cellpadding="4" cellspacing="0" class="wikitable" style="margin: 1em auto 1em auto"
{| class="wikitable" style="margin: 1em auto 1em auto"
! align="center"|Morphological verb categories
! align="center"|Morphological verb categories
|-
|-
Line 27: Line 34:
*direction (towards the speaker)
*direction (towards the speaker)
|}
|}
In the [[verbal]] morphology, [[tense]], [[grammatical mood|mood]] and [[grammatical number|number]] (of the [[subject (grammar)|subject]]) are marked by [[suffix]]es, [[grammatical person|person]] (of the subject, and of the [[object (grammar)|object]] with [[transitive]] verbs) by [[prefix]]es. Prefixes can also reduce a transitive verb's [[verb argument|argument]] [[valency (linguistics)|valency]], making it either [[unaccusative]] (''ta-'') or [[unergative]] (''mu-''). A further prefix (''(w)al-'') adds a [[directional]] meaning component (roughly 'towards the speaker') to some verbs, though no longer [[productive]]ly. [[derivation (linguistics)|Derivational]] suffixes can add to a verb's valency, acting as [[causative]]s (which add a new subject as [[agent (grammar)|agent]]) or [[applicative (grammar)|applicative]]s (which add a new object as [[recipient (grammar)|recipient]]). A few cases of paradigmatic [[suppletion]] occur (''witz'' 'comes' : ''walaj'' 'came'; ''ki-uni'' 'he drinks it (transitive)' : ''ati'' 'he drinks (unaccusative)'.
In the [[Words|verbal]] morphology, [[Grammatical tense|tense]], [[grammatical mood|mood]] and [[grammatical number|number]] (of the [[subject (grammar)|subject]]) are marked by [[suffix]]es, [[grammatical person|person]] (of the subject, and of the [[object (grammar)|object]] with [[transitive verb|transitive]] verbs) by [[prefix]]es. Prefixes can also reduce a transitive verb's [[verb argument|argument]] [[valency (linguistics)|valency]], making it either [[unaccusative]] (''ta-'') or [[unergative]] (''mu-''). A further prefix (''(w)al-'') adds a [[Relative direction|directional]] meaning component (roughly 'towards the speaker') to some verbs, though no longer [[productive]]ly. [[derivation (linguistics)|Derivational]] suffixes can add to a verb's valency, acting as [[causative]]s (which add a new subject as [[agent (grammar)|agent]]) or [[applicative (grammar)|applicative]]s (which add a new object as [[recipient (grammar)|recipient]]). A few cases of paradigmatic [[suppletion]] occur (''witz'' 'comes' : {{transl|ppl|walaj}} 'came'; ''ki-uni'' 'he drinks it (transitive)' : ''ati'' 'he drinks (unaccusative)'.


==Syntax==
==Syntax==
===Word order===


===Word order===
{| border="1" cellpadding="4" cellspacing="0" class="wikitable" style="margin: 1em auto 1em auto"
{| class="wikitable" style="margin: 1em auto 1em auto"
! align="center"|Word order
! align="center"|Word order
|-
|-
|
|
*flexible VO
*flexible VO
*pro-drop (subject and object)
*determiner + head
*determiner + head
*quantifier + head
*quantifier + head
Line 47: Line 53:
[[sentence (linguistics)|Sentence]] [[word order]] shows considerable flexibility. [[Pronominal]] [[verb argument|arguments]] (which may represent [[subject (grammar)|subject]] or [[object (grammar)|object]]) usually precede the [[verb]], and are only used for emphasis: compare ''Naja nikita'' 'I see him/her/it' and ''Naja nechita'' 'He/she/it sees me' (''naja'' 'I, me').
[[sentence (linguistics)|Sentence]] [[word order]] shows considerable flexibility. [[Pronominal]] [[verb argument|arguments]] (which may represent [[subject (grammar)|subject]] or [[object (grammar)|object]]) usually precede the [[verb]], and are only used for emphasis: compare ''Naja nikita'' 'I see him/her/it' and ''Naja nechita'' 'He/she/it sees me' (''naja'' 'I, me').


