Flynn effect: Difference between revisions
mNo edit summary |
MusikBot II (talk | contribs) m Adding missing protection template (more info) |
||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{pp|small=yes}} |
|||
The '''Flynn effect''' is the rise of average [[Intelligence Quotient]] (IQ) test scores, an effect seen in most parts of the world, although at greatly varying rates. It is named after [[James R. Flynn]], who did much to document it and promote awareness of its implications. This increase has been continuous and roughly linear from the earliest days of testing to the present. "Test scores are certainly going up all over the world, but |
|||
{{Short description|20th-century rise in intelligence test scores}} |
|||
whether intelligence itself has risen remains controversial," psychologist [[Ulric Neisser]] wrote in an article in [[1997]] in ''The American Scientist.''<ref name="Neisser97">[http://www.americanscientist.org/template/AssetDetail/assetid/24612?fulltext=true&print=yes Rising Scores on Intelligence Test] Neisser, U. (1997). American Scientist, 85, 440-447.</ref> The Flynn effect may have ended in some developed nations starting in the mid 1990s although other studies, such as ''Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples'' (Dickens, Flynn;2006), still show gain between 1972 and 2002. |
|||
{{Use mdy dates|date=January 2019}} |
|||
[[File:Ourworldindata wisc-iq-gains-over-time-flynn-2007.png|thumb|Composition of IQ Gains]] |
|||
The '''Flynn effect''' is the substantial and long-sustained increase in both [[fluid and crystallized intelligence]] test scores that were measured in many parts of the world over the 20th century, named after researcher [[James Flynn (academic)|James Flynn]] (1934–2020).<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Trahan |first1=Lisa H. |last2=Stuebing |first2=Karla K. |last3=Fletcher |first3=Jack M. |last4=Hiscock |first4=Merrill |date=2014 |title=The Flynn effect: A meta-analysis. |journal=Psychological Bulletin |language=en |volume=140 |issue=5 |pages=1332–1360 |doi=10.1037/a0037173 |pmid=24979188 |pmc=4152423 |issn=1939-1455}}</ref><ref name=baker>{{Cite journal |last1=Baker |first1=David P. |last2=Eslinger |first2=Paul J. |last3=Benavides |first3=Martin |last4=Peters |first4=Ellen |last5=Dieckmann |first5=Nathan F. |last6=Leon |first6=Juan |date=March 2015 |title=The cognitive impact of the education revolution: A possible cause of the Flynn Effect on population IQ |journal=Intelligence |volume=49 |pages=144–58 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2015.01.003 |issn=0160-2896}}</ref> When [[intelligence quotient]] (IQ) tests are initially [[Standard score#Standardizing in mathematical statistics|standardized]] using a [[Sample (statistics)|sample]] of test-takers, by convention the average of the test results is set to 100 and their [[standard deviation]] is set to 15 or 16 IQ points. When IQ tests are revised, they are again standardized using a new sample of test-takers, usually born more recently than the first; the average result is set to 100. When the new test subjects take the older tests, in almost every case their average scores are significantly above 100. |
|||
Test score increases have been continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to the present. For example, a study published in the year 2009 found that British children's average scores on the [[Raven's Progressive Matrices]] test rose by 14 IQ points from 1942 to 2008.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R. |date=March 2009 |title=Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938–2008 |journal=Economics and Human Biology |volume=7 |issue=1 |pages=18–27 |doi=10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009 |issn=1873-6130 |pmid=19251490}}</ref> Similar gains have been observed in many other countries in which IQ testing has long been widely used, including other Western European countries, as well as Japan and South Korea.<ref name=baker/> Improvements have also been reported for [[semantic memory|semantic]] and [[episodic memory]].<ref name="Rönnlund">{{cite journal |vauthors=Rönnlund M, Nilsson LG |title=Flynn effects on sub-factors of episodic and semantic memory: parallel gains over time and the same set of determining factors |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=47 |issue=11 |pages=2174–80 |date=September 2009 |pmid=19056409 |doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.007 |s2cid=15706086 }}</ref> |
|||
== The rise == |
|||
IQ tests are re-[[Normal distribution#Distribution in testing and intelligence|normal]]ized periodically, such that the average score is reset to 100. The revised versions are [[standardized]] on new samples and scored with respect to those samples alone, so the only way to compare the difficulty of two versions of a test is to conduct a separate study in which the same subjects take both versions.<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
|||
There are numerous proposed explanations of the Flynn effect, such as the rise in efficiency of education, along with skepticism concerning its implications. Some researchers have suggested the possibility of a mild reversal in the Flynn effect (i.e., a decline in IQ scores) in developed countries, beginning in the 1990s.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Al-Shahomee | display-authors = etal | year = 2018 | title = An increase of intelligence in Libya from 2008 to 2017 | journal = Personality and Individual Differences | volume = 130| pages = 147–149| doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.004 | s2cid = 149095461 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.029|title = A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse|journal = Personality and Individual Differences|volume = 39|issue = 4|pages = 837–43|year = 2005|last1 = Teasdale|first1 = Thomas W|last2 = Owen|first2 = David R}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2015.10.004|title = A reversal of the Flynn effect for spatial perception in German-speaking countries: Evidence from a cross-temporal IRT-based meta-analysis (1977–2014)|journal = Intelligence|volume = 53|pages = 145–53|year = 2015|last1 = Pietschnig|first1 = Jakob|last2 = Gittler|first2 = Georg}}</ref><ref name="pnas2018">{{Cite journal|last1=Bratsberg|first1=Bernt|last2=Rogeberg|first2=Ole|date=June 6, 2018|title=Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|language=en|volume=115|issue=26|pages=6674–78|doi=10.1073/pnas.1718793115|issn=0027-8424|pmc=6042097|pmid=29891660|bibcode=2018PNAS..115.6674B |doi-access=free}}</ref> In certain cases, this apparent reversal may be due to cultural changes rendering parts of intelligence tests obsolete.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gonthier |first1=Corentin |last2=Grégoire |first2=Jacques |last3=Besançon |first3=Maud |title=No negative Flynn effect in France: Why variations of intelligence should not be assessed using tests based on cultural knowledge |journal=Intelligence |date=January 2021 |volume=84 |pages=101512 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2020.101512|s2cid=230538271 |doi-access=free }}</ref> Meta-analyses indicate that, overall, the Flynn effect continues, either at the same rate,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Trahan |first1=Lisa H. |last2=Stuebing |first2=Karla K. |last3=Fletcher |first3=Jack M. |last4=Hiscock |first4=Merrill |title=The Flynn effect: A meta-analysis. |journal=Psychological Bulletin |date=2014 |volume=140 |issue=5 |pages=1332–1360 |doi=10.1037/a0037173|pmid=24979188 |pmc=4152423 }}</ref> or at a slower rate in developed countries.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Pietschnig |first1=Jakob |last2=Voracek |first2=Martin |title=One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-Analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013) |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |date=May 2015 |volume=10 |issue=3 |pages=282–306 |doi=10.1177/1745691615577701|pmid=25987509 |s2cid=12604392 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Wongupparaj |first1=Peera |last2=Kumari |first2=Veena|author2-link=Veena Kumari |last3=Morris |first3=Robin G. |title=A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Raven's Progressive Matrices: Age groups and developing versus developed countries |journal=Intelligence |date=March 2015 |volume=49 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.008}}</ref> |
