Jump to content

Will to live: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Remove spam
Tag: section blanking
 
(29 intermediate revisions by 19 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Philosophical concept}}
{{Expert needed|philosophy|article|date=June 2009}}
<!-- "Merge to Self preservation page" template removed - please see Talk page -->
<!-- "Merge to Self preservation page" template removed - please see Talk page -->
The '''will to live''' or ''Wille zum Leben'' is a concept developed by the German philosopher [[Arthur Schopenhauer]], '''Will''' being an irrational "blind incessant ''impulse'' without knowledge" that drives [[instinctive]] behaviors, causing an endless insatiable striving in human existence, which Nature could not exist without.
The '''will to live''' ([[German language|German]]: ''Wille zum Leben'') is a concept developed by the German philosopher [[Arthur Schopenhauer]], representing an irrational "blind incessant ''impulse'' without knowledge" that drives [[instinctive]] behaviors, causing an endless insatiable striving in human existence.


This is contrasted with the concept of the will to survive under life threatening conditions used in psychology{{citation needed|date=September 2023}} since Schopenhauer’s notion of the will to live is more broadly understood as the “animal[istic] force to endure, reproduce and flourish.”<ref>{{Cite SEP|url-id=schopenhauer|title=Arthur Schopenhauer|first=Robert|last=Wicks|date=September 9, 2021|edition=Fall 2021}}</ref>
This has nothing to do with the concept of the will to survive used in psychology, which is discussed further below in this article, since Schopenhauer was talking about the basic unit behind doing or thinking anything at all, not the basic drive behind running or fighting under life-threatening conditions (which in his terms would be a dissimulation of the mere blind striving for existence without goal, that is, a [[Play (theatre)|theatrical performance]] of Nature in its ever changing forms of matter).


There are significant correlations between the will to live and existential, psychological, social, and physical sources of [[Distress (medicine)|distress]].<ref>"Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death". The Academy of Psychosomatic Medicine (2005)</ref> Many, who overcome [[near-death experience]]s with no explanation, have described the will to live as a direct component of their survival.<ref>No Author (2003). What is the Will to Live. Retrieved from http://www.wisegeek.com/what-is-the-will-to-live.htm</ref> The difference between the wish to die versus the wish to live is also a unique risk factor for [[suicide]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Brown | first1 = Gregory K. | last2 = Steer | first2 = RA | last3 = Henriques | first3 = GR | last4 = Beck | first4 = AT | title = The Internal Struggle Between the Wish to Die and the Wish to Live: A Risk Factor for Suicide | journal = American Journal of Psychiatry | volume = 162 | issue = 10| pages = 1977–1979 | doi = 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1977 | pmid = 16199851 | date = October 2005 }}</ref>
There are significant correlations between the will to live and existential, psychological, social, and physical sources of [[Distress (medicine)|distress]].<ref>"Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death". The Academy of Psychosomatic Medicine (2005)</ref> Many, who overcome [[near-death experience]]s with no explanation, have described the will to live as a direct component of their survival.<ref>{{Cite web |title=What Is the Will to Live? (with picture) |url=https://www.thehealthboard.com/what-is-the-will-to-live.htm |access-date=2024-07-05 |website=The Health Board |language=en-US}}</ref> The difference between the wish to die versus the wish to live is also a unique risk factor for [[suicide]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Brown | first1 = Gregory K. | last2 = Steer | first2 = RA | last3 = Henriques | first3 = GR | last4 = Beck | first4 = AT | title = The Internal Struggle Between the Wish to Die and the Wish to Live: A Risk Factor for Suicide | journal = American Journal of Psychiatry | volume = 162 | issue = 10| pages = 1977–1979 | doi = 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1977 | pmid = 16199851 | date = October 2005 }}</ref>