Within the [[noun phrase]], [[determiner]]s and [[quantifiers]] precede the [[head (linguistics)|head]]. [[Adjective]]s may either precede or follow the head noun. [[possession (linguistics)|Possessors]] follow possessed, and [[relative clause]]s follow their head. There are [[preposition]]al structures.
Within the [[noun phrase]], [[determiner (linguistics)|determiner]]s and [[Quantifiers (linguistics)|quantifiers]] precede the [[head (linguistics)|head]]. [[Adjective]]s may either precede or follow the head noun. [[possession (linguistics)|Possessors]] follow possessed, and [[relative clause]]s follow their head. There are [[preposition]]al structures.


===Head or dependent marking===
===Head or dependent marking===


{| border="1" cellpadding="4" cellspacing="0" class="wikitable" style="margin: 1em auto 1em auto"
{| class="wikitable" style="margin: 1em auto 1em auto"
! align="center"|HEAD-marking constructions
! align="center"|HEAD-marking constructions
|-
|-
Line 60: Line 66:
*RELATIONAL + noun (see below)
*RELATIONAL + noun (see below)
|}
|}
There is [[pro-drop]] for both subject and object (i.e. subject and object pronouns are omitted in [[markedness|unmarked]] sentences). One or two [[verb argument|argument]]s of the verb are indexed for [[grammatical person|person]] and [[grammatical number|number]] in the verb (one when [[intransitive]], two when [[transitive]]). A maximum of one object index is possible. If a transitive verb has both a [[patient (grammar)|patient]] ('direct object' in English grammar) and a [[recipient (grammar)|recipient]] ('indirect object'), it is the latter which is indexed as object of the Nawat verb, e.g. ''Nechmaka at'' 'He gives me water', where ''nech-'' indexes a first-person-singular object. There is no marking on the arguments to indicate the various semantic relations so far mentioned. This is a [[head-marking]] structure, meaning that relations between a head and dependent are marked on the head (here, the verb) and not on the dependent (here, the subject or object). This is also illustrated by the above examples ''Naja nikita'' 'I see him' vs. ''Naja nechita'' 'He sees me', where the 'case' of the pronoun ''naja'' is determined by the indexing on the verb.
There is [[pro-drop]] for both subject and object (i.e. subject and object pronouns are omitted in [[markedness|unmarked]] sentences). One or two [[verb argument|argument]]s of the verb are indexed for [[grammatical person|person]] and [[grammatical number|number]] in the verb (one when [[intransitive]], two when [[transitive verb|transitive]]). A maximum of one object index is possible. If a transitive verb has both a [[patient (grammar)|patient]] ('direct object' in English grammar) and a [[recipient (grammar)|recipient]] ('indirect object'), it is the latter which is indexed as object of the Nawat verb, e.g. ''Nechmaka at'' 'He gives me water', where ''nech-'' indexes a first-person-singular object. There is no marking on the arguments to indicate the various semantic relations so far mentioned. This is a [[head-marking]] structure, meaning that relations between a head and dependent are marked on the head (here, the verb) and not on the dependent (here, the subject or object). This is also illustrated by the above examples ''Naja nikita'' 'I see him' vs. ''Naja nechita'' 'He sees me', where the 'case' of the pronoun ''naja'' is determined by the indexing on the verb.


Head-marking patterns are found in other parts of Nawat syntax too, so the language shows consistency for this [[typological feature]]. The [[canonical]] expression of [[possession (linguistics)|possessive]] or [[genitive]] relations is parallel: the head (i.e. the possessed) is marked with indexes indicating the person of the dependent (the possessor), the noun phrase expressing which is either omitted normally if pronominal (a pro-drop phenomenon) or occurs as a noun phrase following the head and unmarked for the possessive relation, e.g. ''nu-nan'' 'my mother', ''i-nan'' 'his mother', ''i-nan ne kunet'' 'the child's mother'.
Head-marking patterns are found in other parts of Nawat syntax too, so the language shows consistency for this [[linguistic typology|typological feature]]. The canonical expression of [[possession (linguistics)|possessive]] or [[genitive]] relations is parallel: the head (i.e. the possessed) is marked with indices indicating the person of the dependent (the possessor), the noun phrase expressing which is either omitted normally if pronominal (a pro-drop phenomenon) or occurs as a noun phrase following the head and unmarked for the possessive relation, e.g. ''nu-nan'' 'my mother', ''i-nan'' 'his mother', ''i-nan ne kunet'' 'the child's mother'.