|||
The average rate of rise seems to be around three IQ points per decade. Because children attend school longer now and have become much more familiar with the testing of school-related material, one might expect the greatest gains to occur on such school content-related tests as [[vocabulary]], [[arithmetic]] or general information. Just the opposite is the case: abilities such as these have experienced relatively small gains and even occasional declines over the years. The largest Flynn effects appear instead on culture reduced highly [[general intelligence factor|general intelligence factor loaded (g-loaded)]] tests such as [[Raven's Progressive Matrices]]. For example, Dutch conscripts gained 21 points in only 30 years, or 7 points per decade, between 1952 and 1982.<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
|||
==Origin of term== |
|||
Some studies focusing on the distribution of scores have found the Flynn effect to be primarily a phenomenon in the lower end of the distribution. Teasdale and Owen (1987), for example, found the effect primarily reduced the number of low-end scores, resulting in a pile up of moderately high scores, with no increase in very high scores.<ref>Teasdale, Thomas W., and David R. Owen. (1987). ‘National secular trends in intelligence and education: a twenty year cross-sectional study’, Nature, 325, 119-21.</ref> However, Raven (2000) found that, as Flynn suggested, data reported by many previous researchers that had previously been interpreted as showing a decrease in many abilities with increasing age must be re-interpreted as showing that there has been a dramatic increase in these abilities with date of birth. On many tests this occurs at all levels of ability.<ref>Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s Progressive Matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 1-48.</ref> Two large samples of Spanish children were assessed with a 30-year gap. Comparison of the IQ distributions indicated that |
|||
{{external media | width = 210px | float = right | headerimage=[[File:Jim Flynn U of Otago.jpg|210px]] | video1 = [https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9vpqilhW9uI James Flynn: Why our IQ levels are higher than our grandparents'], (18:41), [[TED (conference)|TED talks]] |
|||
#the mean IQ had increased by 9.7 points (the Flynn effect), |
|||
}} |
|||
#the gains were concentrated in the lower half of the distribution and negligible in the top half, and |
|||
The Flynn effect is named for [[James Flynn (academic)|James Robert Flynn]], who did much to document it and promote awareness of its implications. The term was coined by [[Richard Herrnstein]] and [[Charles Murray (political scientist)|Charles Murray]] in their 1994 book ''[[The Bell Curve]]''.<ref name=Flynn>{{Cite book |author=Flynn, James R. |title=What Is Intelligence: Beyond the Flynn Effect |edition=expanded paperback |location=Cambridge |publisher=[[Cambridge University Press]] |isbn=978-0-521-74147-7 |year=2009 |pages=1–2 |quote=The 'Flynn effect' is the name that has become attached to an exciting development, namely, that the twentieth century saw massive IQ gains from one generation to another. To forestall a diagnosis of megalomania, the label was coined by Herrnstein and Murray, the authors of ''The Bell Curve'', and not by myself.}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |first=Cosma |last=Shalizi |title=The Domestication of the Savage Mind |type=Review |url=http://www.cscs.umich.edu/~crshalizi/reviews/flynn-beyond/ |website=University of Michigan |date=27 April 2009 |archive-date=July 19, 2012 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120719062416/http://www.cscs.umich.edu/~crshalizi/reviews/flynn-beyond/ |access-date=August 13, 2010}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book|last1=Herrnstein|first1=Richard J.|url=https://archive.org/details/bellcurveintell00herr/page/307/mode/1up|title=The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life|last2=Murray|first2=Charles|publisher=The Free Press|year=1994|isbn=0-02-914673-9|location=New York|pages=307}}</ref> Flynn stated that, if asked, he would have named the effect after Read D. Tuddenham<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Haig |first=Brian D. |date=2013-07-01 |title=Detecting Psychological Phenomena: Taking Bottom-Up Research Seriously |url=https://scholarlypublishingcollective.org/ajp/article/126/2/135/258002/Detecting-Psychological-Phenomena-Taking-Bottom-Up |journal=The American Journal of Psychology |language=en |volume=126 |issue=2 |pages=135–153 |doi=10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.2.0135 |pmid=23858950 |issn=0002-9556}}</ref> who "was the first to present convincing evidence of massive gains on mental tests using a nationwide sample"<ref>{{Citation |last=Flynn |first=James R. |title=Secular Changes in Intelligence |url=https://james-flynn.net/wp-content/uploads/2020/12/The-Cambridge-Handbook-of-Intelligence.pdf |work=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence |year=2011 |pages=647–665 |access-date=2023-03-27 |doi=10.1017/cbo9780511977244.033 |isbn=978-0-511-97724-4}}</ref> in a 1948 article.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Tuddenham |first=R. D. |date=1948 |title=Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18911933/ |journal=The American Psychologist |volume=3 |issue=2 |pages=54–56 |doi=10.1037/h0054962 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=18911933}}</ref> |
|||
#the gains gradually decreased from low to high IQ.<ref name="Colom2005">{{cite journal|author=Colom, R., Lluis-Font, J.M., and Andrés-Pueyo, A. |date=2005|title=The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: Supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis|journal=Intelligence|volume=33|pages=83-91}}</ref> |
|||
Although the general term for the phenomenon—referring to no researcher in particular—continues to be "[[Saeculum|secular]] rise in IQ scores", many textbooks on psychology and IQ testing have now followed the lead of Herrnstein and Murray in calling the phenomenon the Flynn effect.<ref name="FlynnEffectTerm">{{cite book |last1=Fletcher |first1=Richard B. |last2=Hattie |first2=John |title=Intelligence and Intelligence Testing |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=8pzDawey6akC |access-date=August 31, 2013 |year=2011 |publisher=Taylor & Francis |isbn=978-1-136-82321-3 |page=26 |quote=Indeed, this effect, now called the 'Flynn effect', is well established. Nations, almost without exception, have shown gains of about 20 IQ points per generation (30 years). These gains are highest for IQ tests that are most related to reasoning and the capacity to figure out novel problems (this is often called 'fluid intelligence', see Chapter 5); and least related to knowledge, which arises from better educational opportunity, a history of persistence and good motivation for learning (this is often called 'crystallized intelligence', see Chapter 5).}} |
|||
Taken at face value, these changes are considered large by some. [[Ulric Neisser]], who in 1995 headed an [[American Psychological Association]] task force writing a consensus statement on the state of intelligence research, estimates that if American children of 1932 could take an IQ test normed in 1997 their average IQ would have been only about 80.<ref name="Neisser97"/> In other words, half of the children in 1932 would be classified as having borderline [[mental retardation]] or worse in 1997. Looking at Ravens, Neisser estimates that if you [[extrapolation|extrapolate]] beyond the data, which shows a 21 point gain between 1952 and 1982, an even larger gain of 35 IQ points can be argued, however [[Arthur Jensen]] warns that extrapolating beyond the data leads to results such as an IQ of -1000 for [[Aristotle]] (even assuming he would have scored 200 in his day)<ref>The g factor by Aurthur Jensen pg 328</ref> |