==Concept==
==Concept==
In psychology, the will to live is the [[Drive theory|drive]] for [[self-preservation]], usually coupled with [[Expectation (epistemic)|expectations]] for future improvement in one's state in life.<ref>"Will to Live." McGraw-Hill Concise Dictionary of Modern Medicine. 2002. The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. 28 Nov. 2017 https://medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/will+to+live</ref> The will to live is an important concept when attempting to understand and comprehend why we do what we do in order to stay alive, and for as long as we can. This can be related to either one's push for survival on the brink of death, or someone who is just trying to find a meaning to continuing their life. Some researchers say that people who have a reason or purpose in life during such dreadful and horrific experiences will often appear to fare better than those that may find such experiences overwhelming.<ref>Frankl, V. E. (1963). In Man's search for meaning. Boston: Beacon Press.</ref> Every day, people undergo countless types of negative experiences, some of which may be demoralizing, hurtful, or tragic. An ongoing question continues to be what keeps the will to live in these situations. People who claim to have had experiences involving the will to live have different explanations behind it.<ref>Grohol, J. (2009). The Power of the Will to Live. Psych Central. Retrieved on November 21, 2012, from http://psychcentral.com/blog/archives/2009/12/28/the-power-of-the-will-to-live</ref>
In psychology, the will to live is the [[Drive theory|drive]] for [[self-preservation]], usually coupled with [[Expectation (epistemic)|expectations]] for future improvement in one's state in life.<ref>{{Cite web |title=will to live |url=https://medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/will+to+live |access-date=2024-07-05 |website=TheFreeDictionary.com |language=en}}</ref> The will to live is an important concept when attempting to understand and comprehend why we do what we do in order to stay alive, and for as long as we can. This can be related to either one's push for survival on the brink of death, or someone who is just trying to find a meaning to continuing their life. Some researchers say that people who have a reason or purpose in life during such dreadful and horrific experiences will often appear to fare better than those that may find such experiences overwhelming.<ref>Frankl, V. E. (1963). In Man's search for meaning. Boston: Beacon Press.</ref> Every day, people undergo countless types of negative experiences, some of which may be demoralizing, hurtful, or tragic. An ongoing question continues to be what keeps the will to live in these situations. People who claim to have had experiences involving the will to live have different explanations behind it.<ref>{{Cite web |date=2009-12-28 |title=The Power of the Will to Live |url=https://psychcentral.com/blog/the-power-of-the-will-to-live |access-date=2024-07-05 |website=Psych Central |language=en}}</ref>



==Background==
==Background==
Line 22: Line 21:
| pages = 147
| pages = 147
| isbn = 978-0-7619-2633-7}}
| isbn = 978-0-7619-2633-7}}
</ref> [[Viktor Frankl]], after spending time in a German concentration camp, developed psychotherapy called [[logotherapy]], or the "will to meaning". [[Maslow's hierarchy of needs]] highlights the innate appetite that people possess for love and belonging but before all this there is the very basic and powerful will to live.
</ref> [[Viktor Frankl]], who spent time in German concentration camps, developed psychotherapy called [[logotherapy]], which may be translated as the therapy focused on the "will to meaning". [[Maslow's hierarchy of needs]] highlights the innate appetite that people possess for love and belonging but before all this there is the very basic and powerful will to live.
Psychologists have established that human beings are a goal-oriented species. In assessing the will to live, it should be borne in mind that it could be augmented or diminished by the relative strength of other simultaneously existent drives. Psychologists generally agree{{weasel inline|date=August 2019}} that there is the will to live, the will to pleasure, the will to superiority and the will to connection. There are also usually varying degrees of curiosity with regard to what may be termed the will to identity or establishing meaningful personal responses. The will to live is a platform without which it would not be possible to satisfy the other drives. However, this overlooks the possibility that there is a commonality among all creatures that drives all others urges.
Psychologists have established that human beings are a goal-oriented species. In assessing the will to live, it should be borne in mind that it could be augmented or diminished by the relative strength of other simultaneously existent drives. Psychologists generally agree{{weasel inline|date=August 2019}} that there is the will to live, the will to pleasure, the will to superiority and the will to connection. There are also usually varying degrees of curiosity with regard to what may be termed the will to identity or establishing meaningful personal responses. The will to live is a platform without which it would not be possible to satisfy the other drives. However, this overlooks the possibility that there is a commonality among all creatures that drives all other urges.


