Jump to content

IQ and the Wealth of Nations: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m Reverted edits by 125.237.47.37 (talk) to last revision by ClueBot (HG)
AnomieBOT (talk | contribs)
m Dating maintenance tags: {{Source needed}} {{Citation needed}}
 
(856 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|2002 book by Richard Lynn}}
{{Infobox Book
{{Other uses|Nations and intelligence}}
| name = ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''
{{Use dmy dates|date=December 2021}}
| title_orig =
{{Infobox book
| translator =
| image = [[Image:IQ and the Wealth of Nations.jpg|170px]]
| name = ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''
| title_orig =
| image_caption = ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations'' cover
| author = [[Richard Lynn]]<br>[[Tatu Vanhanen]]
| translator =
| image = IQ and the Wealth of Nations.jpg
| illustrator =
| cover_artist =
| caption = Cover
| author = [[Richard Lynn]]<br>[[Tatu Vanhanen]]
| country =
| language = English
| illustrator =
| series =
| cover_artist =
| subject =
| country =
| genre =
| language = English
| publisher = Praeger/Greenwood
| series =
| subject = Human intelligence, economic inequality, psychology, sociology
| pub_date = 2002
| genre =
| english_pub_date =
| publisher = [[Greenwood Publishing Group|Praeger/Greenwood]]
| media_type =
| pages =
| pub_date = 28 February 2002
| isbn =
| english_pub_date =
| oclc =
| media_type = Print ([[hardcover]])
| preceded_by =
| pages = 320
| isbn = 978-0275975104
| followed_by =
| oclc =
| preceded_by =
| followed_by =
}}
}}
'''''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''''' is a [[controversial]] 2002 book by Dr. [[Richard Lynn]], Professor Emeritus of Psychology at the [[University of Ulster]], [[Northern Ireland]], and Dr. [[Tatu Vanhanen]], Professor Emeritus of Political Science at the [[University of Tampere]], [[Tampere]], [[Finland]].<ref name="main">Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-97510-X</ref> The book argues that differences in national income (in the form of [[per capita]] [[gross domestic product]]) [[correlation|correlate]] with differences in the average national [[intelligence quotient]] (IQ). The authors interpret this correlation as showing that IQ is one important factor contributing to differences in national wealth and rates of economic growth, but that it is not the only determinant of these differences. The data, methodology, and conclusions have been criticized.<ref>[[IQ_and_the_Wealth_of_Nations#Criticism]]</ref> The 2006 book ''[[IQ and Global Inequality]]'' is a follow-up to ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''.
'''''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''''' is a 2002 book by psychologist [[Richard Lynn]] and [[political science|political scientist]] [[Tatu Vanhanen]].<ref name="main">Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). [https://books.google.com/books?id=KQ4rLiAbHQQC IQ and the wealth of nations]. Westport, CT: Praeger. {{ISBN|0-275-97510-X}}</ref> The authors argue that differences in national income (in the form of [[per capita]] [[gross domestic product]]) are [[correlation|correlated]] with differences in the average national [[intelligence quotient]] (IQ). They further argue that differences in average national IQs constitute one important factor, but not the only one, contributing to differences in [[national wealth]] and rates of [[economic growth]].

The book has drawn widespread criticism from other academics. Critiques have included questioning of the methodology used, the incompleteness of the data, and the conclusions drawn from the analysis.<ref name="suz.unizh.ch">[https://web.archive.org/web/20120229125855/http://www.history.ox.ac.uk/hsmt/courses_reading/undergraduate/authority_of_nature/week_8/volken.pdf The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth: A Critique of] [[Richard Lynn]]<span zoompage-fontsize="12"> and Tatu Vanhanens Recent Book</span> by Thomas Volken</ref><ref name="nature.com">[http://www.nature.com/hdy/journal/v92/n4/full/6800418a.html Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations] Heredity April 2004, Volume 92, Number 4, Pages 359–360. K Richardson.</ref> The 2006 book ''[[IQ and Global Inequality]]'' is a follow-up to ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations'' by the same authors.


==Outline==
==Outline==
The book includes the authors' calculation of average IQ scores for 60 countries, based on their analysis of published reports. It reports their observation that national IQ [[Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient|correlates]] with [[gross domestic product]] per capita at 0.82, and with the rate of economic growth from 1950 to 1990 at 0.64.
[[Image:IQatWoN GDP IQ.png|right|thumb|The central thesis of ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations'' is that the average IQ of a nation correlates with its GDP<ref name="main"/> Above is a scatter plot with Lynn and Vanhanen's calculated IQ values (without estimates) and GDP data.<ref name=IQ1>See [http://www.rlynn.co.uk/pages/article_intelligence/t4.asp Intelligence and the Wealth and Poverty of Nations] by [[Richard Lynn]]</ref> Data from Table 7.7 in the book - Real GDP per capita 1998, and IQ. Residual real GDP, and Fitted real GDP columns not displayed. Table 7.7 in the book titled, '''The Results of the Regression Analysis in which Real GDP Per Capita 1998 is Used as The Dependent Variable and National IQ is Used as the Independent Variable for 81 countries'''.]]
The book includes the authors' calculation of average IQ scores for 81 countries, based on their analysis of published reports. It reports their observation that national IQ correlates with [[gross domestic product]] per capita at 0.82, and with the rate of economic growth from 1950–1990 at 0.64.


The authors believe that average IQ differences between nations are due to both [[gene]]tic and economic factors. They also believe that low GDP can cause low IQ, just as low IQ can cause low GDP. (See: [[Positive feedback]])
The authors believe that average IQ differences between nations are due to both [[heritability of IQ|genetic]] and [[Environment and intelligence|environmental factors]]. They also believe that low GDP can cause low IQ, just as low IQ can cause low GDP. (See [[Positive feedback]]).


The authors write that it is the ethical responsibility of rich, high-IQ nations to financially assist poor, low-IQ nations, as it is the responsibility of rich citizens to assist the poor.
The authors write that it is the ethical responsibility of rich, high-IQ nations to assist poor, low-IQ nations financially, as it is the responsibility of rich citizens to assist the poor.

The book was cited several times in the popular press, notably the British newspaper ''[[The Times]]''.{{Citation needed|date=April 2008}} Because Tatu Vanhanen is the father of [[Matti Vanhanen]], the [[Finland|Finnish]] [[Prime minister]], his work has received wide publicity in Finland. It has also been severely criticized.
<ref name="suz.unizh.ch">[http://www.suz.unizh.ch/volken/ThomasVolken/pdfs/IQWealthNation.pdf The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth: A Critique of [[Richard Lynn]] and Tatu Vanhanens Recent Book] by Thomas Volken</ref><ref name="nature.com">[http://www.nature.com/hdy/journal/v92/n4/full/6800418a.html Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations] Heredity April 2004, Volume 92, Number 4, Pages 359-360. K Richardson.</ref>


==National IQ estimates==
==National IQ estimates==
The result claims that [[Hong Kong]] has the highest national IQ estimate with 107, followed by [[South Korea]] with 106.<ref name="main"/>


Central to the book's thesis is a tabulation of what Lynn and Vanhanen believe to be the average IQs of the world's nations. Rather than do their own IQ studies, the authors average and adjust existing studies and use other methods to create estimates.
[[Image:InvertedIQbyCountry.png|right|thumb|National IQ estimates<ref>[http://www.rlynn.co.uk/pages/article_intelligence/t4.asp National IQs Based on the Results of Intelligence Tests]</ref>]]


For 104 of the 185 nations, no studies were available. In those cases, the authors have used an estimated value by taking averages of the IQs of neighboring or comparable nations. For example, the authors arrived at a figure of 84 for El Salvador by averaging their calculations of 79 for Guatemala and 88 for Colombia. Including those estimated IQs, the correlation of IQ and GDP is 0.62.{{citation needed|reason= no source provided for claim. Previous source (not included) did not provide textual foundation.|date=December 2024}}
Central to the book's thesis is a tabulation of what Lynn and Vanhanen believe to be the average IQs of the world's nations. Rather than do their own IQ studies (a potentially massive project), the authors average and adjust existing studies.


To obtain a figure for South Africa, the authors averaged IQ studies done on different ethnic groups, resulting in a figure of 72. The figures for Colombia, Peru, and Singapore were arrived at in a similar manner.
For 104 of the 185 nations, no studies were available. In those cases, the authors have used an estimated value by taking averages of the IQs of neighboring or comparable nations. For example, the authors arrived at a figure of 84 for El Salvador by averaging their calculations of 79 for Guatemala and 88 for Colombia. Including those estimated IQs, the correlation of IQ and GDP is 0.62.


