Major histocompatibility complex and sexual selection: Difference between revisions
ce, needs substantial editing, and it's based on far too much primary research |
Clean up the lede, introduce the basic terminology, remove the specific details of experiments, which belong in the body. |
||
(33 intermediate revisions by 14 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Short description|Adaptive immune gene selection}} |
|||
[[File:Black-Throated Blue Warbler.jpg|thumb|right|240px|MHC sexual selection has been observed in the [[black-throated blue warbler]].]] |
[[File:Black-Throated Blue Warbler.jpg|thumb|right|240px|MHC sexual selection has been observed in the [[black-throated blue warbler]].]] |
||
[[Major histocompatibility complex]] (MHC) genes code for cell surface proteins that facilitate an organism's immune response to pathogens as well as its ability to avoid attacking its own cells. These genes have maintained an unusually high level of [[allele|allelic]] diversity throughout time and throughout different populations. This means that for each MHC gene, many alleles (or gene variants) consistently exist within the population, and many individuals are [[Zygosity|heterozygous]] at MHC [[Locus (genetics)|loci]] (meaning they possess two different alleles for a given gene locus). |
|||
The '''major histocompatibility complex in sexual selection''' concerns how [[major histocompatibility complex]] (MHC) molecules allow for [[immune system]] surveillance of the population of protein molecules in a host's cells. In 1976, Yamazaki et al. demonstrated a [[sexual selection]] [[mate choice]] by male mice for females of a different MHC. |
|||
⚫ | The vast source of [[genetic variation]] affecting an organism's fitness stems from the co-evolutionary arms race between hosts and parasites. There are two hypotheses for explaining the MHC's high diversity, which are not mutually exclusive. One is that there is selection for individuals to possess a diverse set of MHC alleles, which would occur if MHC heterozygotes are more resistant to pathogens than homozygotes—this is called ''[[heterozygote advantage]]''. The second is that there is selection that undergoes a frequency-dependent cycle—this is called the ''[[Red Queen hypothesis]]''. |
||
⚫ | |||
⚫ | There is evidence that many vertebrates, including humans, select their mates based on signals of "compatibility" between their MHC alleles, with a preference for mates with different alleles than their own, resulting in pairings that would tend to produce more heterozygous offspring. There are several proposed hypotheses that address how MHC-associated mating preferences could be adaptive and how an unusually large amount of allelic diversity has been maintained in the MHC.<ref name="Milinski" /><ref name="Dwyer">{{cite journal |vauthors=O'Dwyer TW, Nevitt GA |date=July 2009 |title=Individual odor recognition in procellariiform chicks: potential role for the major histocompatibility complex |journal=[[Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.]] |volume=1170 |pages=442–6 |doi=10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03887.x |pmid=19686174 |s2cid=10004939}}</ref> |
||
⚫ | The vast source of [[genetic variation]] affecting an organism's fitness stems from the co-evolutionary arms race between hosts and parasites. |
||
==Hypotheses== |
==Hypotheses== |
||
In the first hypothesis, if individuals [[heterozygous]] at the MHC are more resistant to [[parasite]]s than those that are [[homozygous]], then it is beneficial for females to choose mates with MHC genes different from their own, and would result in MHC-heterozygous offspring—this is known as [[disassortative mating]]. The hypothesis states that individuals with a heterozygous MHC would be capable of recognizing a wider range of [[pathogen]]s and therefore of inciting a specific immune response against a greater number of pathogens—thus having an [[immunity (medical)|immunity]] advantage. Unfortunately, the MHC-heterozygote advantage hypothesis has not been adequately tested.<ref name="Dwyer"/> A non-MHC immune genes across species exhibit heterozygote disadvantage, or no advantage.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Quéméré |first1=Erwan |last2=Rossi |first2=Sophie |last3=Petit |first3=Elodie |last4=Marchand |first4=Pascal |last5=Merlet |first5=Joël |last6=Game |first6=Yvette |last7=Galan |first7=Maxime |last8=Gilot-Fromont |first8=Emmanuelle |date=2020-03-10 |title=Genetic epidemiology of the Alpine ibex reservoir of persistent and virulent brucellosis outbreak |journal=Scientific Reports |language=en |volume=10 |issue=1 |pages=4400 |doi=10.1038/s41598-020-61299-2 |pmid=32157133 |pmc=7064506 |bibcode=2020NatSR..10.4400Q |issn=2045-2322}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Minias |first1=Piotr |last2=Vinkler |first2=Michal |date=2022-05-01 |title=Selection Balancing at Innate Immune Genes: Adaptive Polymorphism Maintenance in Toll-Like Receptors |url=https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msac102 |journal=Molecular Biology and Evolution |volume=39 |issue=5 |pages=msac102 |doi=10.1093/molbev/msac102 |pmid=35574644 |pmc=9132207 |issn=1537-1719}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Morger |first1=Jennifer |last2=Bajnok |first2=Jaroslav |last3=Boyce |first3=Kellyanne |last4=Craig |first4=Philip S. |last5=Rogan |first5=Michael T. |last6=Lun |first6=Zhao-Rong |last7=Hide |first7=Geoff |last8=Tschirren |first8=Barbara |date=2014-08-01 |title=Naturally occurring Toll-like receptor 11 (TLR11) and Toll-like receptor 12 (TLR12) polymorphisms are not associated with Toxoplasma gondii infection in wild wood mice |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1567134814002019 |journal=Infection, Genetics and Evolution |language=en |volume=26 |pages=180–184 |doi=10.1016/j.meegid.2014.05.032 |pmid=24910107 |issn=1567-1348}}</ref><ref name="Antonides 107">{{Cite journal |last1=Antonides |first1=Jennifer |last2=Mathur |first2=Samarth |last3=Sundaram |first3=Mekala |last4=Ricklefs |first4=Robert |last5=DeWoody |first5=J. Andrew |date=2019-05-22 |title=Immunogenetic response of the bananaquit in the face of malarial parasites |journal=BMC Evolutionary Biology |volume=19 |issue=1 |pages=107 |doi=10.1186/s12862-019-1435-y |issn=1471-2148 |pmc=6529992 |pmid=31113360 |doi-access=free |bibcode=2019BMCEE..19..107A }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Cornetti |first1=Luca |last2=Hilfiker |first2=Daniela |last3=Lemoine |first3=Mélissa |last4=Tschirren |first4=Barbara |date=2018-08-06 |title=Small-scale spatial variation in infection risk shapes the evolution of a''Borrelia''resistance gene in wild rodents |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/mec.14812 |journal=Molecular Ecology |volume=27 |issue=17 |pages=3515–3524 |doi=10.1111/mec.14812 |pmid=30040159 |bibcode=2018MolEc..27.3515C |hdl=10871/33429 |s2cid=51711551 |issn=0962-1083|hdl-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nelson-Flower |first1=Martha J |last2=Germain |first2=Ryan R |last3=MacDougall-Shackleton |first3=Elizabeth A |last4=Taylor |first4=Sabrina S |last5=Arcese |first5=Peter |date=2018-06-27 |title=Purifying Selection in the Toll-Like Receptors of Song Sparrows Melospiza melodia |journal=Journal of Heredity |volume=109 |issue=5 |pages=501–509 |doi=10.1093/jhered/esy027 |pmid=29893971 |issn=0022-1503|doi-access=free }}</ref> In mice, increased MHC heterozygosity reduces fitness, challenging this hypothesis. MHC-heterozygous females had significantly reduced fitness compared to homozygotes.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Ilmonen |first1=Petteri |last2=Penn |first2=Dustin J |last3=Damjanovich |first3=Kristy |last4=Morrison |first4=Linda |last5=Ghotbi |first5=Laleh |last6=Potts |first6=Wayne K |date=2007-08-01 |title=Major Histocompatibility Complex Heterozygosity Reduces Fitness in Experimentally Infected Mice |url=https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.107.074815 |journal=Genetics |volume=176 |issue=4 |pages=2501–2508 |doi=10.1534/genetics.107.074815 |pmid=17603099 |pmc=1950649 |issn=1943-2631}}</ref> This finding has been replicated in another study in mice and again in fish<ref>{{Cite book |last=Joe. |first=Demas, Gregory E. Nelson, Randy |url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/777401230 |title=Ecoimmunology |date=2012 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-987624-2 |pages=238 |oclc=777401230}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=McClelland |first1=Erin E. |last2=Granger |first2=Donald L. |last3=Potts |first3=Wayne K. |date=August 2003 |title=Major Histocompatibility Complex-Dependent Susceptibility to Cryptococcus neoformans in Mice |journal=Infection and Immunity |language=en |volume=71 |issue=8 |pages=4815–4817 |doi=10.1128/IAI.71.8.4815-4817.2003 |issn=0019-9567 |pmc=166009 |pmid=12874366}}</ref> In some cases, excess heterozygosity can lead to decreased fitness.<ref>{{Citation |last=Takahata |first=Naoyuki |title=Polymorphism at MHC Loci and Isolation by the Immune System in Vertebrates |date=1994 |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-2383-3_19 |work=Non-Neutral Evolution: Theories and Molecular Data |pages=233–246 |editor-last=Golding |editor-first=Brian |place=Boston, MA |publisher=Springer US |language=en |doi=10.1007/978-1-4615-2383-3_19 |isbn=978-1-4615-2383-3 |access-date=2022-07-28}}</ref> |