== Similarity to self-preservation ==
== Similarity to self-preservation ==
Line 32: Line 31:


==Correlations==
==Correlations==
“Existential, psychiatric, social, and, to a lesser degree, physical variables are highly correlated with the will to live”.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Hack | first2 = T. | last3 = Hassard | first3 = T. | last4 = Kristjanson | first4 = L. J. | last5 = McClement | first5 = S. | last6 = Harlos | first6 = M. | year = 2005 | title = Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 46 | issue = 1| pages = 7–10 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7| pmid = 15765815 }}</ref> Existential issues found to correlate significantly include hopelessness, the desire for death, sense of dignity, and burden to others. Psychiatric issues found to be strongly associated are such as depression, anxiety, and lack of concentration. Physical issues that showed the strongest associations were appetite and appearance which did not show the same consistent degree of correlation. The four main predictor variables of the will to live changing over time are anxiety, shortness of breath, depression, and sense of well-being<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Tataryn | first2 = D. | last3 = Clinch | first3 = J. J. | last4 = Dudgeon | first4 = D. | year = 1999 | title = Will to Live in the Terminally Ill | journal = The Lancet | volume = 354 | issue = 9181| pages = 816–819 | doi=10.1016/s0140-6736(99)80011-7| pmid = 10485723 | s2cid = 44586921 }}</ref> which correlate with the other variable predictors as well. Social variables and quality of life measures are shown to correlate significantly with the will to live such as support and satisfaction with support from family, friends, and health care providers.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Hack | first2 = T. | last3 = Hassard | first3 = T. | last4 = Kristjanson | first4 = L. J. | last5 = McClement | first5 = S. | last6 = Harlos | first6 = M. | year = 2005 | title = Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 46 | issue = 1| pages = 7–10 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7| pmid = 15765815 }}</ref> Findings on the will to live have suggested that psychological variables are replaced by physical mediators of variation as death draws nearer. The will to live has also proven to be highly unstable.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Tataryn | first2 = D. | last3 = Clinch | first3 = J. J. | last4 = Dudgeon | first4 = D. | year = 1999 | title = Will to Live in the Terminally Ill | journal = The Lancet | volume = 354 | issue = 9181| pages = 816–819 | doi=10.1016/s0140-6736(99)80011-7| pmid = 10485723 | s2cid = 44586921 }}</ref>
“Existential, psychiatric, social, and, to a lesser degree, physical variables are highly correlated with the will to live”.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Hack | first2 = T. | last3 = Hassard | first3 = T. | last4 = Kristjanson | first4 = L. J. | last5 = McClement | first5 = S. | last6 = Harlos | first6 = M. | year = 2005 | title = Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 46 | issue = 1| pages = 7–10 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7| pmid = 15765815 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Existential issues found to correlate significantly include hopelessness, the desire for death, sense of dignity, and burden to others. Psychiatric issues found to be strongly associated are such as depression, anxiety, and lack of concentration. Physical issues that showed the strongest associations were appetite and appearance which did not show the same consistent degree of correlation. The four main predictor variables of the will to live changing over time are anxiety, shortness of breath, depression, and sense of well-being<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Tataryn | first2 = D. | last3 = Clinch | first3 = J. J. | last4 = Dudgeon | first4 = D. | year = 1999 | title = Will to Live in the Terminally Ill | journal = The Lancet | volume = 354 | issue = 9181| pages = 816–819 | doi=10.1016/s0140-6736(99)80011-7| pmid = 10485723 | s2cid = 44586921 }}</ref> which correlate with the other variable predictors as well. Social variables and quality of life measures are shown to correlate significantly with the will to live such as support and satisfaction with support from family, friends, and health care providers.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Hack | first2 = T. | last3 = Hassard | first3 = T. | last4 = Kristjanson | first4 = L. J. | last5 = McClement | first5 = S. | last6 = Harlos | first6 = M. | year = 2005 | title = Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 46 | issue = 1| pages = 7–10 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7| pmid = 15765815 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Findings on the will to live have suggested that psychological variables are replaced by physical mediators of variation as death draws nearer. The will to live has also proven to be highly unstable.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Tataryn | first2 = D. | last3 = Clinch | first3 = J. J. | last4 = Dudgeon | first4 = D. | year = 1999 | title = Will to Live in the Terminally Ill | journal = The Lancet | volume = 354 | issue = 9181| pages = 816–819 | doi=10.1016/s0140-6736(99)80011-7| pmid = 10485723 | s2cid = 44586921 }}</ref>


