Jump to content

Sex and gender differences in autism: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Derfboy (talk | contribs)
m Added the word 'is' to the sentence.
Tags: Visual edit Mobile edit Mobile web edit Advanced mobile edit
m Fixed a reference. Please see Category:CS1 errors: dates.
 
(31 intermediate revisions by 22 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Differences in autism diagnosis frequencies with respect to gender}}
{{Short description|Differences in autism diagnosis frequencies with respect to gender}}
{{Sex differences}}
{{Sex differences}}
{{Lead too long|date=June 2023}}


'''Sex and gender differences in autism''' exist regarding [[prevalence]], [[Presentation (medical)|presentation]], and [[Medical diagnosis|diagnosis]].
'''Sex and gender differences in autism''' exist regarding [[prevalence]], [[Presentation (medical)|presentation]], and [[Medical diagnosis|diagnosis]].


Men and boys are more frequently diagnosed with [[autism]] than women and girls. It is debated whether this is due to a sex difference in rates of [[autism spectrum disorders]] (ASD) or whether females are underdiagnosed.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Halladay |first1=Alycia K |last2=Bishop |first2=Somer |last3=Constantino |first3=John N |last4=Daniels |first4=Amy M |last5=Koenig |first5=Katheen |last6=Palmer |first6=Kate |last7=Messinger |first7=Daniel |last8=Pelphrey |first8=Kevin |last9=Sanders |first9=Stephan J |last10=Singer |first10=Alison Tepper |last11=Taylor |first11=Julie Lounds |last12=Szatmari |first12=Peter |title=Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority |journal=Molecular Autism |date=December 2015 |volume=6 |issue=1 |pages=36 |doi=10.1186/s13229-015-0019-y |pmid=26075049 |pmc=4465158 }}</ref><ref name="Loomes R" /> The prevalence ratio is often cited as about 4 males for every 1 female diagnosed.<ref name="Fombonne">{{cite journal |last1=Fombonne |first1=Eric |title=Epidemiology of Pervasive Developmental Disorders |journal=Pediatric Research |date=June 2009 |volume=65 |issue=6 |pages=591–598 |doi=10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203 |pmid=19218885 |s2cid=20373463 |doi-access=free }}</ref> Other research indicates that it is closer to 3:1 or 2:1.<ref name="Loomes R">{{cite journal |last1=Loomes |first1=Rachel |last2=Hull |first2=Laura |last3=Mandy |first3=William Polmear Locke |title=What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis |journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry |date=June 2017 |volume=56 |issue=6 |pages=466–474 |doi=10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013 |pmid=28545751 |s2cid=20420861 |url=https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/1558343/ }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hull |first1=Laura |last2=Petrides |first2=K. V. |last3=Mandy |first3=William |title=The Female Autism Phenotype and Camouflaging: a Narrative Review |journal=Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=December 2020 |volume=7 |issue=4 |pages=306–317 |doi=10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9 |s2cid=214314845 |doi-access=free }}</ref> One in every 42 males and one in 189 females in the United States is diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.<ref>{{cite web |title=Data and Statistics on Autism Spectrum Disorder |url=https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/data.html |website=Centers for Disease Control and Prevention |date=25 September 2020 }}</ref> There is some evidence that females may also receive diagnoses somewhat later than males; however, thus far results have been contradictory.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Begeer |first1=Sander |last2=Mandell |first2=David |last3=Wijnker-Holmes |first3=Bernadette |last4=Venderbosch |first4=Stance |last5=Rem |first5=Dorien |last6=Stekelenburg |first6=Fred |last7=Koot |first7=Hans M. |title=Sex Differences in the Timing of Identification Among Children and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=May 2013 |volume=43 |issue=5 |pages=1151–1156 |doi=10.1007/s10803-012-1656-z |pmid=23001766 |s2cid=2705562 |url=https://research.vu.nl/en/publications/0f767749-03a9-4ac3-9669-492398d5a7eb }}</ref>
Men and boys are more frequently diagnosed with [[autism]] than women and girls. It is debated whether this is due to a sex difference in rates of [[autism spectrum disorders]] (ASD) or whether females are underdiagnosed.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Halladay |first1=Alycia K |last2=Bishop |first2=Somer |last3=Constantino |first3=John N |last4=Daniels |first4=Amy M |last5=Koenig |first5=Katheen |last6=Palmer |first6=Kate |last7=Messinger |first7=Daniel |last8=Pelphrey |first8=Kevin |last9=Sanders |first9=Stephan J |last10=Singer |first10=Alison Tepper |last11=Taylor |first11=Julie Lounds |last12=Szatmari |first12=Peter |title=Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority |journal=Molecular Autism |date=December 2015 |volume=6 |issue=1 |pages=36 |doi=10.1186/s13229-015-0019-y |pmid=26075049 |pmc=4465158 |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name="Loomes R" /> The prevalence ratio is often cited as about 4 males for every 1 female diagnosed.<ref name="Fombonne">{{cite journal |last1=Fombonne |first1=Eric |title=Epidemiology of Pervasive Developmental Disorders |journal=Pediatric Research |date=June 2009 |volume=65 |issue=6 |pages=591–598 |doi=10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203 |pmid=19218885 |s2cid=20373463 |doi-access=free }}</ref> Other research indicates that it is closer to 3:1 or 2:1.<ref name="Loomes R">{{cite journal |last1=Loomes |first1=Rachel |last2=Hull |first2=Laura |last3=Mandy |first3=William Polmear Locke |title=What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis |journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry |date=June 2017 |volume=56 |issue=6 |pages=466–474 |doi=10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013 |pmid=28545751 |s2cid=20420861 |url=https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/1558343/ }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hull |first1=Laura |last2=Petrides |first2=K. V. |last3=Mandy |first3=William |title=The Female Autism Phenotype and Camouflaging: a Narrative Review |journal=Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=December 2020 |volume=7 |issue=4 |pages=306–317 |doi=10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9 |s2cid=214314845 |doi-access=free }}</ref> One in every 42 males and one in 189 females in the United States is diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.<ref>{{cite web |title=Data and Statistics on Autism Spectrum Disorder |url=https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/data.html |website=Centers for Disease Control and Prevention |date=25 September 2020 }}</ref> There is some evidence that females may also receive diagnoses somewhat later than males; however, thus far results have been contradictory.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Begeer |first1=Sander |last2=Mandell |first2=David |last3=Wijnker-Holmes |first3=Bernadette |last4=Venderbosch |first4=Stance |last5=Rem |first5=Dorien |last6=Stekelenburg |first6=Fred |last7=Koot |first7=Hans M. |title=Sex Differences in the Timing of Identification Among Children and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=May 2013 |volume=43 |issue=5 |pages=1151–1156 |doi=10.1007/s10803-012-1656-z |pmid=23001766 |s2cid=2705562 |url=https://research.vu.nl/en/publications/0f767749-03a9-4ac3-9669-492398d5a7eb }}</ref>

Several theories exist to explain the sex-based discrepancy, such as a genetic protective effect,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gockley |first1=Jake |last2=Willsey |first2=A Jeremy |last3=Dong |first3=Shan |last4=Dougherty |first4=Joseph D |last5=Constantino |first5=John N |last6=Sanders |first6=Stephan J |title=The female protective effect in autism spectrum disorder is not mediated by a single genetic locus |journal=Molecular Autism |date=December 2015 |volume=6 |issue=1 |pages=25 |doi=10.1186/s13229-015-0014-3 |pmid=25973162 |pmc=4429476 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Robinson |first1=E. B. |last2=Lichtenstein |first2=P. |last3=Anckarsater |first3=H. |last4=Happe |first4=F. |last5=Ronald |first5=A. |title=Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |date=26 March 2013 |volume=110 |issue=13 |pages=5258–5262 |doi=10.1073/pnas.1211070110 |pmid=23431162 |pmc=3612665 |bibcode=2013PNAS..110.5258R |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name="lai2015">{{cite journal |last1=Lai |first1=Meng-Chuan |last2=Baron-Cohen |first2=Simon |last3=Buxbaum |first3=Joseph D |title=Understanding autism in the light of sex/gender |journal=Molecular Autism |date=December 2015 |volume=6 |issue=1 |pages=24 |doi=10.1186/s13229-015-0021-4 |pmid=25973161 |pmc=4429357 }}</ref> the [[extreme male brain theory]]<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Baron-Cohen |first1=Simon |title=The extreme male brain theory of autism |journal=Trends in Cognitive Sciences |date=June 2002 |volume=6 |issue=6 |pages=248–254 |doi=10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6 |pmid=12039606 |s2cid=8098723 }}</ref><ref name="lai2011">{{cite journal |last1=Lai |first1=Meng-Chuan |last2=Lombardo |first2=Michael V. |last3=Pasco |first3=Greg |last4=Ruigrok |first4=Amber N. V. |last5=Wheelwright |first5=Sally J. |last6=Sadek |first6=Susan A. |last7=Chakrabarti |first7=Bhismadev |last8=Baron-Cohen |first8=Simon |title=A Behavioral Comparison of Male and Female Adults with High Functioning Autism Spectrum Conditions |journal=PLOS ONE |date=13 June 2011 |volume=6 |issue=6 |pages=e20835 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0020835 |pmid=21695147 |pmc=3113855 |bibcode=2011PLoSO...620835L |doi-access=free }}</ref> and [[Phenotype|phenotypic]] differences in the presentation between sexes,<ref name="lai2015" /><ref name="lai2012">{{cite journal |last1=Lai |first1=Meng-Chuan |last2=Lombardo |first2=Michael V. |last3=Ruigrok |first3=Amber N. V. |last4=Chakrabarti |first4=Bhismadev |last5=Wheelwright |first5=Sally J. |last6=Auyeung |first6=Bonnie |last7=Allison |first7=Carrie |last8=Baron-Cohen |first8=Simon |title=Cognition in Males and Females with Autism: Similarities and Differences |journal=PLOS ONE |date=17 October 2012 |volume=7 |issue=10 |pages=e47198 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0047198 |pmid=23094036 |pmc=3474800 |bibcode=2012PLoSO...747198L |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name="ludlow2015">{{cite journal |last1=Ludlow |first1=Amanda K. |last2=Roberts |first2=Hannah |last3=Gutierrez |first3=Roberto |title=Social Anxiety and Response to Touch: A Preliminary Exploration of Broader Autism Phenotype in Females |journal=SAGE Open |date=1 April 2015 |volume=5 |issue=2 |pages=215824401558085 |doi=10.1177/2158244015580854 |s2cid=14324454 |hdl=2299/16551 |hdl-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Torres |first1=Elizabeth B. |last2=Isenhower |first2=Robert W. |last3=Yanovich |first3=Polina |last4=Rehrig |first4=Gwendolyn |last5=Stigler |first5=Kimberly |last6=Nurnberger |first6=John |last7=José |first7=Jorge V. |title=Strategies to develop putative biomarkers to characterize the female phenotype with autism spectrum disorders |journal=Journal of Neurophysiology |date=1 October 2013 |volume=110 |issue=7 |pages=1646–1662 |doi=10.1152/jn.00059.2013 |pmid=23864377 }}</ref> which may all be intertwined. Researchers have also debated whether a diagnostic gender bias has played a role in females being underdiagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Sex differences in structural organization of motor systems and their dissociable links with repetitive/restricted behaviors in children with autism|journal = Molecular Autism|date = 2015-09-04|pmc = 4559968|pmid = 26347127|volume = 6|issue = 1|pages = 50|doi = 10.1186/s13229-015-0042-z|first1 = Kaustubh|last1 = Supekar|first2 = Vinod|last2 = Menon}}</ref> Researchers have also speculated a gender bias in parental reporting due to the expectations and socialization of gender roles in society.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Holtmann|first1=Martin|last2=Bölte|first2=Sven|last3=Poustka|first3=Fritz|date=2007|title=Autism spectrum disorders: sex differences in autistic behaviour domains and coexisting psychopathology|journal=Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology|language=en|volume=49|issue=5|pages=361–366|doi=10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00361.x |pmid=17489810|doi-access=free}}</ref>