|||
In the first hypothesis, if individuals [[heterozygous]] at the MHC are more resistant to [[parasite]]s than those that are [[homozygous]], then it is beneficial for females to choose mates with MHC genes different from their own, and would result in MHC-heterozygous offspring—this is known as [[disassortative mating]]. Individuals with a heterozygous MHC would be capable of recognizing a wider range of [[pathogen]]s and therefore of inciting a specific immune response against a greater number of pathogens—thus having an [[immunity (medical)|immunity]] advantage. Unfortunately, the MHC-heterozygote advantage hypothesis has not been adequately tested.<ref name="Dwyer"/> |
|||
The optimality hypothesis states too much variability in the MHC can result in a failure of T-cells to distinguish themselves non-selves, and thereby increase the risk of autoimmune disease. This would confer greater fitness to individuals without a large degree MHC diversity.<ref name="Antonides 107"/><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nowak |first1=M A |last2=Tarczy-Hornoch |first2=K |last3=Austyn |first3=J M |date=1992-11-15 |title=The optimal number of major histocompatibility complex molecules in an individual. |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |language=en |volume=89 |issue=22 |pages=10896–10899 |doi=10.1073/pnas.89.22.10896 |issn=0027-8424 |pmc=50449 |pmid=1438295|bibcode=1992PNAS...8910896N |doi-access=free }}</ref> Autoimmune diseases are associated with MHC loci. In humans, those with greater MHC diversity have a greater risk for autoimmune disorders. MHC diversity may be low "because foreign peptides have to stand out against the self-background." On an individual level, MHC diversity tends to be low. Across many species, there is intermediate heterozygosity in the MHC. Overall evidence supports intermediate MHC heterozygosity is best.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Woelfing |first1=Benno |last2=Traulsen |first2=Arne |last3=Milinski |first3=Manfred |last4=Boehm |first4=Thomas |date=2009-01-12 |title=Does intra-individual major histocompatibility complex diversity keep a golden mean? |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |volume=364 |issue=1513 |pages=117–128 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2008.0174 |pmc=2666699 |pmid=18926972}}</ref> |
|||
⚫ | The |
||
⚫ | The Red Queen hypothesis asserts that MHC diversity is maintained by parasites. If individuals' MHC [[allele]]s render different resistances to a particular parasite, then the allele with the highest resistance is favored, selected for, and consequently spread throughout the population. [[Genetic recombination|Recombination]] and [[mutation]] cause generation of new variants among offspring, which may facilitate a quick response to rapidly evolving parasites or pathogens with much shorter generation times. However, if this particular allele becomes common, selection pressure on parasites to avoid recognition by this common allele increases. An advantageous characteristic that allows a parasite to escape recognition spreads, and causes selection against what was formerly a resistant allele. This enables the parasite to escape this cycle of frequency-dependent selection, and such a cycle eventually leads to a co-evolutionary arms race that may support the maintenance of MHC diversity. This hypothesis has empirical support.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Lampert |first1=K. P. |last2=Fischer |first2=P. |last3=Schartl |first3=M. |date=March 2009 |title=Major histocompatibility complex variability in the clonal Amazon molly, Poecilia formosa : is copy number less important than genotype? |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04097.x |journal=Molecular Ecology |language=en |volume=18 |issue=6 |pages=1124–1136 |doi=10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04097.x|pmid=19226318 |bibcode=2009MolEc..18.1124L |s2cid=13068773 }}</ref><ref name="Dwyer" /><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Šimková |first1=Andrea |last2=Košař |first2=Martin |last3=Vetešník |first3=Lukáš |last4=Vyskočilová |first4=Martina |date=2013-06-14 |title=MHC genes and parasitism in Carassius gibelio, a diploid-triploid fish species with dual reproduction strategies |journal=BMC Evolutionary Biology |volume=13 |issue=1 |pages=122 |doi=10.1186/1471-2148-13-122 |issn=1471-2148 |pmc=3691641 |pmid=23768177 |doi-access=free |bibcode=2013BMCEE..13..122S }}</ref> |
||
[[File:Parasitismus.jpg|right|thumb|Parasites are in a constant arms race with their host: [[harvestman]] suffering from [[mite]] pest]] |
[[File:Parasitismus.jpg|right|thumb|Parasites are in a constant arms race with their host: [[harvestman]] suffering from [[mite]] pest]] |
||
The [[inbreeding avoidance]] hypothesis has less to do with host-parasite relationships than does the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis. |
The [[inbreeding avoidance]] hypothesis has less to do with host-parasite relationships than does the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis. The extreme diversity in the MHC would cause individuals sharing MHC alleles to be more likely to be related. As a result, one function of MHC-disassortative mating would be to avoid mating with family members and any harmful genetic consequences that could occur as a result. The hypothesis states that inbreeding increases the amount of overall homozygosity—not just locally in the MHC, so an increase in genetic homozygosity may be accompanied not only by the expression of recessive diseases and mutations, but by the loss of any potential heterozygote advantage as well.<ref name="Westemeier">{{cite journal |vauthors=Westemeier RL, Brawn JD, Simpson SA, etal |title=Tracking the long-term decline and recovery of an isolated population |journal=[[Science (journal)|Science]] |volume=282 |issue=5394 |pages=1695–8 |date=November 1998 |pmid=9831558 |doi= 10.1126/science.282.5394.1695 |bibcode=1998Sci...282.1695W |s2cid=19726112 }}</ref><ref name="Dwyer"/> Animals only rarely avoid inbreeding.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Schlupp |first1=Ingo |last2=Berbel-Filho |first2=Waldir |date=2021-06-03 |title=Faculty Opinions recommendation of Meta-analytic evidence that animals rarely avoid inbreeding. |doi=10.3410/f.740048135.793586159 |doi-access=free|s2cid=243521297 }}</ref> The inbreeding avoidance hypothesis has been "ruled out as an explanation for the observed pattern of MHC-dependent mate preference" because relatedness is not associated with mate choice.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Bernatchez |first1=L. |last2=Landry |first2=C. |date=May 2003 |title=MHC studies in nonmodel vertebrates: what have we learned about natural selection in 15 years? |journal=Journal of Evolutionary Biology |language=en |volume=16 |issue=3 |pages=363–377 |doi=10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00531.x |pmid=14635837 |s2cid=28094212 |issn=1010-061X|doi-access=free }}</ref> |
||
In the course of searching for potential mates, it would benefit females to be able to discriminate against "bad" genes in order to increase the health and viability of their offspring. |
In the course of searching for potential mates, it would benefit females to be able to discriminate against "bad" genes in order to increase the health and viability of their offspring. If female [[mate choice]] occurs for "good" genes, then it is implied that genetic variation exists among males. Furthermore, one would presume that said difference in genes would impart a difference in fitness as well, which could potentially be chosen or selected for. |
||
Generally, the extreme [[Genetic polymorphism|polymorphism]] of MHC genes is selected for by host-parasite arms races (the Red Queen hypothesis); however, disassortative mate choice may maintain [[genetic diversity]] in some species. |
Generally, the extreme [[Genetic polymorphism|polymorphism]] of MHC genes is selected for by host-parasite arms races (the [[Red Queen hypothesis]]); however, disassortative mate choice may maintain [[genetic diversity]] in some species. Depending on how parasites alter selection on MHC alleles, MHC-dependent mate-choice may increase the fitness of the offspring by enhancing its immunity, as mentioned earlier. |
||
If this is the case, either through the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis, then selection also favors mating practices that are MHC-dependent. |
If this is the case, either through the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis, then selection also favors mating practices that are MHC-dependent. |
||