==Research==
==Research==
Many studies have been conducted on the theory of the will to live. Among these studies are subject to the difference in gender and the elderly and also in the terminally ill. One study focused on a simple question that asked about rating one’s will to live and presented the findings that elderly participants reporting a stronger will to live and strengthened or stable will to live survived longer in comparison to those with a weak will to live. This study found that women were able to cope with life-threatening situations, but suggested that the participants could not have been stable and requires future replication.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Hack | first2 = T. | last3 = Hassard | first3 = T. | last4 = Kristjanson | first4 = L. J. | last5 = McClement | first5 = S. | last6 = Harlos | first6 = M. | year = 2005 | title = Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 46 | issue = 1| pages = 7–10 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7| pmid = 15765815 }}</ref> The second study presented the idea of the will to live in the terminally ill specifically cancer patients termed as older. In this study researchers were able to suggest that patients who had tested as having a low sustained will to live died soonest, as opposed to having a moderate level of the will to live, lived the longest while high will to live could affect individuals in any direction. This study needs future replication that can show the effects of will to live in the terminally ill from different diseases and age categories.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Tataryn | first1 = D. | last2 = Chochoniv | first2 = H. M. | year = 2002 | title = Predicting the trajectory of will to live in terminally ill patients | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 43 | issue = 5| pages = 370–377 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.43.5.370| pmid = 12297605 }}</ref>
Several studies have been conducted testing the theory of the will to live. These studies varied in their focus, but broadly sought to understand the will to live as it differs by [[Demography|demographics]], especially as it concerns the [[Old age|elderly]] and the [[Terminal illness|terminally ill]]. A study conducted in 2005 asked elderly participants to rate their will to live and tracked this data across time. It found that those who reported a high or stable will to live generally lived longer than those who reported a weak will to live. Additionally, this study proposed that women were generally better able to cope with life-altering or life-threatening conditions and situations than men. However, it also suggested that the participants may not have been of stable health, and that further study was required before drawing any definite conclusions.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chochinov | first1 = H. M. | last2 = Hack | first2 = T. | last3 = Hassard | first3 = T. | last4 = Kristjanson | first4 = L. J. | last5 = McClement | first5 = S. | last6 = Harlos | first6 = M. | year = 2005 | title = Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 46 | issue = 1| pages = 7–10 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7| pmid = 15765815 | doi-access = free }}</ref> An earlier study conducted in 2002 tested the idea in terminally ill [[cancer]] patients, with most participants being elderly. This study found that those with the weakest will to live typically died sooner than those with a moderate will to live. Those with a high will to live could either die sooner or live as long as those with a moderate will to live. The authors went on to specify that further research is required, testing this theory against other terminal illnesses and in different age categories.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Tataryn | first1 = D. | last2 = Chochoniv | first2 = H. M. | year = 2002 | title = Predicting the trajectory of will to live in terminally ill patients | journal = Psychosomatics | volume = 43 | issue = 5| pages = 370–377 | doi=10.1176/appi.psy.43.5.370| pmid = 12297605 | doi-access = free }}</ref>


