Jump to content

Self-domestication: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m v2.05b - Bot T20 CW#61 - Fix errors for CW project (Reference before punctuation)
 
(38 intermediate revisions by 20 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Scientific hypothesis in ethnobiology}}
'''Self-domestication''' is the process of [[adaptation]] of for example [[wild animals]] to cohabiting with [[human]]s, without direct human [[selective breeding]] of the animals. The human self-domestication hypothesis argues that, like mammalian domesticates, humans have gone through a process of selection against aggression – a process that in the case of humans was self-induced, in favor of social behavior from which the group as a whole benefited, such as intelligence, soft skills, emotional intelligence and where individuals with an [[antisocial personality disorder]] would be eliminated by the group. For this to happen, sophisticated language was necessary to plot against the bully or individual with excessive aggressive behavior, so one would not be killed themselves. It is hypothesized that this is what differentiated [[Homo erectus]] and [[Homo neanderthalensis]] from ''H. sapiens'': the ability of sophisticated language, allowing better social collaboration, elimination of excessive aggressive behavior in the group, leading to self-domestication and could explain why only homo sapiens survived from all the hominae.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Shilton |first1=D |last2=Breski |first2=M |last3=Dor |first3=D |last4=Jablonka |first4=E |date=February 14, 2020 |title=Human Social Evolution: Self-Domestication or Self-Control? |journal=Frontiers in Psychology |volume=11 |page=134 |doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00134 |pmid=32116937 |pmc=7033472 |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=acOZT240bTA&ab_channel=UniversityofCaliforniaTelevision%28UCTV%29 Harvard Prof Richard Wrangham</ref>
[[File:Human self-domestication and DNA.jpg|thumb|Experiment conducted by the [[University of Barcelona]] to demonstrate the hypothesis of self-domestication.<ref name="eaaw7908">{{Cite journal |last1=Zanella |first1=Matteo |last2=Vitriolo |first2=Alessandro |last3=Andirko |first3=Alejandro |last4=Martins |first4=Pedro Tiago |last5=Sturm |first5=Stefanie |last6=O’Rourke |first6=Thomas |last7=Laugsch |first7=Magdalena |last8=Malerba |first8=Natascia |last9=Skaros |first9=Adrianos |last10=Trattaro |first10=Sebastiano |last11=Germain |first11=Pierre-Luc |last12=Mihailovic |first12=Marija |last13=Merla |first13=Giuseppe |last14=Rada-Iglesias |first14=Alvaro |last15=Boeckx |first15=Cedric |date=2019-12-06 |title=Dosage analysis of the 7q11.23 Williams region identifies BAZ1B as a major human gene patterning the modern human face and underlying self-domestication |journal=Science Advances |language=en |volume=5 |issue=12 |at=eaaw7908 |doi=10.1126/sciadv.aaw7908 |issn=2375-2548 |pmc=6892627 |pmid=31840056|bibcode=2019SciA....5.7908Z }}</ref>]]
Dogs and cats have undergone this kind of self-domestication. Self-domestication also refers to the [[evolution]] of [[hominid]]s, particularly humans<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Theofanopoulou|first1=Constantina|last2=Gastaldon|first2=Simone|last3=O’Rourke|first3=Thomas|last4=Samuels|first4=Bridget D.|last5=Messner|first5=Angela|last6=Martins|first6=Pedro Tiago|last7=Delogu|first7=Francesco|last8=Alamri|first8=Saleh|last9=Boeckx|first9=Cedric|date=2017-10-18|title=Self-domestication in Homo sapiens: Insights from comparative genomics|journal=PLOS ONE|language=en|volume=12|issue=10|pages=e0185306|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0185306|issn=1932-6203|pmc=5646786|pmid=29045412|bibcode=2017PLoSO..1285306T|doi-access=free}}</ref> and [[bonobo]]s, toward [[Sociality|collaborative]], docile behavior. As described by British biological anthropologist [[Richard Wrangham]], self-domestication involves being in an environment that favors reduction in aggression, including interspecific and intraspecific antagonism, for survival.<ref name="Wrangham2">{{cite book|last1=Wrangham|first1=Richard|url=https://archive.org/details/newhumanistsscie00broc/page/99|title=The New Humanists: Science at the Edge|publisher=Sterling Publishing|year=2003|isbn=978-0-7607-4529-8|editor1-last=Brockman|editor1-first=John|pages=[https://archive.org/details/newhumanistsscie00broc/page/99 99–110]|chapter=The Evolution of Cooking|chapter-url={{Google books|Fb0EGAq5UmcC|page=99|plainurl=yes}}}}</ref> [[Spandrel (biology)|Spandrels]], or evolutionary byproducts, also accompany self-domestication, including depigmentation, [[arrested development]], and reduced [[sexual dimorphism]].


'''Self-domestication''' is a [[scientific hypothesis]] that suggests that, similar to [[Domestication|domesticated animals]], there has been a process of [[Artificial Selection|artificial selection]] among members of the human species conducted by humans themselves.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Theofanopoulou|first1=Constantina|last2=Gastaldon|first2=Simone|last3=O’Rourke|first3=Thomas|last4=Samuels|first4=Bridget D.|last5=Messner|first5=Angela|last6=Martins|first6=Pedro Tiago|last7=Delogu|first7=Francesco|last8=Alamri|first8=Saleh|last9=Boeckx|first9=Cedric|date=2017-10-18|title=Self-domestication in Homo sapiens: Insights from comparative genomics|journal=PLOS ONE|language=en|volume=12|issue=10|pages=e0185306|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0185306|issn=1932-6203|pmc=5646786|pmid=29045412|bibcode=2017PLoSO..1285306T |doi-access=free }}</ref> In this way, during the process of [[hominization]], a preference for individuals with collaborative and social behaviors would have been shown to optimize the benefit of the entire group: docility, language, and [[emotional intelligence]] would have been enhanced during this process of [[Artificial Selection|artificial selection]]. The hypothesis is raised that this is what differentiated ''[[Homo sapiens]]'' from ''[[Homo neanderthalensis]]'' and ''[[Homo erectus]]''.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Shilton |first1=D |last2=Breski |first2=M |last3=Dor |first3=D |last4=Jablonka |first4=E |date=February 14, 2020 |title=Human Social Evolution: Self-Domestication or Self-Control? |journal=Frontiers in Psychology |volume=11 |page=134 |doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00134 |pmid=32116937 |pmc=7033472 |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=acOZT240bTA&ab_channel=UniversityofCaliforniaTelevision%28UCTV%29 Harvard Prof Richard Wrangham</ref>
==In animals==
Wild animals may self-domesticate when less aggressive behavior enhances their survival in the vicinity of human beings. This facilitates their ability to take advantage of increased food availability arising from domestic niches. Alternatively, when occurring in non-human environments, self-domestication may be favored by prosociality, as traits arising from self-domestication lead to stronger social structures. An environment that supports the survival of self-domesticated animals can lead to other apparent changes in behavior and appearance that deviate from their wild phenotypes. These traits include, but are not limited to, depigmentation, floppy ears, curly tails, smaller teeth, smaller cranial anatomy, juvenile behavior, reduced sexual dimorphism, and arrested development.<ref name=":5">Carlos A. Driscoll, David W. Macdonald, & Stephen J. O'Brien. (2009). From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 106(Supplement_1), 9971-9978.</ref> Smaller skulls, increased playfulness, and reduced aggression have also been observed in self-domesticated species.