[[File:Paonroue.JPG|thumb|left|The exaggerated, elongated upper tail coverts make up the "train" of the peacock.]]Therefore, mate choice—with respect to the MHC—has probably evolved so that females choose males either based on diverse genes (heterozygote advantage and inbreeding avoidance hypotheses) or "good" genes. |
[[File:Paonroue.JPG|thumb|left|The exaggerated, elongated upper tail coverts make up the "train" of the peacock.]]Therefore, mate choice—with respect to the MHC—has probably evolved so that females choose males either based on diverse genes (heterozygote advantage and inbreeding avoidance hypotheses) or "good" genes. The fact that females choose is naturally selected, as it would be an advantageous trait for females to be able to choose a male that provided either an indirect or direct benefit. As a result of female choice, sexual selection is imposed on males. This is evidenced by genetic "advertisement"—an example of this would be the existence of exaggerated traits, such as the elaborate tail-feathers of male peacocks. However, in humans, both sexes exert mate choice. |
||
== The relationship between olfaction and MHC == |
== The relationship between olfaction and MHC == |
||
MHC-based sexual selection is known to involve olfactory mechanisms in such vertebrate taxa as fish, mice, humans, primates, birds, and reptiles.<ref name="Milinski">{{cite journal |vauthors=Milinski M, Griffiths S, Wegner KM, Reusch TB, Haas-Assenbaum A, Boehm T |title=Mate choice decisions of stickleback females predictably modified by MHC peptide ligands |journal=[[Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.]] |volume=102 |issue=12 |pages=4414–8 |date=March 2005 |pmid=15755811 |pmc=555479 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0408264102 | |
MHC-based sexual selection is known to involve olfactory mechanisms in such vertebrate taxa as fish, mice, humans, primates, birds, and reptiles.<ref name="Milinski">{{cite journal |vauthors=Milinski M, Griffiths S, Wegner KM, Reusch TB, Haas-Assenbaum A, Boehm T |title=Mate choice decisions of stickleback females predictably modified by MHC peptide ligands |journal=[[Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.]] |volume=102 |issue=12 |pages=4414–8 |date=March 2005 |pmid=15755811 |pmc=555479 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0408264102 |bibcode=2005PNAS..102.4414M |doi-access=free }}</ref> At its simplest level, humans have long been acquainted with the sense of olfaction for its use in determining the pleasantness or the unpleasantness of one's resources, food, etc. At a deeper level, it has been predicted that olfaction serves to personally identify individuals based upon the genes of the MHC.<ref name="Yamazaki">{{cite journal |vauthors=Yamazaki K, Beauchamp GK, Singer A, Bard J, Boyse EA |title=Odortypes: their origin and composition |journal=[[Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.]] |volume=96 |issue=4 |pages=1522–5 |date=February 1999 |pmid=9990056 |pmc=15502 |doi= 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1522|bibcode=1999PNAS...96.1522Y |doi-access=free }}</ref> |
||
[[File:Olfactory system.svg|thumb|right|340px|Human olfactory system. 1: [[Olfactory bulb]] 2: [[Mitral cells]] 3: [[Bone]] 4: Nasal [[epithelium]] 5: [[Glomerulus (olfaction)]] 6: [[Olfactory receptor|Olfactory receptor cells]]]] |
[[File:Olfactory system.svg|thumb|right|340px|Human olfactory system. 1: [[Olfactory bulb]] 2: [[Mitral cells]] 3: [[Bone]] 4: Nasal [[epithelium]] 5: [[Glomerulus (olfaction)]] 6: [[Olfactory receptor|Olfactory receptor cells]]]] |
||
Chemosensation, which is one of the most primitive senses, has evolved into a specialized sensory system. |
Chemosensation, which is one of the most primitive senses, has evolved into a specialized sensory system. Humans can not only detect, but also assess, and respond to environmental (chemical) olfactory cues—especially those used to evoke behavioral and sexual responses from other individuals, also known as [[pheromone]]s. Pheromones function to communicate one's species, sex, and perhaps most importantly one's genetic identity. The genes of the MHC provide the basis from which a set of unique olfactory coding develops.<ref name="Yamazaki"/> |
||
Although it is not known exactly how MHC-specific odors are recognized, it is currently believed that proteins bound to the peptide-binding groove of the MHC may produce the odorant. |
Although it is not known exactly how MHC-specific odors are recognized, it is currently believed that proteins bound to the peptide-binding groove of the MHC may produce the odorant. Each MHC protein binds to a specific peptide sequence, yielding a set of uniquely bound peptide-MHC complexes for each individual. During cellular turnover, the MHC-peptide complex is shed from the cell surface and the fragments are dispensed in bodily fluids such as blood serum, saliva, and urine. Scientists believe that commensal microflora, microorganisms that line epithelial surfaces open to the external environment such as the gastrointestinal tract and vagina, further degrade these fragments, which are made volatile by this process. |
||
Recently, it has been shown that receptors in the [[vomeronasal organ]] of mice are activated by peptides having similar characteristics to MHC proteins; further studies may hopefully soon clarify the exact transformation between MHC genotype and an olfactory mechanism.<ref name="Milinski"/><ref name="Yamazaki"/><ref name="Bhutta">{{cite journal |author=Bhutta MF |title=Sex and the nose: human pheromonal responses |journal=[[J R Soc Med]] |volume=100 |issue=6 |pages=268–74 |date=June 2007 |pmid=17541097 |pmc=1885393 |doi=10. |
Recently, it has been shown that receptors in the [[vomeronasal organ]] of mice are activated by peptides having similar characteristics to MHC proteins; further studies may hopefully soon clarify the exact transformation between MHC genotype and an olfactory mechanism.<ref name="Milinski"/><ref name="Yamazaki"/><ref name="Bhutta">{{cite journal |author=Bhutta MF |title=Sex and the nose: human pheromonal responses |journal=[[J R Soc Med]] |volume=100 |issue=6 |pages=268–74 |date=June 2007 |pmid=17541097 |pmc=1885393 |doi=10.1177/014107680710000612 |url=http://www.jrsm.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=17541097}}</ref> |
||
== Empirical evidence == |
== Empirical evidence == |
||
=== In humans === |
=== In humans === |
||
MHC similarity in humans has been studied in three broad ways: odor, facial attractiveness, and actual mate choice.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal |title=MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review |doi=10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007 | volume=34 |journal=Psychoneuroendocrinology |pages=497–512 |pmid=19054623 |date=May 2009 |vauthors=Havlicek J, Roberts SC}}</ref> Studies of odor find MHC-dissimilarity preferences but vary in details, while facial attractiveness favors MHC-similarity and actual mating studies are varied.<ref name=":0" /> |
MHC similarity in humans has been studied in three broad ways: odor, facial attractiveness, and actual mate choice.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal |title=MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review |doi=10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007 | volume=34 |issue=4 |journal=Psychoneuroendocrinology |pages=497–512 |pmid=19054623 |date=May 2009 |vauthors=Havlicek J, Roberts SC|s2cid=40332494 }}</ref> Studies of odor find MHC-dissimilarity preferences but vary in details, while facial attractiveness favors MHC-similarity and actual mating studies are varied.<ref name=":0" /> |
||
==== Specific studies ==== |
==== Specific studies ==== |
||
Several studies suggest that MHC-related odor preferences and mate choice are demonstrated by humans. |
Several studies suggest that MHC-related odor preferences and mate choice are demonstrated by humans. However, the role of MHC in human mate choice has been relatively controversial. One study conducted by Ober et al. examined HLA types from 400 couples in the [[Hutterite]] community and found dramatically fewer HLA matches between husbands and wives than expected when considering the social structure of their community.<ref name="Chaix">{{cite journal |vauthors=Chaix R, Cao C, Donnelly P |title=Is mate choice in humans MHC-dependent? |journal=[[PLoS Genet.]] |volume=4 |issue=9 |pages=e1000184 |year=2008 |pmid=18787687 |pmc=2519788 |doi=10.1371/journal.pgen.1000184 |doi-access=free }}</ref> On the other hand, there was no evidence of MHC-based mate choice in the same study of 200 couples from South Amerindian tribes.<ref name="Chaix" /> |
||
Other studies have approached mate choice based on odor preference. |