In some cases, the IQ of a country is estimated by averaging the IQs of countries that are not actually neighbors of the country in question. For example, [[Kyrgyzstan]]'s IQ is estimated by averaging the IQs of Iran and Turkey, neither of which is close to Kyrgyzstan—China, which is a geographic neighbor, is not counted as such by Lynn and Vanhanen. This is because ethnic background is assumed to be more important than proximity to other nations when determining national IQ.{{source needed|reason= claim of authors assumptions without support. Statement on averages is found in the primary source, but specific study needs proof/ reference.|date=December 2024}}
To obtain a figure for South Africa, the authors averaged IQ studies done on different ethnic groups, resulting in a figure of 72. The figures for Colombia, Peru, and Singapore were arrived at in a similar manner. For [[People's Republic of China]], the authors used a figure of 109.4 for [[Shanghai]] and adjusted it down by an arbitrary 6 points because they believed the average across China's rural areas was probably less than that in Shanghai. Another figure from a study done in [[Beijing]] was not adjusted downwards. Those two studies formed the resultant score for China (PRC). For the figure of [[Macau]], the average IQ is 104 which is obtained from the score of the [[Programme for International Student Assessment]] (PISA) and in such a way transformed into an IQ score.<ref>See Prenzel, Manfred et al. (eds).: PISA 2003. Münster: Waxmann 2004, p. 70, Table 2.9; or: PISA 2003: ''A Profile of Student Performance in Mathematics''</ref>


To account for the [[Flynn effect]] (an increase in IQ scores over time), the authors adjusted the results of older studies upward by a number of points.
In some cases, the IQ of a country is estimated by averaging the IQs of countries that are not actually neighbors of the country in question. For example, [[Kyrgyzstan]]'s IQ is estimated by averaging the IQs of Iran and Turkey, neither of which is close to Kyrgyzstan—China, which is a geographic neighbor, is not counted as such by Lynn and Vanhanen. This is presumably because the ethnic groups of the area speak Iranian and Turkic languages, but do not include Chinese.


===Scores that do not support the theory===
To account for the [[Flynn effect]] (an increase in IQ scores over time), the authors adjusted the results of older studies upward by a number of points.
In several cases the actual GDP did not correspond with that predicted by IQ. In these cases, the authors argued that differences in GDP were caused by differences in natural resources and whether the nation used a [[planned economy|planned]] or [[market economy]].


One example of this was [[Economy of Qatar|Qatar]], whose IQ was estimated by Lynn and Vanhanen to be about 78, yet had a disproportionately high per capita GDP of roughly US$17,000. The authors explain Qatar's disproportionately high GDP by its high [[petroleum]] resources. Similarly, the authors think that large resources of [[diamond]]s explain the economic growth of the African nation [[Education in Botswana|Botswana]], the fastest in the world for several decades.
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
{| class="wikitable sortable"
|-
! Rank !! Country !! IQ estimate<ref name=IQ1/>
|-
| 1 || {{flagcountry|Hong Kong}} || 107
|-
| 2 || {{flagcountry|South Korea}} || 106
|-
| 3 || {{flagcountry|Japan}} || 105
|-
| 4 || {{flagcountry|Taiwan}} || 104
|-
| 5 || {{flagcountry|Singapore}} || 103
|-
| 6 || {{flagcountry|Austria}} || 102
|-
| 6 || {{flagcountry|Germany}} ||102
|-
| 6 || {{flagcountry|Italy}} || 102
|-
| 6 || {{flagcountry|Netherlands}} || 102
|-
| 10 || {{flagcountry|Sweden}} || 101
|-
| 10 || {{flagcountry|Switzerland}} || 101
|-
| 12 || {{flagcountry|Belgium}} || 100
|-
| 12 || {{flagcountry|People's Republic of China|China (PRC)}} || 100
|-
| 12 || {{flagcountry|New Zealand}} || 100
|-
| 12 || {{flagcountry|United Kingdom}} || 100
|-
| 16 || {{flagcountry|Hungary}} || 99
|-
| 16 || {{flagcountry|Poland}} || 99
|-
| 16 || {{flagcountry|Spain}} || 99
|-
| 19 || {{flagcountry|Australia}} || 98
|-
| 19 || {{flagcountry|Denmark}} || 98
|-
| 19 || {{flagcountry|France}} || 98
|-
| 19 || {{flagcountry|Mongolia}} || 98
|-
| 19 || {{flagcountry|Norway}} || 98
|-
| 19 || {{flagcountry|United States}} || 98
|-
| 25 || {{flagcountry|Canada}} || 97
|-
| 25 || {{flagcountry|Czech Republic}} || 97
|-
| 25 || {{flagcountry|Finland}} || 97
|-
| 28 || {{flagcountry|Argentina}} || 96
|-
| 28 || {{flagcountry|Russia}} || 96
|-
| 28 || {{flagcountry|Slovakia}} || 96
|-
| 28 || {{flagcountry|Uruguay}} || 96
|-
| 32 || {{flagcountry|Portugal}} || 95
|-
| 32 || {{flagcountry|Slovenia}} || 95
|-
| 34 || {{flagcountry|Israel}} || 94
|-
| 34 || {{flagcountry|Romania}} || 94
|-
| 36 || {{flagcountry|Bulgaria}} || 93
|-
| 36 || {{flagcountry|Ireland}} || 93
|-
| 36 || {{flagcountry|Greece}} || 93
|-
| 39 || {{flagcountry|Malaysia}} || 92
|-
| 40 || {{flagcountry|Thailand}} || 91
|-
| 41 || {{flagcountry|Croatia}} || 90
|-
| 41 || {{flagcountry|Peru}} || 90
|-
| 41 || {{flagcountry|Turkey}} || 90
|-
|}
{{col-break}}
{| class="wikitable sortable"
|-
! Rank !! Country !! IQ estimate<ref name=IQ1/>
|-
| 44 || {{flagcountry|Colombia}} || 89
|-
| 44 || {{flagcountry|Indonesia}} || 89
|-
| 44 || {{flagcountry|Suriname}} || 89
|-
| 47 || {{flagcountry|Brazil}} || 87
|-
| 47 || {{flagcountry|Iraq}} || 87
|-
| 47 || {{flagcountry|Mexico}} || 87
|-
| 47 || {{flagcountry|Samoa}} || 87
|-
| 47 || {{flagcountry|Tonga}} || 87
|-
| 52 || {{flagcountry|Lebanon}} || 86
|-
| 52 || {{flagcountry|Philippines}} || 86
|-
| 54 || {{flagcountry|Cuba}} || 85
|-
| 54 || {{flagcountry|Morocco}} ||85
|-
| 56 || {{flagcountry|Fiji}} || 84
|-
| 56 || {{flagcountry|Iran}} || 84
|-
| 56 || {{flagcountry|Marshall Islands}} || 84
|-
| 56 || {{flagcountry|Puerto Rico}} || 84
|-
| 60 || {{flagcountry|Egypt}} || 83
|-
| 60 || {{flagcountry|Saudi Arabia}} || 83
|-
| 60 || {{flagcountry|United Arab Emirates}} || 83
|-
| 61 || {{flagcountry|India}} || 81
|-
| 62 || {{flagcountry|Ecuador}} || 80
|-
| 63 || {{flagcountry|Guatemala}} || 79
|-
| 64 || {{flagcountry|Barbados}} || 78
|-
| 64 || {{flagcountry|Nepal}} || 78
|-
| 64 || {{flagcountry|Qatar}} || 78
|-
| 67 || {{flagcountry|Zambia}} || 77
|-
| 68 || {{flagcountry|Republic of the Congo}} || 73
|-
| 68 || {{flagcountry|Uganda}} || 73
|-
| 70 || {{flagcountry|Jamaica}} || 72
|-
| 70 || {{flagcountry|Kenya}} || 72
|-
| 70 || {{flagcountry|South Africa}} || 72
|-
| 70 || {{flagcountry|Sudan}} || 72
|-
| 70 || {{flagcountry|Tanzania}} || 72
|-
| 75 || {{flagcountry|Ghana}} || 71
|-
| 76 || {{flagcountry|Nigeria}} || 67
|-
| 77 || {{flagcountry|Guinea}} || 66
|-
| 77 || {{flagcountry|Zimbabwe}} || 66
|-
| 79 || {{flagcountry|Democratic Republic of the Congo}} || 65
|-
| 80 || {{flagcountry|Sierra Leone}} || 64
|-
| 81 || {{flagcountry|Ethiopia}} || 63
|-
| 82 || {{flagcountry|Equatorial Guinea}} || 59
|}
{{col-end}}


The authors argued that the [[People's Republic of China]]'s per capita GDP of at the time roughly US$4,500 could be explained by its use of a [[Communism in China|communist economic system]] for much of its [[Ideology of the Communist Party of China#Economics|recent history]]. The authors also predicted that communist nations whom they believe have comparatively higher IQs, including [[Economy of the People's Republic of China|China]] and [[Economy of North Korea|North Korea]], can be expected to rapidly gain GDP growth by moving from centrally planned economies to more [[capitalism in China|capitalist based economic systems]], while predicting continued poverty for sub-Saharan African nations no matter what economic systems are used.
*Although not a nation, Hong Kong appears on this list. The sovereignty of Hong Kong was returned to China by the United Kingdom in 1997. However, Hong Kong still maintains most of the colonial administrative system and hence appears in many "international" rankings.