== Origin and status of the hypothesis ==
===Cats===
In general, domesticated animals possess common characteristics that differentiate them from their non-domesticated counterparts (for example, in the case of ''[[Canis familiaris]]'' compared to their relatives, ''[[Canis lupus]]'', among many other cases): they tend to be more docile and playful, exhibit less aggressive behavior, and show marked [[Neoteny in humans|neoteny]], often resulting in a smaller body, a slightly smaller brain and skull, as well as shorter teeth and snout.<ref name=":0">{{Cite book |last=Sauer |first=Hanno |title=La invención del bien y del mal |publisher=Paidós |year=2023 |isbn=9788449340963}}</ref>
As grain plants and livestock became domesticated 9,000 years ago in the [[Fertile Crescent]], hunter-gatherers built urban villages. After a 100,000-year history of nomadism, these hunter-gatherers transitioned to adopting a sedentary lifestyle. Though many societies domesticated barnyard animals for food resources—an example of artificial selection—villagers had little desire or motivation to domesticate wildcats to be house pets. Instead, wildcats, such as the species ''[[Felis lybica]]'', began exploiting new resources offered by human environments, such as a proliferation of rodents in grain stores. These cats were tolerated by people, supporting their natural evolution to deviate further from their wild counterparts. This favored the perpetuation of reduced aggressive behavior and increased “tameness,” which made the cats increasingly tolerable in human society.<ref name=":5" /><ref>Yamaguchi, Nobuyuki, Kitchener, Andrew C, Driscoll, Carlos A, Ward, Jennifer M, & Macdonald, David W. (2004). Craniological differentiation amongst wild-living cats in Britain and southern Africa: Natural variation or the effects of hybridisation? Animal Conservation, 7(4), 339-351.</ref>


One of the first to scientifically observe that humans present similar traits was the naturalist, anthropologist, and physician [[Johann Friedrich Blumenbach]] around 1800.<ref>{{Cite news |last=Hawks |first=John |title=The Goodness Paradox" Review: The Benefits of Good Breeding (book review) |edition=Wall Street Journal |publication-date=2019 |url=https://www.wsj.com/articles/the-goodness-paradox-review-good-breeding-11548427524}}</ref> The author of the thesis "De generis humani varietate nativa" ('On the natural variations in the human lineage') consequently proposed the hypothesis that humans could have been domesticated.
===Dogs===
{{See also|Origin of the domestic dog}}
Noticing that a dog's skull looks like that of a juvenile wolf, [[Richard Wrangham]] suggested that this species could self-domesticate. While some humans may have intentionally domesticated [[Wolf|wolves]] into [[dog]]s, this alternate hypothesis states that wolves effectively domesticated themselves by establishing a [[Mutualism (biology)|mutually beneficial]] relationship with prehistoric humans. They scavenged on the remains of the prey animals left by the prehistoric people at the human settlements or the kill sites. Those wolves that were less anxious and aggressive thrived, continued to follow the prehistoric humans, and colonized the human-dominated environments, generation after generation. Gradually, the first primitive dogs emerged from this group.<ref name="crockford20002">{{cite book|author=Crockford, S.|title=A commentary on dog evolution: Regional variation, breed development and hybridization with wolves|publisher=Archaeopress BAR International Series 889|year=2000|isbn=978-1841710891|editor=Crockford, S.|pages=11–20}}</ref><ref name="coppinger20012">{{cite book|author=Coppinger, R.|url=https://archive.org/details/dogsstartlingnew00raym|title=Dogs: A Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin, Behavior & Evolution|year=2001|isbn=978-0684855301}}{{page needed|date=March 2017}}</ref><ref name="russell20122">{{cite book|author=Russell, N.|title=Social Zooarchaeology: Humans and Animals in Prehistory|publisher=Cambridge University Press|year=2012|isbn=978-0-521-14311-0}}{{page needed|date=March 2017}}</ref>


A few years later, [[Charles Darwin]] addressed the topic using the [[theory of evolution]], which already considered the process of [[Selective breeding|artificial selection]] in animals. Unable to explain the concept of human domestication from an exclusively scientific perspective (the question of ''who domesticated humans'' could only be answered in [[Religion|religious]] or [[Theism|theistic]] terms), he eventually dismissed the hypothesis.<ref name=":0" />
===Bonobos (''Pan paniscus'')===
The evolutionary anthropologist [[Brian Hare]] proposed that [[bonobo]]s (''Pan paniscus'') have also undergone self-domestication.<ref name=":0">Hare, Brian, Wobber, Victoria, & Wrangham, Richard. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: Evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83(3), 573-585.</ref> Despite their close relation to chimpanzees, bonobos exhibit significantly lower levels of aggression. Male chimpanzees use intimidating displays to compete for resources, access to mating, and dominance rank. Both female and male chimpanzees may instigate infanticide.<ref>Pusey, A., Murray, C., Wallauer, W., Wilson, M., Wroblewski, E. & Goodall, J. 2008. Severe aggression among female Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology, 29, 949e973.</ref> In comparison, bonobos deliver calm displays, the most aggressive action ever being using branches merely as a prop, never to make physical contact with another bonobo. Females are organized in coalitions, minimizing if not eliminating intimidation by males for mating. Males do not form alliances with other male bonobo; instead, male-female bonobo alliances are prolific, with strong bonds between mother and sons. Intergroup tolerance is much higher in bonobos in contrast to chimpanzees. Additionally, bonobo adults are known to engage in play much more frequently than chimpanzee adults, suggesting that bonobos showcase more juvenilized behavior.<ref>Palagi, E. 2006. Social play in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implications for natural social systems and interindividual relationships. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 129, 418e426.</ref> The cognitive traits that have caused these phenotypic differences to arise are not fully clear; however, cognitive differences between bonobos and chimpanzees have been established in the orbitofrontal cortex, motor cortices, and hippocampus.<ref>Schenker, N., Desgouttes, A. & Semendeferi, K. 2005. Neural connectivity and cortical substrates of cognition in hominoids. Journal of Human Evolution, 49, 547e569.</ref><ref>Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K. & Van Hoesen, G. 1998. Limbic frontal cortex in hominoids: a comparative study of area 13. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 106, 129e155.</ref><ref>Hopkins, W., Lyn, H. & Cantalupo, C. 2009. Volumetric and lateralized differences in selected brain regions of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). American Journal of Primatology, 71, 988e997.</ref> These neural regions are associated with feeding habits, motor coordination, and emotions.<ref name=":0" />