|||
*{{cite book |title=Gifted Lives: What Happens when Gifted Children Grow Up |last=Freeman |first=Joan |date=2010 |publisher=Routledge |location=London |isbn=978-0-415-47009-4 |pages=290–91 |quote=A strange new phenomenon has been growing since about 1950, called the 'Flynn Effect' after Professor James Flynn of the University of Otago, New Zealand. In his book ''What is Intelligence ?'', Flynn describes a year-on-year rise in measured intelligence, about three IQ points a decade.}} |
|||
*{{cite news |first=Annalisa |last=Barbieri |url=https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2010/oct/09/gifted-children-joan-freeman-psychologist |title=Young, gifted and likely to suffer for it |newspaper=The Guardian |date=8 October 2010 |access-date=December 14, 2016 |archive-date=November 11, 2020 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201111215132/https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2010/oct/09/gifted-children-joan-freeman-psychologist |url-status=live }} |
|||
*{{cite book |last=Urbina |first=Susana |title=Essentials of Psychological Testing |date=2004 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons |isbn=978-0-471-41978-5 |page=103 |quote=A puzzling longitudinal trend in the opposite direction, known as the 'Flynn effect', has been well documented in successive revisions of major intelligence tests (like the S-B and the Wechsler scales) that invariably involve the administration of both the old and new versions to a segment of the newer standardization sample, for comparative purposes. Data from revisions of various intelligence tests in the United States as well as in other countries—extensively analyzed by J.R. Flynn (1984, 1987)—show a pronounced, long-term upward trend in the level of performance required to obtain any given IQ score. The Flynn effect presumably reflects population gains over time in the kinds of cognitive performance that intelligence tests sample.}} {{cite book |last=Wasserman |first=John D. |editor1-last=Weiner |editor1-first=Irving B. |editor2-last=Graham |editor2-first=John R. |editor3-last=Naglieri |editor3-first=Jack A. |title=Handbook of Psychology |volume=10: Assessment Psychology |chapter=Chapter 18: Assessment of Intellectual Functioning |year=2012 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons |isbn=978-0-470-89127-8 |page=486 |quote=Both definitions also specify that the intellectual functioning criterion for a diagnosis of intellectual disability is approximately 2 ''SD''s or more below the normative mean, but factors such as test score statistical error (standard error of measurement), test fairness, normative expectations for the population of interest, the Flynn effect, and practice effects from previous testing need to be considered before arriving at any diagnosis.}} |
|||
*{{cite book |last=Chamorro-Premuzic |first=Tomas |author-link=Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic |title=Personality and Individual Differences |date=2011 |publisher=Wiley |isbn=978-1-4051-9927-8 |page=221 |quote='''Flynn effect''' The finding by sociologist James Flynn that there are generational increases in IQ across nations.}}</ref> |
|||
==Rise in IQ== |
|||
IQ tests are updated periodically. For example, the [[Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children]] (WISC), originally developed in 1949, was updated in 1974, 1991, 2003, and again in 2014. The revised versions are [[standardized]] based on the performance of test-takers in standardization samples. A standard score of IQ 100 is defined as the mean performance of the standardization sample. Thus one way to see changes in norms over time is to conduct a study in which the same test-takers take both an old and new version of the same test. Doing so confirms IQ gains over time. Some IQ tests - for example, tests used for military draftees in [[NATO]] countries in Europe - report raw scores, and those also confirm a trend of rising scores over time. The average rate of increase seems to be about three IQ points per decade in the United States, as scaled by the Wechsler tests. The increasing test performance over time appears on every major test, in every age range, at every ability level, and in every modern industrialized country, although not necessarily at the same rate as in the United States. The increase was continuous and roughly linear from the earliest days of testing to the mid-1990s.<ref name="Neisser97">{{cite journal |author=Neisser U |title=Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests |journal=American Scientist |volume=85 |issue=5 |pages=440–47 |year=1997 |url=http://www.americanscientist.org/issues/feature/rising-scores-on-intelligence-tests/99999 |url-status=bot: unknown |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161104214157/http://www.americanscientist.org/issues/feature/rising-scores-on-intelligence-tests/99999 |archive-date=November 4, 2016 |bibcode=1997AmSci..85..440N }}</ref>{{better source needed|date=November 2017}} Though the effect is most associated with IQ increases, a similar effect has been found with increases in attention and of [[Semantic memory|semantic]] and [[episodic memory]].<ref name="Rönnlund"/> |
|||
[[Ulric Neisser]] estimated that using the IQ values of 1997, the average IQ of the United States in 1932, according to the first [[Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales]] standardization sample, was 80. Neisser states that "Hardly any of them would have scored 'very superior', but nearly one-quarter would have appeared to be 'deficient.'" He also wrote that "Test scores are certainly going up all over the world, but whether intelligence itself has risen remains controversial."<ref name="Neisser97"/> Quantitative psychologist, [[Joseph Lee Rodgers]] argues that the effect occurs outside of families in any case.<ref>[[Joseph Lee Rodgers]] (2014). "http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/rodgers2014.pdf Intelligence Are birth order effects on intelligence really Flynn Effects? Reinterpreting Belmont and Marolla 40 years later" (PDF). ''Intelligence'' 42: 128-133. "No within-family data exist that document an increase in intelligence over birth order, suggesting that its source derives from outside the family and will only manifest in data and analyses that account for between-family variance (such as cross-sectional data)." (p. 130)</ref> |
|||
Trahan et al. (2014) found that the effect was about 2.93 points per decade,{{clarify|reason=Over what time interval?|date=March 2023}} based on both Stanford–Binet and Wechsler tests; they also found no evidence the effect was diminishing.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Trahan|first1=LH|last2=Stuebing|first2=KK|last3=Fletcher|first3=JM|last4=Hiscock|first4=M|title=The Flynn effect: a meta-analysis.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|date=September 2014|volume=140|issue=5|pages=1332–60|doi=10.1037/a0037173|pmid=24979188|pmc=4152423}}</ref> In contrast, Pietschnig and Voracek (2015) reported, in their meta-analysis of studies involving nearly 4 million participants, that the Flynn effect had decreased in recent decades. They also reported that the magnitude of the effect was different for different types of intelligence ("0.41, 0.30, 0.28, and 0.21 IQ points annually for fluid, spatial, full-scale, and crystallized IQ test performance, respectively"), and that the effect was stronger for adults than for children.<ref name="Pietschnig">{{Cite journal |last1=Jakob Pietschnig |last2=Martin Voracek |s2cid=12604392 |date=May 1, 2015 |title=One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-Analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013) |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |language=en |volume=10 |issue=3 |pages=282–306 |doi=10.1177/1745691615577701 |pmid=25987509 |issn=1745-6916}}</ref> |
|||