== Reception and impact ==
===Special cases===
[[File:IQ by Country.png|thumb|right|390px|Map depicting average IQ values as presented in the follow-up scientific study, 'Intelligence and the Wealth and Poverty of Nations']]
In several cases the actual GDP did not correspond with that predicted by IQ. In these cases, the authors argued that differences in GDP were caused by differences in natural resources and whether the nation used a "[[planned economy|planned]]" or [[market economy|"market" economy]].
Several negative reviews of the book have been published in the scholarly literature. Susan Barnett and Wendy Williams wrote that "we see an edifice built on layer upon layer of arbitrary assumptions and selective [[data manipulation]]. The data on which the entire book is based are of questionable validity and are used in ways that cannot be justified." They also wrote that cross country comparisons are "virtually meaningless."<ref>{{Cite journal | journal = Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books | date = August 2004 | volume = 49 | issue = 4 | pages = 389–396 | author1 = Barnett, Susan M. | author2 = Williams, Wendy | title = National Intelligence and the Emperor's New Clothes | doi = 10.1037/004367 }}</ref>


Richardson (2004) argued, citing the [[Flynn effect]] as the best evidence, that Lynn has the causal connection backwards and suggested that "the average IQ of a population is simply an index of the size of its middle class, both of which are results of industrial development". The review concludes that "This is not so much science, then, as a social crusade."<ref name="nature.com"/> A review by Michael Palairet criticized the book's methodology, particularly the imprecise estimates of GDP and the fact that IQ data were only available for 81 of the 185 countries studied. However, the review concluded that the book was "a powerful challenge to economic historians and development economists who prefer not to use IQ as an analytical input", but that it's likely those scholars will deliberately ignore this work instead of improving it.<ref>{{citation|author=Palairet, M. R.|date=2004|title=Book review, IQ and the Wealth of Nations|journal=Heredity|volume=92|issue=4|pages=361–362|doi=10.1038/sj.hdy.6800427|doi-access=free}}</ref>
One example of this was Qatar, whose IQ was estimated by Lynn and Vanhanen to be about 78, yet had a disproportionately high per capita GDP of roughly USD $17,000. The authors explain Qatar's disproportionately high GDP by its high [[petroleum]] resources. Similarly, the authors think that large resources of [[diamond]]s explain the economic growth of the African nation [[Botswana]], the fastest in the world for several decades.


===By economists===
The authors argued that the [[People's Republic of China]]'s per capita GDP of roughly USD $4,500 could be explained by its use of a communist [[economic system]] for much of its [[Economy of the People's Republic of China|recent history]]. The authors also predicted that communist nations whom they believe have comparatively higher IQs, including the PRC, Vietnam, and North Korea, can be expected to gain GDP by moving from centrally-planned to market economic systems, while predicting continued poverty for African nations. Recent trends in the [[economy of the People's Republic of China]] and [[Vietnam]] seem to confirm this prediction, as China's GDP has grown rapidly since introducing market reforms. South Korea has a higher average IQ and a market economy. However, South Korea still has a lower [[List of countries by GDP (PPP) per capita|GDP/Capita]] than many Western nations (but relatively high overall), but South Korean economic reform started in 1970s and she is one of the fastest growing economies in the world. It should be noticed that South Korea is one of the rare countries that successfully developed economy among the poorest countries in the past 50 years. Contrary to the theory of correlation between IQ and economy type many planned economies had higher literacy rates than most market economies. Still, South Korea went from amongst the poorest nations in the world to advanced economy by recording among fastest growth rate in the world. Despite a supposedly higher average IQ and a market economy since the [[Meiji Restoration]] in 1867, [[Japan]] still has a lower [[List of countries by GDP (PPP) per capita|GDP/Capita]] than many Western nations. <!--[http://www.delawareonline.com/apps/pbcs.dll/article?AID=/20070225/OPINION09/702250302/1110/OPINION] This URL is broken, and articles in the delawareonline archive are subscription based. the date of the article appears to be 2007-02-25 -->
In a book review for the ''[[Journal of Economic Literature]]'', Thomas Nechyba wrote that "such sweeping conclusions based on relatively weak statistical evidence and dubious presumptions seem misguided at best and quite dangerous if taken seriously. It is therefore difficult to find much to recommend in this book."<ref>{{citation|title=Reviewed Work: IQ and the Wealth of Nations by Richard Lynn, Tatu Vanhanen|first=Thomas J.|last=Nechyba|journal=[[Journal of Economic Literature]]|volume=42|issue=1|date=Mar 2004|pages=220–221}}</ref>


Writing in the ''[[Economic Journal]]'', Astrid Oline Ervik said that the book may be "thought provoking", but there is nothing that economists can learn from it. She criticized the book's authors for not establishing cross country comparability and reliability of IQ scores, for relying on simple bivariate correlations, for not considering or controlling for other hypotheses, and for confusing correlation with causation. Ervik stated, "The arguments put forward in the book to justify such comparisons [between the average IQ in different countries and their GDP] seem at best vague and unconvincing. At worst, passages in the book appear to be biased and unscientific...The authors fail to present convincing evidence and appear to jump to conclusions."<ref>{{citation|title=Reviewed Work: IQ and the Wealth of Nations by Richard Lynn, Tatu Vanhanen|first=Astrid Oline|last=Ervik|journal=[[The Economic Journal]]|volume=113|issue=488, Features|pages=F406–F408|date=Jun 2003|jstor=3590222|doi=10.1111/1468-0297.13916}}</ref>
The two most striking exceptions, however, may be [[Ireland]] and the [[United States]]. Ireland, whose average I.Q. is listed at 93, had the fourth highest per capita GDP ([[Purchasing power parity|PPP]] adjusted) of any country in the world (after tiny [[Luxembourg]], Norway and the United States).<ref>[http://stats.oecd.org/wbos/Default.aspx?usercontext=sourceoecd] OECD figures] for GDP PPP adjusted for 2005</ref><ref>[ GDP and GNI Ranking country performance is an interesting and useful exercise, but it can be misleading, in part because of what the indicators do not show], [http://www.oecdobserver.org/info/about_us.php OECD Observer], March 2005. "Ireland is another country where GDP has to be read with care. Ireland's position has risen up the GDP per head rankings since 1999, and is now in the top five countries in the OECD. This remarkable transformation has been put down to a mix of factors, of which inward investment in high value-added businesses is one. But does GDP per head accurately reflects Ireland’s actual wealth, since all that inward investment (and foreign labour) generates profits and other revenues, some of which inevitably flows back to the countries of origin?"</ref> The United States, with an average I.Q. of 98, has the third-highest per capita GDP (PPP adjusted), and is by far the most populous of the richest 10 countries. Both of these countries have I.Q. averages considerably below those of countries such as South Korea, Taiwan, and Germany, but have per capita GDPs about 1.5 times higher.