However, the studies of [[Dmitry Belyayev (zoologist)|Dimitri Beliayev]] in the 20th century were important for the proposal: research on the [[Silver fox (animal)|silver fox]] demonstrated that in the process of [[Domestication of animals|animal domestication]], simultaneous changes occurred in behavior (lower levels of adrenaline were observed) and in coat color (alterations in pigmentation): [[adrenaline]] could share a biochemical pathway with [[melanin]], a pathway that would be altered during the process of [[Selective breeding|artificial selection]].<ref name=bestfriend>{{cite web|access-date=23 May 2014|date=6 September 2010|first=Jason|last=Goldman|publisher=Scientific American|title=Man's new best friend? A forgotten Russian experiment in fox domestication|url=http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/guest-blog/2010/09/06/mans-new-best-friend-a-forgotten-russian-experiment-in-fox-domestication/}}<!-- auto-translated by Module:CS1 translator --></ref>
It remains a point of discussion why the mechanism of natural selection has favored self-domestication in bonobos over time. One theory suggests that self-domestication reinforces stable social structures, favoring prosocial behavior; thus, self-domestication has predominantly been motivated by changing intraspecific dynamics. Bonobo groups are more stable than chimpanzee groups, due to their decreased reliance on scramble competition.<ref name=":1">Furuichi, T. 2011. Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society. Evolutionary Anthropology, 20, 131e142</ref> Bonobo social groups consist of a significant percentage of each local community, often 16-21% more inclusive of the total population than chimpanzee groups.<ref name=":1" /> Since female-female coalitions are so strong, intimidating, coercive approaches for mating and high-ranks are not as fruitful; males find greater reproductive success from kinship ties with mothers.<ref name=":0" />


In 2014, scientists Adam Wilkins (from [[Humboldt University of Berlin|Humboldt University]], Berlin), [[Richard Wrangham]] (from [[Harvard University]], Massachusetts), and [[W. Tecumseh Fitch|Tecumseh Fitch]] (from the [[University of Vienna]]) proposed that the common origin of these changes lay in [[neural crest]] cells, exclusive [[Stem cell|stem cells]] of [[Vertebrate|vertebrates]] that migrate to different parts of the body during the [[Embryo|embryonic phase]], giving rise to the [[Adrenal gland|adrenal glands]] (responsible for managing the fear and stress response through [[adrenaline]] production), [[Melanocyte|melanocytes]] (responsible for producing skin or coat [[melanin]]), and jaws simultaneously. The deficit of these cells would explain the common characteristics of all [[Domestication|domesticated animals]]: tameness, cranial and mandibular reduction, and alterations in pigmentation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Wilkins |first1=Adam S |last2=Wrangham |first2=Richard W |last3=Fitch |first3=W Tecumseh |date=2014-07-01 |title=The "Domestication Syndrome" in Mammals: A Unified Explanation Based on Neural Crest Cell Behavior and Genetics |journal=Genetics |language=en |volume=197 |issue=3 |pages=795–808 |doi=10.1534/genetics.114.165423 |issn=1943-2631 |pmc=4096361 |pmid=25024034}}</ref>
In addition to these behavioral observations, morphological evidence supports the hypothesis that bonobos, unlike the closely related chimpanzees, have undergone self-domestication. Bonobos, who also exhibit less aggressive demeanors, have a cranial reduction up to 20%, flattening of facial projection, and diminished sexual dimorphism. Bonobos also have smaller teeth. Their white tail-tufts and pink lips, coloration typically observed in juvenile primates, is persistent in phenotypes of adult bonobos; this depigmentation signals extended periods of juvenilized traits.<ref name=":0" />


Of the three members of the research team, it was primatologist [[Richard Wrangham]] who translated these results to humans, asserting that humans have "domesticated" themselves through a process of self-selection (a proposal he would elaborate in "[[The Goodness Paradox|The Goodness Paradox: The Strange Relationship Between Virtue and Violence in Human Evolution]]").
=== Marmoset monkeys (''Callithrix jacchus'') ===
The neuroscientist [[Asif A. Ghazanfar]] revisited the self-domestication hypothesis in marmoset monkeys, a previously undocumented species in application to the theory.<ref name=":2">Ghazanfar, Asif, Kelly, Lauren, Takahashi, Daniel, Winters, Sandra, Terrett, Rebecca, & Higham, James. (2020). Domestication phenotype linked to vocal behavior in marmoset monkeys.</ref> The study sought to elucidate how affiliative behavior facilitates the development of domestic phenotypes and determine the social underpinnings behind self-domestication's natural selection. So, the researchers identified both an affiliative behavior and a hallmark morphological trait indicating domestication: in marmoset monkeys, these would be vocal exchanges and a species-distinctive white facial fur patch. Their study found that, when marmoset parents provide more vocal feedback to their offspring, juvenile marmosets correspondingly undergo a larger growth of their white facial fur patch. This white facial patch lacked melanocytes, which originate from neural crest cells, suggesting that there exists a pleiotropic linkage with neural crest cells. This is a significant finding in support of the hypothesis, as neural crest cell abundance is directly related to the adrenal gland size. Lower aggression, as arises from self-domestication, also is accompanied by a smaller adrenal gland, due to a decreased urgency to mitigate stressful conditions. A smaller adrenal gland means that there will be fewer neural crest cells, and thereby melanocytes; the phenotypic result will be a reduction in pigmentation, a common byproduct of self-domestication, as is observed in the marmoset monkeys.<ref name=":2" />