Raven (2000) found that, as Flynn suggested, data interpreted as showing a decrease in many abilities with increasing age must be re-interpreted as showing that there has been a dramatic increase of these abilities with the date of birth. On many tests this occurs at all levels of ability.<ref name=R2000>{{cite journal | author = Raven John | year = 2000 | title = The Raven's Progressive Matrices: Change and Stability over Culture and Time | url = http://eyeonsociety.co.uk/resources/RPMChangeAndStability.pdf | journal = Cognitive Psychology | volume = 41 | issue = 1 | pages = 1–48 | doi = 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735 | pmid = 10945921 | s2cid = 26363133 | access-date = July 9, 2011 | archive-date = April 28, 2019 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20190428221444/http://eyeonsociety.co.uk/resources/RPMChangeAndStability.pdf | url-status = live }}</ref> |
|||
Some studies have found the gains of the Flynn effect to be particularly concentrated at the lower end of the distribution. Teasdale and Owen (1989), for example, found the effect primarily reduced the number of low-end scores, resulting in an increased number of moderately high scores, with no increase in very high scores.<ref name=TO1987>{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1016/0160-2896(89)90021-4 | last1 = Teasdale | first1 = T. | title = Continuing secular increases in intelligence and a stable prevalence of high intelligence levels | journal = Intelligence | volume = 13 | issue = 3 | pages = 255–62 | year = 1989 }}</ref> In another study, two large samples of Spanish children were assessed with a 30-year gap. Comparison of the IQ distributions indicated that the mean IQ scores on the test had increased by 9.7 points (the Flynn effect), the gains were concentrated in the lower half of the distribution and negligible in the top half, and the gains gradually decreased as the IQ of the individuals increased.<ref name="Colom2005">{{cite journal | url=http://synapse.princeton.edu/~brained/chapter15/colom_andres-pueyo05_intelligence_Spanish-schoolchildren-nutrition-hypothesis.pdf | title=The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: Supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis | author1=Colom, R. | author2=Lluis-Font, J.M. | author3=Andrés-Pueyo, A. | name-list-style=amp | journal=Intelligence | year=2005 | volume=33 | issue=1 | pages=83–91 | doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010 | access-date=October 4, 2012 | archive-date=August 13, 2012 | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120813042004/http://synapse.princeton.edu/~brained/chapter15/colom_andres-pueyo05_intelligence_Spanish-schoolchildren-nutrition-hypothesis.pdf | url-status=live }}</ref> Some studies have found a reverse Flynn effect with declining scores for those with high IQ.<ref name="Pietschnig" /> |
|||
In 1987, Flynn took the position that the very large increase indicates that IQ tests do not measure intelligence but only a minor sort of "abstract problem-solving ability" with little practical significance. He argued that if IQ gains did reflect intelligence increases, there would have been consequent changes of our society that have not been observed (a presumed non-occurrence of a "cultural renaissance").<ref name="Neisser97"/> By 2012 Flynn no longer endorsed this view of intelligence, having elaborated and refined his view of what rising IQ scores meant.<ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.theguardian.com/technology/2012/sep/23/james-flynn-iq-scores-environment|title=James Flynn: IQ may go up as well as down|last=Tucker|first=Ian|date=2012-09-22|work=The Guardian|access-date=2019-01-21|language=en-GB|issn=0261-3077|archive-date=January 21, 2019|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190121232649/https://www.theguardian.com/technology/2012/sep/23/james-flynn-iq-scores-environment|url-status=live}}</ref> |
|||
===Precursors to Flynn's publications=== |
|||
Earlier investigators had discovered rises in raw IQ test scores in some study populations, but had not published general investigations of that issue in particular. Historian Daniel C. Calhoun cited earlier psychology literature on IQ score trends in his book ''The Intelligence of a People'' (1973).<ref>{{cite book |last=Calhoun |first=Daniel |title=The Intelligence of a People |date=1973 |publisher=Princeton University Press |isbn=978-0-691-04619-8 |url=https://archive.org/details/intelligenceofpe0000calh |url-access=registration}}</ref> [[Robert L. Thorndike]] – not to be confused with his famous father ''[[Edward Thorndike|Edward]]'' – drew attention to rises in Stanford-Binet scores in a 1975 review of the history of intelligence testing.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Thorndike |first=Robert L. |author-link=Robert L. Thorndike|title=Mr. Binet's Test 70 Years Later |journal=Educational Researcher |volume=4 |issue=5 |year=1975 |pages=3–7 |issn=0013-189X |doi=10.3102/0013189X004005003 |jstor=1174855 |s2cid=145355731 }}</ref> In 1982, [[Richard Lynn]] recorded an increase in average IQ among the population of Japan.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Lynn |first=Richard |author-link=Richard Lynn|date=May 1982 |title=IQ in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity |url=https://www.nature.com/articles/297222a0 |journal=Nature |language=en |volume=297 |issue=5863 |pages=222–223 |doi=10.1038/297222a0 |bibcode=1982Natur.297..222L |s2cid=4331657 |issn=1476-4687}}</ref> |
|||
===Intelligence=== |
|||
{{See also|g factor (psychometrics)|Intelligence (trait)}} |
|||
There is debate about whether the rise in IQ scores also corresponds to a rise in general intelligence, or only a rise in special skills related to taking IQ tests. Because children attend school longer now and have become much more familiar with the testing of school-related material, one might expect the greatest gains to occur on such school content-related tests as [[vocabulary]], [[arithmetic]] or general information. Just the opposite is the case: abilities such as these have experienced relatively small gains and even occasional decreases over the years. Meta-analytic findings indicate that Flynn effects occur for tests assessing both fluid and crystallized abilities. For example, Dutch conscripts gained 21 points during only 30 years, or 7 points per decade, between 1952 and 1982.<ref name="Neisser97"/> This rise in IQ test scores is not wholly explained by an increase in general intelligence. Studies have shown that while test scores have improved over time, the improvement is not fully correlated with latent factors related to intelligence.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Must O, Must A, Raudik V | year = 2003 | title = The secular rise in IQs: In Estonia, the Flynn effect is not a Jensen effect | journal = [[Intelligence (journal)|Intelligence]] | volume = 31 | issue = 5 | pages = 461–71 | doi = 10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00013-8 | url = http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/Must2003.pdf | access-date = September 13, 2011 | archive-date = October 11, 2017 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20171011193700/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/Must2003.pdf | url-status = live }}</ref> Other researchers argue that the IQ gains described by the Flynn effect are due in part to increasing intelligence, and in part to increases in test-specific skills.