Other studies have approached mate choice based on odor preference. In one study done by Wedekind et al., women were asked to smell male axillary odors collected on T-shirts worn by different males. Women that were ovulating rated the odors of MHC-dissimilar men as more pleasant than those of the MHC-similar men. Furthermore, odors of MHC-dissimilar men often reminded women of current or former partners, suggesting that odor—specifically odor for MHC-dissimilarity—plays a role in mate choice.<ref name="Roberts">{{cite journal |vauthors=Roberts SC, Gosling LM, Carter V, Petrie M |title=MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=275 |issue=1652 |pages=2715–22 |date=December 2008 |pmid=18700206 |pmc=2605820 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.0825 }}</ref> |
||
In another study done by Wedekind et al., 121 women and men were asked to rank the pleasantness of the odors of sweaty T-shirts. |
In another study done by Wedekind et al., 121 women and men were asked to rank the pleasantness of the odors of sweaty T-shirts. Upon smelling the shirts, it was found that men and women who were reminded of their own mate or ex-mate had dramatically fewer MHC alleles in common with the wearer than would be expected by chance. If the selection for shirts was not random, and actually selected for MHC-dissimilar alleles, this suggests that MHC genetic composition does influence mate choice. Furthermore, when the degree of similarity between the wearer and the smeller was statistically accounted for, there was no longer a significant influence of MHC on odor preference. The results show that MHC similarity or dissimilarity certainly plays a role in mate choice. Specifically, MHC-disassortative mate choice and less similar MHC combinations are selected for.<ref name="Wedekind">{{cite journal |vauthors=Wedekind C, Füri S |title=Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity? |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=264 |issue=1387 |pages=1471–9 |date=October 1997 |pmid=9364787 |pmc=1688704 |doi=10.1098/rspb.1997.0204 }}</ref> One interesting aspect of the Wedekind's experiment was that in contrast to normally cycling women, women taking oral contraceptives preferred odors of MHC-similar men. This would suggest that the pill may interfere with the adaptive preference for dissimilarity.<ref name="Roberts"/><ref name="Wedekind"/> |
||
=== In primates === |
=== In primates === |
||
There is evidence of MHC-associated mate choice in other primates. |
There is evidence of MHC-associated mate choice in other primates. In the grey mouse lemur ''[[Microcebus murinus]]'', post-copulatory mate-choice is associated with genetic constitution. Fathers are more MHC-dissimilar from the mother than are randomly tested males. Fathers have more differences in amino acid and microsatellite diversity than did randomly tested males. It is hypothesized that this is caused by female cryptic choice.<ref name="Schwensow">{{cite journal |vauthors=Schwensow N, Eberle M, Sommer S |title=Compatibility counts: MHC-associated mate choice in a wild promiscuous primate |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=275 |issue=1634 |pages=555–64 |date=March 2008 |pmid=18089539 |pmc=2596809 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2007.1433 }}</ref> |
||
=== In other animals === |
=== In other animals === |
||
In mice, both males and females choose MHC-dissimilar partners. Mice develop the ability to identify family members during early growth and are known to avoid inbreeding with kin, which would support the MHC-mediated mate choice hypothesis for inbreeding avoidance.<ref name="Dwyer"/> |
In mice, both males and females choose MHC-dissimilar partners. Mice develop the ability to identify family members during early growth and are known to avoid inbreeding with kin, which would support the MHC-mediated mate choice hypothesis for inbreeding avoidance.<ref name="Dwyer"/> |
||
Fish are another group of vertebrates shown to display MHC-associated mate choice. |
Fish are another group of vertebrates shown to display MHC-associated mate choice. Scientists tested the Atlantic salmon, ''[[Salmo salar]]'', by observing effects of MHC upon natural spawning salmon that resided in the river versus artificial crosses that were carried out in hatcheries. Logically, the artificial crosses would be bereft of the benefits of mate choice that would naturally be available. The results showed that the offspring of the artificially bred salmon were more infected with parasites: almost four times more than the naturally-spawned offspring were. In addition, wild offspring were more MHC-heterozygous than the artificially-bred offspring. These results support the Heterozygous Advantage hypothesis of sexual selection for MHC-dissimilar mate choice.<ref name="Consuegra">{{cite journal |vauthors=Consuegra S, Garcia de Leaniz C |title=MHC-mediated mate choice increases parasite resistance in salmon |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=275 |issue=1641 |pages=1397–403 |date=June 2008 |pmid=18364312 |pmc=2602703 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.0066 }}</ref> In another fish, the three-spined stickleback, it has been shown that females desire MHC diversity in their offspring, which affects their mate choice.<ref name="Kurtz">{{cite journal |vauthors=Kurtz J, Kalbe M, Aeschlimann PB, etal |title=Major histocompatibility complex diversity influences parasite resistance and innate immunity in sticklebacks |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=271 |issue=1535 |pages=197–204 |date=January 2004 |pmid=15058398 |pmc=1691569 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2003.2567 }}</ref> |
||
Female [[Savannah sparrow]]s, ''Passerculus sandwichensis'', chose MHC-dissimilar males to mate with. Females are more likely to engage in extra-pair relationships if paired with MHC-similar mates and more dissimilar mates are available. Similarly, MHC diversity in house sparrows, ''Passer domesticus'', suggests that MHC-disassortative mate choice occurs.<ref name="Dwyer"/> |
Female [[Savannah sparrow]]s, ''Passerculus sandwichensis'', chose MHC-dissimilar males to mate with. Females are more likely to engage in extra-pair relationships if paired with MHC-similar mates and more dissimilar mates are available. Similarly, MHC diversity in house sparrows, ''Passer domesticus'', suggests that MHC-disassortative mate choice occurs.<ref name="Dwyer"/> |
||
MHC-mediated mate choice has been shown to exist in Swedish sand lizards, ''[[Lacerta agilis]]''. Females preferred to associate with odor samples obtained from males more distantly related at the MHC I loci.<ref name="Olsson2003">{{cite journal |vauthors=Olsson M, Madsen T, Nordby J, Wapstra E, Ujvari B, Wittsell H |title=Major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in sand lizards |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=270 |
MHC-mediated mate choice has been shown to exist in Swedish sand lizards, ''[[Lacerta agilis]]''. Females preferred to associate with odor samples obtained from males more distantly related at the MHC I loci.<ref name="Olsson2003">{{cite journal |vauthors=Olsson M, Madsen T, Nordby J, Wapstra E, Ujvari B, Wittsell H |title=Major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in sand lizards |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=270 |pages=S254–6 |date=November 2003 |issue=Suppl 2 |pmid=14667398 |pmc=1809963 |doi=10.1098/rsbl.2003.0079 }}</ref> |
||
Even though many species are socially monogamous, females can accept or actively seek mating outside of the relationship;<ref name="Suter">{{cite journal |vauthors=Suter SM, Keiser M, Feignoux R, Meyer DR |title=Reed bunting females increase fitness through extra-pair mating with genetically dissimilar males |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=274 |issue=1627 |pages=2865–71 |date=November 2007 |pmid=17785270 |pmc=2288684 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2007.0799 |
Even though many species are socially monogamous, females can accept or actively seek mating outside of the relationship;<ref name="Suter">{{cite journal |vauthors=Suter SM, Keiser M, Feignoux R, Meyer DR |title=Reed bunting females increase fitness through extra-pair mating with genetically dissimilar males |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=274 |issue=1627 |pages=2865–71 |date=November 2007 |pmid=17785270 |pmc=2288684 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2007.0799 }}</ref> extra-pair paternity is a mating pattern known to be affiliated with MHC-associated mate choice. Birds are one of the more commonly studied groups of animals to exhibit this sexual behavior. In the scarlet rosefinch ''[[Carpocus erythrinus]]'', females engaged in extra-pair paternity much less frequently when their mates were MHC-heterozygous.<ref>Promerová Vinkler. Occurrence of extra-pair paternity is connected to social male's MHC-variability in the scarlet rosefinch ''Carpodacus erythrinus.'' Journal of Avian Biology 42, 5-10(2011).</ref> In the Seychelles warbler ''[[Acrocephalus sechellensis]]'', there was no evidence of MHC variation between social mates. However, when females' social mates were MHC-similar, they were more likely to participate in extra-pair paternity; in most cases, the extra-pair male was significantly more MHC-dissimilar than the social mate.<ref name="Richardson">{{cite journal |vauthors=Richardson DS, Komdeur J, Burke T, von Schantz T |title=MHC-based patterns of social and extra-pair mate choice in the Seychelles warbler |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=272 |issue=1564 |pages=759–67 |date=April 2005 |pmid=15870038 |pmc=1602051 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2004.3028 }}</ref> |