In July 2019, a team from the Institute of Marine Sciences of Barcelona described, through the [[DNA methylation|methylation]] of certain genes in DNA, the [[Epigenetics|epigenetic]] and genetic changes through which [[neural crest]] cells were reduced.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Anastasiadi |first1=Dafni |last2=Piferrer |first2=Francesc |date=2019-10-01 |editor-last=Wittkopp |editor-first=Patricia |title=Epimutations in Developmental Genes Underlie the Onset of Domestication in Farmed European Sea Bass |url=https://academic.oup.com/mbe/article/36/10/2252/5525707 |journal=Molecular Biology and Evolution |language=en |volume=36 |issue=10 |pages=2252–2264 |doi=10.1093/molbev/msz153 |pmid=31289822 |pmc=6759067 |issn=0737-4038|doi-access=free }}</ref> Subsequently, another research team from the [[University of Barcelona]] discovered that the [[BAZ1B]] gene controls the behavior of neural crest cells. Comparable with the [[Neanderthal]] genome, BAZ1B is also related to genes that have many [[Mutation|mutations]] not present in the homologous genes of our past [[hominini]].<ref name="eaaw7908" /> Cedric Boeckx, one of the researchers in this study, states:<blockquote>"We believe this means that the genetic network of BAZ1B is an important reason why our face is different compared to other already extinct ancestors, like the Neanderthals [...]. In broad terms, it gives us, for the first time, experimental validation of the autodomestication hypothesis based on the neural crest."<ref>{{Cite news |date=5 December 2019 |title=Universidad de Barcelona |work=Primera demostración experimental de la hipótesis de la autodomesticación del ser humano |url=https://web.ub.edu/es/web/actualitat/enwiki/w/first-experimental-genetic-evidence-of-the-human-self-domestication-hypothesis}}</ref></blockquote>
Ghazanfar's study with marmoset monkeys further substantiated the self-domestication hypothesis, which has also emerged in humans. He proposed that the common denominator, and thus a likely driver and selective pressure of domestication, between both marmosets and humans was cooperative breeding. In marmosets, cooperative breeding was a mating system driven by their production of dizygotic twins, whereas in humans, it may be driven due to the extensive amount of parental care that goes into an offspring's early years of development.<ref name=":2"/>


==In humans==
== Hominids ==

=== Hominids ===
Clark & Henneberg argue that during the earliest stages of human evolution a more [[paedomorphic]] skull arose through self-domestication.<ref name="Clark & Henneberg 20152">{{cite journal|last1=Clark|first1=Gary|last2=Henneberg|first2=Maciej|year=2015|title=The life history of Ardipithecus ramidus: A heterochronic model of sexual and social maturation|journal=Anthropological Review|volume=78|issue=2|pages=109–132|doi=10.1515/anre-2015-0009|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Clark & Henneberg 20172">{{cite journal|last1=Clark|first1=Gary|last2=Henneberg|first2=Maciej|year=2017|title=Ardipithecus ramidus and the evolution of language and singing: An early origin for hominin vocal capability|journal=HOMO: Journal of Comparative Human Biology|volume=68|issue=2|pages=101–121|doi=10.1016/j.jchb.2017.03.001|pmid=28363458}}</ref> This assertion is based upon a comparison of the skull of [[Ardipithecus]] and chimpanzees of various ages. It was found that Ardipithecus clustered with the infant and juvenile species. The consequent lack of a pubertal growth spurt in males of the species and the consequent growth of aggressive canine armoury was taken as evidence that Ardipithecus evolved its paedomorphic skull through self domestication. As the authors state, comparing the species with [[bonobos]]:
Clark & Henneberg argue that during the earliest stages of human evolution a more [[paedomorphic]] skull arose through self-domestication.<ref name="Clark & Henneberg 20152">{{cite journal|last1=Clark|first1=Gary|last2=Henneberg|first2=Maciej|year=2015|title=The life history of Ardipithecus ramidus: A heterochronic model of sexual and social maturation|journal=Anthropological Review|volume=78|issue=2|pages=109–132|doi=10.1515/anre-2015-0009|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Clark & Henneberg 20172">{{cite journal|last1=Clark|first1=Gary|last2=Henneberg|first2=Maciej|year=2017|title=Ardipithecus ramidus and the evolution of language and singing: An early origin for hominin vocal capability|journal=HOMO: Journal of Comparative Human Biology|volume=68|issue=2|pages=101–121|doi=10.1016/j.jchb.2017.03.001|pmid=28363458}}</ref> This assertion is based upon a comparison of the skull of [[Ardipithecus]] and chimpanzees of various ages. It was found that Ardipithecus clustered with the infant and juvenile species. The consequent lack of a pubertal growth spurt in males of the species and the consequent growth of aggressive canine armoury was taken as evidence that Ardipithecus evolved its paedomorphic skull through self domestication. As the authors state, comparing the species with [[bonobos]]:


Line 35: Line 28:
The most comprehensive case for human self-domestication has been proposed for the changes that account for the much later transition from robust humans such as Neanderthals or Denisovans to anatomically modern humans. Occurring between 40,000 and 25,000 years ago, this rapid neotenization has been explained as the result of cultural selection of mating partners<ref name="Bednarik2">[[Robert G. Bednarik|Bednarik, Robert G.]] (2008). "The Domestication of Humans". Anthropologie. 46 (1): 1-17.</ref> on the basis of variables lacking evolutionary benefits, such as perceived attractiveness, facial symmetry, youth, specific body ratios, skin tone or hair, none of which play any role in any other animal species. This unintentional auto-domestication, coinciding with the introduction of imagery of female sexuality, occurred simultaneously in four continents then occupied by hominins. It led to rapid changes typical for domestication, such as in cranial morphology, skeletal architecture, reduction in brain volume, to playful and exploratory behavior, and the establishment of thousands of deleterious conditions, syndromes, disorders and illnesses presumed absent in robust humans.<ref>Bednarik, Robert G. (2011). The Human Condition. Springer, New York, pp. 127-141. {{ISBN|978-1-4419-9352-6}}.</ref>
The most comprehensive case for human self-domestication has been proposed for the changes that account for the much later transition from robust humans such as Neanderthals or Denisovans to anatomically modern humans. Occurring between 40,000 and 25,000 years ago, this rapid neotenization has been explained as the result of cultural selection of mating partners<ref name="Bednarik2">[[Robert G. Bednarik|Bednarik, Robert G.]] (2008). "The Domestication of Humans". Anthropologie. 46 (1): 1-17.</ref> on the basis of variables lacking evolutionary benefits, such as perceived attractiveness, facial symmetry, youth, specific body ratios, skin tone or hair, none of which play any role in any other animal species. This unintentional auto-domestication, coinciding with the introduction of imagery of female sexuality, occurred simultaneously in four continents then occupied by hominins. It led to rapid changes typical for domestication, such as in cranial morphology, skeletal architecture, reduction in brain volume, to playful and exploratory behavior, and the establishment of thousands of deleterious conditions, syndromes, disorders and illnesses presumed absent in robust humans.<ref>Bednarik, Robert G. (2011). The Human Condition. Springer, New York, pp. 127-141. {{ISBN|978-1-4419-9352-6}}.</ref>