[[Edward M. Miller|Edward Miller]], an economist who has published many controversial papers on [[race and intelligence]], gave the book positive reviews in two different [[white nationalist]] publications, the ''[[Journal of Social, Political, and Economic Studies]]'' and ''[[The Occidental Quarterly]]''.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Miller | first1 = E | year = 2002 | title = BOOK REVIEW ARTICLE: Differential Intelligence and National Income |url=https://www.jspes.org/winter2002_miller.html | journal = The Journal of Social, Political and Economic Studies | volume = 27 | pages = 513–522}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last=Miller |first=Edward M. |url=http://www.toqonline.com/archives/v2n4/TOQv2n4Miller.pdf |title=IQ and the Wealth of Nations |type=book review |journal=[[The Occidental Quarterly]] |volume=2 |issue=4 |pages=79–83}}</ref>
==Related studies==
<!--If you restore the cleanup tag here, please leave a note on the talk page suggesting items that need attention. Thank you.-->
''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''' was not peer-reviewed before publication but was published by a publisher of academic literature. Peer-reviewed articles have used the IQ scores presented in the book and some have also commented on the claims in the book.

Several negative reviews have been published in the scholarly literature. Susan Barnett and Wendy Williams wrote that "we see an edifice built on layer upon layer of arbitrary assumptions and selective [[data manipulation]]. The data on which the entire book is based are of questionable validity and are used in ways that cannot be justified."<ref>{{Cite journal | journal = Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books | month = August | year = 2004 | volume = 49 | issue = 4 | pages = 389–396 | author = Barnett, Susan M. and Williams, Wendy | title = National Intelligence and the Emperor's New Clothes | url = http://psycinfo.apa.org/psyccritiques/display/?uid=2004-17780-001}}</ref> They also wrote that cross country comparisons are "virtually meaningless." Ken Richardson wrote "This is not so much science, then, as a social crusade. The [[Pioneer Fund]] of America, champion of many dubious causes in the past, will obtain little credit from having assisted this one."<ref>[http://www.nature.com/hdy/journal/v92/n4/full/6800418a.html Heredity April 2004, Volume 92, Number 4, Pages 359-360]</ref> [[Thomas Nechyba]] wrote of "relatively weak statistical evidence and dubious presumptions."<ref>Nechyba, T. (2004). Review of IQ and the Wealth of Nations. Journal of
Economic Literature, 42, 220–221. (p. 220)</ref> Astrid Ervik asked "are people in rich countries smarter than those in poorer countries?" and concluded that "the authors fail to present convincing evidence and appear to jump to conclusions."<ref>Ervik, A. O. (2003). IQ and the Wealth of Nations. The Economic Journal, 113, No. 488, F406–F407.</ref>

Denny Borsboom (2006) finds that mainstream contemporary test analysis does not reflect substantial recent developments in the field and "bears an uncanny resemblance to the psychometric state of the art as it existed in the 1950s." For example, it notes that ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations'', in order to show that the tests are unbiased, uses outdated methodology, if anything indicative that test bias exist.<ref>[http://users.fmg.uva.nl/dborsboom/papers.htm The attack of the psychometricians]. DENNY BORSBOOM. PSYCHOMETRIKA VOL 71, NO 3, 425–440. SEPTEMBER 2006.</ref>

Thomas Volken wrote that the study is "neither methodologically nor theoretically convincing."<ref>[http://www.suz.unizh.ch/volken/ThomasVolken/pdfs/IQWealthNation.pdf ''The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth'']</ref> Although critical of the IQ data, for the sake of argument Volken assumes that the data is correct but then criticizes the statistical methods used, finding no effect on growth or income. Using the same assumption, Garett Jones
and W. Joel Schneider report a strong connection between intelligence and economic growth.<ref>{{cite journal | title = Intelligence, Human Capital, and Economic Growth: An Extreme-Bounds Analysis | url = http://tv.isg.si/site/ftpaccess/elogedusavoir/Intelligence,%20Human%20Capital,%20and%20Economic%20Growth%20-%20An%20Extreme-Bounds%20Analysis.pdf | format = pdf | author = Jones, G, Schneider, WJ | journal = Journal of Economic Growth | year = 2004 or 2006?<!-- First page states 2004; other sources seem to cite this as 2006; need volume and page -->}}</ref>

Erich Weede and Sebastian Kampf wrote that "there is one clear and robust result: average IQ does promote growth."<ref>Weede, E. and Kämpf, S. (2002). The Impact of Intelligence and Institutional Improvements on Economic Growth. Kyklos, 55, Fasc. 3, 361–380. (p. 376)</ref> Edward Miller wrote that "the theory helps significantly to explain why some countries are rich and some poor."<ref>Miller, E. (2002). Differential Intelligence and National Income. A review of IQ and the Wealth of Nations. Journal of Social, Political & Economic Studies, 27, 413–524. (p. 522)</ref> Michael Palairet wrote that "Lynn and Vanhanen have launched a powerful challenge to economic historians and development economists who prefer not to use IQ as an analytical input."<ref>Palairet, M. R. (2004). Book review, IQ and the Wealth of Nations. Heredity, 92, 361–362.</ref>
In a reanalysis of the Lynn and Vanhanen's hypothesis, Dickerson (2006) finds that IQ and GDP data is best fitted by an exponential function, with IQ explaining approximately 70% of the variation in GDP.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Dickerson, R. E. | year = 2006 | month = May-June | title = Exponential correlation of IQ and the wealth of nations | journal = Intelligence | volume = 34 | issue = 3 | pages = 291–295 | id = | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.006 }}</ref> Dickerson concludes that as a rough approximation "an increase of 10 points in mean IQ results in a doubling of the per capita GDP."

Whetzel and McDaniel (2006) conclude that the book's "results regarding the relationship between IQ, democracy and economic freedom are robust".<ref>{{cite journal | author = Whetzel, D. L. & McDaniel, M. A. | year = 2006 | month = September-October | title = Prediction of national wealth | journal = Intelligence | volume = 34 | issue = 5 | pages = 449–458 | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.003 }}[http://www.people.vcu.edu/~mamcdani/Publications/Whetzel%20&%20McDaniel%20(2006,%20Intelligence)%20Prediction%20of%20national%20wealth.pdf PDF]</ref> Moreover, they address "criticisms concerning the measurement of IQ in purportedly low IQ countries", finding that by setting "all IQ scores below 90 to equal 90, the relationship between IQ and wealth of nations remained strong and actually increased in magnitude." On this question they conclude that their findings "argue against claims made by some that inaccuracies in IQ estimation of low IQ countries invalidate conclusions about the relationship between IQ and national wealth."

Voracek (2004) used the national IQ data to examine the relationship between intelligence and suicide, finding national IQ was positively correlated with national male and female suicide rates.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Voracek, M. | year = 2004 | month = | title = National intelligence and suicide rate: an ecological study of 85 countries | journal = Personality and Individual Differences | volume = 37 | issue = 3 | pages = 543–553 | id = | doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.025 }}</ref> The effect was not attenuated by controlling for GDP.

Barber (2005) found that national IQ was associated with rates of secondary education enrollment, illiteracy, and agricultural employment.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Barber, N. | year = 2005 | month = | title = Educational and ecological correlates of IQ: A cross-national investigation | journal = Intelligence | volume = 33 | issue = 3 | pages = 273–284 | id = | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2005.01.001 }}</ref> The effect on illiteracy and agricultural employment remained with national wealth, infant mortality, and geographic continent controlled.

Both Lynn and [[J. Philippe Rushton|Rushton]] have suggested that high IQ is associated with colder climates. To test this hypothesis, Templer and Arikawa (2006) compare the national IQ data from Lynn and Vanhanen with data sets that describe national average skin color and average winter and summer temperatures.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Templer, D. I. and Arikawa, H. | year = 2006 | month = | title = Temperature, skin color, per capita income, and IQ: An international perspective | journal = Intelligence | volume = 34 | issue = 2 | pages = 121–139| id = | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2005.04.002 }}
see also discussion [http://theoccidentalquarterly.com/vol4no3/dt-tbc.html]</ref> They find that the strongest correlations to national IQ were −0.92 for skin color and −0.76 for average high winter temperature. They interpret this finding as strong support for IQ-climate association. Other studies using different data sets find no correlation [http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/101/35/12824][http://dienekes.angeltowns.net/articles/greekiq/].

[[Satoshi Kanazawa|Kanazawa]] (2006), "IQ and the wealth of states" (in press in ''Intelligence''), replicates across U.S. states Lynn and Vanhanen's demonstration that national IQs strongly correlate with macroeconomic performance.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Kanazawa, S. | year = 2006 | month = | title = IQ and the wealth of states | journal = Intelligence | volume = 34 | issue = 6 | pages = 593–600 | id = | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.003 | format = | accessdate = }}</ref> Kanazawa finds that state cognitive ability scores, based on the SAT data, correlate moderately with state economic performance, explaining about a quarter of the variance in gross state product per capita.