||
MHC-mediated mate choice may occur after copulation, at the gametic level, through sperm competition or female cryptic choice. |
MHC-mediated mate choice may occur after copulation, at the gametic level, through sperm competition or female cryptic choice. The [[Atlantic salmon]], ''Salmo salar'', is one species in which sperm competition is influenced by the variation in the major histocompatibility complex, specifically that of the Class I alleles. Atlantic salmon males have higher rates of successful fertilization when competing for eggs from females genetically similar at the class I genes of the MHC.<ref name="Yeates">{{cite journal |vauthors=Yeates SE, Einum S, Fleming IA, etal |title=Atlantic salmon eggs favour sperm in competition that have similar major histocompatibility alleles |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=276 |issue=1656 |pages=559–66 |date=February 2009 |pmid=18854296 |pmc=2592554 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.1257 }}</ref> |
||
Another species that exhibits MHC-associated cryptic choice is the Arctic charr ''[[Salvelinus alpinus]]''. |
Another species that exhibits MHC-associated cryptic choice is the Arctic charr ''[[Salvelinus alpinus]]''. In this case, however, it seems that sperm selection is more dependent on the ovum. |
||
MHC-heterozygous males were found to have significantly more fertilization success than MHC-homozygous males; sperm count, motility, and swimming velocity were not shown to significantly co-vary with similarity or dissimilarity at the MHC. |
MHC-heterozygous males were found to have significantly more fertilization success than MHC-homozygous males; sperm count, motility, and swimming velocity were not shown to significantly co-vary with similarity or dissimilarity at the MHC. It is proposed that there is a chemo-attraction system responsible for the egg itself being able to discriminate and selectively choose between MHC-heterozygous and MHC-homozygous males.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Skarstein F ''et al'' | year = 2005 | title = MHC and fertilization success in the Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) | journal = Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | volume = 57 | issue = 4| pages = 374–380 | doi = 10.1007/s00265-004-0860-z | s2cid = 42727644 }}</ref> |
||
Contrary to the Atlantic salmon and the Arctic char, red junglefowl ''[[Gallus gallus]]'' males instead of females exert cryptic preference. |
Contrary to the Atlantic salmon and the Arctic char, red junglefowl ''[[Gallus gallus]]'' males instead of females exert cryptic preference. Male junglefowl showed no preference when simultaneously presented with both an MHC-dissimilar and an MHC-similar female. However, they did show a cryptic preference by allocating more sperm to the more MHC-dissimilar of the two.<ref name="Gillingham">{{cite journal |vauthors=Gillingham MA, Richardson DS, Løvlie H, Moynihan A, Worley K, Pizzari T |title=Cryptic preference for MHC-dissimilar females in male red junglefowl, Gallus gallus |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=276 |issue=1659 |pages=1083–92 |date=March 2009 |pmid=19129124 |pmc=2679071 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.1549 }}</ref> |
||
Male sand lizards ''[[Lacerta agilis]]'' behave similarly to the male junglefowl. Initial copulation between a male and a female without any rivals was shown to be extended when the male sensed a higher female fecundity. However, second males adjusted the duration of their copulation depending on the relatedness between the female and the first male, believed to be determined by the MHC-odor of the copulatory plug. |
Male sand lizards ''[[Lacerta agilis]]'' behave similarly to the male junglefowl. Initial copulation between a male and a female without any rivals was shown to be extended when the male sensed a higher female fecundity. However, second males adjusted the duration of their copulation depending on the relatedness between the female and the first male, believed to be determined by the MHC-odor of the copulatory plug. A closer genetic relatedness between a male and a female sand lizard increased the chances for a successful fertilization and rate of paternity for the second male.<ref name="Olsson2004">{{cite journal |vauthors=Olsson M, Madsen T, Ujvari B, Wapstra E |title=Fecundity and MHC affects ejaculation tactics and paternity bias in sand lizards |journal=[[Evolution (journal)|Evolution]] |volume=58 |issue=4 |pages=906–9 |date=April 2004 |pmid=15154566 |doi= 10.1554/03-610|hdl=10536/DRO/DU:30066491 |s2cid=198152712 |hdl-access=free }}</ref> |
||
Abortional selection may be a form of cryptic female choice. Many studies on humans and rodents have found that females may spontaneously abort pregnancies in which the offspring is too MHC-similar.{{Citation needed|date=October 2016}} |
Abortional selection may be a form of cryptic female choice. Many studies on humans and rodents have found that females may spontaneously abort pregnancies in which the offspring is too MHC-similar.{{Citation needed|date=October 2016}} In addition, in vitro fertilizations are more likely to fail when couples have similar MHC genes.{{Citation needed|date=October 2016}} |
||
== MHC and sexual conflict == |
== MHC and sexual conflict == |
||
Line 72: | Line 75: | ||
If males attempt to thwart female mate choice by mating with a female against her will, sexual conflict may interfere with the choice for compatibility at the MHC genes. |
If males attempt to thwart female mate choice by mating with a female against her will, sexual conflict may interfere with the choice for compatibility at the MHC genes. |
||
In [[Chinook salmon]] ''Oncorhyncus tshawytscha'', females act more aggressively towards MHC-similar males than MHC-dissimilar males, suggesting the presence of female mate choice. |
In [[Chinook salmon]] ''Oncorhyncus tshawytscha'', females act more aggressively towards MHC-similar males than MHC-dissimilar males, suggesting the presence of female mate choice. Furthermore, males directed aggression at MHC-similar females. This was accompanied by male harassment of unreceptive females; however, there was a positive correlation between male aggression and reproductive success. The ability of the males to over-power the females' original mate choice resulted in the offspring of the targets of male aggression having low genetic diversity. Offspring with high genetic diversity seemed to happen only when the operational sex ratio was female-biased, when females were more likely to be able to exert mate choice, and males were less likely to harass females. These results suggest that sexual conflict may interfere with female mate choice for 'good' MHC genes.<ref name="Garner">{{cite journal |vauthors=Garner SR, Bortoluzzi RN, Heath DD, Neff BD |title=Sexual conflict inhibits female mate choice for major histocompatibility complex dissimilarity in Chinook salmon |journal=[[Proc. Biol. Sci.]] |volume=277 |issue=1683 |pages=885–94 |date=March 2010 |pmid=19864282 |pmc=2842720 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2009.1639 }}</ref> |
||
== See also == |
== See also == |
||
Line 78: | Line 81: | ||
*[[Body odor and subconscious human sexual attraction]] |
*[[Body odor and subconscious human sexual attraction]] |
||
*[[Pheromone]] |
*[[Pheromone]] |
||
*''[[The Compatibility Gene]]'' |
|||
== References == |
== References == |
Latest revision as of 16:17, 7 July 2024
Major histocompatibility complex (MHC) genes code for cell surface proteins that facilitate an organism's immune response to pathogens as well as its ability to avoid attacking its own cells. These genes have maintained an unusually high level of allelic diversity throughout time and throughout different populations. This means that for each MHC gene, many alleles (or gene variants) consistently exist within the population, and many individuals are heterozygous at MHC loci (meaning they possess two different alleles for a given gene locus).
The vast source of genetic variation affecting an organism's fitness stems from the co-evolutionary arms race between hosts and parasites. There are two hypotheses for explaining the MHC's high diversity, which are not mutually exclusive. One is that there is selection for individuals to possess a diverse set of MHC alleles, which would occur if MHC heterozygotes are more resistant to pathogens than homozygotes—this is called heterozygote advantage. The second is that there is selection that undergoes a frequency-dependent cycle—this is called the Red Queen hypothesis.
