Jump to content

Disruptive eye mask: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
top: analysed
History: Updated citation
Tags: Mobile edit Mobile web edit
 
(44 intermediate revisions by 14 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{good article}}
[[File:Bitis gabonica rhinoceros.JPG|thumb|upright=1.5|Gaboon viper ''[[Bitis gabonica]]'', its eye concealed by a disruptive mask]]
{{short description|Camouflage to conceal the eye}}
'''Disruptive eye masks''' camouflage the [[eye]]s of a variety of animals, both invertebrates such as [[grasshopper]]s and [[vertebrate]]s including [[fish]], [[frog]]s, [[birds]] and [[snake]]s. The eye has a distinctive shape and dark coloration dictated by its function, and it is housed in the vulnerable head, making it a natural target for [[predator]]s. It can be [[camouflage]]d by a suitable [[disruptive coloration|disruptive pattern]] arranged to run up to or through the eye, sometimes forming an eyestripe. The illusion is sometimes completed by the provision of a [[eyespot (mimicry)|false eye]] or head somewhere else on the body, in a form of [[automimicry]].
[[File:Bitis gabonica rhinoceros.JPG|thumb|upright=1.35|[[Bitis gabonica|Gaboon viper]], its eye concealed by a disruptive mask]]


'''Disruptive eye masks''' are [[camouflage]] markings that conceal the eyes of an animal from its [[predator]]s or prey. They are used by prey, to avoid being seen by predators, and by predators to help them approach their prey.
Eye masks were first noticed by the artist [[Abbott Handerson Thayer]] in 1909, and analysed extensively by the zoologist [[Hugh Cott]] in 1940. However, in 2005 [[Tim Caro]] could still observe that the assumption that eye masks served as camouflage had not been tested systematically.

The eye has a distinctive shape and dark coloration dictated by its function, and it is housed in the vulnerable head, making it a natural target for predators. It can be camouflaged by a suitable [[disruptive coloration|disruptive pattern]] arranged to run up to or through the eye, sometimes forming a '''camouflage eyestripe'''.<!--redirects here--> The illusion is completed in some animals by a [[eyespot (mimicry)|false eye]] or false head somewhere else on the body, in a form of [[automimicry]].

Disruptive eye masks are seen on a variety of animals, both invertebrates such as [[grasshopper]]s and [[vertebrate]]s, including [[fish]], [[frog]]s, [[birds]] and [[snake]]s. Eye masks were first noticed by the American artist [[Abbott Handerson Thayer]] in 1909, and analysed extensively by the zoologist [[Hugh Cott]] in 1940. However, in 2005 the evolutionary zoologist [[Tim Caro]] could still observe that the assumption that eye masks served as camouflage had not been tested systematically.


==History==
==History==
[[File:Little ringed plover - chick.jpg|thumb|Early investigators [[Abbott Handerson Thayer]] and [[Hugh Cott]] noted that [[nidifugous|active juvenile birds]] like this [[little ringed plover]] chick have boldly disruptive patterns that camouflage the eye.]]
[[File:Cott's_diagram_illustrating_the_inherent_conspicuousness_of_an_eye-spot.jpg|thumb|Cott used this diagram to illustrate "the inherent conspicuousness of an eye-spot", and hence to justify the need for a disruptive eye mask.{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=82}}]]


The artist [[Abbott Handerson Thayer]] mentioned the "masking" of the eyes of birds and mammals in his 1909 book ''[[Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom]]'', stating that this was found mainly in birds, such as plovers, and predatory mamamls. He noted that "it is very effective .. as it completely breaks the eye's otherwise conspicuous circular or oval outline.{{sfn|Thayer|1909|pages=81–82}}
The American artist [[Abbott Handerson Thayer]] mentioned the "masking" of the eyes of birds and mammals in his 1909 book ''[[Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom]]'', stating that this was found mainly in birds, such as [[plover]]s, and predatory mammals. He noted that "it is very effective .. as it completely breaks the eye's otherwise conspicuous circular or oval outline.{{sfn|Thayer|1909|pages=81–82}}