<ref>{{cite journal |author1=Wicherts, J.M. |author2=Dolan, C.V. |author3=Hessen, D.J. |author4=Oosterveld, P. |author5=Baal, G.C.M. van |author6=Boomsma, D.I. |author7=Span, M.M. |name-list-style=amp |year=2004 |title=Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect |journal=[[Intelligence (journal)|Intelligence]] |volume=32 |issue=5 |pages=509537 |url=http://users.fmg.uva.nl/jwicherts/wicherts2004.pdf |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002 |quote=The overall conclusion of the present paper is that factorial invariance with respect to cohorts is not tenable . . . . The fact that the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure, should not sit well with explanations that appeal solely to changes at the level of the latent variables. |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20050529034027/http://users.fmg.uva.nl/jwicherts/wicherts2004.pdf |archive-date=May 29, 2005 |citeseerx=10.1.1.207.4350 }}</ref><ref name=Nijenhuis04>{{cite journal | vauthors= Te Nijenhuis J, De Jong MJ, Evers A, Van Der Flier H | title= Are cognitive differences between immigrant and majority groups diminishing? | journal= [[European Journal of Personality]] | year= 2004 | volume= 18 | issue= 5 | pages= 405–34 | doi= 10.1002/per.511 | s2cid= 4806581 | url= https://research.vu.nl/ws/files/1957002/Nijenhuis%20European%20Journal%20of%20Personality%2018%202004%20u.pdf | access-date= November 10, 2019 | archive-date= February 21, 2021 | archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20210221012426/https://research.vu.nl/ws/files/1957002/Nijenhuis%20European%20Journal%20of%20Personality%2018%202004%20u.pdf | url-status= live }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |title=Secular Gains in Fluid Intelligence: Evidence from the Culture-Fair Intelligence Test |vauthors=Colom R, Garcia-Lopez O| journal= [[Journal of Biosocial Science]] | year= 2003 | volume= 35 | pages= 33–39 | doi= 10.1017/S0021932003000336 | pmid= 12537154 | issue= 1|s2cid=24493926}}</ref> One study suggested that the IQ gains reflected changes in modes of thinking that better reflected cognitive skills assessed by IQ tests rather than raw intelligence itself.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=James Robert |last2=Te Nijenhuis |author2-link= Jan te Nijenhuis |first2=Jan |last3=Metzen |first3=Daniel |date=May–June 2014 |title=The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0160289614000105 |journal=[[Intelligence (journal)|Intelligence]] |volume=44 |pages=1–10 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.009 |access-date=2 July 2023}}</ref> |
|||
==Proposed explanations== |
==Proposed explanations== |
||
{{See also| |
{{See also|Impact of health on intelligence}} |
||
===Schooling and test familiarity=== |
|||
Attempted explanations have included improved nutrition, a trend towards smaller families, better education, greater environmental complexity, and [[heterosis]]<ref>{{cite journal | author=Mingroni, M.A. | title=The secular rise in IQ: Giving heterosis a closer look | journal=Intelligence | year=2004 | volume=32 | pages=65–83}}</ref>. Another proposition is greater familiarity with [[multiple-choice questions]] and experience with brain-teaser IQ problems. <ref> (Ulric Neisser et al 1998) [http://adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/1993JRScT..30..709H]</ref> |
|||
The duration of average schooling has increased steadily. However, a criticism of this explanation is that if (in the United States) older and younger subjects, with similar educational levels, are compared together, then the IQ gains appear almost undiminished in each group compared to when they are considered individually.<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
|||
Many studies find that children who do not attend school |
Many studies find that children who do not attend school score drastically lower on the tests than their regularly attending peers. During the 1960s, when some [[Virginia]] counties [[Massive resistance|closed their public schools to avoid racial integration]], compensatory private schooling was available only for White children. On average, the scores of African-American children who received no formal education during that period decreased at a rate of about six IQ points per year.<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
||
Another explanation is an increased familiarity of the general |
Another explanation is an increased familiarity of the general population with tests and testing. For example, children who take the very same IQ test a second time usually gain five or six points. However, this seems to set an upper limit on the effects of test sophistication. One problem with this explanation and others related to schooling is that in the US, the groups with greater test familiarity show smaller IQ increases.<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
||
[[Early intervention]] programs have shown mixed results. Some preschool (ages 3–4) intervention programs like "[[Head Start Program|Head Start]]" do not produce lasting changes of IQ, although they may confer other benefits.{{which|date=July 2014}} The "[[Abecedarian Early Intervention Project]]", an all-day program that provided various forms of [[Environmental enrichment (neural)|environmental enrichment]] to children from infancy onward, showed IQ gains that did not diminish over time. The IQ gains in the experimental group compared to the control group was 4.4 points. These gains persisted until at least age 21.<ref>{{cite book |author1=Plomin R. |author1-link=Robert Plomin|author2=DeFries J.C. |author3=Craig I.W. |author4=McGuffin P. |title=Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era |year=2003 |edition=4th }}</ref> |
|||
Citing a high correlation between rising literacy rates and gains in IQ, [[David Marks (psychologist)|David Marks]] has argued that the Flynn effect is caused by changes in literacy rates.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Marks |first=David Francis |date=2010-06-01 |title=IQ Variations across Time, Race, and Nationality: An Artifact of Differences in Literacy Skills |url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664 |journal=Psychological Reports |language=en |volume=106 |issue=3 |pages=643–664 |doi=10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664 |pmid=20712152 |issn=0033-2941 |via=[[Sage Publishing]]}}</ref> |
|||
Still another is that the general environment is today much more complex and stimulating. One of the most striking 20th-century change in the human intellectual environment has come from the increase in exposure to many types of visual media. From pictures on the wall to movies to television to video games to computers, each successive generation has been exposed to far richer optical displays than the one before and may have become more adept at visual analysis. This would explain why tests like the Raven's have shown the greatest increases, since they depend on such analysis. This explanation may imply that IQ tests do not necessarily measure a general intelligence factor, especially not Raven's as often argued, but instead may measure different forms of intelligence that are developed by different experiences. An increase only in particular form(s) of intelligence would explain why the Flynn effect has not caused a "cultural renaissance too great to be overlooked."<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
|||
===Nutrition=== |
|||
Improved nutrition is another explanation. Today's average adult from an industrialized nation stands much taller than the comparable adult of a century ago. That increase in stature, almost certainly the result general improvements in nutrition and health, has come at a rate of more than a centimeter per decade. Available data suggest that these gains have been accompanied by analogous increases in head size, and presumably by an increase in the average size of the brain. One problem with this, and other explanations arguing that the IQ gains reflect real gains in general intelligence, is that if all of the gains are real, then this would imply changes in average mental ability too great to seem plausible.<ref name="Neisser97"/> A 2005 study presented data supporting the nutrition hypothesis, which predicts that gains in IQ will predominantly occur at the low end of the distribution where nutritional deprivation is most severe.<ref name="Colom2005"/> |
|||
{{See also|Iodine deficiency#Deficient populations}} |
|||