There is evidence that many vertebrates, including humans, select their mates based on signals of "compatibility" between their MHC alleles, with a preference for mates with different alleles than their own, resulting in pairings that would tend to produce more heterozygous offspring. There are several proposed hypotheses that address how MHC-associated mating preferences could be adaptive and how an unusually large amount of allelic diversity has been maintained in the MHC.[1][2]
Hypotheses
[edit]In the first hypothesis, if individuals heterozygous at the MHC are more resistant to parasites than those that are homozygous, then it is beneficial for females to choose mates with MHC genes different from their own, and would result in MHC-heterozygous offspring—this is known as disassortative mating. The hypothesis states that individuals with a heterozygous MHC would be capable of recognizing a wider range of pathogens and therefore of inciting a specific immune response against a greater number of pathogens—thus having an immunity advantage. Unfortunately, the MHC-heterozygote advantage hypothesis has not been adequately tested.[2] A non-MHC immune genes across species exhibit heterozygote disadvantage, or no advantage.[3][4][5][6][7][8] In mice, increased MHC heterozygosity reduces fitness, challenging this hypothesis. MHC-heterozygous females had significantly reduced fitness compared to homozygotes.[9] This finding has been replicated in another study in mice and again in fish[10][11] In some cases, excess heterozygosity can lead to decreased fitness.[12]
The optimality hypothesis states too much variability in the MHC can result in a failure of T-cells to distinguish themselves non-selves, and thereby increase the risk of autoimmune disease. This would confer greater fitness to individuals without a large degree MHC diversity.[6][13] Autoimmune diseases are associated with MHC loci. In humans, those with greater MHC diversity have a greater risk for autoimmune disorders. MHC diversity may be low "because foreign peptides have to stand out against the self-background." On an individual level, MHC diversity tends to be low. Across many species, there is intermediate heterozygosity in the MHC. Overall evidence supports intermediate MHC heterozygosity is best.[14]
The Red Queen hypothesis asserts that MHC diversity is maintained by parasites. If individuals' MHC alleles render different resistances to a particular parasite, then the allele with the highest resistance is favored, selected for, and consequently spread throughout the population. Recombination and mutation cause generation of new variants among offspring, which may facilitate a quick response to rapidly evolving parasites or pathogens with much shorter generation times. However, if this particular allele becomes common, selection pressure on parasites to avoid recognition by this common allele increases. An advantageous characteristic that allows a parasite to escape recognition spreads, and causes selection against what was formerly a resistant allele. This enables the parasite to escape this cycle of frequency-dependent selection, and such a cycle eventually leads to a co-evolutionary arms race that may support the maintenance of MHC diversity. This hypothesis has empirical support.[15][2][16]
The inbreeding avoidance hypothesis has less to do with host-parasite relationships than does the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis. The extreme diversity in the MHC would cause individuals sharing MHC alleles to be more likely to be related. As a result, one function of MHC-disassortative mating would be to avoid mating with family members and any harmful genetic consequences that could occur as a result. The hypothesis states that inbreeding increases the amount of overall homozygosity—not just locally in the MHC, so an increase in genetic homozygosity may be accompanied not only by the expression of recessive diseases and mutations, but by the loss of any potential heterozygote advantage as well.[17][2] Animals only rarely avoid inbreeding.[18] The inbreeding avoidance hypothesis has been "ruled out as an explanation for the observed pattern of MHC-dependent mate preference" because relatedness is not associated with mate choice.[19]
In the course of searching for potential mates, it would benefit females to be able to discriminate against "bad" genes in order to increase the health and viability of their offspring. If female mate choice occurs for "good" genes, then it is implied that genetic variation exists among males. Furthermore, one would presume that said difference in genes would impart a difference in fitness as well, which could potentially be chosen or selected for.
Generally, the extreme polymorphism of MHC genes is selected for by host-parasite arms races (the Red Queen hypothesis); however, disassortative mate choice may maintain genetic diversity in some species. Depending on how parasites alter selection on MHC alleles, MHC-dependent mate-choice may increase the fitness of the offspring by enhancing its immunity, as mentioned earlier. If this is the case, either through the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis, then selection also favors mating practices that are MHC-dependent.
Therefore, mate choice—with respect to the MHC—has probably evolved so that females choose males either based on diverse genes (heterozygote advantage and inbreeding avoidance hypotheses) or "good" genes. The fact that females choose is naturally selected, as it would be an advantageous trait for females to be able to choose a male that provided either an indirect or direct benefit. As a result of female choice, sexual selection is imposed on males. This is evidenced by genetic "advertisement"—an example of this would be the existence of exaggerated traits, such as the elaborate tail-feathers of male peacocks. However, in humans, both sexes exert mate choice.
The relationship between olfaction and MHC
[edit]MHC-based sexual selection is known to involve olfactory mechanisms in such vertebrate taxa as fish, mice, humans, primates, birds, and reptiles.[1] At its simplest level, humans have long been acquainted with the sense of olfaction for its use in determining the pleasantness or the unpleasantness of one's resources, food, etc. At a deeper level, it has been predicted that olfaction serves to personally identify individuals based upon the genes of the MHC.[20]
Chemosensation, which is one of the most primitive senses, has evolved into a specialized sensory system. Humans can not only detect, but also assess, and respond to environmental (chemical) olfactory cues—especially those used to evoke behavioral and sexual responses from other individuals, also known as pheromones. Pheromones function to communicate one's species, sex, and perhaps most importantly one's genetic identity. The genes of the MHC provide the basis from which a set of unique olfactory coding develops.[20]
Although it is not known exactly how MHC-specific odors are recognized, it is currently believed that proteins bound to the peptide-binding groove of the MHC may produce the odorant. Each MHC protein binds to a specific peptide sequence, yielding a set of uniquely bound peptide-MHC complexes for each individual. During cellular turnover, the MHC-peptide complex is shed from the cell surface and the fragments are dispensed in bodily fluids such as blood serum, saliva, and urine. Scientists believe that commensal microflora, microorganisms that line epithelial surfaces open to the external environment such as the gastrointestinal tract and vagina, further degrade these fragments, which are made volatile by this process. Recently, it has been shown that receptors in the vomeronasal organ of mice are activated by peptides having similar characteristics to MHC proteins; further studies may hopefully soon clarify the exact transformation between MHC genotype and an olfactory mechanism.[1][20][21]
Empirical evidence
[edit]In humans
[edit]MHC similarity in humans has been studied in three broad ways: odor, facial attractiveness, and actual mate choice.[22] Studies of odor find MHC-dissimilarity preferences but vary in details, while facial attractiveness favors MHC-similarity and actual mating studies are varied.[22]
Specific studies
[edit]Several studies suggest that MHC-related odor preferences and mate choice are demonstrated by humans. However, the role of MHC in human mate choice has been relatively controversial. One study conducted by Ober et al. examined HLA types from 400 couples in the Hutterite community and found dramatically fewer HLA matches between husbands and wives than expected when considering the social structure of their community.[23] On the other hand, there was no evidence of MHC-based mate choice in the same study of 200 couples from South Amerindian tribes.[23]
Other studies have approached mate choice based on odor preference. In one study done by Wedekind et al., women were asked to smell male axillary odors collected on T-shirts worn by different males. Women that were ovulating rated the odors of MHC-dissimilar men as more pleasant than those of the MHC-similar men. Furthermore, odors of MHC-dissimilar men often reminded women of current or former partners, suggesting that odor—specifically odor for MHC-dissimilarity—plays a role in mate choice.[24]