Since autism is a largely genetic and [[Heritability of autism|hereditary condition]], genetic factors that lead to differences depending on sex come into play, such as the role of [[androgen]] signaling in male development or [[X-linked]] mutations, whose associated genetic conditions are typically more common and severe in males. The extreme male brain theory suggests that autistic brains show an exaggeration of the features associated with male brains, such as [[Megalencephaly#Epidemiology|increased size]] and decreased relative connectivity as well as systematic thinking over empathetic thinking.<ref name="Baron-Cohen, S. 2005">{{cite journal |last1=Baron-Cohen |first1=Simon |last2=Knickmeyer |first2=Rebecca C. |last3=Belmonte |first3=Matthew K. |title=Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism |journal=Science |date=4 November 2005 |volume=310 |issue=5749 |pages=819–823 |doi=10.1126/science.1115455 |pmid=16272115 |bibcode=2005Sci...310..819B |s2cid=44330420 |url=http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/2710/1/219535_PubSub1971_Belmonte.pdf |author-link1=Simon Baron-Cohen }}</ref> The [[imprinted brain hypothesis]] suggests genomic imprinting is at least partly responsible for the sex differences in autism and points to the evidence for a [[Causes of schizophrenia#Genetic candidates|common genetic cause]] with [[schizophrenia]].<ref name=":0">{{cite journal |last1=Crespi |first1=Bernard |last2=Badcock |first2=Christopher |title=Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |date=June 2008 |volume=31 |issue=3 |pages=241–261 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X08004214 |pmid=18578904 |url=http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/21571/1/Psychosis%20and%20autism%20as%20diametrical%20disorders%20of%20the%20social%20brain%20%28LSERO%29.pdf }}</ref><ref name=":2">{{cite journal |last1=Crespi |first1=Bernard |last2=Stead |first2=Philip |last3=Elliot |first3=Michael |title=Comparative genomics of autism and schizophrenia |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |date=26 January 2010 |volume=107 |issue=Suppl 1 |pages=1736–1741 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0906080106 |pmid=19955444 |pmc=2868282 |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name=":4">{{cite journal |last1=Ciaramidaro |first1=Angela |last2=Bölte |first2=Sven |last3=Schlitt |first3=Sabine |last4=Hainz |first4=Daniela |last5=Poustka |first5=Fritz |last6=Weber |first6=Bernhard |last7=Bara |first7=Bruno G |last8=Freitag |first8=Christine |last9=Walter |first9=Henrik |title=Schizophrenia and Autism as Contrasting Minds: Neural Evidence for the Hypo-Hyper-Intentionality Hypothesis |journal=Schizophrenia Bulletin |date=1 January 2015 |volume=41 |issue=1 |pages=171–179 |doi=10.1093/schbul/sbu124 |pmid=25210055 |pmc=4266299 }}</ref>

Compared to men, women are generally required to be more impaired by their autism<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Russell |first1=Ginny |last2=Ford |first2=Tamsin |last3=Steer |first3=Colin |last4=Golding |first4=Jean |title=Identification of children with the same level of impairment as children on the autistic spectrum, and analysis of their service use |journal=Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry |date=2010 |volume=51 |issue=6 |pages=643–651 |doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02233.x |pmid=20345841 }}</ref> or have more cognitive or behavioral conditions<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Dworzynski |first1=Katharina |last2=Ronald |first2=Angelica |last3=Bolton |first3=Patrick |last4=Happé |first4=Francesca |title=How Different Are Girls and Boys Above and Below the Diagnostic Threshold for Autism Spectrum Disorders? |journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry |date=1 August 2012 |volume=51 |issue=8 |pages=788–797 |doi=10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.018 |pmid=22840550 }}</ref> than their male counterparts to meet [[autism spectrum]] criteria. There is evidence of increased incidence of [[social anxiety]],<ref name="ludlow2015" /> [[anorexia nervosa]]<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Baron-Cohen |first1=Simon |last2=Jaffa |first2=Tony |last3=Davies |first3=Sarah |last4=Auyeung |first4=Bonnie |last5=Allison |first5=Carrie |last6=Wheelwright |first6=Sally |title=Do girls with anorexia nervosa have elevated autistic traits? |journal=Molecular Autism |date=December 2013 |volume=4 |issue=1 |pages=24 |doi=10.1186/2040-2392-4-24 |pmid=23915495 |pmc=3735388 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Rhind |first1=Charlotte |last2=Bonfioli |first2=Elena |last3=Hibbs |first3=Rebecca |last4=Goddard |first4=Elizabeth |last5=Macdonald |first5=Pamela |last6=Gowers |first6=Simon |last7=Schmidt |first7=Ulrike |last8=Tchanturia |first8=Kate |last9=Micali |first9=Nadia |last10=Treasure |first10=Janet |title=An examination of autism spectrum traits in adolescents with anorexia nervosa and their parents |journal=Molecular Autism |date=2014 |volume=5 |issue=1 |pages=56 |doi=10.1186/2040-2392-5-56 |pmid=25553237 |pmc=4280745 }}</ref> and [[self-harm]] in autistic females,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Cohen |first1=Ira L. |last2=Tsiouris |first2=John A. |last3=Flory |first3=Michael J. |last4=Kim |first4=Soh-Yule |last5=Freedland |first5=Robert |last6=Heaney |first6=Glenn |last7=Pettinger |first7=Jill |last8=Ted Brown |first8=W. |title=A Large Scale Study of the Psychometric Characteristics of the IBR Modified Overt Aggression Scale: Findings and Evidence for Increased Self-Destructive Behaviors in Adult Females with Autism Spectrum Disorder |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=May 2010 |volume=40 |issue=5 |pages=599–609 |doi=10.1007/s10803-009-0908-z |pmid=19941156 |s2cid=23789973 }}</ref> though the increased rates of anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Huke |first1=Vanessa |last2=Turk |first2=Jeremy |last3=Saeidi |first3=Saeideh |last4=Kent |first4=Andy |last5=Morgan |first5=John F. |title=Autism Spectrum Disorders in Eating Disorder Populations: A Systematic Review |journal=European Eating Disorders Review |date=2013 |volume=21 |issue=5 |pages=345–351 |doi=10.1002/erv.2244 |pmid=23900859 }}</ref> may be due to confusion or conflation with [[avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder]] (ARFID), which is particularly common in autism.<ref>{{cite web |last1=Shea |first1=Elizabeth |title=Eating disorder or disordered eating? Eating patterns in autism |url=https://www.autism.org.uk/advice-and-guidance/professional-practice/avoidant-eating |website=The National Autistic Society |date=24 May 2016 }}</ref>
Autistic girls and women show higher social motivation and a greater capacity for typical friendships than autistic boys and men,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Head |first1=Alexandra M. |last2=McGillivray |first2=Jane A. |last3=Stokes |first3=Mark A. |title=Gender differences in emotionality and sociability in children with autism spectrum disorders |journal=Molecular Autism |date=28 February 2014 |volume=5 |issue=1 |pages=19 |doi=10.1186/2040-2392-5-19 |pmid=24576331 |pmc=3945617 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Sedgewick |first1=Felicity |last2=Hill |first2=Vivian |last3=Yates |first3=Rhiannon |last4=Pickering |first4=Leanne |last5=Pellicano |first5=Elizabeth |title=Gender Differences in the Social Motivation and Friendship Experiences of Autistic and Non-autistic Adolescents |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=1 April 2016 |volume=46 |issue=4 |pages=1297–1306 |doi=10.1007/s10803-015-2669-1 |pmid=26695137 |pmc=4786616 }}</ref> are less likely to be hyperactive, impulsive, have [[Conduct disorder|issues with conduct]] or stereotyped behavioral traits,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Mandy |first1=William |last2=Chilvers |first2=Rebecca |last3=Chowdhury |first3=Uttom |last4=Salter |first4=Gemma |last5=Seigal |first5=Anna |last6=Skuse |first6=David |title=Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Evidence from a Large Sample of Children and Adolescents |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=1 July 2012 |volume=42 |issue=7 |pages=1304–1313 |doi=10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0 |pmid=21947663 |s2cid=18639019 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Van Wijngaarden-Cremers |first1=Patricia J. M. |last2=van Eeten |first2=Evelien |last3=Groen |first3=Wouter B. |last4=Van Deurzen |first4=Patricia A. |last5=Oosterling |first5=Iris J. |last6=Van der Gaag |first6=Rutger Jan |title=Gender and age differences in the core triad of impairments in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=March 2014 |volume=44 |issue=3 |pages=627–635 |doi=10.1007/s10803-013-1913-9 |pmid=23989936 |s2cid=1705914 }}</ref> and have been shown to [[Autistic masking|mask their autistic behaviors and social difficulties]] more frequently than autistic men.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Schuck |first1=Rachel K. |last2=Flores |first2=Ryan E. |last3=Fung |first3=Lawrence K. |title=Brief Report: Sex/Gender Differences in Symptomology and Camouflaging in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=June 2019 |volume=49 |issue=6 |pages=2597–2604 |doi=10.1007/s10803-019-03998-y |pmid=30945091 |pmc=6753236 }}</ref> Autistic males often exhibit more easily observed behaviors at a younger age resulting in parental observance and subsequent evaluation of the child. In contrast, behavior of young females is more often overlooked, regardless of any associated [[Causes of autism#:~:text=The risk of autism is,though these cases are rare.|at-risk factors]] for ASD or other developmental delays. Ultimately, this may contribute to females more frequently receiving their ASD diagnosis later in life than their male counterpart.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Ramsey |first1=Riane K. |last2=Nichols |first2=Lashae |last3=Ludwig |first3=Natasha N. |last4=Fein |first4=Deborah |last5=Adamson |first5=Lauren B. |last6=Robins |first6=Diana L. |date=2018-12-01 |title=Brief Report: Sex Differences in Parental Concerns for Toddlers with Autism Risk |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3583-0 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=48 |issue=12 |pages=4063–4069 |doi=10.1007/s10803-018-3583-0 |pmid=29700708 |pmc=6203663 |issn=1573-3432}}</ref> There is a growing consensus among neuroscientists that the number of autistic women has been vastly underrepresented due to the assumption that it is primarily a male condition.<ref>{{cite news |last1=Devlin |first1=Hannah |title=Thousands of autistic girls and women 'going undiagnosed' due to gender bias |url=https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/sep/14/thousands-of-autistic-girls-and-women-going-undiagnosed-due-to-gender-bias |access-date=13 September 2021 |work=The Guardian |date=14 September 2018}}</ref>


== Background ==
== Background ==
Line 18: Line 10:
[[Hans Asperger]] was one of the first people to study autism, with all of his four study subjects being male. Another early researcher, [[Leo Kanner]] described "autistic disturbances of affective contact" in the group consisting of eight boys and three girls.<ref>{{cite journal |id={{NAID|10007611258}} |last1=Kanner |first1=Leo |year=1943 |title=Autistic disturbances of affective contact |journal=Nervous Child |volume=2 |issue=3 |pages=217–250 }}</ref>
[[Hans Asperger]] was one of the first people to study autism, with all of his four study subjects being male. Another early researcher, [[Leo Kanner]] described "autistic disturbances of affective contact" in the group consisting of eight boys and three girls.<ref>{{cite journal |id={{NAID|10007611258}} |last1=Kanner |first1=Leo |year=1943 |title=Autistic disturbances of affective contact |journal=Nervous Child |volume=2 |issue=3 |pages=217–250 }}</ref>