Improved nutrition is another possible explanation. Today's average adult from an industrialized nation is taller than a comparable adult of a century ago. That increase of stature, likely the result of general improvements in nutrition and health, has been at a rate of more than a centimeter per decade. Available data suggest that these gains have been accompanied by analogous increases in head size, and by an increase in the average size of the brain.<ref name="Neisser97"/><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Jantz |first1=R. |last2=Meadows Jantz |first2=L. |year=2000 |title=Secular change in craniofacial morphology |journal=American Journal of Human Biology |volume=12 |issue= 3|pages=327–38 |doi=10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(200005/06)12:3<327::AID-AJHB3>3.0.CO;2-1 |pmid=11534023|s2cid=22059721 |doi-access=free }}</ref> This argument had been thought to suffer the difficulty that groups who tend to be of smaller overall body size (e.g. women, or people of Asian ancestry) do not have lower average IQs.<ref name=TO1987/> |
|||
A 2005 study presented data supporting the nutrition hypothesis, which predicts that gains will occur predominantly at the low end of the IQ distribution, where nutritional deprivation is probably most severe.<ref name="Colom2005"/> An alternative interpretation of [[skewed]] IQ gains could be that improved education has been particularly important for this group.<ref name=TO1987/> |
|||
Possibly related to the Flynn effect is change in [[cranial vault]] size and shape during the last 150 years in the US. These changes must occur by early childhood because of the early development of the vault.<ref>"<cite>Changes in vault dimensions must occur by early childhood because of the early development of the vault.</cite>" [http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/cgi-bin/abstract/71007970/ABSTRACT Secular change in craniofacial morphology] |
|||
"<cite>During the 125 years under consideration, cranial vaults have become markedly higher, somewhat narrower, with narrower faces. The changes in cranial morphology are probably in large part due to changes in growth at the cranial base due to improved environmental conditions. The changes are likely a combination of phenotypic plasticity and genetic changes over this period.</cite>" [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=11451056&dopt=Abstract Cranial change in Americans: 1850-1975.]</ref> |
|||
A century ago, [[micronutrient|nutritional]] deficiencies may have limited body and organ functionality, including skull volume. The first two years of life are a critical time for nutrition. The consequences of malnutrition can be irreversible and may include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity.<ref>{{cite web |title=The Lancet Series on Maternal and Child Undernutrition |year=2008 |url=http://www.thelancet.com/series/maternal-and-child-undernutrition |access-date=February 11, 2011 |archive-date=July 17, 2011 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110717005704/http://www.thelancet.com/series/maternal-and-child-undernutrition |url-status=live }}</ref> On the other hand, Flynn has pointed to 20-point gains on Dutch military ([[Raven's Progressive Matrices|Raven's]] type) IQ tests between 1952, 1962, 1972, and 1982. In 1962 he observed that Dutch 18-year-olds had a major nutritional handicap. They were either in the womb or were recently born, during the great [[Dutch famine of 1944]]—when German troops monopolized food and 18,000 people died of starvation.<ref>C. Banning (1946). "Food Shortage and Public Health, First Half of 1945". ''Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science'' Vol. 245, The Netherlands during German Occupation (May 1946), pp. 93–110</ref> Yet, concludes Flynn, "they do not show up even as a blip in the pattern of Dutch IQ gains. It is as if the famine had never occurred."<ref name="PB101-171">{{cite journal | author = Flynn J.R. | year = 1987 | title = Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 101 | issue = 2| pages = 171–91 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171}}</ref><ref>Flynn, James R. (2009). ''What Is Intelligence?'' (p. 103). Cambridge University Press. Kindle Edition.</ref> It appears that the effects of diet are gradual, taking effect over decades (affecting mother as well as the child) rather than a few months.{{Citation needed|reason=Reliable source needed for the whole sentence|date=February 2015}} |
|||
Flynn earlier argued that the very large increase indicates that IQ tests do not measure intelligence well but only a minor sort of "abstract problem-solving ability" with little practical significance.<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
|||
In support of the nutritional hypothesis, it is known that, in the United States, the average height before 1900 was about 10 cm (~4 inches) shorter than it is today.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Samaras |first1=Thomas T. |last2=Elrick |first2=Harold |date=May 2002 |title=Group Height, body size, and longevity: is smaller better for the human body? |journal=West J Med |volume=176 |issue=3 |pages=206–08 |pmc=1071721 |pmid=12016250 |doi=10.1136/ewjm.176.3.206}}</ref> Possibly related to the Flynn effect is a similar change of [[cranial vault|skull]] size and shape during the last 150 years. A Norwegian study found that height gains were strongly correlated with intelligence gains until the cessation of height gains in military conscript cohorts towards the end of the 1980s.<ref name="doi10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.004">{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.004| last1 = Sundet | first1 = J. | last2 = Barlaug | first2 = D. | last3 = Torjussen | first3 = T. | journal = Intelligence | volume = 32 | issue = 4 | pages = 349–62 |title=The end of the Flynn effect?: A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century| year = 2004 }}</ref> Both height and skull size increases probably result from a combination of [[phenotypic plasticity]] and genetic [[Selection (biology)|selection]] over this period.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Jantz RL, Meadows Jantz L |title=Secular change in craniofacial morphology |journal=Am. J. Hum. Biol. |volume=12 |issue=3 |pages=327–38 |date=May 2000 |pmid=11534023 |doi=10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(200005/06)12:3<327::AID-AJHB3>3.0.CO;2-1|s2cid=22059721 |doi-access=free }}<br /> |
|||
Dickens and Flynn in 2001 presented a model for resolving several contradictory findings regarding IQ. They argue that the measure "[[heritability]]" includes both a direct effect of the [[genotype]] on IQ and also indirect effects where the genotype changes the environment, in turn affecting IQ. That is, those with a higher IQ tend to seek out stimulating environments that further increase IQ. The direct effect can initially have been very small but feedback loops can create large differences in IQ. In their model an environmental stimulus can have a very large effect on IQ, even in adults, but this effect also decays over time unless the stimulus continues (the model could be adapted to include possible factors, like nutrition in early childhood, that may cause permanent effects). The Flynn effect can be explained by a generally more stimulating environment for all people. The authors suggest that programs aiming to increase IQ would be most likely to produce long-term IQ gains if they taught children how to replicate outside the program the kinds of cognitively demanding experiences that produce IQ gains while they are in the program and motivate them to persist in that replication long after they have left the program.<ref>William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn, [http://www.apa.org/journals/features/rev1082346.pdf Heritability Estimates Versus Large Environmental Effects:The IQ Paradox Resolved], ''Psychological Review'' 2001. Vol. 108, No. 2. 346-369.</ref><ref>William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn, "[http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20020205.pdf The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved]," ''Psychological Review'' 109, no. 4 (2002).</ref> However if the Flynn Effect is caused by intellectual stimulation, this may suggest that the Flynn Effect is unrelated to g{{dubious}} because according to Jensen "the preponderance of evidence argues that variance in the level of g is not a psychologically manipulable variable, but rather a biological phenomenon under the control both of the genes and of those external physical variables that affect the physiological and biochemical functioning of the central nervous system, which mediates the behavioral manifestations of g<ref>The g factor by Arthur Jensen pg 336</ref>...Anything less than very early and intensive intervention, including medical and nutritional advances, during the preschool years (and also prenatally) is probably inadequate to cause a lasting increase in the child's level of g."<ref>The g factor by Aurthur Jensen pg 344</ref> However, Dickens and Flynn's paper, which was written after Jensen's book, disputes Jensen's claims, for example arguing that using Jensen's methodology the Flynn effect is found to be substantially due to genetic improvements, an extremely unlikely cause. Their theory explains such contradictions.{{dubious}} |