Other accounts of the will to live exist in many extreme medical cases, where patients have overcome extraordinary odds to survive. The Holocaust has provided many instances of this phenomenon, and is a good example of this as well.<ref>Goldenberg, J. Retrieved from http://www.councilforrelationships.org/resources/goldenberg-survival.pdf {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120418051335/http://www.councilforrelationships.org/resources/goldenberg-survival.pdf |date=2012-04-18 }}</ref> A proposed mechanism for the will to live is the idea that positive mental thinking tends to lower one’s risk for disease and health complications. One study showed that women who thought positively were more likely to carry more antibodies against certain strains of the flu, thus having a stronger immune system than those who were told to think negative thoughts.<ref>Goode, E. (2003, September 02). Power of positive thinking may have a health benefit, study says. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2003/09/02/health/power-of-positive-thinking-may-have-a-health-benefit-study-says.html</ref>
Anecdotal evidence also suggests a correlation between the individual will to live and survival in [[Psychological trauma|traumatic situations]] that include [[Abuse|maltreatment]]. The [[World War II|Second World War]] and the [[The Holocaust|Holocaust]] provide concrete examples of this, where many individuals survived years of [[Malnutrition|malnourishment]] and mistreatment in [[Nazi concentration camps|concentration camps]], and cited their will to live as a key part of their survival.<ref>Goldenberg, J. Retrieved from http://www.councilforrelationships.org/resources/goldenberg-survival.pdf {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120418051335/http://www.councilforrelationships.org/resources/goldenberg-survival.pdf |date=2012-04-18 }}</ref> A study conducted in 2003 suggested that positive thought (i.e., having a positive outlook on ones' future and life in general) could lower ones' risk for health complications and diseases. This study posited that women who had a more positive outlook were more likely to carry a greater number of [[Antibody|antibodies]] for certain [[Influenza|flu strains]], which suggested a stronger immune system more generally than those who had a negative outlook.<ref>{{Cite news |last=Goode |first=Erica |date=2003-09-02 |title=Power of Positive Thinking May Have a Health Benefit, Study Says |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2003/09/02/health/power-of-positive-thinking-may-have-a-health-benefit-study-says.html |access-date=2024-07-05 |work=The New York Times |language=en-US |issn=0362-4331}}</ref> Further anecdotal evidence can be found through quantitative analysis of [[Death certificate|death records]], which consistently show many people dying shortly after major holidays, suggesting that people will themselves to live until the holiday (or in other cases, a birthday), and then passing shortly thereafter.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Shimizu | first1 = M. | last2 = Pelham | first2 = B.W. | year = 2008 | title = Postponing a date with the Grim Reaper: Ceremonial events and mortality | journal = Basic and Applied Social Psychology | volume = 30 | issue = 1| pages = 36–45 | doi = 10.1080/01973530701866482 | s2cid = 143717879 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |date=2009-12-28 |title=The Power of the Will to Live |url=https://psychcentral.com/blog/the-power-of-the-will-to-live |access-date=2024-07-05 |website=Psych Central |language=en}}</ref>
Powerful examples of humans having a will to live can be seen in death records throughout history showing that people were more likely to die right after a major holiday, such as Christmas and Thanksgiving, and even birthdays, not actually on or before them, but passing shortly after.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Shimizu | first1 = M. | last2 = Pelham | first2 = B.W. | year = 2008 | title = Postponing a date with the Grim Reaper: Ceremonial events and mortality | journal = Basic and Applied Social Psychology | volume = 30 | issue = 1| pages = 36–45 | doi = 10.1080/01973530701866482 | s2cid = 143717879 }}</ref><ref>Grohol, J. (2009). The Power of the Will to Live. Psych Central. Retrieved on November 21, 2012, from http://psychcentral.com/blog/archives/2009/12/28/the-power-of-the-will-to-live</ref>


==See also==
==See also==
Line 68: Line 66:
[[Category:Evolutionary biology]]
[[Category:Evolutionary biology]]
[[Category:Motivation]]
[[Category:Motivation]]
[[Category:Arthur Schopenhauer]]

Latest revision as of 10:53, 5 July 2024

The will to live (German: Wille zum Leben) is a concept developed by the German philosopher Arthur Schopenhauer, representing an irrational "blind incessant impulse without knowledge" that drives instinctive behaviors, causing an endless insatiable striving in human existence.

This is contrasted with the concept of the will to survive under life threatening conditions used in psychology[citation needed] since Schopenhauer’s notion of the will to live is more broadly understood as the “animal[istic] force to endure, reproduce and flourish.”[1]

There are significant correlations between the will to live and existential, psychological, social, and physical sources of distress.[2] Many, who overcome near-death experiences with no explanation, have described the will to live as a direct component of their survival.[3] The difference between the wish to die versus the wish to live is also a unique risk factor for suicide.[4]

Concept

[edit]

In psychology, the will to live is the drive for self-preservation, usually coupled with expectations for future improvement in one's state in life.[5] The will to live is an important concept when attempting to understand and comprehend why we do what we do in order to stay alive, and for as long as we can. This can be related to either one's push for survival on the brink of death, or someone who is just trying to find a meaning to continuing their life. Some researchers say that people who have a reason or purpose in life during such dreadful and horrific experiences will often appear to fare better than those that may find such experiences overwhelming.[6] Every day, people undergo countless types of negative experiences, some of which may be demoralizing, hurtful, or tragic. An ongoing question continues to be what keeps the will to live in these situations. People who claim to have had experiences involving the will to live have different explanations behind it.[7]

Background

[edit]

The will to live is considered to be a very basic drive in humans; but not necessarily the main driving force. In psychotherapy, Sigmund Freud termed the pleasure principle, which is the seeking of pleasure and avoiding of pain.[8] Viktor Frankl, who spent time in German concentration camps, developed psychotherapy called logotherapy, which may be translated as the therapy focused on the "will to meaning". Maslow's hierarchy of needs highlights the innate appetite that people possess for love and belonging but before all this there is the very basic and powerful will to live. Psychologists have established that human beings are a goal-oriented species. In assessing the will to live, it should be borne in mind that it could be augmented or diminished by the relative strength of other simultaneously existent drives. Psychologists generally agree[weasel words] that there is the will to live, the will to pleasure, the will to superiority and the will to connection. There are also usually varying degrees of curiosity with regard to what may be termed the will to identity or establishing meaningful personal responses. The will to live is a platform without which it would not be possible to satisfy the other drives. However, this overlooks the possibility that there is a commonality among all creatures that drives all other urges.