In another study done by Wedekind et al., 121 women and men were asked to rank the pleasantness of the odors of sweaty T-shirts. Upon smelling the shirts, it was found that men and women who were reminded of their own mate or ex-mate had dramatically fewer MHC alleles in common with the wearer than would be expected by chance. If the selection for shirts was not random, and actually selected for MHC-dissimilar alleles, this suggests that MHC genetic composition does influence mate choice. Furthermore, when the degree of similarity between the wearer and the smeller was statistically accounted for, there was no longer a significant influence of MHC on odor preference. The results show that MHC similarity or dissimilarity certainly plays a role in mate choice. Specifically, MHC-disassortative mate choice and less similar MHC combinations are selected for.[25] One interesting aspect of the Wedekind's experiment was that in contrast to normally cycling women, women taking oral contraceptives preferred odors of MHC-similar men. This would suggest that the pill may interfere with the adaptive preference for dissimilarity.[24][25]
In primates
[edit]There is evidence of MHC-associated mate choice in other primates. In the grey mouse lemur Microcebus murinus, post-copulatory mate-choice is associated with genetic constitution. Fathers are more MHC-dissimilar from the mother than are randomly tested males. Fathers have more differences in amino acid and microsatellite diversity than did randomly tested males. It is hypothesized that this is caused by female cryptic choice.[26]
In other animals
[edit]In mice, both males and females choose MHC-dissimilar partners. Mice develop the ability to identify family members during early growth and are known to avoid inbreeding with kin, which would support the MHC-mediated mate choice hypothesis for inbreeding avoidance.[2]
Fish are another group of vertebrates shown to display MHC-associated mate choice. Scientists tested the Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, by observing effects of MHC upon natural spawning salmon that resided in the river versus artificial crosses that were carried out in hatcheries. Logically, the artificial crosses would be bereft of the benefits of mate choice that would naturally be available. The results showed that the offspring of the artificially bred salmon were more infected with parasites: almost four times more than the naturally-spawned offspring were. In addition, wild offspring were more MHC-heterozygous than the artificially-bred offspring. These results support the Heterozygous Advantage hypothesis of sexual selection for MHC-dissimilar mate choice.[27] In another fish, the three-spined stickleback, it has been shown that females desire MHC diversity in their offspring, which affects their mate choice.[28]
Female Savannah sparrows, Passerculus sandwichensis, chose MHC-dissimilar males to mate with. Females are more likely to engage in extra-pair relationships if paired with MHC-similar mates and more dissimilar mates are available. Similarly, MHC diversity in house sparrows, Passer domesticus, suggests that MHC-disassortative mate choice occurs.[2]
MHC-mediated mate choice has been shown to exist in Swedish sand lizards, Lacerta agilis. Females preferred to associate with odor samples obtained from males more distantly related at the MHC I loci.[29]
Even though many species are socially monogamous, females can accept or actively seek mating outside of the relationship;[30] extra-pair paternity is a mating pattern known to be affiliated with MHC-associated mate choice. Birds are one of the more commonly studied groups of animals to exhibit this sexual behavior. In the scarlet rosefinch Carpocus erythrinus, females engaged in extra-pair paternity much less frequently when their mates were MHC-heterozygous.[31] In the Seychelles warbler Acrocephalus sechellensis, there was no evidence of MHC variation between social mates. However, when females' social mates were MHC-similar, they were more likely to participate in extra-pair paternity; in most cases, the extra-pair male was significantly more MHC-dissimilar than the social mate.[32]
MHC-mediated mate choice may occur after copulation, at the gametic level, through sperm competition or female cryptic choice. The Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, is one species in which sperm competition is influenced by the variation in the major histocompatibility complex, specifically that of the Class I alleles. Atlantic salmon males have higher rates of successful fertilization when competing for eggs from females genetically similar at the class I genes of the MHC.[33]
Another species that exhibits MHC-associated cryptic choice is the Arctic charr Salvelinus alpinus. In this case, however, it seems that sperm selection is more dependent on the ovum. MHC-heterozygous males were found to have significantly more fertilization success than MHC-homozygous males; sperm count, motility, and swimming velocity were not shown to significantly co-vary with similarity or dissimilarity at the MHC. It is proposed that there is a chemo-attraction system responsible for the egg itself being able to discriminate and selectively choose between MHC-heterozygous and MHC-homozygous males.[34]
Contrary to the Atlantic salmon and the Arctic char, red junglefowl Gallus gallus males instead of females exert cryptic preference. Male junglefowl showed no preference when simultaneously presented with both an MHC-dissimilar and an MHC-similar female. However, they did show a cryptic preference by allocating more sperm to the more MHC-dissimilar of the two.[35]
Male sand lizards Lacerta agilis behave similarly to the male junglefowl. Initial copulation between a male and a female without any rivals was shown to be extended when the male sensed a higher female fecundity. However, second males adjusted the duration of their copulation depending on the relatedness between the female and the first male, believed to be determined by the MHC-odor of the copulatory plug. A closer genetic relatedness between a male and a female sand lizard increased the chances for a successful fertilization and rate of paternity for the second male.[36]
Abortional selection may be a form of cryptic female choice. Many studies on humans and rodents have found that females may spontaneously abort pregnancies in which the offspring is too MHC-similar.[citation needed] In addition, in vitro fertilizations are more likely to fail when couples have similar MHC genes.[citation needed]
MHC and sexual conflict
[edit]If males attempt to thwart female mate choice by mating with a female against her will, sexual conflict may interfere with the choice for compatibility at the MHC genes.
In Chinook salmon Oncorhyncus tshawytscha, females act more aggressively towards MHC-similar males than MHC-dissimilar males, suggesting the presence of female mate choice. Furthermore, males directed aggression at MHC-similar females. This was accompanied by male harassment of unreceptive females; however, there was a positive correlation between male aggression and reproductive success. The ability of the males to over-power the females' original mate choice resulted in the offspring of the targets of male aggression having low genetic diversity. Offspring with high genetic diversity seemed to happen only when the operational sex ratio was female-biased, when females were more likely to be able to exert mate choice, and males were less likely to harass females. These results suggest that sexual conflict may interfere with female mate choice for 'good' MHC genes.[37]
See also
[edit]References
[edit]- ^ a b c Milinski M, Griffiths S, Wegner KM, Reusch TB, Haas-Assenbaum A, Boehm T (March 2005). "Mate choice decisions of stickleback females predictably modified by MHC peptide ligands". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 (12): 4414–8. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.4414M. doi:10.1073/pnas.0408264102. PMC 555479. PMID 15755811.
- ^ a b c d e f O'Dwyer TW, Nevitt GA (July 2009). "Individual odor recognition in procellariiform chicks: potential role for the major histocompatibility complex". Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1170: 442–6. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03887.x. PMID 19686174. S2CID 10004939.
- ^ Quéméré, Erwan; Rossi, Sophie; Petit, Elodie; Marchand, Pascal; Merlet, Joël; Game, Yvette; Galan, Maxime; Gilot-Fromont, Emmanuelle (2020-03-10). "Genetic epidemiology of the Alpine ibex reservoir of persistent and virulent brucellosis outbreak". Scientific Reports. 10 (1): 4400. Bibcode:2020NatSR..10.4400Q. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-61299-2. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 7064506. PMID 32157133.
- ^ Minias, Piotr; Vinkler, Michal (2022-05-01). "Selection Balancing at Innate Immune Genes: Adaptive Polymorphism Maintenance in Toll-Like Receptors". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 39 (5): msac102. doi:10.1093/molbev/msac102. ISSN 1537-1719. PMC 9132207. PMID 35574644.
- ^ Morger, Jennifer; Bajnok, Jaroslav; Boyce, Kellyanne; Craig, Philip S.; Rogan, Michael T.; Lun, Zhao-Rong; Hide, Geoff; Tschirren, Barbara (2014-08-01). "Naturally occurring Toll-like receptor 11 (TLR11) and Toll-like receptor 12 (TLR12) polymorphisms are not associated with Toxoplasma gondii infection in wild wood mice". Infection, Genetics and Evolution. 26: 180–184. doi:10.1016/j.meegid.2014.05.032. ISSN 1567-1348. PMID 24910107.
- ^ a b Antonides, Jennifer; Mathur, Samarth; Sundaram, Mekala; Ricklefs, Robert; DeWoody, J. Andrew (2019-05-22). "Immunogenetic response of the bananaquit in the face of malarial parasites". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 19 (1): 107. Bibcode:2019BMCEE..19..107A. doi:10.1186/s12862-019-1435-y. ISSN 1471-2148. PMC 6529992. PMID 31113360.
- ^ Cornetti, Luca; Hilfiker, Daniela; Lemoine, Mélissa; Tschirren, Barbara (2018-08-06). "Small-scale spatial variation in infection risk shapes the evolution of aBorreliaresistance gene in wild rodents". Molecular Ecology. 27 (17): 3515–3524. Bibcode:2018MolEc..27.3515C. doi:10.1111/mec.14812. hdl:10871/33429. ISSN 0962-1083. PMID 30040159. S2CID 51711551.