Today, Autism Spectrum Disorder is commonly defined as a neurological developmental disorder with symptoms of poor social communication, repetitive behaviors, sensory sensitivities, executive dysfunction, and hyper-fixations.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Jung |first1=Minyoung |last2=Mody |first2=Maria |last3=Saito |first3=Daisuke N. |last4=Tomoda |first4=Akemi |last5=Okazawa |first5=Hidehiko |last6=Wada |first6=Yuji |last7=Kosaka |first7=Hirotaka |date=2015-11-24 |title=Sex Differences in the Default Mode Network with Regard to Autism Spectrum Traits: A Resting State fMRI Study |journal=PLOS ONE |language=en |volume=10 |issue=11 |pages=e0143126 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0143126 |issn=1932-6203 |pmc=4658035 |pmid=26600385|bibcode=2015PLoSO..1043126J |doi-access=free }}</ref> In the modern day, women are less likely to be diagnosed as autistic than men; they are often misdiagnosed or not noticed to be [[neurodivergent]] by doctors.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Lai |first1=Meng-Chuan |last2=Baron-Cohen |first2=Simon |title=Identifying the lost generation of adults with autism spectrum conditions |journal=The Lancet Psychiatry |date=November 2015 |volume=2 |issue=11 |pages=1013–1027 |doi=10.1016/s2215-0366(15)00277-1 |pmid=26544750 }}</ref> Women are also more likely to be diagnosed as autistic at a later age than men.<ref>{{cite journal|first1=Ellen|last1=Giarelli |first2=Lisa D. |last2=Wiggins |first3=Catherine E. |last3=Rice |first4=Susan E. |last4=Levy |first5=Russell S. |last5=Kirby |first6=Jennifer |last6=Pinto-Martin |first7=David |last7=Mandell |title=Sex differences in the evaluation and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders among children |journal=Disability and Health Journal |date=1 April 2010 |volume=3 |issue=2 |pages=107–116 |doi=10.1016/j.dhjo.2009.07.001 |pmid=21122776 |pmc=4767258 }}</ref> This discrepancy in diagnoses is believed to be caused at least partially by [[Autistic masking|camouflaging]], a common autistic phenotype presented in females, which hides autistic traits.<ref name=":3">Lai, Meng-Chuan; Lombardo, Michael V; Ruigrok, Amber NV; Chakrabarti, Bhismadev; Auyeung, Bonnie; Szatmari, Peter; Happé, Francesca; Baron-Cohen, Simon (2016-11-29). [[doi:10.1177/1362361316671012|"Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism"]]. ''Autism''. '''21''' (6): 690–702. [[Doi (identifier)|doi]]:10.1177/1362361316671012. [[ISSN (identifier)|ISSN]] 1362-3613</ref>
Today, Autism Spectrum Disorder is commonly defined as a neurological developmental disorder with symptoms of poor social communication, repetitive behaviors, sensory sensitivities, executive dysfunction, and hyper-fixations.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Jung |first1=Minyoung |last2=Mody |first2=Maria |last3=Saito |first3=Daisuke N. |last4=Tomoda |first4=Akemi |last5=Okazawa |first5=Hidehiko |last6=Wada |first6=Yuji |last7=Kosaka |first7=Hirotaka |date=2015-11-24 |title=Sex Differences in the Default Mode Network with Regard to Autism Spectrum Traits: A Resting State fMRI Study |journal=PLOS ONE |language=en |volume=10 |issue=11 |pages=e0143126 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0143126 |issn=1932-6203 |pmc=4658035 |pmid=26600385|bibcode=2015PLoSO..1043126J |doi-access=free }}</ref> In the modern day, women are less likely to be diagnosed as autistic than men; they are often misdiagnosed or not noticed to be [[neurodivergent]] by doctors.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Lai |first1=Meng-Chuan |last2=Baron-Cohen |first2=Simon |title=Identifying the lost generation of adults with autism spectrum conditions |journal=The Lancet Psychiatry |date=November 2015 |volume=2 |issue=11 |pages=1013–1027 |doi=10.1016/s2215-0366(15)00277-1 |pmid=26544750 }}</ref> Women are also more likely to be diagnosed as autistic at a later age than men.<ref>{{cite journal|first1=Ellen|last1=Giarelli |first2=Lisa D. |last2=Wiggins |first3=Catherine E. |last3=Rice |first4=Susan E. |last4=Levy |first5=Russell S. |last5=Kirby |first6=Jennifer |last6=Pinto-Martin |first7=David |last7=Mandell |title=Sex differences in the evaluation and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders among children |journal=Disability and Health Journal |date=1 April 2010 |volume=3 |issue=2 |pages=107–116 |doi=10.1016/j.dhjo.2009.07.001 |pmid=21122776 |pmc=4767258 }}</ref> There are many theories to explain this discrepancy in diagnoses, the most prominent being extreme male brain theory, imprinted brain theory, female protective effect theory, and female autism phenotype theory.


==Theories explaining gender diagnosis disparity==
==Theories explaining gender diagnosis disparity==
Line 24: Line 16:
===Extreme male brain theory===
===Extreme male brain theory===
{{Main|Empathizing–systemizing theory}}
{{Main|Empathizing–systemizing theory}}
Extreme Male Brain Theory is an extension of the [[Empathizing-systemizing theory|Empathizing-Systemizing Theory]], which categorizes people into 5 different groups based on their empathizing and systemizing expressions. In the general neurotypical population, females have a greater ability to empathize, and males have a greater ability to systemize.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal |last1=Williams |first1=Olivia O. F. |last2=Coppolino |first2=Madeleine |last3=Perreault |first3=Melissa L. |date=2021-12-09 |title=Sex differences in neuronal systems function and behaviour: beyond a single diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders |journal=Translational Psychiatry |language=en |volume=11 |issue=1 |page=625 |doi=10.1038/s41398-021-01757-1 |issn=2158-3188 |pmc=8660826 |pmid=34887388}}</ref> [[Simon Baron-Cohen|Simon Baron-Cohen's]] extreme male brain theory states that autistic males have higher doses of prenatal [[testosterone]] and on average have a more [[Empathizing–systemizing theory|systemizing]] brain, as opposed to the more empathizing female brain. He suggests that autistic brains show an exaggeration of the features associated with male brains. These are mainly size and connectivity, with [[Megalencephaly#Epidemiology|males generally having a larger brain]],<ref name="Baron-Cohen, S. 2005" /> which is seen in an exaggerated form in those with [[Autism spectrum|ASD]]. Individuals with ASD were found to have widespread abnormalities in interconnectivity and general functioning in specific brain regions.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Anderson |first1=J. S. |last2=Druzgal |first2=T. J. |last3=Froehlich |first3=A. |last4=DuBray |first4=M. B. |last5=Lange |first5=N. |last6=Alexander |first6=A. L. |last7=Abildskov |first7=T. |last8=Nielsen |first8=J. A. |last9=Cariello |first9=A. N. |last10=Cooperrider |first10=J. R. |last11=Bigler |first11=E. D. |last12=Lainhart |first12=J. E. |date=2010-10-12 |title=Decreased Interhemispheric Functional Connectivity in Autism |url=https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq190 |journal=Cerebral Cortex |volume=21 |issue=5 |pages=1134–1146 |doi=10.1093/cercor/bhq190 |issn=1047-3211 |pmc=3077433 |pmid=20943668}}</ref> This could explain the [[Sex differences in psychology#Empathy|different results on empathy tests between men and women]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Teatero |first1=Missy L. |last2=Netley |first2=Charles |date=November 2013 |title=A Critical Review of the Research on the Extreme Male Brain Theory and Digit Ratio (2D:4D) |url=http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10803-013-1819-6 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=43 |issue=11 |pages=2664–2676 |doi=10.1007/s10803-013-1819-6 |pmid=23575643 |s2cid=254569210 |issn=0162-3257}}</ref> as well as the deficiencies in empathy seen in ASD, as empathy requires several brain regions to be activated which need information from many different areas of the brain.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Baron-Cohen |first1=Simon |last2=Knickmeyer |first2=Rebecca C. |last3=Belmonte |first3=Matthew K. |date=2005-11-04 |title=Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism |url=https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.1115455 |journal=Science |language=en |volume=310 |issue=5749 |pages=819–823 |doi=10.1126/science.1115455 |pmid=16272115 |bibcode=2005Sci...310..819B |s2cid=44330420 |issn=0036-8075}}</ref> Baron-Cohen therefore argues that genetic factors play a role in autism prevalence and that children with technically minded parents are more likely to be diagnosed with autism.<ref>Baron-Cohen, Simon (November 2012). "Autism and the technical mind: live chat with Simon Baron-Cohen, November 9, 10 A.M. EST". ''[[Scientific American]]''. Vol. 307, no. 5. pp. 72–75.</ref> Although autistic females have been documented to have higher testosterone levels, which could support the Extreme Male Brain theory, not all autistic females show male-specific symptoms, leaving the Extreme Male Brain theory with Autism Spectrum Disorder to be controversial.<ref name=":5" />


Extreme male brain theory is an extension of the [[empathizing-systemizing theory]], which categorizes people into 5 different groups based on their empathizing and systemizing expressions. In the general neurotypical population, females have a greater ability to empathize, and males have a greater ability to systemize.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal |last1=Williams |first1=Olivia O. F. |last2=Coppolino |first2=Madeleine |last3=Perreault |first3=Melissa L. |date=2021-12-09 |title=Sex differences in neuronal systems function and behaviour: beyond a single diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders |journal=Translational Psychiatry |language=en |volume=11 |issue=1 |page=625 |doi=10.1038/s41398-021-01757-1 |issn=2158-3188 |pmc=8660826 |pmid=34887388}}</ref> [[Simon Baron-Cohen]]'s extreme male brain theory states that autistic males have higher doses of prenatal [[testosterone]] and on average have a more [[Empathizing–systemizing theory|systemizing]] brain, as opposed to the more empathizing female brain. He suggests that autistic brains show an exaggeration of the features associated with male brains. These are mainly size and connectivity, with [[Megalencephaly#Epidemiology|males generally having a larger brain]],<ref name="Baron-Cohen, S. 2005">{{cite journal |last1=Baron-Cohen |first1=Simon |author-link1=Simon Baron-Cohen |last2=Knickmeyer |first2=Rebecca C. |last3=Belmonte |first3=Matthew K. |date=4 November 2005 |title=Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism |url=http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/2710/1/219535_PubSub1971_Belmonte.pdf |journal=Science |volume=310 |issue=5749 |pages=819–823 |bibcode=2005Sci...310..819B |doi=10.1126/science.1115455 |pmid=16272115 |s2cid=44330420}}</ref> which is seen in an exaggerated form in those with [[Autism spectrum|ASD]].
=== Imprinted brain hypothesis ===