===Argument valency restrictions===
===Argument valency restrictions===


Another kind of typological consistency between verbal and nominal constructions is seen in the fact that just as verbs may be classified as intransitives, which do not require an object, and transitives, which require one, so some nouns in Nawat need not have a possessor while others must have one.
Another kind of typological consistency between verbal and nominal constructions is seen in the fact that just as verbs may be classified as intransitives, which do not require an object, and transitives, which require one, so some nouns in Nawat need not have a possessor while others must have one.


Some nouns change their form depending on whether they have a possessor or not, alternating between absolute and construct states, such as absolute ''kunet'', construct ''-kunew'' 'child'; this is reminiscent of how verbs may change their forms depending on their transitivity (i.e. whether they take an object or not), e.g. intransitive ''waki'', transitive ''-watza'' 'dry', transitive ''miki'' 'die', transitive ''-miktia'' 'kill', etc. Once again, it is changes in the head that determine the nature of the grammatical relation between the head and its dependent(s).
Some nouns change their form depending on whether they have a possessor or not, alternating between absolute and [[construct state]]s, such as absolute ''kunet'', construct {{transl|ppl|-kunew}} 'child'; this is reminiscent of how verbs may change their forms depending on their transitivity (i.e. whether they take an object or not), e.g. intransitive ''waki'', transitive {{transl|ppl|-watza}} 'dry', transitive ''miki'' 'die', transitive ''-miktia'' 'kill', etc. Once again, it is changes in the head that determine the nature of the grammatical relation between the head and its dependent(s).


===Adpositions===
===Adpositions===


Other relations between a verb and its noun phrase complements or adjuncts are expressed by means of a small number of prepositions or relational noun constructions. The relational noun construction itself, e.g. ''nu-wan'' 'with me', ''i-wan'' 'with him/her/it/', ''i-wan ne kunet'' 'with the child', are internally just like possessive constructions, with ''wan'' playing the same role as a construct noun.
Other relations between a verb and its noun phrase complements or [[Adjunct (grammar)|adjuncts]] are expressed by means of a small number of [[prepositions]] or [[relational (grammar)|relational]] constructions. The relational construction itself, e.g. ''nu-wan'' 'with me', ''i-wan'' 'with him/her/it/', ''i-wan ne kunet'' 'with the child', is internally just like possessive constructions, with ''wan'' playing the same role as a construct noun.


===Predication and sentence types===
===Predication and sentence types===


There is no general copula; instead, a nominal (or other non-verbal) predicate with no verbal component may be made a clause's grammatical nucleus. Some of these take subject indices just like verbal predicates, but tense can only be expressed periphrastically in such sentences.
There is no general [[copula (linguistics)|copula]]; instead, a nominal (or other non-verbal) [[predicate (grammar)|predicate]] with no verbal component may be made a [[clause]]'s grammatical nucleus. Some of these take subject indices just like verbal predicates, but [[tense (grammar)|tense]] can only be expressed [[periphrastic]]ally in such sentences.


Negation is achieved by placing a negative particle in front of the predicate. Yes-no questions have no special grammatical marking, while wh-questions are identified by the presence of a question word, which usually precedes the verb (or other predicate).
[[negation (linguistics)|Negation]] is achieved by placing a negative particle in front of the predicate. [[Yes–no question]]s have no special grammatical marking, while [[wh-question]]s are identified by the presence of a [[question word]], which usually precedes the verb (or other predicate).