Of course, these specific views are very clearly based on multi-regionalist perspectives of human evolution which claim modern human populations evolved from relevant archaics present in each world region, as demonstrated in robust skeletal fossils. Such views are largely disproven by genetic evidence supporting the [[Recent African origin of modern humans|Out of Africa hypothesis]] with minor inter-breeding and genetic introgression. Despite this, however, human self-domestication entirely within Africa, say, during transition from earlier hominins, especially H. heidelbergensis to H. sapiens remains an open possibility.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Wrangham |first=Richard W. |date=2021 |title=Targeted conspiratorial killing, human self-domestication and the evolution of groupishness |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/evolutionary-human-sciences/article/targeted-conspiratorial-killing-human-selfdomestication-and-the-evolution-of-groupishness/B70C0490CEFFFB3B5231A5426A1D1577 |journal=Evolutionary Human Sciences |language=en |volume=3 |pages=e26 |doi=10.1017/ehs.2021.20 |issn=2513-843X}}</ref> This would mean archaics in each region (e.g., neanderthals, denisovans) were largely replaced by self-domesticated H. sapiens as they spread around the globe. This possibility suggests self-domestication played a role in the success of H. sapiens, and the extinction of other lineages.
Of course, these specific views are very clearly based on multi-regionalist perspectives of human evolution which claim modern human populations evolved from relevant archaics present in each world region, as demonstrated in robust skeletal fossils. Such views are largely disproven by genetic evidence supporting the [[Recent African origin of modern humans|Out of Africa hypothesis]] with minor inter-breeding and genetic introgression. Despite this, however, human self-domestication entirely within Africa, say, during transition from earlier hominins, especially ''H. heidelbergensis'' to ''H. sapiens'' remains an open possibility.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Wrangham |first=Richard W. |date=2021 |title=Targeted conspiratorial killing, human self-domestication and the evolution of groupishness |journal=Evolutionary Human Sciences |language=en |volume=3 |pages=e26 |doi=10.1017/ehs.2021.20 |pmid=37588548 |pmc=10427284 |s2cid=233029730 |issn=2513-843X|doi-access=free }}</ref> This would mean archaics in each region (e.g., neanderthals, denisovans) were largely replaced by self-domesticated ''H. sapiens'' as they spread around the globe. This possibility suggests self-domestication played a role in the success of modern humans, and the extinction of other lineages.


The idea of self-domestication was used by early [[Social Darwinism]] which, according to psychiatrist Martin Brüne in an article "On human self-domestication",<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Brüne|first1=Martin|year=2007|title=On human self-domestication, psychiatry, and eugenics|journal=Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine|volume=2|pages=21|doi=10.1186/1747-5341-2-21|pmc=2082022|pmid=17919321}}</ref> developed from the idea that humans could "perfect" themselves biologically. The idea of self-domestication is also related to the concept of [[sociodicy]].
The idea of self-domestication was used by early [[Social Darwinism]] which, according to psychiatrist Martin Brüne in an article "On human self-domestication",<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Brüne|first1=Martin|year=2007|title=On human self-domestication, psychiatry, and eugenics|journal=Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine|volume=2|pages=21|doi=10.1186/1747-5341-2-21|pmc=2082022|pmid=17919321 |doi-access=free }}</ref> developed from the idea that humans could "perfect" themselves biologically. The idea of self-domestication is also related to the concept of [[sociodicy]].


=== Modern humans ===
== Modern humans ==


==== Physical anatomy ====
=== Physical anatomy ===
Based on the dating of the fossil record, archaeologists have concluded that self-domestication likely occurred during the Pleistocene, over 300,000 years ago. Using the fossil record to compare Homo sapiens to pre-sapiens ancestors, archaeologists observed many of the same telling phenotypic characteristics that emerge as a consequence of self-domestication in animals. These features include diminished sexual dimorphism, smaller tooth size, reduction of the cranium, and smaller body size. H. sapiens fossils also demonstrated the flattening of brow-ridge projection and shortening of faces.<ref name=":3">Wrangham, R. W. (2019). Hypotheses for the Evolution of Reduced Reactive Aggression in the Context of Human Self-Domestication. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1914.</ref>
Based on the dating of the fossil record, archaeologists have concluded that self-domestication likely occurred during the Pleistocene, over 300,000 years ago. Using the fossil record to compare ''Homo sapiens'' to pre-sapiens ancestors, archaeologists observed many of the same telling phenotypic characteristics that emerge as a consequence of self-domestication in animals. These features include diminished sexual dimorphism, smaller tooth size, reduction of the cranium, and smaller body size. ''H. sapiens'' fossils also demonstrated the flattening of brow-ridge projection and shortening of faces.<ref name=":3">Wrangham, R. W. (2019). Hypotheses for the Evolution of Reduced Reactive Aggression in the Context of Human Self-Domestication. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1914.</ref>
{| class="wikitable"
{| class="wikitable"
|
|
Line 105: Line 98:
|}
|}


==== Reactive aggression ====
=== Reactive aggression ===
[[Richard Wrangham]] further built upon this body of research, addressing how bonobos and chimpanzees could elucidate development of aggression in humans. Academics have raised concerns with inconsistencies with the self-domestication hypothesis, pointing out that it isn't logical that humans could potentially be domesticated given the profundity of violent acts for which they are responsible. Reconciling this paradox, Wrangham posited that self-domestication is the outcome of two different kinds of aggression: proactive and reactive aggression.<ref name=":4">Wrangham, R. W. (2018). Two types of aggression in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 115(2), 245-253.</ref>
[[Richard Wrangham]] further built upon this body of research, addressing how bonobos and chimpanzees could elucidate development of aggression in humans. Academics have raised concerns with inconsistencies with the self-domestication hypothesis, pointing out that it isn't logical that humans could potentially be domesticated given the profundity of violent acts for which they are responsible. Reconciling this paradox, Wrangham posited that self-domestication is the outcome of two different kinds of aggression: proactive and reactive aggression.<ref name=":4">Wrangham, R. W. (2018). Two types of aggression in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 115(2), 245-253.</ref>


Proactive aggression, which is commonly observed in chimpanzees, is defined as an attack that was planned, motivated by achieving an end goal. Generally, humans demonstrate lower aggression within groups. Reactive aggression, much more closely associated with anger, is characterized as an immediate response to a threat—the human equivalent being "bar fights". Aligned with the behavior of self-domesticated bonobos, humans do not have a high propensity for reactive aggression. This lends further evidence to supporting the self-domestication hypothesis, of which reduced reactive aggression is a central trait.<ref name=":4" /><ref>Steele, T. E., and Weaver, T. D. (2014). Comment on Cieri et al. Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. ''Curr. Anthropol.'' 55, 434–435. : doi: 10.1086/677209</ref>
Proactive aggression, which is commonly observed in chimpanzees, is defined as an attack that was planned, motivated by achieving an end goal. Generally, humans demonstrate lower aggression within groups. Reactive aggression, much more closely associated with anger, is characterized as an immediate response to a threat—the human equivalent being "bar fights". Aligned with the behavior of self-domesticated bonobos, humans do not have a high propensity for reactive aggression. This lends further evidence to supporting the self-domestication hypothesis, of which reduced reactive aggression is a central trait.<ref name=":4" /><ref>Steele, T. E., and Weaver, T. D. (2014). Comment on Cieri et al. Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. ''Curr. Anthropol.'' 55, 434–435. : doi: 10.1086/677209</ref>