Similarity to self-preservation

[edit]

Self-preservation is a behavior that ensures the survival of an organism.[9] Pain and fear are integral parts of this mechanism. Pain motivates the individual to withdraw from damaging situations, to protect a damaged body part while it heals, and to avoid similar experiences in the future.[10] Most pain resolves promptly once the painful stimulus is removed and the body has healed, but sometimes pain persists despite removal of the stimulus and apparent healing of the body; and sometimes pain arises in the absence of any detectable stimulus, damage or disease.[11] Fear causes the organism to seek safety and may cause a release of adrenaline,[12][13] which has the effect of increased strength and heightened senses such as hearing, smell, and sight. Self-preservation may also be interpreted figuratively, in regard to the coping mechanisms one needs to prevent emotional trauma from distorting the mind (see: defence mechanism.)

Even the most simple of living organisms (for example, the single-celled bacteria) are typically under intense selective pressure to evolve a response that would help avoid a damaging environment, if such an environment exists. Organisms also evolve while adapting - even thriving - in a benign environment (for example, a marine sponge modifies its structure in response to current changes, in order to better absorb and process nutrients). Self-preservation is therefore an almost universal hallmark of life. However, when introduced to a novel threat, many species will have a self-preservation response either too specialised, or not specialised enough, to cope with that particular threat.[citation needed] An example is the dodo, which evolved in the absence of natural predators and hence lacked an appropriate, general self-preservation response to heavy predation by humans and rats, showing no fear of them.

Correlations

[edit]

“Existential, psychiatric, social, and, to a lesser degree, physical variables are highly correlated with the will to live”.[14] Existential issues found to correlate significantly include hopelessness, the desire for death, sense of dignity, and burden to others. Psychiatric issues found to be strongly associated are such as depression, anxiety, and lack of concentration. Physical issues that showed the strongest associations were appetite and appearance which did not show the same consistent degree of correlation. The four main predictor variables of the will to live changing over time are anxiety, shortness of breath, depression, and sense of well-being[15] which correlate with the other variable predictors as well. Social variables and quality of life measures are shown to correlate significantly with the will to live such as support and satisfaction with support from family, friends, and health care providers.[16] Findings on the will to live have suggested that psychological variables are replaced by physical mediators of variation as death draws nearer. The will to live has also proven to be highly unstable.[17]

Research

[edit]

Several studies have been conducted testing the theory of the will to live. These studies varied in their focus, but broadly sought to understand the will to live as it differs by demographics, especially as it concerns the elderly and the terminally ill. A study conducted in 2005 asked elderly participants to rate their will to live and tracked this data across time. It found that those who reported a high or stable will to live generally lived longer than those who reported a weak will to live. Additionally, this study proposed that women were generally better able to cope with life-altering or life-threatening conditions and situations than men. However, it also suggested that the participants may not have been of stable health, and that further study was required before drawing any definite conclusions.[18] An earlier study conducted in 2002 tested the idea in terminally ill cancer patients, with most participants being elderly. This study found that those with the weakest will to live typically died sooner than those with a moderate will to live. Those with a high will to live could either die sooner or live as long as those with a moderate will to live. The authors went on to specify that further research is required, testing this theory against other terminal illnesses and in different age categories.[19]