Subordinate clauses are either introduced by a subordinator in clause-initial position or else are juxtaposed with no subordinating conjunction.
[[Subordinate clause]]s are either introduced by a [[subordinator (grammar)|subordinator]] in clause-initial position or else are [[juxtaposition (grammar)|juxtapose]]d with no subordinating [[grammatical conjunction|conjunction]].


==See also==
==See also==
Line 89: Line 95:
==Bibliography==
==Bibliography==


* Campbell, Lyle. (1985). ''The Pipil language of El Salvador''. Mouton grammar library (No. 1). Berlin: Mouton Publishers. ISBN 0-89925-040-8 (U.S.), ISBN 3-11-010344-3.
* [[Lyle Campbell|Campbell, Lyle]] (1985). ''The Pipil language of El Salvador''. Mouton Grammar Library (No. 1). Berlin: Mouton Publishers. {{ISBN|0-89925-040-8}} (U.S.), {{ISBN|3-11-010344-3}}.
* Campbell, Lyle, Terrence Kaufman and Thomas C. Smith-Stark (1986). "Meso-America as a Linguistic Area." ''Language'' 62:3, p. 530–570.
* Campbell, Lyle, [[Terrence Kaufman]] and Thomas C. Smith-Stark (1986). "Meso-America as a Linguistic Area." ''Language'' 62:3, p. 530–570.

[[Category:Pipil|Language typology]]
[[Category:Indigenous languages of Central America]]
[[Category:Mesoamerican languages]]
[[Category:Linguistic typology]]

Latest revision as of 18:06, 15 July 2023

This rather technical article provides a typological sketch of the Pipil language (also known as Nawat). Another related article outlines Pipil grammar in fuller detail. The distinctive purpose of the present article is to single out those specific features of Nawat linguistic structure that are relevant to this language's general typological classification and characterization, answering the question: What major features make this language similar to or different from other languages? Most of the assertions in this article are generalizations from information found in the Pipil grammar article.

This article is likely to be of most interest and use to readers interested in general linguistics, language typology, and related areas such as areal typology, and especially (though not exclusively) to professional linguists. Another possible use of this article is as a thumbnail sketch of the language, a checklist of its most salient points, serving as a brief guide to the fuller language description in the Pipil grammar article.

Phonology

[edit]

Phoneme inventory

[edit]

The Nawat phoneme inventory is smaller than that of most languages in the area. Phonemically relevant voice distinctions are generally absent: plosives are normally voiceless (though there exist some voiced allophones), as are fricatives and affricates; liquids, nasals and semivowels are normally voiced (though there exist voiceless allophones).

Other aspects

[edit]

Syllables consist of a vowel nucleus preceded and followed by a maximum of one consonant: (C)V(C). Word stress is normally phonologically determined, and rarely distinctive.

Morphology

[edit]

Inflectional and derivational morphology are of moderate complexity, with a fairly balanced mix of prefixing and suffixing mechanisms.

Nominal morphology

[edit]

In the nominal morphology there is no inflection for case or definiteness, the morphological categories being number, state (absolute vs. construct) and person (of the possessor, with construct state).

Verbal morphology

[edit]
Morphological verb categories
  • tense
  • mood
  • person/number of subject
  • person/number of object
  • valency decrease (unaccusative or unergative)
  • valency increase (causative, applicative)
  • direction (towards the speaker)

In the verbal morphology, tense, mood and number (of the subject) are marked by suffixes, person (of the subject, and of the object with transitive verbs) by prefixes. Prefixes can also reduce a transitive verb's argument valency, making it either unaccusative (ta-) or unergative (mu-). A further prefix ((w)al-) adds a directional meaning component (roughly 'towards the speaker') to some verbs, though no longer productively. Derivational suffixes can add to a verb's valency, acting as causatives (which add a new subject as agent) or applicatives (which add a new object as recipient). A few cases of paradigmatic suppletion occur (witz 'comes' : walaj 'came'; ki-uni 'he drinks it (transitive)' : ati 'he drinks (unaccusative)'.