|||
{{cite journal |author=Jantz RL |title=Cranial change in Americans: 1850–1975 |journal=J. Forensic Sci. |volume=46 |issue=4 |pages=784–87 |date=July 2001 |doi=10.1520/JFS15047J |pmid=11451056 }}</ref> With only five or six human generations in 150 years, time for [[natural selection]] has been very limited, suggesting that increased skeletal size resulting from changes in population [[phenotype]]s is more likely than recent genetic evolution. |
|||
It is well known that [[micronutrient]] deficiencies change the development of intelligence. For instance, one study has found that [[iodine deficiency]] causes a fall, on average, of 12 IQ points in China.<ref>{{Cite journal|author=Qian M |title=The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China |journal=Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition |volume=14 |issue=1 |pages=32–42 |year=2005 |pmid=15734706|author2=Wang D|author3=Watkins WE|display-authors=3|last4=Gebski|first4=V|last5=Yan|first5=YQ|last6=Li|first6=M|last7=Chen|first7=ZP}}</ref> |
|||
Some researchers, such as [[J. Phillipe Rushton]]<ref>{{cite journal | author = Rushton, J. P. | year = 1999 | title = Secular Gains in IQ Not Related to the g Factor and Inbreeding Depression--Unlike Black-White Differences: A Reply to Flynn | journal = Personality and Individual Difference | volume = 26 | pages = 381-389 | id = {{doi|10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00148-2}} | url = http://www.ssc.uwo.ca/psychology/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PAID-1999.pdf }}</ref> argue the Flynn effect largely has not changed the [[general intelligence factor]] (''g''), which would mean practical significance of the effect would be limited. More recent studies have found that ''g'' has improved substantially<ref>[http://cat.inist.fr/?aModele=afficheN&cpsidt=16026650 Are cognitive differences between immigrant and majority groups diminishing?]TE NIJENHUIS Jan ; DE JONG Mart-Jan ; EVERS Arne ; VAN DER FLIER Henk ; European journal of personality (Eur. j. pers.) 2004, vol. 18, no5, pp. 405-434</ref><ref>[http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayAbstract?fromPage=online&aid=135589 Secular Gains in Fluid Intelligence: Evidence from the Culture-Fair Intelligence Test] Roberto Colom and Oscar Garcia-Lopez, Journal of Biosocial Science (2003), 35: 33-39 Cambridge University Press</ref> |
|||
Scientists James Feyrer, Dimitra Politi, and David N. Weil have found in the U.S. that the proliferation of iodized salt increased IQ by 15 points in some areas. Journalist Max Nisen has stated that with this type of salt becoming popular, that "the aggregate effect has been extremely positive."<ref>{{Cite web|last=Nisen|first=Max|title=How Adding Iodine To Salt Resulted In A Decade's Worth Of IQ Gains For The United States|url=https://www.businessinsider.com/iodization-effect-on-iq-2013-7|date=2013-07-22|access-date=2023-01-23|website=Business Insider|language=en-US}}</ref> |
|||
Studies that make use of multigroup confirmatory factor analysis test for "measurement invariance." Where tenable, invariance demonstrates that group differences exist in the latent constructs the tests contain and not, for example, as a result of measurement artifacts or cultural bias. Wicherts ''et al.'' (2004) found evidence from five data sets that IQ scores are not measurement invariant over time, and thus "the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure". In other words, according to this study, some of the inter-generational difference in IQ is attributable to bias or other artifacts, and not real gains in [[general intelligence factor|general intelligence]] or higher-order ability factors.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Wicherts, J.M., Dolan, C.V., Hessen, D.J., Oosterveld, P., Baal, G.C.M. van, Boomsma, D.I., & Span, M.M. |date=2004|title=Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect|journal=Intelligence|volume= 32|pages= 509–537|url=http://users.fmg.uva.nl/jwicherts/wicherts2004.pdf }} (''links to PDF file'')</ref> |
|||
Daley et al. (2003) found a significant Flynn effect among children in rural [[Kenya]], and concluded that nutrition was one of the hypothesized explanations that best explained their results (the others were parental literacy and family structure).<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Daley|first1=TC|last2=Whaley|first2=SE|last3=Sigman|first3=MD|last4=Espinosa|first4=MP|last5=Neumann|first5=C|s2cid=12315212|title=IQ on the rise: the Flynn effect in rural Kenyan children.|journal=Psychological Science|date=May 2003|volume=14|issue=3|pages=215–19|pmid=12741743|doi=10.1111/1467-9280.02434}}</ref> |
|||
A 2003 study looking at the Flynn effect in Kenya between 1984 and 1998 found that the increase was best explained by parents' literacy, family structure, and children's nutrition and health.<ref>[http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/links/doi/10.1111/1467-9280.02434/abs/ Iq on the rise: The Flynn Effect in Rural Kenyan Children] amara C. Daley, Shannon E. Whaley,Marian D. Sigman, Michael P. Espinosa, and Charlotte Neumann. Psychological Science 14 (3), 215–219.</ref> |
|||
===Generally more stimulating environment=== |
|||
A 2006 study from Brazil looked at data from testing children in 1930 and 2002-2004, the largest gap ever considered. The results are consistent with both the cognitive stimulation and the nutritional hypotheses.<ref>[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=16441963&dopt=Citation Generational changes on the draw-a-man test: a comparison of brazilian urban and rural children tested in 1930, 2002 and 2004] Colom R, Flores-Mendoza CE, Abad FJ. J Biosoc Sci. 2007 Jan;39(1):79-89. Epub 2006 Jan 27.</ref> |
|||
Still, another theory is that the general environment today is much more complex and stimulating. One of the most striking 20th-century changes in the human intellectual environment has come from the increase of exposure to many types of [[visual media]]. From pictures on the wall to movies to television to video games to computers, each successive generation has been exposed to richer optical displays than the one before and may have become more adept at visual analysis. This would explain why visual tests like the Raven's have shown the greatest increases. An increase only of particular forms of intelligence would explain why the Flynn effect has not caused a "cultural renaissance too great to be overlooked."<ref name="Neisser97"/> |
|||