Hunt and Wittmann (in press) use data from the [[Programme for International Student Assessment]] (PISA) to conclude that "in spite of the weaknesses [in] several of their data points Lynn and Vanhanen's empirical conclusion was correct, but we question the simple explanation that national intelligence causes national wealth. We argue that the relationship is more complex".<ref>{{cite journal | author = Hunt, E., Wittmann, W. | year = in press | month = | title = National intelligence and national prosperity | journal = Intelligence | volume = 36| issue = | pages = 1| id = | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2006.11.002}}</ref>

The book was followed by Lynn's 2006 ''[[Race Differences in Intelligence]]'', which expands the data by nearly four times and concludes the average human IQ is presently 90 when compared to a norm of 100 based on UK data, or two thirds of a standard deviation below the UK norm, and Lynn and Vanhanen's 2006 ''[[IQ and Global Inequality]]''.<ref>Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2006). IQ and global inequality. Athens, GA: Washington Summit Books. see also {{cite journal | author = Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. | title = National differences in intelligence and educational attainment | journal = Intelligence | volume = In Press, Corrected Proof. | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2006.06.001 | year = 2007 | pages = 115}}</ref>

[[Jared Diamond]]'s ''[[Guns, Germs and Steel]]'' instead argues that historical differences in economic and technological development for different areas can be explained by differences in geography (which affects factors like population density and spread of new technology) and differences in available crops and domesticatable animals. [[Richard E. Nisbett|Richard Nisbett]] argues in his 2004 ''The Geography of Thought'' that some of these regional differences shaped lasting cultural traits, such as the collectivism required by East Asian rice [[irrigation]], compared with the individualism of [[Ancient Greece|ancient Greek]] herding, maritime mercantilism, and money crops wine and olive oil (pp.&nbsp;34–35).

== Criticism ==
===Criticism of research funding sources===
Lynn has been frequently criticized as a [[Pioneer Fund]] grantee.{{Clarify|date=April 2010}}


===Criticism of data sets===
===Criticism of data sets===
Academic reviews of the book generally criticized both its methodology and conclusions.
The figures were obtained by taking [[weighted mean|equally-weighted averages]] of different IQ tests. The number of studies is very limited; the IQ figure is based on one study in 34 nations, two studies in 30 nations. There were actual tests for IQ in 81 nations. In 104 of the world's nations there were no IQ studies at all and IQ was estimated based on IQ in surrounding nations.<ref name="suz.unizh.ch"/> The number of participants in each study was usually limited, often numbering under a few hundred. The exceptions to this were the [[United States]] and [[Japan]], for which studies using more than several thousand participants are available.


The methods of the study were criticized by [[Richard E. Nisbett]] for relying on small and haphazard samples and for ignoring data that did not support the conclusions.<ref name="Nisbett, Richard 2009. pp. 215">Nisbett, Richard. 2009. Intelligence and how to get it. pp. 215.</ref>
Many nations are very heterogeneous ethnically. This is true for many [[developing countries]]. It is very doubtful that an often limited number of participants from one or a few areas are representative for the population as whole.


[[University of Reading]] geographer Stephen Morse also criticized the book (as well as ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''), arguing that the authors' hypothesis rests on "serious flaws". Morse also argued: "The central dilemma of the Lynn and Vanhanen case rests with their assumption that national IQ data are primarily (not wholly) a function of innate ability, which in turn is at least partly generated by genes. There are many assumptions of cause–effect in here, and some of them involve substantial leaps of faith."<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Morse |first=Stephen |date=September 2008 |title=The geography of tyranny and despair: development indicators and the hypothesis of genetic inevitability of national inequality |journal=Geographical Journal |language=en |volume=174 |issue=3 |pages=195–206 |doi=10.1111/j.1475-4959.2008.00296.x |issn=0016-7398|url=http://epubs.surrey.ac.uk/351102/1/The%20geography%20of%20tyranny%20and%20dispair%20~%20The%20Geographical%20Journal.pdf |doi-access=free |bibcode=2008GeogJ.174..195M }}</ref>
Studies that were averaged together often used different methods of IQ testing, different scales for IQ values and/or were done decades apart. IQ in children is different although correlated with IQ later in life and many of the studies tested only young children.


In the 2010 paper "A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans", also published in ''Intelligence'', [[Jelte M. Wicherts]] and colleagues stated:
A test of 108 9-15-year olds in Barbados, of 50 13–16-year olds in Colombia, of 104 5–17-year olds in Ecuador, of 129 6–12-year olds in Egypt, of 48 10–14-year olds in Equatorial Guinea, and so on, all were taken as measures of 'national IQ'.<ref name="nature.com"/>


{{blockquote|1="For instance, Lynn and Vanhanen (2006) accorded a national IQ of 69 to Nigeria on the basis of three samples (Fahrmeier, 1975; Ferron, 1965; Wober, 1969), but they did not consider other relevant published studies that indicated that average IQ in Nigeria is considerably higher than 70 (Maqsud, 1980a, b; Nenty & Dinero, 1981; Okunrotifa, 1976). As Lynn rightly remarked during the 2006 conference of the [[International Society for Intelligence Research]] (ISIR), performing a literature review involves making a lot of choices. Nonetheless, an important drawback of Lynn (and Vanhanen)'s reviews of the literature is that they are [[systematic review|unsystematic]]."<ref name="Africans, Intelligence 2009">{{cite journal | last1 = Wicherts | first1 = J. M. |display-authors=etal | year = 2009 | title = A systematic literature review of the average IQ of Sub-Saharan Africans | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002 | journal = Intelligence | volume = 38| pages = 1–20}}</ref>}}
The notion that there is such a thing as a culturally neutral intelligence test is disputed.<ref>[https://www.tcrecord.org/Content.asp?ContentID=1544 ''The Cultural Context of Learning and Thinking: An Exploration in Experimental Anthropology''] Gay, Glick and Sharp (1971) made the following observation: "Cultural differences in cognition reside more in the situations to which particular cognitive processes are applied than in the existence of a process in one cultural group, and its absence in another." A similar position is held by Berry in ''Acculturative Stress'' 1974 [http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/5/4/382]</ref><ref>''Educational Handicap, Public Policy, and Social History: A Broadened Perspective on Mental Retardation'' ISBN 0029279208 Sarason and Doris (1979) view intelligence as a cultural invention that does not hold true across cultures.</ref><ref>[http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons (2004)] 1*, Leah K. Hamilton1, Betty R. Onyura1 and Andrew S. Winston International Journal of Selection and Assessment Volume 14 Issue 3 Page 278 - September 2006</ref><ref>[http://asm.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/12/3/303 Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment] Steven P. Verney Assessment, Vol. 12, No. 3, 303-319 (2005)</ref><ref>[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=15742541&dopt=Abstract Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance.] Shuttleworth-Edwards AB, Kemp RD, Rust AL, Muirhead JG, Hartman NP, Radloff SE. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2004 Oct;26(7):903-20.</ref> There are many difficulties when one is measuring IQ scores across cultures, and in multiple languages. Use of the same set of exams requires translation, with all its attendant difficulties and possible misunderstandings in other cultures.<ref>Instruments developed to quantify smartness are culturally based and cannot simply be "transplanted" to a culture with different values (Greenfield, 1997). In ''Culture as process: Empirical methods for cultural psychology''</ref> To adapt to this, some IQ tests rely on non-verbal approaches, which involve pictures, diagrams, and conceptual relationships (such as ''in-out'', ''great-small'', and so on).