Individuals with ASD were found to have widespread abnormalities in interconnectivity and general functioning in specific brain regions.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Anderson |first1=J. S. |last2=Druzgal |first2=T. J. |last3=Froehlich |first3=A. |last4=DuBray |first4=M. B. |last5=Lange |first5=N. |last6=Alexander |first6=A. L. |last7=Abildskov |first7=T. |last8=Nielsen |first8=J. A. |last9=Cariello |first9=A. N. |last10=Cooperrider |first10=J. R. |last11=Bigler |first11=E. D. |last12=Lainhart |first12=J. E. |date=2010-10-12 |title=Decreased Interhemispheric Functional Connectivity in Autism |url=https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq190 |journal=Cerebral Cortex |volume=21 |issue=5 |pages=1134–1146 |doi=10.1093/cercor/bhq190 |issn=1047-3211 |pmc=3077433 |pmid=20943668}}</ref> This could explain the [[Sex differences in psychology#Empathy|different results on empathy tests between men and women]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Teatero |first1=Missy L. |last2=Netley |first2=Charles |date=November 2013 |title=A Critical Review of the Research on the Extreme Male Brain Theory and Digit Ratio (2D:4D) |url=http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10803-013-1819-6 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=43 |issue=11 |pages=2664–2676 |doi=10.1007/s10803-013-1819-6 |pmid=23575643 |s2cid=254569210 |issn=0162-3257}}</ref> as well as the deficiencies in empathy seen in ASD, as empathy requires several brain regions to be activated which need information from many different areas of the brain.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Baron-Cohen |first1=Simon |last2=Knickmeyer |first2=Rebecca C. |last3=Belmonte |first3=Matthew K. |date=2005-11-04 |title=Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism |url=http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/2710/1/219535_PubSub1971_Belmonte.pdf |journal=Science |language=en |volume=310 |issue=5749 |pages=819–823 |doi=10.1126/science.1115455 |pmid=16272115 |bibcode=2005Sci...310..819B |s2cid=44330420 |issn=0036-8075|url-access= |url-status= |archive-url= |archive-date= }}</ref> Baron-Cohen therefore argues that genetic factors play a role in autism prevalence and that children with technically minded parents are more likely to be diagnosed with autism.<ref>Baron-Cohen, Simon (November 2012). "Autism and the technical mind: live chat with Simon Baron-Cohen, November 9, 10 A.M. EST". ''[[Scientific American]]''. Vol. 307, no. 5. pp. 72–75.</ref> Although autistic females have been documented to have higher testosterone levels, which could support the extreme male brain theory, not all autistic females show male-specific symptoms, leaving the extreme male brain theory with Autism Spectrum Disorder to be controversial.<ref name=":5" />

=== Imprinted brain theory ===
{{Main|Imprinted brain hypothesis}}
{{Main|Imprinted brain hypothesis}}
The imprinted brain theory suggests genomic imprinting is at least partly responsible for the sex differences in autism and implicates [[schizophrenia]] as well, claiming that genetic and physiological evidence suggests the two conditions are [[Causes of schizophrenia#Genetic candidates|on a spectrum]] in which some mutations in certain genes cause lower social cognition but higher practical cognition (autism) while other mutations in the same genes cause lower practical cognition with higher social cognition (schizophrenia).<ref name=":0" /><ref name=":2" /><ref name=":4" />
The imprinted brain theory suggests genomic imprinting is at least partly responsible for the sex differences in autism and implicates [[schizophrenia]] as well, claiming that genetic and physiological evidence suggests the two conditions are [[Causes of schizophrenia#Genetic candidates|on a spectrum]] in which some mutations in certain genes cause lower social cognition but higher practical cognition (autism) while other mutations in the same genes cause lower practical cognition with higher social cognition (schizophrenia).<ref name=":0">{{cite journal |last1=Crespi |first1=Bernard |last2=Badcock |first2=Christopher |date=June 2008 |title=Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain |url=http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/21571/1/Psychosis%20and%20autism%20as%20diametrical%20disorders%20of%20the%20social%20brain%20%28LSERO%29.pdf |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |volume=31 |issue=3 |pages=241–261 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X08004214 |pmid=18578904}}</ref><ref name=":2">{{cite journal |last1=Crespi |first1=Bernard |last2=Stead |first2=Philip |last3=Elliot |first3=Michael |date=26 January 2010 |title=Comparative genomics of autism and schizophrenia |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |volume=107 |issue=Suppl 1 |pages=1736–1741 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0906080106 |pmc=2868282 |pmid=19955444 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name=":4">{{cite journal |last1=Ciaramidaro |first1=Angela |last2=Bölte |first2=Sven |last3=Schlitt |first3=Sabine |last4=Hainz |first4=Daniela |last5=Poustka |first5=Fritz |last6=Weber |first6=Bernhard |last7=Bara |first7=Bruno G |last8=Freitag |first8=Christine |last9=Walter |first9=Henrik |date=1 January 2015 |title=Schizophrenia and Autism as Contrasting Minds: Neural Evidence for the Hypo-Hyper-Intentionality Hypothesis |journal=Schizophrenia Bulletin |volume=41 |issue=1 |pages=171–179 |doi=10.1093/schbul/sbu124 |pmc=4266299 |pmid=25210055}}</ref>


===Female protective effect hypothesis===
===Female protective effect theory===
According to the female protective effect hypothesis, more extreme genetic mutations are required for a girl to develop autism than for a boy. In 2012, [[Harvard University|Harvard]] researchers published findings suggesting that, on average, more genetic and environmental risk factors are required for girls to develop autism, compared to boys. The researchers analyzed DNA samples of nearly 800 families affected by autism and nearly 16,000 individuals with a variety of [[neurodevelopmental disorder]]s. They looked for various types of [[Mutation|gene mutations]]. Overall, they found that females diagnosed with autism or another neurodevelopmental disorder had a greater number of harmful mutations throughout the [[genome]] than did males with the same disorders.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Jacquemont |first1=Sébastien |last2=Coe |first2=Bradley P. |last3=Hersch |first3=Micha |last4=Duyzend |first4=Michael H. |last5=Krumm |first5=Niklas |last6=Bergmann |first6=Sven |last7=Beckmann |first7=Jacques S. |last8=Rosenfeld |first8=Jill A. |last9=Eichler |first9=Evan E. |title=A Higher Mutational Burden in Females Supports a "Female Protective Model" in Neurodevelopmental Disorders |journal=The American Journal of Human Genetics |date=6 March 2014 |volume=94 |issue=3 |pages=415–425 |doi=10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.02.001 |pmid=24581740 |pmc=3951938 }}</ref> Women with an extra X chromosome, 47,XXX or [[Trisomy X|triple X syndrome]], have autism-like social impairments in 32% of cases.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Otter |first1=Maarten |last2=Crins |first2=Peter M. L. |last3=Campforts |first3=Bea C. M. |last4=Stumpel |first4=Constance T. R. M. |last5=van Amelsvoort |first5=Thérèse A. M. J. |last6=Vingerhoets |first6=Claudia |title=Social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with triple X syndrome |journal=BJPsych Open |date=March 2021 |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=e51 |doi=10.1192/bjo.2021.8 |pmid=33583482 |pmc=8058878 }}</ref>
According to the female protective effect hypothesis, more genetic mutations are required for a girl to develop autism than for a boy. In 2012, [[Harvard University|Harvard]] researchers published findings suggesting that, on average, more genetic and environmental risk factors are required for girls to develop autism, compared to boys. The researchers analyzed DNA samples of nearly 800 families affected by autism and nearly 16,000 individuals with a variety of [[neurodevelopmental disorder]]s. They looked for various types of [[Mutation|gene mutations]]. Overall, they found that females diagnosed with autism or another neurodevelopmental disorder had a greater number of harmful mutations throughout the [[genome]] than did males with the same disorders.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Jacquemont |first1=Sébastien |last2=Coe |first2=Bradley P. |last3=Hersch |first3=Micha |last4=Duyzend |first4=Michael H. |last5=Krumm |first5=Niklas |last6=Bergmann |first6=Sven |last7=Beckmann |first7=Jacques S. |last8=Rosenfeld |first8=Jill A. |last9=Eichler |first9=Evan E. |title=A Higher Mutational Burden in Females Supports a "Female Protective Model" in Neurodevelopmental Disorders |journal=The American Journal of Human Genetics |date=6 March 2014 |volume=94 |issue=3 |pages=415–425 |doi=10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.02.001 |pmid=24581740 |pmc=3951938 }}</ref> Women with an extra X chromosome, 47,XXX or [[Trisomy X|triple X syndrome]], have autism-like social impairments in 32% of cases.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Otter |first1=Maarten |last2=Crins |first2=Peter M. L. |last3=Campforts |first3=Bea C. M. |last4=Stumpel |first4=Constance T. R. M. |last5=van Amelsvoort |first5=Thérèse A. M. J. |last6=Vingerhoets |first6=Claudia |title=Social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with triple X syndrome |journal=BJPsych Open |date=March 2021 |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=e51 |doi=10.1192/bjo.2021.8 |pmid=33583482 |pmc=8058878 }}</ref>


=== Hypothesis of female under-diagnosis ===
=== Female autism phenotype theory ===
The prevalence ratio is often cited as about 4 males for every 1 female diagnosed.<ref name="Fombonne"/> Other research indicates that it closer to 3:1 or 2:1.<ref name="Loomes R"/><ref name="phenotype1">{{cite journal |last1=Bargiela |first1=Sarah |last2=Steward |first2=Robyn |last3=Mandy |first3=William |title=The Experiences of Late-diagnosed Women with Autism Spectrum Conditions: An Investigation of the Female Autism Phenotype |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=1 October 2016 |volume=46 |issue=10 |pages=3281–3294 |doi=10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8 |pmid=27457364 |pmc=5040731 }}</ref>
The prevalence ratio is often cited as about 4 males for every 1 female diagnosed.<ref name="Fombonne"/> Other research indicates that it closer to 3:1 or 2:1.<ref name="Loomes R"/><ref name="phenotype1">{{cite journal |last1=Bargiela |first1=Sarah |last2=Steward |first2=Robyn |last3=Mandy |first3=William |title=The Experiences of Late-diagnosed Women with Autism Spectrum Conditions: An Investigation of the Female Autism Phenotype |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=1 October 2016 |volume=46 |issue=10 |pages=3281–3294 |doi=10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8 |pmid=27457364 |pmc=5040731 }}</ref>

Some have suggested a differential [[phenotype]] for autistic women; "a female-specific manifestation of autistic strengths and difficulties, which fits imperfectly with current, male-based conceptualisations" of autism.<ref name="phenotype1" /> The female autism phenotype differs from the typical male autism phenotype in social relationships, relational interests, internalizing problems, and camouflaging.<ref name=":8">{{Cite journal |last1=Hull |first1=Laura |last2=Petrides |first2=K. V. |last3=Mandy |first3=William |date=2020-12-01 |title=The Female Autism Phenotype and Camouflaging: a Narrative Review |url=https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9 |journal=Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=7 |issue=4 |pages=306–317 |doi=10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9 |issn=2195-7185|doi-access=free }}</ref>