The zoologist [[Hugh Cott]] identified the value of concealing the eye in his 1940 book ''[[Adaptive Coloration in Animals]]''. He notes the "inherent conspicuousness of an eye-spot", which "stands out from everything else, and rivets the attention."{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=82}} Therefore, he argues, "no scheme of camouflage will be completely effective which does not mask or modify the appearance of the eye".{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=82}} He mentions, as "beautiful examples" of face patterns that achieve this, the swamp viper ''[[Proatheris superciliaris]]'' and the Gaboon viper ''[[Bitis gabonica]]''.{{sfn|Cott|1940|pages=72–73}} In his words:
The zoologist [[Hugh Cott]] identified the value of concealing the eye in his 1940 book ''[[Adaptive Coloration in Animals]]''. He notes the "inherent conspicuousness of an eye-spot", which "stands out from everything else, and rivets the attention", making the point with a diagram containing one small eyespot and many larger features: the eyespot immediately attracts the viewer's attention.{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=82}} The image has been used elsewhere, for example in Tim Newark's 2007 book on [[camouflage]], where Newark noted that Cott's image proved the point, as "the eye of a vertebrate, with its dense black pupil, stands out from the most jumbled backgrounds, as Cott's illustration demonstrates."<ref>{{cite book |last1=Newark |first1=Tim |title=Camouflage |date=2007 |publisher=Thames & Hudson |isbn=978-0-500-51347-7 |page=33}}</ref>
Cott argued that "no scheme of camouflage will be completely effective which does not mask or modify the appearance of the eye".{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=82}} He mentioned, as "beautiful examples" of face patterns that achieve this, the [[Proatheris superciliaris|swamp viper]] and the [[Bitis gabonica|Gaboon viper]].{{sfn|Cott|1940|pages=72–73}} In his words:


{{quote|The disruptive value of a pattern lies in its tendency to hide the real form of an animal by suggesting a false form to the eye. So long as the false configuration is recognized in preference to the real one, concealment will be effected.{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=70}}}}
{{quote|The disruptive value of a pattern lies in its tendency to hide the real form of an animal by suggesting a false form to the eye. So long as the false configuration is recognized in preference to the real one, concealment will be effected.{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=70}}}}


Cott described disruptive eye masks as a special case of a [[coincident disruptive pattern]], one that provides camouflage by joining together parts of the body to create a new appearance which contradicts the actual structures present.{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=83}} He noted that "more or less well-defined ocular bands or stripes" are found in many species of bird, including the [[nuthatch]], [[snipe]], [[whimbrel]], [[ringed plover]], and [[turnstone]], and thought it significant that these patterns were associated with active [[nidifugous]] young, as in the ringed plover. He recorded that "what appear to be markings of similar significance" are found in [[mammal]]s such as [[gemsbok]], [[sable antelope]], [[Grant's gazelle]] and [[vizcacha]].{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=88}}
Cott described disruptive eye masks as a special case of a [[coincident disruptive pattern]], one that provides camouflage by joining together parts of the body to create a new appearance which contradicts the actual structures present.{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=83}} On camouflage eyestripes, he noted that "more or less well-defined ocular bands or stripes" are found in many species of bird, including the [[nuthatch]], [[snipe]], [[Eurasian whimbrel|whimbrel]], [[ringed plover]], and [[turnstone]], and thought it significant that these patterns were associated with active young [[nidifugous|that leave the nest early]], as in the ringed plover. He recorded that "what appear to be markings of similar significance" are found in [[mammal]]s such as [[gemsbok]], [[sable antelope]], [[Grant's gazelle]] and [[vizcacha]].{{sfn|Cott|1940|page=88}}

In 1989, [[J. L. Cloudsley-Thompson]] noted that camouflage eyestripes are also found in many reptiles including [[Oxybelis|slender arboreal vine snakes]], numerous tropical fish such as the angelfish ''[[Pterophyllum scalare]]'' and the gar ''[[Lepisosteus platostomus]]'', and a wide variety of amphibians including the [[common frog]].<ref>{{cite journal |last=Cloudsley-Thompson |first=J. L. |authorlink=J. L. Cloudsley-Thompson |title=Some Aspects of Camouflage in Animals (review article) |journal=Qatar University Science Bulletin |date=1989 |issue=9 |pages=141–158 |url=http://qspace.qu.edu.qa/bitstream/handle/10576/9697/Some%20aspects%20of%20camouflage%20in%20animals.pdf?sequence=10|access-date=6 January 2024|via=Qatar University Digital Hub}}</ref>