Lynn and [[Gerhard Meisenberg]] replied that "critical evaluation of the studies presented by WDM shows that many of these are based on unrepresentative elite samples" and that a further literature review, including taking into account results in mathematics, science, and reading, gave "an IQ of 68 as the best reading of the IQ in sub-Saharan Africa".<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Lynn | first1 = Richard | last2 = Meisenberg | first2 = Gerhard | year = 2010 | title = The average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans: Comments on Wicherts, Dolan, and van der Maas | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2009.09.009 | journal = Intelligence | volume = 38 | issue = 1| pages = 21–29 }}</ref> Wicherts and colleagues in yet another reply stated: "In light of all the available IQ data of over 37,000 African testtakers, only the use of unsystematic methods to exclude the vast majority of data could result in a mean IQ close to 70. On the basis of sound methods, the average IQ remains close to 80. Although this mean IQ is clearly lower than 100, we view it as unsurprising in light of the potential of the [[Flynn effect]] in Africa (Wicherts, Borsboom, & Dolan, 2010) and common psychometric problems associated with the use of western IQ tests among Africans."<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Wicherts | first1 = Jelte M. | last2 = Dolan | first2 = Conor V. | last3 = van der Maas | first3 = Han L.J. | year = 2010 | title = The dangers of unsystematic selection methods and the representativeness of 46 samples of African test-takers | journal = Intelligence | volume = 38 | issue = 1| pages = 30–37 | doi=10.1016/j.intell.2009.11.003}}</ref>
===Criticism of data set sources===
There are also errors in the raw data presented by authors. The results from Vinko Buj's 1981 study of 21 European cities and the Ghanaian capital [[Accra]] used different scaling from Lynn and Vanhanen's. A comparison of the reported to actual data from only a single study found 5 errors in 19 reported IQ scores.<ref>[http://dienekes.blogspot.com/2004/08/richard-lynns-massaged-iq-data.html Richard Lynn's Massaged IQ Data]</ref><ref>[http://dienekes.angeltowns.net/articles/greekiq/ Greek IQ] by [[Dienekes Pontikos]]</ref>


Some criticisms have focused on the limited number of studies upon which the book is based. The IQ figures are based on 3 different studies for 17 nations, two studies for 30 nations, and one study for 34 nations. There were actual tests for IQ in the case of 81 countries out of the 185 countries studied. For 104 nations there were no IQ studies at all and IQ was estimated based on the average IQ of surrounding nations.<ref name="suz.unizh.ch"/> The limited number of participants in some studies as well as outdated data has also been criticized. A test of 108 9- to 15-year-olds in Barbados, of 50 13- to 16-year-olds in Colombia, of 104 5- to 17-year-olds in Ecuador, of 129 6- to 12-year-olds in Egypt, and of 48 10- to 14-year-olds in Equatorial Guinea, all were taken as measures of national IQ.<ref name="nature.com"/>
The national IQ of Ethiopia was estimated from a study done on 250 15-year-old Ethiopian Jews one year after their migration to Israel. The research compares their level of performance with native Israelis using progressive matrices tests. However, another study showed that after intensive training, the cognitive ability of Ethiopian Jewish immigrants improved and caught up with that of their native Israeli peer groups.<ref>Kaniel, S and Fisherman S. (1991). Level of performance and distribution of errors in the progressive matrices test: a comparison of Ethiopian immigrant and native Israeli adolescents. International Journal of Psychology, 26, 25-33</ref>


[[Denny Borsboom]] argued that mainstream contemporary test analysis does not reflect substantial recent developments in the field and "bears an uncanny resemblance to the psychometric state of the art as it existed in the 1950s". For example, he argued that ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations'', in order to show that the tests are unbiased, uses outdated methodology{{spaced ndash}} if anything, indicative that test bias exists.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Borsboom | first1 = Denny | year = 2006 | title = The attack of the psychometricians | url = http://users.fmg.uva.nl/dborsboom/BorsboomPM2006.pdf | journal = Psychometrika | volume = 71 | issue = 3 | pages = 425–440 | doi = 10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6 | pmid = 19946599 | pmc = 2779444 | url-status = bot: unknown | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20100215043343/http://users.fmg.uva.nl/dborsboom/BorsboomPM2006.pdf | archive-date = 2010-02-15 }}</ref> Girma Berhanu, in an essay review of the book, concentrated on the discussion of Ethiopian Jews. The review criticized the principal assertion of the authors that differences in intelligence, attributed to genetics, account for the gap between rich and poor countries. Berhanu criticized the book as being based in a "racist, sexist, and antihuman" research tradition and alleged that "the low standards of scholarship evident in the book render it largely irrelevant for modern science".<ref>{{Cite journal|journal=Education Review|year=2007|first=Girma|last=Bernahu|title=Black Intellectual Genocide: An Essay Review of ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''|url=http://edrev.asu.edu/index.php/ER/article/view/1373/44|pages=1–28| issn=1094-5296}}</ref>
=== Criticism of statistical manipulation===
As noted earlier, in many cases arbitrary adjustments were made by authors to account for the [[Flynn effect]] or when the authors thought that the studies were not representative of the ethnic or social composition of the nation.


===Impact on psychology===
One critic writes: "Their scheme is to take the British Ravens IQ in 1979 as 100, and simply add or subtract 2 or 3 to the scores from other countries for each decade that the relevant date of test departs from that year. The assumptions of size, linearity and universal applicability of this correction across all countries are, of course, hugely questionable if not breathtaking. Flynn's original results were from only 14 (recently extended to twenty) industrialised nations, and even those gains varied substantially with test and country and were not linear. For example, recent studies report increases of eight points per decade among Danes; six points per decade in Spain; and 26 points over 14 years in Kenya (confirming the expectation that newly developing countries would show more rapid gains)."<ref name="nature.com"/>
In 2006, Lynn and Vanhanen followed ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations'' with their book ''[[IQ and Global Inequality]]'', which contained additional data and analyses, but the same general conclusions as the earlier book. Discussing both books, [[Earl B. Hunt|Earl Hunt]] writes that although Lynn and Vanhanen's methodology and conclusions are questionable, they deserve credit for raising important questions about international IQ comparisons. Hunt writes that Lynn and Vanhanen are correct that national IQs correlate strongly with measures of social well-being, but they are unjustified in their rejection of the idea that national IQs could change as a result of improved education.<ref name="Hunt">Hunt, E. ''Human Intelligence''. Cambridge University Press, 2011, page 426-445.</ref>


On July 27, 2020, the European Human Behavior and Evolution Association issued a formal statement opposing the utilization of Lynn's national IQ dataset, as well as all updated forms of it, citing various criticisms of its methodology and data collection. They concluded that "any conclusions drawn from analyses which
There is controversy about the definition and usage of [[IQ]] and [[intelligence]]. See also [[race and intelligence]].
use these data are therefore unsound, and no reliable evolutionary work should be using these data."<ref>{{cite web|title=EHBEA Statement on National IQ Datasets|website=European Human Behaviour and Evolution Association|date=27 July 2020|url=https://ehbea2020.com/wp-content/uploads/2020/07/EHBEA_IQ_statement.pdf|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200810182007/https://ehbea2020.com/wp-content/uploads/2020/07/EHBEA_IQ_statement.pdf|archive-date=10 August 2020|url-status=dead}}</ref>

It is generally agreed many factors, including environment, culture, demographics, wealth, pollution, and educational opportunities, affect measured IQ.<ref>[http://faculty.mwsu.edu/psychology/Laura.Spiller/4503_Tests/intelligence_knowns_and_unknowns.pdf] Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns</ref> See also [[Health and intelligence]].

Finally, the [[Flynn effect]] may well reduce or eliminate differences in IQ between nations in the future. One estimate is that the average IQ of the US was below 75 before factors like improved nutrition started to increase IQ scores. Some predict that considering that the Flynn effect started first in more affluent nations, it will also disappear first in these nations. Then the IQ gap between nations will diminish. However, even assuming that the IQ difference will disappear among the babies born today, the differences will remain for decades simply because of the composition of the current workforce. [[Steve Sailer]] noted as much when discussing the workforce in both India and China (see second diagram) [http://www.vdare.com/sailer/060423_lynn.htm].

===Criticism of thesis===
Girma Berhanu, senior lecturer in the department of education at the [[University of Gothenburg]], prepared a detailed review of the book, concentrating on the discussion of [[Ethiopian Jews]].<ref>{{Cite journal|journal=Education Review|year=2007|first=Girma|last=Bernahu|title=Black Intellectual Genocide: An Essay Review of ''IQ and the Wealth of Nations''|url=http://edrev.asu.edu/essays/v10n6.pdf|pages=1–28}}</ref> The review criticizes the principal assertion of the authors that differences in intelligence attributed to genetics account for the gap between rich and poor countries. Carefully surveying related academic literature, the review points out flaws in the methodology of Lynn and Vanhanen, exposing the "racist, sexist and antihuman nature" of their underlying framework. Berhanu concludes that "the low standards of scholarship evident in the book render it largely irrelevant for modern science".