Some authors, clinicians and experts like [[Judith Gould (psychologist)|Judith Gould]], [[Tony Attwood]], [[Lorna Wing]] and [[Christopher Gillberg]]<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.autism.org.uk/about/what-is/gender.aspx |title=Gender and autism |publisher=The National Autistic Society |access-date=2016-06-01}}</ref> have proposed that autism in females may be underdiagnosed due to better natural superficial [[Imitation|social mimicry]] skills in females, partially different set of symptoms and less knowledge about autism in females among experts.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Attwood|first1=Tony|title=Asperger's and Girls|date=2006|publisher=Jessica Kingsley Publishers|location=London|isbn=978-1-932565-40-9|pages=2–6}}</ref> In his [[Foreword|preword]] to the book ''Asperger's and Girls'', Attwood writes: "These tentative explanations for the apparent underrepresentation of girls with Asperger's Syndrome have yet to be examined by objective research studies."<ref>{{cite book|last1=Attwood|first1=Tony|title=Asperger's and Girls|date=2006|publisher=Jessica Kingsley Publishers|location=London|isbn=978-1-932565-40-9|page=6}}</ref>
Some authors, clinicians and experts like [[Judith Gould (psychologist)|Judith Gould]], [[Tony Attwood]], [[Lorna Wing]] and [[Christopher Gillberg]]<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.autism.org.uk/about/what-is/gender.aspx |title=Gender and autism |publisher=The National Autistic Society |access-date=2016-06-01}}</ref> have proposed that autism in females may be underdiagnosed due to better natural superficial [[Imitation|social mimicry]] skills in females, partially different set of symptoms and less knowledge about autism in females among experts.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Attwood|first1=Tony|title=Asperger's and Girls|date=2006|publisher=Jessica Kingsley Publishers|location=London|isbn=978-1-932565-40-9|pages=2–6}}</ref> In his [[Foreword|preword]] to the book ''Asperger's and Girls'', Attwood writes: "These tentative explanations for the apparent underrepresentation of girls with Asperger's Syndrome have yet to be examined by objective research studies."<ref>{{cite book|last1=Attwood|first1=Tony|title=Asperger's and Girls|date=2006|publisher=Jessica Kingsley Publishers|location=London|isbn=978-1-932565-40-9|page=6}}</ref>


Another clinician, William Mandy, hypothesized referrals for ASD assessment are often started by teachers. Girls with ASD may sometimes lack the skills of social communication and this is not noticed until they are in a school setting. Therefore, girls suggested to have ASD may receive delayed or no clinical assessment.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Mandy |first1=William |last2=Chilvers |first2=Rebecca |last3=Chowdhury |first3=Uttom |last4=Salter |first4=Gemma |last5=Seigal |first5=Anna |last6=Skuse |first6=David |date=2012-07-01 |title=Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Evidence from a Large Sample of Children and Adolescents |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=42 |issue=7 |pages=1304–1313 |doi=10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0 |pmid=21947663 |s2cid=18639019 |issn=1573-3432}}</ref> Compared with males, females with autism are more likely to mask their restricted interests (strong or intense interests in specific topics or objects), which could decrease the chances of diagnosis.<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal |last1=Loomes |first1=Rachel |last2=Hull |first2=Laura |last3=Mandy |first3=William Polmear Locke |date=June 2017 |title=What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis |url=https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1558343/1/Loomes.pdf |journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry |language=en |volume=56 |issue=6 |pages=466–474 |doi=10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013|pmid=28545751 |s2cid=20420861 |url-access= |url-status= |archive-url= |archive-date= }}</ref>
Specifically, Gould has discussed the idea that a pervasive developmental disorder called [[pathological demand avoidance]], which is not officially included in [[Psychiatry#Diagnostic manuals|diagnostic manuals]], may offer a glimpse into how autism in females may present in some cases.<ref>{{cite web |url=https://shiggs55.files.wordpress.com/2014/01/pda-conference-plenary-session-jacqui-smith-and-judy-gould.pdf |title=Missed Diagnosis or Misdiagnosis? Women and Girls with PDA |publisher=Dr Judith Gould, Dr Jacqui Ashton Smith |access-date=2016-01-06 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161013030827/https://shiggs55.files.wordpress.com/2014/01/pda-conference-plenary-session-jacqui-smith-and-judy-gould.pdf |archive-date=2016-10-13 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gould |first1=Judith |last2=Ashton-Smith |first2=Jacqui |title=Missed diagnosis or misdiagnosis? Girls and women on the autism spectrum |journal=Good Autism Practice |date=31 May 2011 |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=34–41 |url=https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/bild/gap/2011/00000012/00000001/art00005 }}</ref>


Recent literature has exemplified that autistic females present lower levels of restricted and repetitive interests. However, some studies suggest that autistic females’ interests are in areas that aren’t considered atypical or captured in the diagnostic process as compared to autistic males’ special interests.<ref name=":8" />
Another clinician, William Mandy, hypothesized referrals for ASD assessment are often started by teachers. Girls with ASD may sometimes lack the skills of social communication and this is not noticed until they are in a school setting. Therefore, girls suggested to have ASD may receive delayed or no clinical assessment.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Mandy |first1=William |last2=Chilvers |first2=Rebecca |last3=Chowdhury |first3=Uttom |last4=Salter |first4=Gemma |last5=Seigal |first5=Anna |last6=Skuse |first6=David |date=2012-07-01 |title=Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Evidence from a Large Sample of Children and Adolescents |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=42 |issue=7 |pages=1304–1313 |doi=10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0 |pmid=21947663 |s2cid=18639019 |issn=1573-3432}}</ref> Compared with males, females with autism are more likely to mask their restricted interests (strong or intense interests in specific topics or objects), which could decrease the chances of diagnosis.<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal |last1=Loomes |first1=Rachel |last2=Hull |first2=Laura |last3=Mandy |first3=William Polmear Locke |date=June 2017 |title=What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0890856717301521 |journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry |language=en |volume=56 |issue=6 |pages=466–474 |doi=10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013|pmid=28545751 |s2cid=20420861 }}</ref>


Various studies suggest that autistic females are more likely to have co-occurring internalizing disorders, while their male counterparts are more likely to have co-occurring externalizing disorders. Internalizing problems (the inward expression of emotional difficulties, in contrast to externalizing problems), while not a core feature of autism, could still affect how females present symptoms of autism. For example, more severe expressions of these co-occurring internalizing disorders could mask underlying symptoms of autism. Moreover, if males are more likely to present with co-occurring externalizing disorders, their symptoms could be more disruptive, thereby being noticed by teachers and caregivers sooner than females with autism who have co-occurring internalizing disorders.<ref name=":8" />
====Female phenotype====
Some have suggested a differential [[phenotype]] for autistic women; "a female-specific manifestation of autistic strengths and difficulties, which fits imperfectly with current, male-based conceptualisations" of autism.<ref name="phenotype1"/> Autistic women have been shown to score higher in self-reports of [[autistic masking]], which may factor into the different phenotype.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hull |first1=L |last2=Lai |first2=MC |last3=Baron-Cohen |first3=S |last4=Allison |first4=C |last5=Smith |first5=P |last6=Petrides |first6=KV |last7=Mandy |first7=W |title=Gender differences in self-reported camouflaging in autistic and non-autistic adults. |journal=Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice |date=February 2020 |volume=24 |issue=2 |pages=352–363 |doi=10.1177/1362361319864804 |pmid=31319684 |s2cid=197664299 |url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/1362361319864804 |access-date=25 October 2021}}</ref> One study found evidence for a diagnostic bias against girls who meet criteria for ASD. In some cases where females showed severe autistic traits, they failed to meet the criteria for a diagnosis, because of the lack of sensitivity to the female phenotype.<ref name=":1" />


Camouflaging, the conscious or unconscious manners individuals learn or develop to hide their autistic symptoms, has been found to be more prevalent in autistic girls than boys, but other literature displays varied results. When it comes to social camouflaging, there are three sub-categories according to the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q): Masking, Assimilation, and Compensation.<ref name=":7">{{Cite web |last=Engelbrecht |first=Natalie |title=The Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q) {{!}} Embrace Autism |url=https://embrace-autism.com/cat-q/ |access-date=2023-10-12 |website=embrace-autism.com |language=en-US}}</ref> Masking is the act of constantly monitoring one's behavior in order to hide one's autistic traits and/or putting on a fake persona.<ref name=":6">{{Cite journal |last1=Hull |first1=Laura |last2=Petrides |first2=K. V. |last3=Allison |first3=Carrie |last4=Smith |first4=Paula |last5=Baron-Cohen |first5=Simon |last6=Lai |first6=Meng-Chuan |last7=Mandy |first7=William |date=2017-08-01 |title="Putting on My Best Normal": Social Camouflaging in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=47 |issue=8 |pages=2519–2534 |doi=10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5 |issn=1573-3432 |pmc=5509825 |pmid=28527095}}</ref><ref name=":7" /> Assimilation is known as "hiding in plain sight" or trying to blend in with non-autistic peers.<ref name=":6" /> Finally, compensation is trying to over-compensate for a lack of social abilities. Examples of this can include mimicking real or fictional people, over exaggerating non-verbal expressions, and creating scripts or rules when having a conversation with someone.<ref name=":6" />
====Camouflaging====
{{See also|Autistic masking}}
The DSM-5 mainly looks at two categories of autism spectrum symptoms when diagnosing someone: social deficits and restricted/repetitive behaviors and interests. Both of these categories of symptoms can be hidden by an aspect of the autistic female phenotype known as [[Autistic masking|camouflaging]].<ref name=":6">{{Cite journal |last1=Hull |first1=Laura |last2=Petrides |first2=K. V. |last3=Allison |first3=Carrie |last4=Smith |first4=Paula |last5=Baron-Cohen |first5=Simon |last6=Lai |first6=Meng-Chuan |last7=Mandy |first7=William |date=2017-08-01 |title="Putting on My Best Normal": Social Camouflaging in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=47 |issue=8 |pages=2519–2534 |doi=10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5 |issn=1573-3432 |pmc=5509825 |pmid=28527095}}</ref><ref name=":3" /><ref>Jorgenson, Courtney; Lewis, Timothy; Rose, Chad; Kanne, Stephan (2022-04-08). [https://rdcu.be/cZKmT "Social Camouflaging in Autistic and Neurotypical Adolescents: A Pilot Study of Differences by Sex and Diagnosis"]. ''rdcu.be''. Retrieved 2022-11-15.</ref>

Autistic girls tend to camouflage more than boys, this leads to many of their symptoms being hidden and not noticed by professionals.<ref name=":3" /> When it comes to social camouflaging, there are three sub-categories according to the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q): Masking, Assimilation, and Compensation.<ref name=":7">Engelbrecht, Natalie. [https://embrace-autism.com/cat-q/#What_it_tests "The Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire"]. ''embrace-autism.com''. Retrieved 2022-11-14.</ref> Masking is the act of constantly monitoring one's behavior in order to hide one's autistic traits and/or putting on a fake persona.<ref name=":6" /><ref name=":7" /> Assimilation is known as "hiding in plain sight" or trying to blend in with non-autistic peers.<ref name=":6" /> Finally, compensation is trying to over-compensate for a lack of social abilities. Examples of this can include mimicking real or fictional people, over exaggerating non-verbal expressions, and creating scripts or rules when having a conversion with someone.<ref name=":6" />

Camouflaging can also be used to hide repetitive/restricted behaviors and interests.<ref name=":6" /> In fact, researchers have found that autistic girls are ten times more likely to not originally meet the DSM-5 criteria for restricted/repetitive behaviors.<ref>{{Cite journal |title=Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder based on DSM-5 Criteria: Evidence from Clinician and Teacher Reporting |url=https://rdcu.be/cZKoE |access-date=2022-12-04 |journal=Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology |year=2014 |doi=10.1007/s10802-014-9881-x |language=en |last1=Hiller |first1=Rachel M. |last2=Young |first2=Robyn L. |last3=Weber |first3=Nathan |volume=42 |issue=8 |pages=1381–1393 |pmid=24882502 |s2cid=254580713 }}</ref> Sensory overstimulation is another autistic trait that can be hidden by masking.<ref name=":6" /> Participants of the Hull, et al., would internalize their overwhelming feelings and try to channel it through small and unnoticeable everyday objects. If those objects were not enough to calm them down, then they would try to leave the environment and recuperate by making " regular excuses<nowiki>''</nowiki> as to why they needed to leave.<ref name=":6" />