The evolutionary zoologist [[Tim Caro]] observed in 2005 that "the whole topic of disruptive coloration needs systematic analysis".{{sfn|Caro|2005|page=61}} Caro noted that in mammals, "no systematic tests of this idea are available", but that dark patches around the eyes, which would tend to draw attention to the eye instead of camouflaging it, are associated with grassland and terrestrial carnivores as well as [[riparian zone|riparian]] animals, suggesting the function of reducing glare, or perhaps of [[aposematism]].{{sfn|Caro|2005|page=61}}


==Pattern==
==Pattern==


G. W. Barlow, noting Cott's examples, analysed fish "eye-lines", finding a relationship between angle of line and both body shape and angle of forehead. He found that fast-swimming species had longitudinal lines and long bodies; deep-bodied fish had vertical bars and the ability to turn abruptly. Many barred patterns were in his opinion "obviously an adaptation for crypsis". He concluded that stripes and bars were both [[social signal]]s and [[antipredator adaptation]]s.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Barlow |first1=G. W. |title=The attitude of fish eye-lines in relation to body shape and to stripes and bars |journal=Copeia |date=1972 |volume=1972 |pages=4–12 |jstor=1442777}}</ref>
G. W. Barlow, noting Cott's examples, analysed fish "eye-lines" in 1972, finding a relationship between angle of line and both body shape and angle of forehead. He found that fast-swimming species had longitudinal lines and long bodies; deep-bodied fish had vertical bars and the ability to turn abruptly. Many barred patterns were in his opinion "obviously an adaptation for crypsis" (camouflage). He concluded that stripes and bars were both [[social signal]]s and [[antipredator adaptation]]s.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Barlow |first1=G. W. |title=The attitude of fish eye-lines in relation to body shape and to stripes and bars |journal=Copeia |date=1972 |volume=1972 |issue=1 |pages=4–12 |jstor=1442777 |doi=10.2307/1442777 }}</ref>


Leah and Benjamin Gavish tested patterns that conceal birds' eyes using patterns and human observers. They found that patterns which allow the eye to protrude from the dark area concealed the eye best, calling this the "borderline eye effect".<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gavish |first1=Leah |last2=Gavish |first2=Benjamin |title=Patterns that conceal a bird's eye |journal=Z. Tierpsychol. |date=1981 |volume=56 |pages=193–204 |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228056109_Patterns_that_Conceal_a_Bird%27s_Eye}}</ref>
In 1981, Leah and Benjamin Gavish tested patterns that conceal birds' eyes using patterns and human observers. They found that patterns which allow the eye to protrude from the dark area concealed the eye best, calling this the "borderline eye effect".<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gavish |first1=Leah |last2=Gavish |first2=Benjamin |title=Patterns that conceal a bird's eye |journal=Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie |date=1981 |volume=56 |issue=3 |pages=193–204 |doi=10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01296.x |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228056109}}</ref>


Some animals such as [[butterflyfish]] combine the camouflaging of the <!--real -->eye with an [[eyespot (mimicry)|eyespot]] somewhere else on the body, possibly giving the impression that the animal's [[Automimicry|head is located there]].<ref name="Machin2014">{{cite book |last=Machin |first=David |title=Visual Communication |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=WFXnBQAAQBAJ&pg=PA668 |year=2014 |publisher=De Gruyter|isbn=978-3-11-025549-2 |page=668}}</ref> In 2013, Karin Kjernsmo and Sami Merilaita showed using artificial prey and predatory fish ([[three-spined stickleback]]s) that such eyespots diverted predators' attacks from the vulnerable head.<ref name="Kjernsmo Merilaita 2013">{{cite journal |last1=Kjernsmo |first1=Karin |last2=Merilaita |first2=Sami |title=Eyespots divert attacks by fish |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |volume=280 |issue=1766 |year=2013 |pages=20131458 |issn=0962-8452 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2013.1458|pmid=23864602 |pmc=3730605 |doi-access=free }}</ref>
The evolutionary zoologist [[Tim Caro]] observed in 2005 that "the whole topic of disruptive coloration needs systematic analysis".{{sfn|Caro|2005|page=61}} Caro noted that in mammals, "no systematic tests of this idea are available", but that dark patches around the eyes, which would tend to draw attention to the eye instead of camouflaging it, are associated with grassland and terrestrial carnivores as well as [[riparian zone|riparian]] animals, suggesting the function of reducing glare, or perhaps of [[aposematism]].{{sfn|Caro|2005|page=61}}