Syntax

[edit]

Word order

[edit]
Word order
  • flexible VO
  • determiner + head
  • quantifier + head
  • adjective + head / head + adjective
  • head + possessor
  • head + relative clause
  • preposition + head

Sentence word order shows considerable flexibility. Pronominal arguments (which may represent subject or object) usually precede the verb, and are only used for emphasis: compare Naja nikita 'I see him/her/it' and Naja nechita 'He/she/it sees me' (naja 'I, me').

Within the noun phrase, determiners and quantifiers precede the head. Adjectives may either precede or follow the head noun. Possessors follow possessed, and relative clauses follow their head. There are prepositional structures.

Head or dependent marking

[edit]
HEAD-marking constructions
  • VERB + subject / subject + VERB
  • VERB + object
  • POSSESSED + possessor
  • RELATIONAL + noun (see below)

There is pro-drop for both subject and object (i.e. subject and object pronouns are omitted in unmarked sentences). One or two arguments of the verb are indexed for person and number in the verb (one when intransitive, two when transitive). A maximum of one object index is possible. If a transitive verb has both a patient ('direct object' in English grammar) and a recipient ('indirect object'), it is the latter which is indexed as object of the Nawat verb, e.g. Nechmaka at 'He gives me water', where nech- indexes a first-person-singular object. There is no marking on the arguments to indicate the various semantic relations so far mentioned. This is a head-marking structure, meaning that relations between a head and dependent are marked on the head (here, the verb) and not on the dependent (here, the subject or object). This is also illustrated by the above examples Naja nikita 'I see him' vs. Naja nechita 'He sees me', where the 'case' of the pronoun naja is determined by the indexing on the verb.

Head-marking patterns are found in other parts of Nawat syntax too, so the language shows consistency for this typological feature. The canonical expression of possessive or genitive relations is parallel: the head (i.e. the possessed) is marked with indices indicating the person of the dependent (the possessor), the noun phrase expressing which is either omitted normally if pronominal (a pro-drop phenomenon) or occurs as a noun phrase following the head and unmarked for the possessive relation, e.g. nu-nan 'my mother', i-nan 'his mother', i-nan ne kunet 'the child's mother'.

Argument valency restrictions

[edit]

Another kind of typological consistency between verbal and nominal constructions is seen in the fact that just as verbs may be classified as intransitives, which do not require an object, and transitives, which require one, so some nouns in Nawat need not have a possessor while others must have one.

Some nouns change their form depending on whether they have a possessor or not, alternating between absolute and construct states, such as absolute kunet, construct -kunew 'child'; this is reminiscent of how verbs may change their forms depending on their transitivity (i.e. whether they take an object or not), e.g. intransitive waki, transitive -watza 'dry', transitive miki 'die', transitive -miktia 'kill', etc. Once again, it is changes in the head that determine the nature of the grammatical relation between the head and its dependent(s).

Adpositions

[edit]

Other relations between a verb and its noun phrase complements or adjuncts are expressed by means of a small number of prepositions or relational constructions. The relational construction itself, e.g. nu-wan 'with me', i-wan 'with him/her/it/', i-wan ne kunet 'with the child', is internally just like possessive constructions, with wan playing the same role as a construct noun.

Predication and sentence types

[edit]

There is no general copula; instead, a nominal (or other non-verbal) predicate with no verbal component may be made a clause's grammatical nucleus. Some of these take subject indices just like verbal predicates, but tense can only be expressed periphrastically in such sentences.

Negation is achieved by placing a negative particle in front of the predicate. Yes–no questions have no special grammatical marking, while wh-questions are identified by the presence of a question word, which usually precedes the verb (or other predicate).

Subordinate clauses are either introduced by a subordinator in clause-initial position or else are juxtaposed with no subordinating conjunction.

See also

[edit]

Bibliography

[edit]
  • Campbell, Lyle (1985). The Pipil language of El Salvador. Mouton Grammar Library (No. 1). Berlin: Mouton Publishers. ISBN 0-89925-040-8 (U.S.), ISBN 3-11-010344-3.
  • Campbell, Lyle, Terrence Kaufman and Thomas C. Smith-Stark (1986). "Meso-America as a Linguistic Area." Language 62:3, p. 530–570.