====Downfalls of camouflaging====
====Downfalls of camouflaging====
Studies have shown that high levels of camouflaging is can lead to higher levels of anxiety and depression and can increase the risk of suicidal ideation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Cassidy |first1=S. A. |last2=Gould |first2=K. |last3=Townsend |first3=E. |last4=Pelton |first4=M. |last5=Robertson |first5=A. E. |last6=Rodgers |first6=J. |date=2020-10-01 |title=Is Camouflaging Autistic Traits Associated with Suicidal Thoughts and Behaviours? Expanding the Interpersonal Psychological Theory of Suicide in an Undergraduate Student Sample |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04323-3 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=50 |issue=10 |pages=3638–3648 |doi=10.1007/s10803-019-04323-3 |issn=1573-3432 |pmc=7502035 |pmid=31820344}}</ref><ref name="phenotype1" /><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Cage |first1=Eilidh |last2=Troxell-Whitman |first2=Zoe |date=2019-05-01 |title=Understanding the Reasons, Contexts and Costs of Camouflaging for Autistic Adults |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=49 |issue=5 |pages=1899–1911 |doi=10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x |issn=1573-3432 |pmc=6483965 |pmid=30627892}}</ref><ref name=":3" /> Studies have also found that camouflaging can lead to a skewed sense of self.<ref name="phenotype1" /> This is especially the case for people who had been masking and mimicking other people for long periods of time.<ref name=":6" /> Another factor of masking is mental and physical exhaustion after a camouflaging session.<ref name=":3" /> According to the participants of the Hull, et al (2017)<ref name=":6" /> study, the longer that autistic individuals camouflage, the worse the exhaustion becomes and the longer these individuals need to rest and recharge. This study had also found that there were increased amounts of anxiety and stress revolving around camouflaging because the participants were often worried that they did not mask enough, did not mask correctly, or did not reach the desired effects of masking in that camouflaging session. Another one of the factors that increased anxiety and exhaustion while camouflaging is the fact that it "involved a constant monitoring of the situation, as if training oneself in self-monitoring, self-awareness, and monitoring others' reactions, both during and after the interaction occurred."<ref name=":6" />
Studies have shown that high levels of camouflaging is can lead to higher levels of anxiety and depression and can increase the risk of suicidal ideation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Cassidy |first1=S. A. |last2=Gould |first2=K. |last3=Townsend |first3=E. |last4=Pelton |first4=M. |last5=Robertson |first5=A. E. |last6=Rodgers |first6=J. |date=2020-10-01 |title=Is Camouflaging Autistic Traits Associated with Suicidal Thoughts and Behaviours? Expanding the Interpersonal Psychological Theory of Suicide in an Undergraduate Student Sample |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04323-3 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=50 |issue=10 |pages=3638–3648 |doi=10.1007/s10803-019-04323-3 |issn=1573-3432 |pmc=7502035 |pmid=31820344}}</ref><ref name="phenotype1" /><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Cage |first1=Eilidh |last2=Troxell-Whitman |first2=Zoe |date=2019-05-01 |title=Understanding the Reasons, Contexts and Costs of Camouflaging for Autistic Adults |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=49 |issue=5 |pages=1899–1911 |doi=10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x |issn=1573-3432 |pmc=6483965 |pmid=30627892}}</ref><ref name=":3">{{Cite journal |last1=Lai |first1=Meng-Chuan |last2=Lombardo |first2=Michael V |last3=Ruigrok |first3=Amber NV |last4=Chakrabarti |first4=Bhismadev |last5=Auyeung |first5=Bonnie |last6=Szatmari |first6=Peter |last7=Happé |first7=Francesca |last8=Baron-Cohen |first8=Simon |last9=MRC AIMS Consortium |date=August 2017 |title=Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism |journal=Autism |language=en |volume=21 |issue=6 |pages=690–702 |doi=10.1177/1362361316671012 |issn=1362-3613 |pmc=5536256 |pmid=27899710}}</ref> Studies have also found that camouflaging can lead to a skewed sense of self.<ref name="phenotype1" /> This is especially the case for people who had been masking and mimicking other people for long periods of time.<ref name=":6" /> Another factor of masking is mental and physical exhaustion after a camouflaging session.<ref name=":3" /> According to the participants of the Hull, et al (2017)<ref name=":6" /> study, the longer that autistic individuals camouflage, the worse the exhaustion becomes and the longer these individuals need to rest and recharge. This study had also found that there were increased amounts of anxiety and stress revolving around camouflaging because the participants were often worried that they did not mask enough, did not mask correctly, or did not reach the desired effects of masking in that camouflaging session. Another one of the factors that increased anxiety and exhaustion while camouflaging is the fact that it "involved a constant monitoring of the situation, as if training oneself in self-monitoring, self-awareness, and monitoring others' reactions, both during and after the interaction occurred."<ref name=":6" />


==Differences in gender and sexuality identification==
==Differences in gender and sexuality identification==
{{main|Autism and LGBT identities}}
{{main|Autism and LGBT identities}}
[[Sexuality]] is often discussed within the autistic community, with many observations that identities other than [[Cisgender|cis]]-[[Heterosexuality|hetero]] seem to be more common than is observed in the [[neurotypical]] population. There have not been many formal studies on this to date, however members{{who|date=September 2021}} of the community speculate that autistic individuals generally have different ideals, perceptions and desires than neurotypicals or simply do not comprehend or agree with society's expectation, making them more apt to diverge from the norm.


Growing literature suggests a higher diversity of [[Gender identity|gender identities]] and [[Sexual orientation|sexual orientations]] in autistic populations as compared to neurotypical populations.<ref name=":9">{{Cite journal |last1=Kallitsounaki |first1=Aimilia |last2=Williams |first2=David M. |date=2023-08-01 |title=Autism Spectrum Disorder and Gender Dysphoria/Incongruence. A systematic Literature Review and Meta-Analysis |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=53 |issue=8 |pages=3103–3117 |doi=10.1007/s10803-022-05517-y |issn=1573-3432 |pmc=10313553 |pmid=35596023}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Maggio |first1=Maria Grazia |last2=Calatozzo |first2=Patrizia |last3=Cerasa |first3=Antonio |last4=Pioggia |first4=Giovanni |last5=Quartarone |first5=Angelo |last6=Calabrò |first6=Rocco Salvatore |date=November 2022 |title=Sex and Sexuality in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Scoping Review on a Neglected but Fundamental Issue |journal=Brain Sciences |language=en |volume=12 |issue=11 |pages=1427 |doi=10.3390/brainsci12111427 |doi-access=free |pmid=36358354 |pmc=9688284 |issn=2076-3425}}</ref>
A study looking at the co-occurrence of ASD in patients with [[gender dysphoria]] found 7.8% of patients to be on the autism spectrum.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=de Vries |first1=Annelou L. C. |last2=Noens |first2=Ilse L. J. |last3=Cohen-Kettenis |first3=Peggy T. |last4=van Berckelaer-Onnes |first4=Ina A. |last5=Doreleijers |first5=Theo A. |title=Autism Spectrum Disorders in Gender Dysphoric Children and Adolescents |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |date=August 2010 |volume=40 |issue=8 |pages=930–936 |doi=10.1007/s10803-010-0935-9 |pmid=20094764 |pmc=2904453 }}</ref> Another study consisting of online surveys that included those who identified as [[nonbinary]] and those identifying as [[transgender]] without diagnoses of gender dysphoria found the number to be as high as 24% of gender diverse people having autism, versus around 5% of the surveyed cisgender people.<ref>{{cite news |last1=Dattaro |first1=Laura |title=Largest study to date confirms overlap between autism and gender diversity |url=https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/largest-study-to-date-confirms-overlap-between-autism-and-gender-diversity/ |access-date=5 May 2021 |work=Spectrum |date=2020-09-14}}</ref> A possible hypothesis for the correlation may be that autistic people are less willing or able to conform to societal norms, which may explain the high number of autistic individuals who identify outside the stereotypical gender binary.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Warrier |first1=Varun |last2=Greenberg |first2=David |last3=Weir |first3=Elizabeth |last4=Buckingham |first4=Clara |last5=Smith |first5=Paula |last6=Lai |first6=Meng-Chuan |last7=Allison |first7=Carrie |last8=Baron-Cohen |first8=Simon |title=Elevated rates of autism, other neurodevelopmental and psychiatric diagnoses, and autistic traits in transgender and gender-diverse individuals |journal=Nature Communications |date=2020-08-07 |volume=11 |issue=3959 |page=3959 |doi=10.1038/s41467-020-17794-1 |pmid=32770077 |pmc=7415151 |bibcode=2020NatCo..11.3959W |url=}}</ref> As of yet, there have been no studies specifically addressing the occurrence of autism in [[intersex]] individuals.


A study looking at the co-occurrence of ASD in patients with [[gender dysphoria]] found 7.8% of patients to be on the autism spectrum. Another study consisting of online surveys that included those who identified as [[non-binary]] and those identifying as [[transgender]] without diagnoses of gender dysphoria found the number to be as high as 24% of gender diverse people having autism, versus around 5% of the surveyed cisgender people. A possible hypothesis for the correlation may be that autistic people are less willing or able to conform to societal norms, which may explain the high number of autistic individuals who identify outside the stereotypical gender binary. As of yet, there have been no studies specifically addressing the occurrence of autism in [[intersex]] individuals.
A study conducted by Byers and Nichols (2014) explored the level of sexual satisfaction of [[high-functioning autistic]] individuals, with researchers testing the sexual and relationship satisfaction of neurotypical versus high functioning autistic individuals. The results suggest that men with ASD are generally less satisfied with their relationship or marriage compared to neurotypical men and women, and women with ASD.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Byers |first1=E. Sandra |last2=Nichols |first2=Shana |title=Sexual Satisfaction of High-Functioning Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder |journal=Sexuality and Disability |date=September 2014 |volume=32 |issue=3 |pages=365–382 |doi=10.1007/s11195-014-9351-y |s2cid=143453860 }}</ref>

Recent literature suggests that 11% of people who are gender dysphoric or gender incongruent are autistic.<ref name=":9" /> Many theories exist regarding the suggested link between gender diversity and autism: Vanderlaan et al.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=VanderLaan |first=Doug P. |last2=Leef |first2=Jonathan H. |last3=Wood |first3=Hayley |last4=Hughes |first4=S. Kathleen |last5=Zucker |first5=Kenneth J. |date=June 2015 |title=Autism Spectrum Disorder Risk Factors and Autistic Traits in Gender Dysphoric Children |url=http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10803-014-2331-3 |journal=Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=45 |issue=6 |pages=1742–1750 |doi=10.1007/s10803-014-2331-3 |issn=0162-3257}}</ref> proposed that a high birth weight could be the determinant of this co-occurrence, but this idea is challenged by its association with lower fetal testosterone, contradicting other autism theories such as Baron-Cohen's Extreme Male Brain hypothesis. Social theories, such as Gallucci et al. (2005) and Tateno et al. (2008), argue that individuals with autism may experience gender diversity as a way to avoid conventional sexual relationships or as a result of peer harassment. Psychologically, early theories from Landén et al. (1997) and Williams (1996) linked transidentity in autistic individuals to [[Special interest (autism)|restricted interests or obsessive preoccupations]], though these ideas have largely been refuted. Criticisms of these theories often focus on their reliance on insufficient evidence and their failure to fully capture the complexity of both gender identity and autism.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Wattel |first1=Luna L. |last2=Walsh |first2=Reubs J. |last3=Krabbendam |first3=Lydia |date=2024-06-01 |title=Theories on the Link Between Autism Spectrum Conditions and Trans Gender Modality: a Systematic Review |journal=Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders |language=en |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=275–295 |doi=10.1007/s40489-022-00338-2 |issn=2195-7185 |pmc=11127869 |pmid=38803560}}</ref>