==See also==
==See also==
*[[History of the race and intelligence controversy]]
*[[Economic inequality]]
*[[Economic inequality]]
*''[[IQ and Global Inequality]]''
*''[[IQ and Global Inequality]]''
*[[Race and intelligence]]
*''[[The Bell Curve]]''
*''[[The Bell Curve]]''
*''[[The Mismeasure of Man]]
*''[[The Wealth and Poverty of Nations]]''


==References==
==References==
{{reflist|2}}
{{Reflist|2}}

==External links==
* [http://www.rlynn.co.uk/pages/article_intelligence/1.asp "Intelligence and the Wealth and Poverty of Nations"] - article by Lynn and Vanhanen
* [http://www.isteve.com/IQ_Table.htm 2002 (''IQ and the Wealth of Nations'') data]
* [http://www.v-weiss.de/calibration.html PISA scores transformed into IQ values in comparison with IQ estimated by Lynn and Vanhanen]
* [http://www.iapsych.com/articles/dickerson2006ip.pdf Exponential correlation of IQ and the wealth of nations] - Peer reviewed article to be published in an upcoming edition of [[Intelligence (journal)|Intelligence]]
* [http://www.ssc.uwo.ca/psychology/faculty/rushtonpdfs/L&Vreview.pdf "The Bigger Bell Curve: Intelligence, National Achievement, and The Global Economy"], review by [[J. Philippe Rushton]]
* [http://rpongett.phpwebhosting.com/gdp.html "A Reader's statistical update of IQ & The Wealth of Nations"]
* [http://www.vdare.com/sailer/wealth_of_nations.htm A Few Thoughts on IQ and the Wealth of Nations], Steve Sailer, ''VDARE'', April 2002.

{{Population country lists}}


{{DEFAULTSORT:Iq And The Wealth Of Nations}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Iq And The Wealth Of Nations}}
[[Category:2002 books]]
[[Category:2002 non-fiction books]]
[[Category:Economics books]]
[[Category:Books about human intelligence]]
[[Category:Books about wealth distribution]]
[[Category:Books by Richard Lynn]]
[[Category:Books by Tatu Vanhanen]]
[[Category:Intelligence quotient]]
[[Category:Race and intelligence controversy]]
[[Category:Race and intelligence controversy]]
[[Category:Psychological testing]]
[[Category:Intelligence]]

[[ar:نسبة الذكاء وثروات الشعوب]]
[[az:IQ və millətlərin sərvəti]]
[[es:IQ and the Wealth of Nations]]
[[fa:ضریب هوشی و ثروت ملت‌ها (کتاب)]]
[[fr:IQ and the Wealth of Nations]]
[[id:IQ and the Wealth of Nations]]
[[he:איי קיו ועושר האומות]]
[[pt:IQ and the Wealth of Nations]]
[[ru:Коэффициент интеллекта и богатство народов]]
[[fi:IQ and the Wealth of Nations]]
[[sv:IQ and the Wealth of Nations]]
[[zh:智商與國民財富]]

Latest revision as of 17:15, 19 December 2024

IQ and the Wealth of Nations
Cover
AuthorRichard Lynn
Tatu Vanhanen
LanguageEnglish
SubjectHuman intelligence, economic inequality, psychology, sociology
PublisherPraeger/Greenwood
Publication date
28 February 2002
Media typePrint (hardcover)
Pages320
ISBN978-0275975104

IQ and the Wealth of Nations is a 2002 book by psychologist Richard Lynn and political scientist Tatu Vanhanen.[1] The authors argue that differences in national income (in the form of per capita gross domestic product) are correlated with differences in the average national intelligence quotient (IQ). They further argue that differences in average national IQs constitute one important factor, but not the only one, contributing to differences in national wealth and rates of economic growth.

The book has drawn widespread criticism from other academics. Critiques have included questioning of the methodology used, the incompleteness of the data, and the conclusions drawn from the analysis.[2][3] The 2006 book IQ and Global Inequality is a follow-up to IQ and the Wealth of Nations by the same authors.

Outline

[edit]

The book includes the authors' calculation of average IQ scores for 60 countries, based on their analysis of published reports. It reports their observation that national IQ correlates with gross domestic product per capita at 0.82, and with the rate of economic growth from 1950 to 1990 at 0.64.

The authors believe that average IQ differences between nations are due to both genetic and environmental factors. They also believe that low GDP can cause low IQ, just as low IQ can cause low GDP. (See Positive feedback).

The authors write that it is the ethical responsibility of rich, high-IQ nations to assist poor, low-IQ nations financially, as it is the responsibility of rich citizens to assist the poor.

National IQ estimates

[edit]

The result claims that Hong Kong has the highest national IQ estimate with 107, followed by South Korea with 106.[1]

Central to the book's thesis is a tabulation of what Lynn and Vanhanen believe to be the average IQs of the world's nations. Rather than do their own IQ studies, the authors average and adjust existing studies and use other methods to create estimates.

For 104 of the 185 nations, no studies were available. In those cases, the authors have used an estimated value by taking averages of the IQs of neighboring or comparable nations. For example, the authors arrived at a figure of 84 for El Salvador by averaging their calculations of 79 for Guatemala and 88 for Colombia. Including those estimated IQs, the correlation of IQ and GDP is 0.62.[citation needed]

To obtain a figure for South Africa, the authors averaged IQ studies done on different ethnic groups, resulting in a figure of 72. The figures for Colombia, Peru, and Singapore were arrived at in a similar manner.

In some cases, the IQ of a country is estimated by averaging the IQs of countries that are not actually neighbors of the country in question. For example, Kyrgyzstan's IQ is estimated by averaging the IQs of Iran and Turkey, neither of which is close to Kyrgyzstan—China, which is a geographic neighbor, is not counted as such by Lynn and Vanhanen. This is because ethnic background is assumed to be more important than proximity to other nations when determining national IQ.[citation needed]

To account for the Flynn effect (an increase in IQ scores over time), the authors adjusted the results of older studies upward by a number of points.

Scores that do not support the theory

[edit]

In several cases the actual GDP did not correspond with that predicted by IQ. In these cases, the authors argued that differences in GDP were caused by differences in natural resources and whether the nation used a planned or market economy.

One example of this was Qatar, whose IQ was estimated by Lynn and Vanhanen to be about 78, yet had a disproportionately high per capita GDP of roughly US$17,000. The authors explain Qatar's disproportionately high GDP by its high petroleum resources. Similarly, the authors think that large resources of diamonds explain the economic growth of the African nation Botswana, the fastest in the world for several decades.

The authors argued that the People's Republic of China's per capita GDP of at the time roughly US$4,500 could be explained by its use of a communist economic system for much of its recent history. The authors also predicted that communist nations whom they believe have comparatively higher IQs, including China and North Korea, can be expected to rapidly gain GDP growth by moving from centrally planned economies to more capitalist based economic systems, while predicting continued poverty for sub-Saharan African nations no matter what economic systems are used.

Reception and impact

[edit]
Map depicting average IQ values as presented in the follow-up scientific study, 'Intelligence and the Wealth and Poverty of Nations'

Several negative reviews of the book have been published in the scholarly literature. Susan Barnett and Wendy Williams wrote that "we see an edifice built on layer upon layer of arbitrary assumptions and selective data manipulation. The data on which the entire book is based are of questionable validity and are used in ways that cannot be justified." They also wrote that cross country comparisons are "virtually meaningless."[4]

Richardson (2004) argued, citing the Flynn effect as the best evidence, that Lynn has the causal connection backwards and suggested that "the average IQ of a population is simply an index of the size of its middle class, both of which are results of industrial development". The review concludes that "This is not so much science, then, as a social crusade."[3] A review by Michael Palairet criticized the book's methodology, particularly the imprecise estimates of GDP and the fact that IQ data were only available for 81 of the 185 countries studied. However, the review concluded that the book was "a powerful challenge to economic historians and development economists who prefer not to use IQ as an analytical input", but that it's likely those scholars will deliberately ignore this work instead of improving it.[5]

By economists

[edit]

In a book review for the Journal of Economic Literature, Thomas Nechyba wrote that "such sweeping conclusions based on relatively weak statistical evidence and dubious presumptions seem misguided at best and quite dangerous if taken seriously. It is therefore difficult to find much to recommend in this book."[6]

Writing in the Economic Journal, Astrid Oline Ervik said that the book may be "thought provoking", but there is nothing that economists can learn from it. She criticized the book's authors for not establishing cross country comparability and reliability of IQ scores, for relying on simple bivariate correlations, for not considering or controlling for other hypotheses, and for confusing correlation with causation. Ervik stated, "The arguments put forward in the book to justify such comparisons [between the average IQ in different countries and their GDP] seem at best vague and unconvincing. At worst, passages in the book appear to be biased and unscientific...The authors fail to present convincing evidence and appear to jump to conclusions."[7]

Edward Miller, an economist who has published many controversial papers on race and intelligence, gave the book positive reviews in two different white nationalist publications, the Journal of Social, Political, and Economic Studies and The Occidental Quarterly.[8][9]

Criticism of data sets

[edit]

Academic reviews of the book generally criticized both its methodology and conclusions.