==== Population density hypothesis ====
=== Population density hypothesis ===
The population density hypothesis attempts to explain the decreased reactive aggression that is observed in modern humans. During periods of high population density, higher tolerance of associates may be favored due to an increased reliance upon social networks for reliable access to otherwise limited, scarce resources like food. ''H. sapiens'' began to exhibit this higher degree of social tolerance approximately 300,000 years ago, which—if this hypothesis upholds—would be associated with a higher population size. However, recent genetic data has currently put this hypothesis to rest, as ''H. sapiens'' actually underwent a population decline about 200,000 years ago.<ref name=":3" />
The population density hypothesis attempts to explain the decreased reactive aggression that is observed in modern humans. During periods of high population density, higher tolerance of associates may be favored due to an increased reliance upon social networks for reliable access to otherwise limited, scarce resources like food. ''H. sapiens'' began to exhibit this higher degree of social tolerance approximately 300,000 years ago, which—if this hypothesis upholds—would be associated with a higher population size. However, recent genetic data has currently put this hypothesis to rest, as ''H. sapiens'' actually underwent a population decline about 200,000 years ago.<ref name=":3" />


===== Language-based conspiracy =====
=== Language-based conspiracy ===
The language-based conspiracy provides a convincing argument—and is currently the best-supported theory—explaining why reactive aggression was selected against in modern humans, thereby resulting in self-domestication. ''H. sapiens'' are theorized to have developed an elegant propensity for language that surpassed its predecessors, including ''H. neanderthalensis''. Enhanced linguistic ability would have allowed for greater suppression and control over a power-hungry member of early hunter-gatherer societies. Those who attempted to achieve dominance over others would be subject to capital punishment, which was facilitated by shared intentionality from others that was easily communicated through language. Language allowed subordinates to collaborate, coordinating plans to dampen the attempt at dominance by the instigator. Over time, this resulted in the selection against reactive aggression.<ref name=":3" />
The language-based conspiracy provides a convincing argument—and is currently the best-supported theory—explaining why reactive aggression was selected against in modern humans, thereby resulting in self-domestication. ''H. sapiens'' are theorized to have developed an elegant propensity for language that surpassed its predecessors, including ''H. neanderthalensis''. Enhanced linguistic ability would have allowed for greater suppression and control over a power-hungry member of early hunter-gatherer societies. Those who attempted to achieve dominance over others would be subject to capital punishment, which was facilitated by [[shared intentionality]] from others that was easily communicated through language. Language allowed subordinates to collaborate, coordinating plans to dampen the attempt at dominance by the instigator. Over time, this resulted in the selection against reactive aggression.<ref name=":3" />


=== Theoretical criticism ===
=== Criticism ===

The self-domestication hypothesis has been met with some degree of criticism. Some researchers have argued that the human brain is peramorphic, instead of paedomorphic. Wrangham puts forth that these arguments do not address the evolution of Homo sapiens from their most recent ancestor, instead focusing too heavily on a direct contrast between apes and humans.<ref name=":4" />
{{Expand section|with=Broader overview of the criticisms|date=April 2024}}
A criticism of the theory as applied to [[human]]s, is that a number of differences between us and other great apes are not the result of halted brain development preserving juvenile characteristics into adulthood, but instead arise from accelerated and prolonged brain development - which could indicate other processes are needed to explain important evolutionary changes in humans. Wrangham argues self-domestication in the context of humans is applicable to the more recent evolution of modern humans from [[archaic humans]] over the last 2 million years, and hence the differences between humans and other extant ape species do not disprove human self-domestication.<ref name=":4" />

== See also ==


==See also==
{{Portal|Biology|Evolutionary biology}}
{{Portal|Biology|Evolutionary biology}}
* [[de novo domestication|''De novo'' domestication]]
* [[Domestication]]
* [[Domestication]]
* [[Landrace]]
* [[Ethology]]
* [[Ethology]]
* [[Sociobiology]]
* [[Evolutionary psychology]]
* [[Evolutionary psychology]]
* [[Landrace]]
* [[Rewilding (anarchism)]]
* [[Sociobiology]]
* [[Synanthrope]]
* [[Synanthrope]]



Latest revision as of 09:38, 9 June 2024

Experiment conducted by the University of Barcelona to demonstrate the hypothesis of self-domestication.[1]

Self-domestication is a scientific hypothesis that suggests that, similar to domesticated animals, there has been a process of artificial selection among members of the human species conducted by humans themselves.[2] In this way, during the process of hominization, a preference for individuals with collaborative and social behaviors would have been shown to optimize the benefit of the entire group: docility, language, and emotional intelligence would have been enhanced during this process of artificial selection. The hypothesis is raised that this is what differentiated Homo sapiens from Homo neanderthalensis and Homo erectus.[3][4]

Origin and status of the hypothesis

[edit]

In general, domesticated animals possess common characteristics that differentiate them from their non-domesticated counterparts (for example, in the case of Canis familiaris compared to their relatives, Canis lupus, among many other cases): they tend to be more docile and playful, exhibit less aggressive behavior, and show marked neoteny, often resulting in a smaller body, a slightly smaller brain and skull, as well as shorter teeth and snout.[5]

One of the first to scientifically observe that humans present similar traits was the naturalist, anthropologist, and physician Johann Friedrich Blumenbach around 1800.[6] The author of the thesis "De generis humani varietate nativa" ('On the natural variations in the human lineage') consequently proposed the hypothesis that humans could have been domesticated.