While more research is needed, current literature suggests that there is a link between autistic traits and non-heterosexuality within both neurotypical and autistic samples. This relationship is especially prevalent in autistic women.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Sala |first1=Giorgia |last2=Pecora |first2=Laura |last3=Hooley |first3=Merrilyn |last4=Stokes |first4=Mark A. |date=2020-06-01 |title=As Diverse as the Spectrum Itself: Trends in Sexuality, Gender and Autism |url=https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s40474-020-00190-1 |journal=Current Developmental Disorders Reports |language=en |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=59–68 |doi=10.1007/s40474-020-00190-1 |issn=2196-2987}}</ref>


==See also==
==See also==
Line 70: Line 63:
*[[Sex differences in schizophrenia]]
*[[Sex differences in schizophrenia]]
*[[Autism Diagnostic Interview]]
*[[Autism Diagnostic Interview]]
*[[Autism in France]]


==References==
==References==
Line 76: Line 70:
== Further reading ==
== Further reading ==
* {{Cite book |last=Hendrickx |first=Sarah |title=Women and Girls with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Understanding Life Experiences from Early Childhood to Old Age |date=2015 |publisher=[[Jessica Kingsley Publishers]] |isbn=978-0-85700-982-1 |location=London and Philadelphia, PA}}
* {{Cite book |last=Hendrickx |first=Sarah |title=Women and Girls with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Understanding Life Experiences from Early Childhood to Old Age |date=2015 |publisher=[[Jessica Kingsley Publishers]] |isbn=978-0-85700-982-1 |location=London and Philadelphia, PA}}
* {{Cite book |last=Brady |first=Fern|title=Strong Female Character |date=2023 |publisher=Harmony/Rodale |isbn=978-0-593-58250-3}}



{{Pervasive developmental disorders}}{{Evolutionary psychology}}
{{Pervasive developmental disorders}}{{Evolutionary psychology}}

Latest revision as of 22:26, 24 November 2024

Sex and gender differences in autism exist regarding prevalence, presentation, and diagnosis.

Men and boys are more frequently diagnosed with autism than women and girls. It is debated whether this is due to a sex difference in rates of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) or whether females are underdiagnosed.[1][2] The prevalence ratio is often cited as about 4 males for every 1 female diagnosed.[3] Other research indicates that it is closer to 3:1 or 2:1.[2][4] One in every 42 males and one in 189 females in the United States is diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.[5] There is some evidence that females may also receive diagnoses somewhat later than males; however, thus far results have been contradictory.[6]

Background

[edit]

Hans Asperger was one of the first people to study autism, with all of his four study subjects being male. Another early researcher, Leo Kanner described "autistic disturbances of affective contact" in the group consisting of eight boys and three girls.[7]

Today, Autism Spectrum Disorder is commonly defined as a neurological developmental disorder with symptoms of poor social communication, repetitive behaviors, sensory sensitivities, executive dysfunction, and hyper-fixations.[8] In the modern day, women are less likely to be diagnosed as autistic than men; they are often misdiagnosed or not noticed to be neurodivergent by doctors.[9] Women are also more likely to be diagnosed as autistic at a later age than men.[10] There are many theories to explain this discrepancy in diagnoses, the most prominent being extreme male brain theory, imprinted brain theory, female protective effect theory, and female autism phenotype theory.

Theories explaining gender diagnosis disparity

[edit]

Extreme male brain theory

[edit]

Extreme male brain theory is an extension of the empathizing-systemizing theory, which categorizes people into 5 different groups based on their empathizing and systemizing expressions. In the general neurotypical population, females have a greater ability to empathize, and males have a greater ability to systemize.[11] Simon Baron-Cohen's extreme male brain theory states that autistic males have higher doses of prenatal testosterone and on average have a more systemizing brain, as opposed to the more empathizing female brain. He suggests that autistic brains show an exaggeration of the features associated with male brains. These are mainly size and connectivity, with males generally having a larger brain,[12] which is seen in an exaggerated form in those with ASD.

Individuals with ASD were found to have widespread abnormalities in interconnectivity and general functioning in specific brain regions.[13] This could explain the different results on empathy tests between men and women[14] as well as the deficiencies in empathy seen in ASD, as empathy requires several brain regions to be activated which need information from many different areas of the brain.[15] Baron-Cohen therefore argues that genetic factors play a role in autism prevalence and that children with technically minded parents are more likely to be diagnosed with autism.[16] Although autistic females have been documented to have higher testosterone levels, which could support the extreme male brain theory, not all autistic females show male-specific symptoms, leaving the extreme male brain theory with Autism Spectrum Disorder to be controversial.[11]

Imprinted brain theory

[edit]

The imprinted brain theory suggests genomic imprinting is at least partly responsible for the sex differences in autism and implicates schizophrenia as well, claiming that genetic and physiological evidence suggests the two conditions are on a spectrum in which some mutations in certain genes cause lower social cognition but higher practical cognition (autism) while other mutations in the same genes cause lower practical cognition with higher social cognition (schizophrenia).[17][18][19]

Female protective effect theory

[edit]

According to the female protective effect hypothesis, more genetic mutations are required for a girl to develop autism than for a boy. In 2012, Harvard researchers published findings suggesting that, on average, more genetic and environmental risk factors are required for girls to develop autism, compared to boys. The researchers analyzed DNA samples of nearly 800 families affected by autism and nearly 16,000 individuals with a variety of neurodevelopmental disorders. They looked for various types of gene mutations. Overall, they found that females diagnosed with autism or another neurodevelopmental disorder had a greater number of harmful mutations throughout the genome than did males with the same disorders.[20] Women with an extra X chromosome, 47,XXX or triple X syndrome, have autism-like social impairments in 32% of cases.[21]

Female autism phenotype theory

[edit]

The prevalence ratio is often cited as about 4 males for every 1 female diagnosed.[3] Other research indicates that it closer to 3:1 or 2:1.[2][22]

Some have suggested a differential phenotype for autistic women; "a female-specific manifestation of autistic strengths and difficulties, which fits imperfectly with current, male-based conceptualisations" of autism.[22] The female autism phenotype differs from the typical male autism phenotype in social relationships, relational interests, internalizing problems, and camouflaging.[23]

Some authors, clinicians and experts like Judith Gould, Tony Attwood, Lorna Wing and Christopher Gillberg[24] have proposed that autism in females may be underdiagnosed due to better natural superficial social mimicry skills in females, partially different set of symptoms and less knowledge about autism in females among experts.[25] In his preword to the book Asperger's and Girls, Attwood writes: "These tentative explanations for the apparent underrepresentation of girls with Asperger's Syndrome have yet to be examined by objective research studies."[26]

Another clinician, William Mandy, hypothesized referrals for ASD assessment are often started by teachers. Girls with ASD may sometimes lack the skills of social communication and this is not noticed until they are in a school setting. Therefore, girls suggested to have ASD may receive delayed or no clinical assessment.[27] Compared with males, females with autism are more likely to mask their restricted interests (strong or intense interests in specific topics or objects), which could decrease the chances of diagnosis.[28]

Recent literature has exemplified that autistic females present lower levels of restricted and repetitive interests. However, some studies suggest that autistic females’ interests are in areas that aren’t considered atypical or captured in the diagnostic process as compared to autistic males’ special interests.[23]

Various studies suggest that autistic females are more likely to have co-occurring internalizing disorders, while their male counterparts are more likely to have co-occurring externalizing disorders. Internalizing problems (the inward expression of emotional difficulties, in contrast to externalizing problems), while not a core feature of autism, could still affect how females present symptoms of autism. For example, more severe expressions of these co-occurring internalizing disorders could mask underlying symptoms of autism. Moreover, if males are more likely to present with co-occurring externalizing disorders, their symptoms could be more disruptive, thereby being noticed by teachers and caregivers sooner than females with autism who have co-occurring internalizing disorders.[23]

Camouflaging, the conscious or unconscious manners individuals learn or develop to hide their autistic symptoms, has been found to be more prevalent in autistic girls than boys, but other literature displays varied results. When it comes to social camouflaging, there are three sub-categories according to the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q): Masking, Assimilation, and Compensation.[29] Masking is the act of constantly monitoring one's behavior in order to hide one's autistic traits and/or putting on a fake persona.[30][29] Assimilation is known as "hiding in plain sight" or trying to blend in with non-autistic peers.[30] Finally, compensation is trying to over-compensate for a lack of social abilities. Examples of this can include mimicking real or fictional people, over exaggerating non-verbal expressions, and creating scripts or rules when having a conversation with someone.[30]

Downfalls of camouflaging

[edit]

Studies have shown that high levels of camouflaging is can lead to higher levels of anxiety and depression and can increase the risk of suicidal ideation.[31][22][32][33] Studies have also found that camouflaging can lead to a skewed sense of self.[22] This is especially the case for people who had been masking and mimicking other people for long periods of time.[30] Another factor of masking is mental and physical exhaustion after a camouflaging session.[33] According to the participants of the Hull, et al (2017)[30] study, the longer that autistic individuals camouflage, the worse the exhaustion becomes and the longer these individuals need to rest and recharge. This study had also found that there were increased amounts of anxiety and stress revolving around camouflaging because the participants were often worried that they did not mask enough, did not mask correctly, or did not reach the desired effects of masking in that camouflaging session. Another one of the factors that increased anxiety and exhaustion while camouflaging is the fact that it "involved a constant monitoring of the situation, as if training oneself in self-monitoring, self-awareness, and monitoring others' reactions, both during and after the interaction occurred."[30]

Differences in gender and sexuality identification

[edit]

Growing literature suggests a higher diversity of gender identities and sexual orientations in autistic populations as compared to neurotypical populations.[34][35]

A study looking at the co-occurrence of ASD in patients with gender dysphoria found 7.8% of patients to be on the autism spectrum. Another study consisting of online surveys that included those who identified as non-binary and those identifying as transgender without diagnoses of gender dysphoria found the number to be as high as 24% of gender diverse people having autism, versus around 5% of the surveyed cisgender people. A possible hypothesis for the correlation may be that autistic people are less willing or able to conform to societal norms, which may explain the high number of autistic individuals who identify outside the stereotypical gender binary. As of yet, there have been no studies specifically addressing the occurrence of autism in intersex individuals.