Anecdotal evidence also suggests a correlation between the individual will to live and survival in traumatic situations that include maltreatment. The Second World War and the Holocaust provide concrete examples of this, where many individuals survived years of malnourishment and mistreatment in concentration camps, and cited their will to live as a key part of their survival.[20] A study conducted in 2003 suggested that positive thought (i.e., having a positive outlook on ones' future and life in general) could lower ones' risk for health complications and diseases. This study posited that women who had a more positive outlook were more likely to carry a greater number of antibodies for certain flu strains, which suggested a stronger immune system more generally than those who had a negative outlook.[21] Further anecdotal evidence can be found through quantitative analysis of death records, which consistently show many people dying shortly after major holidays, suggesting that people will themselves to live until the holiday (or in other cases, a birthday), and then passing shortly thereafter.[22][23]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Wicks, Robert (September 9, 2021). "Arthur Schopenhauer". In Zalta, Edward N. (ed.). Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2021 ed.).
  2. ^ "Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death". The Academy of Psychosomatic Medicine (2005)
  3. ^ "What Is the Will to Live? (with picture)". The Health Board. Retrieved 2024-07-05.
  4. ^ Brown, Gregory K.; Steer, RA; Henriques, GR; Beck, AT (October 2005). "The Internal Struggle Between the Wish to Die and the Wish to Live: A Risk Factor for Suicide". American Journal of Psychiatry. 162 (10): 1977–1979. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1977. PMID 16199851.
  5. ^ "will to live". TheFreeDictionary.com. Retrieved 2024-07-05.
  6. ^ Frankl, V. E. (1963). In Man's search for meaning. Boston: Beacon Press.
  7. ^ "The Power of the Will to Live". Psych Central. 2009-12-28. Retrieved 2024-07-05.
  8. ^ Snyder, C. R.; Lopez, Shane J. (2007). Positive Psychology. Sage Publications, Inc. p. 147. ISBN 978-0-7619-2633-7.
  9. ^ "Self-preservation - definition of self-preservation by The Free Dictionary". TheFreeDictionary.com.
  10. ^ Lynn B (1984). "Cutaneous nociceptors". In Winlow W; Holden AV (eds.). The neurobiology of pain: Symposium of the Northern Neurobiology Group, held at Leeds on 18 April 1983. Manchester: Manchester University Press. p. 106. ISBN 0-7190-0996-0.
  11. ^ Raj PP (2007). "Taxonomy and classification of pain". In Niv D; Kreitler S; Diego B; Lamberto A (eds.). The Handbook of Chronic Pain. Nova Biomedical Books. ISBN 978-1-60021-044-0.
  12. ^ Henry Gleitman, Alan J. Fridlund and Daniel Reisberg (2004). Psychology (6 ed.). W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 0-393-97767-6.
  13. ^ "Fear factors". CBC News. 31 October 2007.
  14. ^ Chochinov, H. M.; Hack, T.; Hassard, T.; Kristjanson, L. J.; McClement, S.; Harlos, M. (2005). "Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death". Psychosomatics. 46 (1): 7–10. doi:10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7. PMID 15765815.
  15. ^ Chochinov, H. M.; Tataryn, D.; Clinch, J. J.; Dudgeon, D. (1999). "Will to Live in the Terminally Ill". The Lancet. 354 (9181): 816–819. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(99)80011-7. PMID 10485723. S2CID 44586921.
  16. ^ Chochinov, H. M.; Hack, T.; Hassard, T.; Kristjanson, L. J.; McClement, S.; Harlos, M. (2005). "Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death". Psychosomatics. 46 (1): 7–10. doi:10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7. PMID 15765815.
  17. ^ Chochinov, H. M.; Tataryn, D.; Clinch, J. J.; Dudgeon, D. (1999). "Will to Live in the Terminally Ill". The Lancet. 354 (9181): 816–819. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(99)80011-7. PMID 10485723. S2CID 44586921.
  18. ^ Chochinov, H. M.; Hack, T.; Hassard, T.; Kristjanson, L. J.; McClement, S.; Harlos, M. (2005). "Understanding the Will to Live in Patients Nearing Death". Psychosomatics. 46 (1): 7–10. doi:10.1176/appi.psy.46.1.7. PMID 15765815.
  19. ^ Tataryn, D.; Chochoniv, H. M. (2002). "Predicting the trajectory of will to live in terminally ill patients". Psychosomatics. 43 (5): 370–377. doi:10.1176/appi.psy.43.5.370. PMID 12297605.
  20. ^ Goldenberg, J. Retrieved from http://www.councilforrelationships.org/resources/goldenberg-survival.pdf Archived 2012-04-18 at the Wayback Machine
  21. ^ Goode, Erica (2003-09-02). "Power of Positive Thinking May Have a Health Benefit, Study Says". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 2024-07-05.
  22. ^ Shimizu, M.; Pelham, B.W. (2008). "Postponing a date with the Grim Reaper: Ceremonial events and mortality". Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 30 (1): 36–45. doi:10.1080/01973530701866482. S2CID 143717879.
  23. ^ "The Power of the Will to Live". Psych Central. 2009-12-28. Retrieved 2024-07-05.
[edit]