A few years later, Charles Darwin addressed the topic using the theory of evolution, which already considered the process of artificial selection in animals. Unable to explain the concept of human domestication from an exclusively scientific perspective (the question of who domesticated humans could only be answered in religious or theistic terms), he eventually dismissed the hypothesis.[5]

However, the studies of Dimitri Beliayev in the 20th century were important for the proposal: research on the silver fox demonstrated that in the process of animal domestication, simultaneous changes occurred in behavior (lower levels of adrenaline were observed) and in coat color (alterations in pigmentation): adrenaline could share a biochemical pathway with melanin, a pathway that would be altered during the process of artificial selection.[7]

In 2014, scientists Adam Wilkins (from Humboldt University, Berlin), Richard Wrangham (from Harvard University, Massachusetts), and Tecumseh Fitch (from the University of Vienna) proposed that the common origin of these changes lay in neural crest cells, exclusive stem cells of vertebrates that migrate to different parts of the body during the embryonic phase, giving rise to the adrenal glands (responsible for managing the fear and stress response through adrenaline production), melanocytes (responsible for producing skin or coat melanin), and jaws simultaneously. The deficit of these cells would explain the common characteristics of all domesticated animals: tameness, cranial and mandibular reduction, and alterations in pigmentation.[8]

Of the three members of the research team, it was primatologist Richard Wrangham who translated these results to humans, asserting that humans have "domesticated" themselves through a process of self-selection (a proposal he would elaborate in "The Goodness Paradox: The Strange Relationship Between Virtue and Violence in Human Evolution").

In July 2019, a team from the Institute of Marine Sciences of Barcelona described, through the methylation of certain genes in DNA, the epigenetic and genetic changes through which neural crest cells were reduced.[9] Subsequently, another research team from the University of Barcelona discovered that the BAZ1B gene controls the behavior of neural crest cells. Comparable with the Neanderthal genome, BAZ1B is also related to genes that have many mutations not present in the homologous genes of our past hominini.[1] Cedric Boeckx, one of the researchers in this study, states:

"We believe this means that the genetic network of BAZ1B is an important reason why our face is different compared to other already extinct ancestors, like the Neanderthals [...]. In broad terms, it gives us, for the first time, experimental validation of the autodomestication hypothesis based on the neural crest."[10]

Hominids

[edit]

Clark & Henneberg argue that during the earliest stages of human evolution a more paedomorphic skull arose through self-domestication.[11][12] This assertion is based upon a comparison of the skull of Ardipithecus and chimpanzees of various ages. It was found that Ardipithecus clustered with the infant and juvenile species. The consequent lack of a pubertal growth spurt in males of the species and the consequent growth of aggressive canine armoury was taken as evidence that Ardipithecus evolved its paedomorphic skull through self domestication. As the authors state, comparing the species with bonobos:

"Of course A. ramidus differs significantly from bonobos, bonobos having retained a functional canine honing complex. However, the fact that A. ramidus shares with bonobos reduced sexual dimorphism, and a more paedomorphic form relative to chimpanzees, suggests that the developmental and social adaptations evident in bonobos may be of assistance in future reconstructions of early hominin social and sexual psychology. In fact the trend towards increased maternal care, female mate selection and self-domestication may have been stronger and more refined in A. ramidus than what we see in bonobos."[11]

Further research has confirmed that Ardipithecus possessed paedomorphic cranial base angulation, position of the foramen magnum as well as vocal tract dimensions. This was interpreted as not only evidence of a change in social behavior but also a potentially early emergence of hominid vocal capability. If this thesis is correct then not only human social behavior but also language ability originally evolved through paedomorphic skull morphogenesis via the process of self-domestication.[12]

The most comprehensive case for human self-domestication has been proposed for the changes that account for the much later transition from robust humans such as Neanderthals or Denisovans to anatomically modern humans. Occurring between 40,000 and 25,000 years ago, this rapid neotenization has been explained as the result of cultural selection of mating partners[13] on the basis of variables lacking evolutionary benefits, such as perceived attractiveness, facial symmetry, youth, specific body ratios, skin tone or hair, none of which play any role in any other animal species. This unintentional auto-domestication, coinciding with the introduction of imagery of female sexuality, occurred simultaneously in four continents then occupied by hominins. It led to rapid changes typical for domestication, such as in cranial morphology, skeletal architecture, reduction in brain volume, to playful and exploratory behavior, and the establishment of thousands of deleterious conditions, syndromes, disorders and illnesses presumed absent in robust humans.[14]

Of course, these specific views are very clearly based on multi-regionalist perspectives of human evolution which claim modern human populations evolved from relevant archaics present in each world region, as demonstrated in robust skeletal fossils. Such views are largely disproven by genetic evidence supporting the Out of Africa hypothesis with minor inter-breeding and genetic introgression. Despite this, however, human self-domestication entirely within Africa, say, during transition from earlier hominins, especially H. heidelbergensis to H. sapiens remains an open possibility.[15] This would mean archaics in each region (e.g., neanderthals, denisovans) were largely replaced by self-domesticated H. sapiens as they spread around the globe. This possibility suggests self-domestication played a role in the success of modern humans, and the extinction of other lineages.

The idea of self-domestication was used by early Social Darwinism which, according to psychiatrist Martin Brüne in an article "On human self-domestication",[16] developed from the idea that humans could "perfect" themselves biologically. The idea of self-domestication is also related to the concept of sociodicy.

Modern humans

[edit]

Physical anatomy

[edit]

Based on the dating of the fossil record, archaeologists have concluded that self-domestication likely occurred during the Pleistocene, over 300,000 years ago. Using the fossil record to compare Homo sapiens to pre-sapiens ancestors, archaeologists observed many of the same telling phenotypic characteristics that emerge as a consequence of self-domestication in animals. These features include diminished sexual dimorphism, smaller tooth size, reduction of the cranium, and smaller body size. H. sapiens fossils also demonstrated the flattening of brow-ridge projection and shortening of faces.[17]

Reduced aggression Reduced cranium and skull White patches Floppy ears Flattened facial projection Small teeth Juvenility Curly Tails
Cats Y Y Y N Y N N
Dogs Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Bonobos Y Y Y N Y Y Y NA
Marmosets Y NA Y N NA NA NA NA
Humans Y Y N N Y Y Y NA

Reactive aggression

[edit]

Richard Wrangham further built upon this body of research, addressing how bonobos and chimpanzees could elucidate development of aggression in humans. Academics have raised concerns with inconsistencies with the self-domestication hypothesis, pointing out that it isn't logical that humans could potentially be domesticated given the profundity of violent acts for which they are responsible. Reconciling this paradox, Wrangham posited that self-domestication is the outcome of two different kinds of aggression: proactive and reactive aggression.[18]

Proactive aggression, which is commonly observed in chimpanzees, is defined as an attack that was planned, motivated by achieving an end goal. Generally, humans demonstrate lower aggression within groups. Reactive aggression, much more closely associated with anger, is characterized as an immediate response to a threat—the human equivalent being "bar fights". Aligned with the behavior of self-domesticated bonobos, humans do not have a high propensity for reactive aggression. This lends further evidence to supporting the self-domestication hypothesis, of which reduced reactive aggression is a central trait.[18][19]

Population density hypothesis

[edit]