- ^ Nelson-Flower, Martha J; Germain, Ryan R; MacDougall-Shackleton, Elizabeth A; Taylor, Sabrina S; Arcese, Peter (2018-06-27). "Purifying Selection in the Toll-Like Receptors of Song Sparrows Melospiza melodia". Journal of Heredity. 109 (5): 501–509. doi:10.1093/jhered/esy027. ISSN 0022-1503. PMID 29893971.
- ^ Ilmonen, Petteri; Penn, Dustin J; Damjanovich, Kristy; Morrison, Linda; Ghotbi, Laleh; Potts, Wayne K (2007-08-01). "Major Histocompatibility Complex Heterozygosity Reduces Fitness in Experimentally Infected Mice". Genetics. 176 (4): 2501–2508. doi:10.1534/genetics.107.074815. ISSN 1943-2631. PMC 1950649. PMID 17603099.
- ^ Joe., Demas, Gregory E. Nelson, Randy (2012). Ecoimmunology. Oxford University Press. p. 238. ISBN 978-0-19-987624-2. OCLC 777401230.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ McClelland, Erin E.; Granger, Donald L.; Potts, Wayne K. (August 2003). "Major Histocompatibility Complex-Dependent Susceptibility to Cryptococcus neoformans in Mice". Infection and Immunity. 71 (8): 4815–4817. doi:10.1128/IAI.71.8.4815-4817.2003. ISSN 0019-9567. PMC 166009. PMID 12874366.
- ^ Takahata, Naoyuki (1994), Golding, Brian (ed.), "Polymorphism at MHC Loci and Isolation by the Immune System in Vertebrates", Non-Neutral Evolution: Theories and Molecular Data, Boston, MA: Springer US, pp. 233–246, doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-2383-3_19, ISBN 978-1-4615-2383-3, retrieved 2022-07-28
- ^ Nowak, M A; Tarczy-Hornoch, K; Austyn, J M (1992-11-15). "The optimal number of major histocompatibility complex molecules in an individual". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 89 (22): 10896–10899. Bibcode:1992PNAS...8910896N. doi:10.1073/pnas.89.22.10896. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 50449. PMID 1438295.
- ^ Woelfing, Benno; Traulsen, Arne; Milinski, Manfred; Boehm, Thomas (2009-01-12). "Does intra-individual major histocompatibility complex diversity keep a golden mean?". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 364 (1513): 117–128. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0174. PMC 2666699. PMID 18926972.
- ^ Lampert, K. P.; Fischer, P.; Schartl, M. (March 2009). "Major histocompatibility complex variability in the clonal Amazon molly, Poecilia formosa : is copy number less important than genotype?". Molecular Ecology. 18 (6): 1124–1136. Bibcode:2009MolEc..18.1124L. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04097.x. PMID 19226318. S2CID 13068773.
- ^ Šimková, Andrea; Košař, Martin; Vetešník, Lukáš; Vyskočilová, Martina (2013-06-14). "MHC genes and parasitism in Carassius gibelio, a diploid-triploid fish species with dual reproduction strategies". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 13 (1): 122. Bibcode:2013BMCEE..13..122S. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-13-122. ISSN 1471-2148. PMC 3691641. PMID 23768177.
- ^ Westemeier RL, Brawn JD, Simpson SA, et al. (November 1998). "Tracking the long-term decline and recovery of an isolated population". Science. 282 (5394): 1695–8. Bibcode:1998Sci...282.1695W. doi:10.1126/science.282.5394.1695. PMID 9831558. S2CID 19726112.
- ^ Schlupp, Ingo; Berbel-Filho, Waldir (2021-06-03). "Faculty Opinions recommendation of Meta-analytic evidence that animals rarely avoid inbreeding". doi:10.3410/f.740048135.793586159. S2CID 243521297.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Bernatchez, L.; Landry, C. (May 2003). "MHC studies in nonmodel vertebrates: what have we learned about natural selection in 15 years?". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 16 (3): 363–377. doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00531.x. ISSN 1010-061X. PMID 14635837. S2CID 28094212.
- ^ a b c Yamazaki K, Beauchamp GK, Singer A, Bard J, Boyse EA (February 1999). "Odortypes: their origin and composition". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96 (4): 1522–5. Bibcode:1999PNAS...96.1522Y. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.4.1522. PMC 15502. PMID 9990056.
- ^ Bhutta MF (June 2007). "Sex and the nose: human pheromonal responses". J R Soc Med. 100 (6): 268–74. doi:10.1177/014107680710000612. PMC 1885393. PMID 17541097.
- ^ a b Havlicek J, Roberts SC (May 2009). "MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 34 (4): 497–512. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007. PMID 19054623. S2CID 40332494.
- ^ a b Chaix R, Cao C, Donnelly P (2008). "Is mate choice in humans MHC-dependent?". PLoS Genet. 4 (9): e1000184. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000184. PMC 2519788. PMID 18787687.
- ^ a b Roberts SC, Gosling LM, Carter V, Petrie M (December 2008). "MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives". Proc. Biol. Sci. 275 (1652): 2715–22. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0825. PMC 2605820. PMID 18700206.
- ^ a b Wedekind C, Füri S (October 1997). "Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?". Proc. Biol. Sci. 264 (1387): 1471–9. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0204. PMC 1688704. PMID 9364787.
- ^ Schwensow N, Eberle M, Sommer S (March 2008). "Compatibility counts: MHC-associated mate choice in a wild promiscuous primate". Proc. Biol. Sci. 275 (1634): 555–64. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1433. PMC 2596809. PMID 18089539.
- ^ Consuegra S, Garcia de Leaniz C (June 2008). "MHC-mediated mate choice increases parasite resistance in salmon". Proc. Biol. Sci. 275 (1641): 1397–403. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0066. PMC 2602703. PMID 18364312.
- ^ Kurtz J, Kalbe M, Aeschlimann PB, et al. (January 2004). "Major histocompatibility complex diversity influences parasite resistance and innate immunity in sticklebacks". Proc. Biol. Sci. 271 (1535): 197–204. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2567. PMC 1691569. PMID 15058398.
- ^ Olsson M, Madsen T, Nordby J, Wapstra E, Ujvari B, Wittsell H (November 2003). "Major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in sand lizards". Proc. Biol. Sci. 270 (Suppl 2): S254–6. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0079. PMC 1809963. PMID 14667398.
- ^ Suter SM, Keiser M, Feignoux R, Meyer DR (November 2007). "Reed bunting females increase fitness through extra-pair mating with genetically dissimilar males". Proc. Biol. Sci. 274 (1627): 2865–71. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0799. PMC 2288684. PMID 17785270.
- ^ Promerová Vinkler. Occurrence of extra-pair paternity is connected to social male's MHC-variability in the scarlet rosefinch Carpodacus erythrinus. Journal of Avian Biology 42, 5-10(2011).
- ^ Richardson DS, Komdeur J, Burke T, von Schantz T (April 2005). "MHC-based patterns of social and extra-pair mate choice in the Seychelles warbler". Proc. Biol. Sci. 272 (1564): 759–67. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3028. PMC 1602051. PMID 15870038.
- ^ Yeates SE, Einum S, Fleming IA, et al. (February 2009). "Atlantic salmon eggs favour sperm in competition that have similar major histocompatibility alleles". Proc. Biol. Sci. 276 (1656): 559–66. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1257. PMC 2592554. PMID 18854296.
- ^ Skarstein F, et al. (2005). "MHC and fertilization success in the Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 57 (4): 374–380. doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0860-z. S2CID 42727644.
- ^ Gillingham MA, Richardson DS, Løvlie H, Moynihan A, Worley K, Pizzari T (March 2009). "Cryptic preference for MHC-dissimilar females in male red junglefowl, Gallus gallus". Proc. Biol. Sci. 276 (1659): 1083–92. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1549. PMC 2679071. PMID 19129124.
- ^ Olsson M, Madsen T, Ujvari B, Wapstra E (April 2004). "Fecundity and MHC affects ejaculation tactics and paternity bias in sand lizards". Evolution. 58 (4): 906–9. doi:10.1554/03-610. hdl:10536/DRO/DU:30066491. PMID 15154566. S2CID 198152712.
- ^ Garner SR, Bortoluzzi RN, Heath DD, Neff BD (March 2010). "Sexual conflict inhibits female mate choice for major histocompatibility complex dissimilarity in Chinook salmon". Proc. Biol. Sci. 277 (1683): 885–94. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1639. PMC 2842720. PMID 19864282.