<gallery mode=packed>
<gallery mode=packed>
File:Elegant Grass-mimicking Grasshopper (Leptacris elegans) (14008906395) (cropped).jpg|Elegant grass-mimicking [[grasshopper]], ''[[Leptacris elegans]]'', has stripes that join head and eye to the body
File:Elegant Grass-mimicking Grasshopper (Leptacris elegans) (14008906395) (cropped).jpg|[[Leptacris|Elegant grass-mimicking grasshopper]] has stripes that join head and eye to the body
File:Equetus lanceolatus in Madagascar Reef.jpg|The jack-knifefish, ''[[Equetus lanceolatus]]'',<!--see Barlow 1972--> has a strongly disruptive pattern on body and through the eye.
File:Equetus lanceolatus in Madagascar Reef.jpg|The [[Equetus lanceolatus|jack-knifefish]]<!--see Barlow 1972--> has a strongly disruptive pattern on body and through the eye.
<!--File:Bep chaetodon oxycephalus.jpg|The [[eye]] of ''[[Chaetodon oxycephalus]]'', the spot-nape butterflyfish, is concealed by a bold eyestripe.-->
File:Bep chaetodon oxycephalus.jpg|The eye of the [[Chaetodon oxycephalus|spot-nape butterflyfish]] is concealed by a bold eyestripe.
<!--File:Four-Eye Butterflyfish.jpg|In ''[[Chaetodon capistratus]]'', the four-eye butterflyfish, eyestripe camouflage is combined with a conspicuous [[eyespot (mimicry)|eyespot]] near the tail. -->
File:Four-Eye Butterflyfish.jpg|In the [[Chaetodon capistratus|four-eye butterflyfish]], eyestripe camouflage is combined with a conspicuous [[eyespot (mimicry)|eyespot]] near the tail.
File:Tail-spot butterflyfish 8 (cropped).jpg|''[[Chaetodon ocellicaudus]]'', the tail-spot butterflyfish is conspicuously coloured, but its eye is camouflaged and the tail has a [[automimicry|false eye]], distracting predators' attention from the head.
File:Tail-spot butterflyfish 8 (cropped).jpg|The [[Chaetodon ocellicaudus|tail-spot butterflyfish]] is conspicuously coloured, but its eye is camouflaged and the tail has a [[automimicry|false eye]], adding to the illusion by distracting predators' attention from the head.
File:2014.07.17.-28-Zadlitzgraben Pressel--Grasfrosch-Weibchen.jpg|''[[Rana temporaria]]'', the common [[frog]], has a [[disruptively patterned]] body and an eyestripe concealing the eye.
File:2014.07.17.-28-Zadlitzgraben Pressel--Grasfrosch-Weibchen.jpg|The [[Rana temporaria|common frog]] has a [[disruptively patterned]] body and an eyestripe concealing the eye.
File:Little ringed plover - chick.jpg|[[Little ringed plover]] chick is boldly disruptive with a pattern that obliterates the eye at a short distance.
File:Sitta europaea wildlife 2 1.jpg|The [[Eurasian nuthatch]] has a stripe joining the beak, eye, and body
File:Oxybelis aeneus (detail).jpg|The [[Oxybelis aeneus|Mexican vine snake]] has a dark, strongly contrasting eyestripe to conceal the eye.
File:Sitta europaea wildlife 2 1.jpg|[[Eurasian nuthatch]] has an stripe joining the beak, eye, and body
File:Oxybelis aeneus (detail).jpg|''[[Oxybelis aeneus]]'', the Mexican vine [[snake]], has a dark, strongly contrasting eyestripe to conceal the eye.
File:Oryx gazella - Etosha 2014.jpg|[[Gemsbok]] has a disruptive facial mask that obscures the eye.
File:Oryx gazella - Etosha 2014.jpg|[[Gemsbok]] has a disruptive facial mask that obscures the eye.
</gallery>
</gallery>
Line 39: Line 50:
{{reflist|30em}}
{{reflist|30em}}