The population density hypothesis attempts to explain the decreased reactive aggression that is observed in modern humans. During periods of high population density, higher tolerance of associates may be favored due to an increased reliance upon social networks for reliable access to otherwise limited, scarce resources like food. H. sapiens began to exhibit this higher degree of social tolerance approximately 300,000 years ago, which—if this hypothesis upholds—would be associated with a higher population size. However, recent genetic data has currently put this hypothesis to rest, as H. sapiens actually underwent a population decline about 200,000 years ago.[17]

Language-based conspiracy

[edit]

The language-based conspiracy provides a convincing argument—and is currently the best-supported theory—explaining why reactive aggression was selected against in modern humans, thereby resulting in self-domestication. H. sapiens are theorized to have developed an elegant propensity for language that surpassed its predecessors, including H. neanderthalensis. Enhanced linguistic ability would have allowed for greater suppression and control over a power-hungry member of early hunter-gatherer societies. Those who attempted to achieve dominance over others would be subject to capital punishment, which was facilitated by shared intentionality from others that was easily communicated through language. Language allowed subordinates to collaborate, coordinating plans to dampen the attempt at dominance by the instigator. Over time, this resulted in the selection against reactive aggression.[17]

Criticism

[edit]

A criticism of the theory as applied to humans, is that a number of differences between us and other great apes are not the result of halted brain development preserving juvenile characteristics into adulthood, but instead arise from accelerated and prolonged brain development - which could indicate other processes are needed to explain important evolutionary changes in humans. Wrangham argues self-domestication in the context of humans is applicable to the more recent evolution of modern humans from archaic humans over the last 2 million years, and hence the differences between humans and other extant ape species do not disprove human self-domestication.[18]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Zanella, Matteo; Vitriolo, Alessandro; Andirko, Alejandro; Martins, Pedro Tiago; Sturm, Stefanie; O’Rourke, Thomas; Laugsch, Magdalena; Malerba, Natascia; Skaros, Adrianos; Trattaro, Sebastiano; Germain, Pierre-Luc; Mihailovic, Marija; Merla, Giuseppe; Rada-Iglesias, Alvaro; Boeckx, Cedric (2019-12-06). "Dosage analysis of the 7q11.23 Williams region identifies BAZ1B as a major human gene patterning the modern human face and underlying self-domestication". Science Advances. 5 (12). eaaw7908. Bibcode:2019SciA....5.7908Z. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaw7908. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 6892627. PMID 31840056.
  2. ^ Theofanopoulou, Constantina; Gastaldon, Simone; O’Rourke, Thomas; Samuels, Bridget D.; Messner, Angela; Martins, Pedro Tiago; Delogu, Francesco; Alamri, Saleh; Boeckx, Cedric (2017-10-18). "Self-domestication in Homo sapiens: Insights from comparative genomics". PLOS ONE. 12 (10): e0185306. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1285306T. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0185306. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 5646786. PMID 29045412.
  3. ^ Shilton, D; Breski, M; Dor, D; Jablonka, E (February 14, 2020). "Human Social Evolution: Self-Domestication or Self-Control?". Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 134. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00134. PMC 7033472. PMID 32116937.
  4. ^ https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=acOZT240bTA&ab_channel=UniversityofCaliforniaTelevision%28UCTV%29 Harvard Prof Richard Wrangham
  5. ^ a b Sauer, Hanno (2023). La invención del bien y del mal. Paidós. ISBN 9788449340963.
  6. ^ Hawks, John (2019). "The Goodness Paradox" Review: The Benefits of Good Breeding (book review)" (Wall Street Journal ed.).
  7. ^ Goldman, Jason (6 September 2010). "Man's new best friend? A forgotten Russian experiment in fox domestication". Scientific American. Retrieved 23 May 2014.
  8. ^ Wilkins, Adam S; Wrangham, Richard W; Fitch, W Tecumseh (2014-07-01). "The "Domestication Syndrome" in Mammals: A Unified Explanation Based on Neural Crest Cell Behavior and Genetics". Genetics. 197 (3): 795–808. doi:10.1534/genetics.114.165423. ISSN 1943-2631. PMC 4096361. PMID 25024034.
  9. ^ Anastasiadi, Dafni; Piferrer, Francesc (2019-10-01). Wittkopp, Patricia (ed.). "Epimutations in Developmental Genes Underlie the Onset of Domestication in Farmed European Sea Bass". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 36 (10): 2252–2264. doi:10.1093/molbev/msz153. ISSN 0737-4038. PMC 6759067. PMID 31289822.
  10. ^ "Universidad de Barcelona". Primera demostración experimental de la hipótesis de la autodomesticación del ser humano. 5 December 2019.
  11. ^ a b Clark, Gary; Henneberg, Maciej (2015). "The life history of Ardipithecus ramidus: A heterochronic model of sexual and social maturation". Anthropological Review. 78 (2): 109–132. doi:10.1515/anre-2015-0009.
  12. ^ a b Clark, Gary; Henneberg, Maciej (2017). "Ardipithecus ramidus and the evolution of language and singing: An early origin for hominin vocal capability". HOMO: Journal of Comparative Human Biology. 68 (2): 101–121. doi:10.1016/j.jchb.2017.03.001. PMID 28363458.
  13. ^ Bednarik, Robert G. (2008). "The Domestication of Humans". Anthropologie. 46 (1): 1-17.
  14. ^ Bednarik, Robert G. (2011). The Human Condition. Springer, New York, pp. 127-141. ISBN 978-1-4419-9352-6.
  15. ^ Wrangham, Richard W. (2021). "Targeted conspiratorial killing, human self-domestication and the evolution of groupishness". Evolutionary Human Sciences. 3: e26. doi:10.1017/ehs.2021.20. ISSN 2513-843X. PMC 10427284. PMID 37588548. S2CID 233029730.
  16. ^ Brüne, Martin (2007). "On human self-domestication, psychiatry, and eugenics". Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine. 2: 21. doi:10.1186/1747-5341-2-21. PMC 2082022. PMID 17919321.
  17. ^ a b c Wrangham, R. W. (2019). Hypotheses for the Evolution of Reduced Reactive Aggression in the Context of Human Self-Domestication. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1914.
  18. ^ a b c Wrangham, R. W. (2018). Two types of aggression in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 115(2), 245-253.
  19. ^ Steele, T. E., and Weaver, T. D. (2014). Comment on Cieri et al. Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. Curr. Anthropol. 55, 434–435. : doi: 10.1086/677209

Further reading

[edit]
  • Brian Hare and Vanessa Woods, "Survival of the Friendliest: Natural selection for hypersocial traits enabled Earth's apex species to best Neandertals and other competitors", Scientific American, vol. 323, no. 2 (August 2020), pp. 58–63.