The methods of the study were criticized by Richard E. Nisbett for relying on small and haphazard samples and for ignoring data that did not support the conclusions.[10]

University of Reading geographer Stephen Morse also criticized the book (as well as IQ and the Wealth of Nations), arguing that the authors' hypothesis rests on "serious flaws". Morse also argued: "The central dilemma of the Lynn and Vanhanen case rests with their assumption that national IQ data are primarily (not wholly) a function of innate ability, which in turn is at least partly generated by genes. There are many assumptions of cause–effect in here, and some of them involve substantial leaps of faith."[11]

In the 2010 paper "A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans", also published in Intelligence, Jelte M. Wicherts and colleagues stated:

"For instance, Lynn and Vanhanen (2006) accorded a national IQ of 69 to Nigeria on the basis of three samples (Fahrmeier, 1975; Ferron, 1965; Wober, 1969), but they did not consider other relevant published studies that indicated that average IQ in Nigeria is considerably higher than 70 (Maqsud, 1980a, b; Nenty & Dinero, 1981; Okunrotifa, 1976). As Lynn rightly remarked during the 2006 conference of the International Society for Intelligence Research (ISIR), performing a literature review involves making a lot of choices. Nonetheless, an important drawback of Lynn (and Vanhanen)'s reviews of the literature is that they are unsystematic."[12]

Lynn and Gerhard Meisenberg replied that "critical evaluation of the studies presented by WDM shows that many of these are based on unrepresentative elite samples" and that a further literature review, including taking into account results in mathematics, science, and reading, gave "an IQ of 68 as the best reading of the IQ in sub-Saharan Africa".[13] Wicherts and colleagues in yet another reply stated: "In light of all the available IQ data of over 37,000 African testtakers, only the use of unsystematic methods to exclude the vast majority of data could result in a mean IQ close to 70. On the basis of sound methods, the average IQ remains close to 80. Although this mean IQ is clearly lower than 100, we view it as unsurprising in light of the potential of the Flynn effect in Africa (Wicherts, Borsboom, & Dolan, 2010) and common psychometric problems associated with the use of western IQ tests among Africans."[14]

Some criticisms have focused on the limited number of studies upon which the book is based. The IQ figures are based on 3 different studies for 17 nations, two studies for 30 nations, and one study for 34 nations. There were actual tests for IQ in the case of 81 countries out of the 185 countries studied. For 104 nations there were no IQ studies at all and IQ was estimated based on the average IQ of surrounding nations.[2] The limited number of participants in some studies as well as outdated data has also been criticized. A test of 108 9- to 15-year-olds in Barbados, of 50 13- to 16-year-olds in Colombia, of 104 5- to 17-year-olds in Ecuador, of 129 6- to 12-year-olds in Egypt, and of 48 10- to 14-year-olds in Equatorial Guinea, all were taken as measures of national IQ.[3]

Denny Borsboom argued that mainstream contemporary test analysis does not reflect substantial recent developments in the field and "bears an uncanny resemblance to the psychometric state of the art as it existed in the 1950s". For example, he argued that IQ and the Wealth of Nations, in order to show that the tests are unbiased, uses outdated methodology – if anything, indicative that test bias exists.[15] Girma Berhanu, in an essay review of the book, concentrated on the discussion of Ethiopian Jews. The review criticized the principal assertion of the authors that differences in intelligence, attributed to genetics, account for the gap between rich and poor countries. Berhanu criticized the book as being based in a "racist, sexist, and antihuman" research tradition and alleged that "the low standards of scholarship evident in the book render it largely irrelevant for modern science".[16]

Impact on psychology

[edit]

In 2006, Lynn and Vanhanen followed IQ and the Wealth of Nations with their book IQ and Global Inequality, which contained additional data and analyses, but the same general conclusions as the earlier book. Discussing both books, Earl Hunt writes that although Lynn and Vanhanen's methodology and conclusions are questionable, they deserve credit for raising important questions about international IQ comparisons. Hunt writes that Lynn and Vanhanen are correct that national IQs correlate strongly with measures of social well-being, but they are unjustified in their rejection of the idea that national IQs could change as a result of improved education.[17]

On July 27, 2020, the European Human Behavior and Evolution Association issued a formal statement opposing the utilization of Lynn's national IQ dataset, as well as all updated forms of it, citing various criticisms of its methodology and data collection. They concluded that "any conclusions drawn from analyses which use these data are therefore unsound, and no reliable evolutionary work should be using these data."[18]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-97510-X
  2. ^ a b The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth: A Critique of Richard Lynn and Tatu Vanhanens Recent Book by Thomas Volken
  3. ^ a b c Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations Heredity April 2004, Volume 92, Number 4, Pages 359–360. K Richardson.
  4. ^ Barnett, Susan M.; Williams, Wendy (August 2004). "National Intelligence and the Emperor's New Clothes". Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books. 49 (4): 389–396. doi:10.1037/004367.
  5. ^ Palairet, M. R. (2004), "Book review, IQ and the Wealth of Nations", Heredity, 92 (4): 361–362, doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800427
  6. ^ Nechyba, Thomas J. (March 2004), "Reviewed Work: IQ and the Wealth of Nations by Richard Lynn, Tatu Vanhanen", Journal of Economic Literature, 42 (1): 220–221
  7. ^ Ervik, Astrid Oline (June 2003), "Reviewed Work: IQ and the Wealth of Nations by Richard Lynn, Tatu Vanhanen", The Economic Journal, 113 (488, Features): F406 – F408, doi:10.1111/1468-0297.13916, JSTOR 3590222
  8. ^ Miller, E (2002). "BOOK REVIEW ARTICLE: Differential Intelligence and National Income". The Journal of Social, Political and Economic Studies. 27: 513–522.
  9. ^ Miller, Edward M. "IQ and the Wealth of Nations" (PDF). The Occidental Quarterly (book review). 2 (4): 79–83.
  10. ^ Nisbett, Richard. 2009. Intelligence and how to get it. pp. 215.
  11. ^ Morse, Stephen (September 2008). "The geography of tyranny and despair: development indicators and the hypothesis of genetic inevitability of national inequality" (PDF). Geographical Journal. 174 (3): 195–206. Bibcode:2008GeogJ.174..195M. doi:10.1111/j.1475-4959.2008.00296.x. ISSN 0016-7398.
  12. ^ Wicherts, J. M.; et al. (2009). "A systematic literature review of the average IQ of Sub-Saharan Africans". Intelligence. 38: 1–20. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002.
  13. ^ Lynn, Richard; Meisenberg, Gerhard (2010). "The average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans: Comments on Wicherts, Dolan, and van der Maas". Intelligence. 38 (1): 21–29. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.09.009.
  14. ^ Wicherts, Jelte M.; Dolan, Conor V.; van der Maas, Han L.J. (2010). "The dangers of unsystematic selection methods and the representativeness of 46 samples of African test-takers". Intelligence. 38 (1): 30–37. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.11.003.
  15. ^ Borsboom, Denny (2006). "The attack of the psychometricians" (PDF). Psychometrika. 71 (3): 425–440. doi:10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6. PMC 2779444. PMID 19946599. Archived from the original on 15 February 2010.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  16. ^ Bernahu, Girma (2007). "Black Intellectual Genocide: An Essay Review of IQ and the Wealth of Nations". Education Review: 1–28. ISSN 1094-5296.
  17. ^ Hunt, E. Human Intelligence. Cambridge University Press, 2011, page 426-445.
  18. ^ "EHBEA Statement on National IQ Datasets" (PDF). European Human Behaviour and Evolution Association. 27 July 2020. Archived from the original (PDF) on 10 August 2020.