==Sources==
==Bibliography==

{{Spoken Wikipedia|date=2023-03-11|Disruptive Eye Mask.ogg}}
* {{cite book |authorlink=Tim Caro |last=Caro |first=Tim |title=Antipredator Defenses in Birds and Mammals |date=2005 |publisher=University of Chicago Press }}
* {{cite book |authorlink=Hugh B. Cott |last=Cott |first=Hugh B. |title=Adaptive Coloration in Animals |date=1940 |publisher=Oxford University Press |url=https://archive.org/details/adaptivecolorati00cott }}
* {{cite book |last=Thayer |first=Abbott Handerson |authorlink=Abbott Handerson Thayer |year=1909 |title=Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom |publisher=Macmillan |url=https://archive.org/details/cu31924022546406 }}


{{Camouflage}}
* {{cite book |authorlink=Tim Caro |last=Caro |first=Tim |title=Antipredator Defenses in Birds and Mammals |date=2005 |publisher=University of Chicago Press |ref=harv}}
* {{cite book |authorlink=Hugh B. Cott |last=Cott |first=Hugh B. |title=Adaptive Coloration in Animals |date=1940 |publisher=Oxford University Press |url=https://archive.org/details/adaptivecolorati00cott |ref=harv}}
* {{cite book |last=Thayer |first=Abbott Handerson |authorlink=Abbott Handerson Thayer |year=1909 |title=Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom |publisher=Macmillan |url=https://archive.org/details/cu31924022546406 |ref=harv}}


[[Category:Camouflage]]
[[Category:Disruptive coloration]]
[[Category:Eye]]

Latest revision as of 03:39, 7 January 2024

Gaboon viper, its eye concealed by a disruptive mask

Disruptive eye masks are camouflage markings that conceal the eyes of an animal from its predators or prey. They are used by prey, to avoid being seen by predators, and by predators to help them approach their prey.

The eye has a distinctive shape and dark coloration dictated by its function, and it is housed in the vulnerable head, making it a natural target for predators. It can be camouflaged by a suitable disruptive pattern arranged to run up to or through the eye, sometimes forming a camouflage eyestripe. The illusion is completed in some animals by a false eye or false head somewhere else on the body, in a form of automimicry.

Disruptive eye masks are seen on a variety of animals, both invertebrates such as grasshoppers and vertebrates, including fish, frogs, birds and snakes. Eye masks were first noticed by the American artist Abbott Handerson Thayer in 1909, and analysed extensively by the zoologist Hugh Cott in 1940. However, in 2005 the evolutionary zoologist Tim Caro could still observe that the assumption that eye masks served as camouflage had not been tested systematically.

History

[edit]
Early investigators Abbott Handerson Thayer and Hugh Cott noted that active juvenile birds like this little ringed plover chick have boldly disruptive patterns that camouflage the eye.
Cott used this diagram to illustrate "the inherent conspicuousness of an eye-spot", and hence to justify the need for a disruptive eye mask.[1]

The American artist Abbott Handerson Thayer mentioned the "masking" of the eyes of birds and mammals in his 1909 book Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom, stating that this was found mainly in birds, such as plovers, and predatory mammals. He noted that "it is very effective .. as it completely breaks the eye's otherwise conspicuous circular or oval outline.[2]

The zoologist Hugh Cott identified the value of concealing the eye in his 1940 book Adaptive Coloration in Animals. He notes the "inherent conspicuousness of an eye-spot", which "stands out from everything else, and rivets the attention", making the point with a diagram containing one small eyespot and many larger features: the eyespot immediately attracts the viewer's attention.[1] The image has been used elsewhere, for example in Tim Newark's 2007 book on camouflage, where Newark noted that Cott's image proved the point, as "the eye of a vertebrate, with its dense black pupil, stands out from the most jumbled backgrounds, as Cott's illustration demonstrates."[3] Cott argued that "no scheme of camouflage will be completely effective which does not mask or modify the appearance of the eye".[1] He mentioned, as "beautiful examples" of face patterns that achieve this, the swamp viper and the Gaboon viper.[4] In his words:

The disruptive value of a pattern lies in its tendency to hide the real form of an animal by suggesting a false form to the eye. So long as the false configuration is recognized in preference to the real one, concealment will be effected.[5]

Cott described disruptive eye masks as a special case of a coincident disruptive pattern, one that provides camouflage by joining together parts of the body to create a new appearance which contradicts the actual structures present.[6] On camouflage eyestripes, he noted that "more or less well-defined ocular bands or stripes" are found in many species of bird, including the nuthatch, snipe, whimbrel, ringed plover, and turnstone, and thought it significant that these patterns were associated with active young that leave the nest early, as in the ringed plover. He recorded that "what appear to be markings of similar significance" are found in mammals such as gemsbok, sable antelope, Grant's gazelle and vizcacha.[7]

In 1989, J. L. Cloudsley-Thompson noted that camouflage eyestripes are also found in many reptiles including slender arboreal vine snakes, numerous tropical fish such as the angelfish Pterophyllum scalare and the gar Lepisosteus platostomus, and a wide variety of amphibians including the common frog.[8]

The evolutionary zoologist Tim Caro observed in 2005 that "the whole topic of disruptive coloration needs systematic analysis".[9] Caro noted that in mammals, "no systematic tests of this idea are available", but that dark patches around the eyes, which would tend to draw attention to the eye instead of camouflaging it, are associated with grassland and terrestrial carnivores as well as riparian animals, suggesting the function of reducing glare, or perhaps of aposematism.[9]

Pattern

[edit]

G. W. Barlow, noting Cott's examples, analysed fish "eye-lines" in 1972, finding a relationship between angle of line and both body shape and angle of forehead. He found that fast-swimming species had longitudinal lines and long bodies; deep-bodied fish had vertical bars and the ability to turn abruptly. Many barred patterns were in his opinion "obviously an adaptation for crypsis" (camouflage). He concluded that stripes and bars were both social signals and antipredator adaptations.[10]

In 1981, Leah and Benjamin Gavish tested patterns that conceal birds' eyes using patterns and human observers. They found that patterns which allow the eye to protrude from the dark area concealed the eye best, calling this the "borderline eye effect".[11]

Some animals such as butterflyfish combine the camouflaging of the eye with an eyespot somewhere else on the body, possibly giving the impression that the animal's head is located there.[12] In 2013, Karin Kjernsmo and Sami Merilaita showed using artificial prey and predatory fish (three-spined sticklebacks) that such eyespots diverted predators' attacks from the vulnerable head.[13]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b c Cott 1940, p. 82.
  2. ^ Thayer 1909, pp. 81–82.
  3. ^ Newark, Tim (2007). Camouflage. Thames & Hudson. p. 33. ISBN 978-0-500-51347-7.
  4. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 72–73.
  5. ^ Cott 1940, p. 70.
  6. ^ Cott 1940, p. 83.
  7. ^ Cott 1940, p. 88.
  8. ^ Cloudsley-Thompson, J. L. (1989). "Some Aspects of Camouflage in Animals (review article)" (PDF). Qatar University Science Bulletin (9): 141–158. Retrieved 6 January 2024 – via Qatar University Digital Hub.
  9. ^ a b Caro 2005, p. 61.
  10. ^ Barlow, G. W. (1972). "The attitude of fish eye-lines in relation to body shape and to stripes and bars". Copeia. 1972 (1): 4–12. doi:10.2307/1442777. JSTOR 1442777.
  11. ^ Gavish, Leah; Gavish, Benjamin (1981). "Patterns that conceal a bird's eye". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 56 (3): 193–204. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01296.x.
  12. ^ Machin, David (2014). Visual Communication. De Gruyter. p. 668. ISBN 978-3-11-025549-2.
  13. ^ Kjernsmo, Karin; Merilaita, Sami (2013). "Eyespots divert attacks by fish". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 280 (1766): 20131458. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.1458. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 3730605. PMID 23864602.

Bibliography

[edit]
Listen to this article (9 minutes)
Spoken Wikipedia icon
This audio file was created from a revision of this article dated 11 March 2023 (2023-03-11), and does not reflect subsequent edits.