Recent literature suggests that 11% of people who are gender dysphoric or gender incongruent are autistic.[34] Many theories exist regarding the suggested link between gender diversity and autism: Vanderlaan et al.[36] proposed that a high birth weight could be the determinant of this co-occurrence, but this idea is challenged by its association with lower fetal testosterone, contradicting other autism theories such as Baron-Cohen's Extreme Male Brain hypothesis. Social theories, such as Gallucci et al. (2005) and Tateno et al. (2008), argue that individuals with autism may experience gender diversity as a way to avoid conventional sexual relationships or as a result of peer harassment. Psychologically, early theories from Landén et al. (1997) and Williams (1996) linked transidentity in autistic individuals to restricted interests or obsessive preoccupations, though these ideas have largely been refuted. Criticisms of these theories often focus on their reliance on insufficient evidence and their failure to fully capture the complexity of both gender identity and autism.[37]

While more research is needed, current literature suggests that there is a link between autistic traits and non-heterosexuality within both neurotypical and autistic samples. This relationship is especially prevalent in autistic women.[38]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Halladay, Alycia K; Bishop, Somer; Constantino, John N; Daniels, Amy M; Koenig, Katheen; Palmer, Kate; Messinger, Daniel; Pelphrey, Kevin; Sanders, Stephan J; Singer, Alison Tepper; Taylor, Julie Lounds; Szatmari, Peter (December 2015). "Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority". Molecular Autism. 6 (1): 36. doi:10.1186/s13229-015-0019-y. PMC 4465158. PMID 26075049.
  2. ^ a b c Loomes, Rachel; Hull, Laura; Mandy, William Polmear Locke (June 2017). "What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 56 (6): 466–474. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013. PMID 28545751. S2CID 20420861.
  3. ^ a b Fombonne, Eric (June 2009). "Epidemiology of Pervasive Developmental Disorders". Pediatric Research. 65 (6): 591–598. doi:10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203. PMID 19218885. S2CID 20373463.
  4. ^ Hull, Laura; Petrides, K. V.; Mandy, William (December 2020). "The Female Autism Phenotype and Camouflaging: a Narrative Review". Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 7 (4): 306–317. doi:10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9. S2CID 214314845.
  5. ^ "Data and Statistics on Autism Spectrum Disorder". Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 25 September 2020.
  6. ^ Begeer, Sander; Mandell, David; Wijnker-Holmes, Bernadette; Venderbosch, Stance; Rem, Dorien; Stekelenburg, Fred; Koot, Hans M. (May 2013). "Sex Differences in the Timing of Identification Among Children and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 43 (5): 1151–1156. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1656-z. PMID 23001766. S2CID 2705562.
  7. ^ Kanner, Leo (1943). "Autistic disturbances of affective contact". Nervous Child. 2 (3): 217–250. NAID 10007611258.
  8. ^ Jung, Minyoung; Mody, Maria; Saito, Daisuke N.; Tomoda, Akemi; Okazawa, Hidehiko; Wada, Yuji; Kosaka, Hirotaka (2015-11-24). "Sex Differences in the Default Mode Network with Regard to Autism Spectrum Traits: A Resting State fMRI Study". PLOS ONE. 10 (11): e0143126. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1043126J. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0143126. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4658035. PMID 26600385.
  9. ^ Lai, Meng-Chuan; Baron-Cohen, Simon (November 2015). "Identifying the lost generation of adults with autism spectrum conditions". The Lancet Psychiatry. 2 (11): 1013–1027. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(15)00277-1. PMID 26544750.
  10. ^ Giarelli, Ellen; Wiggins, Lisa D.; Rice, Catherine E.; Levy, Susan E.; Kirby, Russell S.; Pinto-Martin, Jennifer; Mandell, David (1 April 2010). "Sex differences in the evaluation and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders among children". Disability and Health Journal. 3 (2): 107–116. doi:10.1016/j.dhjo.2009.07.001. PMC 4767258. PMID 21122776.
  11. ^ a b Williams, Olivia O. F.; Coppolino, Madeleine; Perreault, Melissa L. (2021-12-09). "Sex differences in neuronal systems function and behaviour: beyond a single diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders". Translational Psychiatry. 11 (1): 625. doi:10.1038/s41398-021-01757-1. ISSN 2158-3188. PMC 8660826. PMID 34887388.
  12. ^ Baron-Cohen, Simon; Knickmeyer, Rebecca C.; Belmonte, Matthew K. (4 November 2005). "Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism" (PDF). Science. 310 (5749): 819–823. Bibcode:2005Sci...310..819B. doi:10.1126/science.1115455. PMID 16272115. S2CID 44330420.
  13. ^ Anderson, J. S.; Druzgal, T. J.; Froehlich, A.; DuBray, M. B.; Lange, N.; Alexander, A. L.; Abildskov, T.; Nielsen, J. A.; Cariello, A. N.; Cooperrider, J. R.; Bigler, E. D.; Lainhart, J. E. (2010-10-12). "Decreased Interhemispheric Functional Connectivity in Autism". Cerebral Cortex. 21 (5): 1134–1146. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq190. ISSN 1047-3211. PMC 3077433. PMID 20943668.
  14. ^ Teatero, Missy L.; Netley, Charles (November 2013). "A Critical Review of the Research on the Extreme Male Brain Theory and Digit Ratio (2D:4D)". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 43 (11): 2664–2676. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1819-6. ISSN 0162-3257. PMID 23575643. S2CID 254569210.
  15. ^ Baron-Cohen, Simon; Knickmeyer, Rebecca C.; Belmonte, Matthew K. (2005-11-04). "Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism" (PDF). Science. 310 (5749): 819–823. Bibcode:2005Sci...310..819B. doi:10.1126/science.1115455. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 16272115. S2CID 44330420.
  16. ^ Baron-Cohen, Simon (November 2012). "Autism and the technical mind: live chat with Simon Baron-Cohen, November 9, 10 A.M. EST". Scientific American. Vol. 307, no. 5. pp. 72–75.
  17. ^ Crespi, Bernard; Badcock, Christopher (June 2008). "Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (3): 241–261. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004214. PMID 18578904.
  18. ^ Crespi, Bernard; Stead, Philip; Elliot, Michael (26 January 2010). "Comparative genomics of autism and schizophrenia". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (Suppl 1): 1736–1741. doi:10.1073/pnas.0906080106. PMC 2868282. PMID 19955444.
  19. ^ Ciaramidaro, Angela; Bölte, Sven; Schlitt, Sabine; Hainz, Daniela; Poustka, Fritz; Weber, Bernhard; Bara, Bruno G; Freitag, Christine; Walter, Henrik (1 January 2015). "Schizophrenia and Autism as Contrasting Minds: Neural Evidence for the Hypo-Hyper-Intentionality Hypothesis". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 41 (1): 171–179. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu124. PMC 4266299. PMID 25210055.
  20. ^ Jacquemont, Sébastien; Coe, Bradley P.; Hersch, Micha; Duyzend, Michael H.; Krumm, Niklas; Bergmann, Sven; Beckmann, Jacques S.; Rosenfeld, Jill A.; Eichler, Evan E. (6 March 2014). "A Higher Mutational Burden in Females Supports a "Female Protective Model" in Neurodevelopmental Disorders". The American Journal of Human Genetics. 94 (3): 415–425. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.02.001. PMC 3951938. PMID 24581740.
  21. ^ Otter, Maarten; Crins, Peter M. L.; Campforts, Bea C. M.; Stumpel, Constance T. R. M.; van Amelsvoort, Thérèse A. M. J.; Vingerhoets, Claudia (March 2021). "Social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with triple X syndrome". BJPsych Open. 7 (2): e51. doi:10.1192/bjo.2021.8. PMC 8058878. PMID 33583482.
  22. ^ a b c d Bargiela, Sarah; Steward, Robyn; Mandy, William (1 October 2016). "The Experiences of Late-diagnosed Women with Autism Spectrum Conditions: An Investigation of the Female Autism Phenotype". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 46 (10): 3281–3294. doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8. PMC 5040731. PMID 27457364.
  23. ^ a b c Hull, Laura; Petrides, K. V.; Mandy, William (2020-12-01). "The Female Autism Phenotype and Camouflaging: a Narrative Review". Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 7 (4): 306–317. doi:10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9. ISSN 2195-7185.
  24. ^ "Gender and autism". The National Autistic Society. Retrieved 2016-06-01.
  25. ^ Attwood, Tony (2006). Asperger's and Girls. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. pp. 2–6. ISBN 978-1-932565-40-9.
  26. ^ Attwood, Tony (2006). Asperger's and Girls. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. p. 6. ISBN 978-1-932565-40-9.
  27. ^ Mandy, William; Chilvers, Rebecca; Chowdhury, Uttom; Salter, Gemma; Seigal, Anna; Skuse, David (2012-07-01). "Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Evidence from a Large Sample of Children and Adolescents". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 42 (7): 1304–1313. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0. ISSN 1573-3432. PMID 21947663. S2CID 18639019.
  28. ^ Loomes, Rachel; Hull, Laura; Mandy, William Polmear Locke (June 2017). "What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" (PDF). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 56 (6): 466–474. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013. PMID 28545751. S2CID 20420861.
  29. ^ a b Engelbrecht, Natalie. "The Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q) | Embrace Autism". embrace-autism.com. Retrieved 2023-10-12.
  30. ^ a b c d e f Hull, Laura; Petrides, K. V.; Allison, Carrie; Smith, Paula; Baron-Cohen, Simon; Lai, Meng-Chuan; Mandy, William (2017-08-01). ""Putting on My Best Normal": Social Camouflaging in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 47 (8): 2519–2534. doi:10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5. ISSN 1573-3432. PMC 5509825. PMID 28527095.
  31. ^ Cassidy, S. A.; Gould, K.; Townsend, E.; Pelton, M.; Robertson, A. E.; Rodgers, J. (2020-10-01). "Is Camouflaging Autistic Traits Associated with Suicidal Thoughts and Behaviours? Expanding the Interpersonal Psychological Theory of Suicide in an Undergraduate Student Sample". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 50 (10): 3638–3648. doi:10.1007/s10803-019-04323-3. ISSN 1573-3432. PMC 7502035. PMID 31820344.
  32. ^ Cage, Eilidh; Troxell-Whitman, Zoe (2019-05-01). "Understanding the Reasons, Contexts and Costs of Camouflaging for Autistic Adults". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 49 (5): 1899–1911. doi:10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x. ISSN 1573-3432. PMC 6483965. PMID 30627892.
  33. ^ a b Lai, Meng-Chuan; Lombardo, Michael V; Ruigrok, Amber NV; Chakrabarti, Bhismadev; Auyeung, Bonnie; Szatmari, Peter; Happé, Francesca; Baron-Cohen, Simon; MRC AIMS Consortium (August 2017). "Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism". Autism. 21 (6): 690–702. doi:10.1177/1362361316671012. ISSN 1362-3613. PMC 5536256. PMID 27899710.
  34. ^ a b Kallitsounaki, Aimilia; Williams, David M. (2023-08-01). "Autism Spectrum Disorder and Gender Dysphoria/Incongruence. A systematic Literature Review and Meta-Analysis". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 53 (8): 3103–3117. doi:10.1007/s10803-022-05517-y. ISSN 1573-3432. PMC 10313553. PMID 35596023.
  35. ^ Maggio, Maria Grazia; Calatozzo, Patrizia; Cerasa, Antonio; Pioggia, Giovanni; Quartarone, Angelo; Calabrò, Rocco Salvatore (November 2022). "Sex and Sexuality in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Scoping Review on a Neglected but Fundamental Issue". Brain Sciences. 12 (11): 1427. doi:10.3390/brainsci12111427. ISSN 2076-3425. PMC 9688284. PMID 36358354.
  36. ^ VanderLaan, Doug P.; Leef, Jonathan H.; Wood, Hayley; Hughes, S. Kathleen; Zucker, Kenneth J. (June 2015). "Autism Spectrum Disorder Risk Factors and Autistic Traits in Gender Dysphoric Children". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 45 (6): 1742–1750. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2331-3. ISSN 0162-3257.
  37. ^ Wattel, Luna L.; Walsh, Reubs J.; Krabbendam, Lydia (2024-06-01). "Theories on the Link Between Autism Spectrum Conditions and Trans Gender Modality: a Systematic Review". Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 11 (2): 275–295. doi:10.1007/s40489-022-00338-2. ISSN 2195-7185. PMC 11127869. PMID 38803560.
  38. ^ Sala, Giorgia; Pecora, Laura; Hooley, Merrilyn; Stokes, Mark A. (2020-06-01). "As Diverse as the Spectrum Itself: Trends in Sexuality, Gender and Autism". Current Developmental Disorders Reports. 7 (2): 59–68. doi:10.1007/s40474-020-00190-1. ISSN 2196-2987.

Further reading

[edit]