In 2001, [[William Dickens]] and James Flynn presented a model for resolving several contradictory findings regarding IQ. They argue that the measure "[[heritability]]" includes both a direct effect of the [[genotype]] on IQ and also indirect effects such that the genotype changes the [[Environment (biophysical)|environment]], thereby affecting IQ. That is, those with a greater IQ tend to seek stimulating environments that further increase IQ. These reciprocal effects result in [[gene environment correlation]]. The direct effect could initially have been very small, but [[feedback]] can create large differences in IQ. In their model, an environmental stimulus can have a very great effect on IQ, even for adults, but this effect also decays over time unless the stimulus continues (the model could be adapted to include possible factors, like nutrition during early childhood, that may cause permanent effects). The Flynn effect can be explained by a generally more stimulating environment for all people. The authors suggest that any program designed to increase IQ may produce long-term IQ gains if that program teaches children how to replicate the types of cognitively demanding experiences that produce IQ gains outside the program. To maximize lifetime IQ, the programs should also motivate them to continue searching for cognitively demanding experiences after they have left the program.<ref name=Dickens01>{{cite journal |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346 |vauthors=Dickens WT, Flynn JR |title=Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved |journal=Psychological Review |volume=108 |issue=2 |pages=346–369 |year=2001 |pmid=11381833 |url=https://www.psychologytoday.com/files/u81/Dickens_and_Flynn__2001_.pdf|citeseerx=10.1.1.139.2436 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|vauthors=Dickens WT, Flynn JR |title=The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved |journal=Psychological Review |volume=109 |issue=4 |year=2002 |url=http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20020205.pdf |doi=10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.764 |pages=764–71 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070319031706/http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20020205.pdf |archive-date=March 19, 2007 }}</ref> |
|||
In the end a number of varied phenomena may be contributing to the Flynn effect. |
|||
Flynn, in his 2007 book ''[[What Is Intelligence?]]'', further expanded on this theory. Environmental changes resulting from modernization—such as more intellectually demanding work, greater use of technology, and smaller families—have meant that a much larger proportion of people are more accustomed to manipulating abstract concepts such as hypotheses and categories than a century ago. Substantial portions of IQ tests deal with these abilities. Flynn gives, as an example, the question 'What do a dog and a rabbit have in common?' A modern respondent might say they are both mammals (an abstract, or ''a priori'' answer, which depends only on the meanings of the words ''dog'' and ''rabbit''), whereas someone a century ago might have said that humans catch rabbits with dogs (a concrete, or ''a posteriori'' answer, which depended on what happened to be the case at that time).<ref>{{cite book |last=Flynn |first=James R. |author-link=James Flynn (academic) |title=[[What Is Intelligence?|What Is Intelligence? Beyond the Flynn Effect]] |date=August 2012 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9780511605253 |pages=24–29}}</ref><ref>{{cite magazine |last1=Gladwell |first1=Malcolm |title=None of the Above |url=https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2007/12/17/none-of-the-above |magazine=The New Yorker |access-date=July 6, 2024 |date=December 10, 2007 |url-access=limited}}</ref>{{Better source needed|date=July 2024}} |
|||
== Has progression ended? == |
|||
[[Image:Blacktest score rise.jpg|250px|right|thumb|William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn write that blacks have gained 5 or 6 IQ points on non-Hispanic whites between 1972 and 2002. This graph shows the gains for various tests.<ref>''[http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples]'' William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn. Oct. 2006</ref>]] |
|||
The Flynn effect may have ended in some developed nations starting in the mid 1990s. Teasdale & Owen (2005) "report intelligence test results from over 500,000 young [[Denmark|Danish]] men, tested between 1959 and 2004, showing that performance peaked in the late 1990s, and has since declined moderately to pre-1991 levels." They speculate that "a contributing factor in this recent fall could be a simultaneous decline in proportions of students entering 3-year advanced-level school programs for 16–18 year olds."<ref>* Teasdale, Thomas W., and David R. Owen. (2005). "A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse." ''Personality and Individual Differences.'' 39(4):837-843.</ref> |
|||
===Infectious diseases=== |
|||
In 2004, Jon Martin Sundet of the [[University of Oslo]] and colleagues published an article documenting scores on intelligence tests given to [[Norway|Norwegian]] conscripts between the 1950s and 2002, showing that the increase in scores of general intelligence stopped after the mid-1990s and in numerical reasoning subtests, declined.<ref>{{Cite web |
|||
{{See also|Parasite load#Host stress|Impact of health on intelligence}} |
|||
|url=http://72.14.207.104/search?q=cache:sLUpdtOiKmoJ:www.missouri.edu/~aab2b3/LFE_GNXP/sdarticle.pdf+%2B%22Jon+Martin+Sundet%22+%2BIQ&hl=en&gl=us&ct=clnk&cd=9 |
|||
Eppig, Fincher, and Thornhill (2011) conducted a study looking at different US states found that states with a higher prevalence of [[Infection|infectious diseases]] had lower average IQ. The effect remained after controlling for the effects of wealth and educational variation.<ref>{{Cite journal |vauthors=Eppig C, Fincher CL, Thornhill R |title=Parasite prevalence and the distribution of intelligence among the states of the USA |journal=Intelligence |volume=39 |issue=2–3 |pages=155–60 |year=2011 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2011.02.008}}</ref> |
|||
|title=The end of the Flynn Effect. A study of secular trends in mean intelligence scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century. |
|||
|accessmonthday=August 6 |accessyear=2006 |
|||
|date= |
|||
|author= |
|||
|publisher= |
|||
}}</ref> |
|||
Atheendar Venkataramani (2010) studied the effect of [[malaria]] on IQ in a sample of Mexicans. Malaria eradication during the birth year was associated with increases in IQ. It also increased the probability of employment in a skilled occupation. The author suggests that this may be one explanation for the Flynn effect and that this may be an important explanation for the link between national malaria burden and economic development.<ref>{{cite SSRN |vauthors=Venkataramani A |title=Early Life Exposure to Malaria and Cognition and Skills in Adulthood: Evidence from Mexico |date=September 18, 2010 |ssrn=1679164}}</ref> A literature review of 44 papers states that cognitive abilities and school performance were shown to be impaired in sub-groups of patients (with either cerebral malaria or uncomplicated malaria) when compared with healthy controls. Studies comparing cognitive functions before and after treatment for acute malarial illness continued to show significantly impaired school performance and cognitive abilities even after recovery. Malaria [[prophylaxis]] was shown to improve cognitive function and school performance in clinical trials when compared to placebo groups.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Fernando SD, Rodrigo C, Rajapakse S |title=The 'hidden' burden of malaria: cognitive impairment following infection |journal=Malar. J. |volume=9 |issue=1 |pages=366 |year=2010 |pmid=21171998 |pmc=3018393 |doi=10.1186/1475-2875-9-366 |doi-access=free }}</ref> |
|||
Some have claimed that the Flynn effect was masking a [[dysgenic]] decline in human reproduction and that in developed countries the only direction that IQ scores will now move is downwards. However, even if there is a decline, then this may have other causes than dysgenics. Genetic changes usually happen relatively slowly. For example, the Flynn effect has been too rapid for a genetic explanation.<ref>[http://language.la.psu.edu/~thorne/Intelligence2005.pdf Rising mean IQ: Cognitive demand of mathematics education for young children, population exposure to formal schooling, and the neurobiology of the prefrontal cortex] Clancy Blair, David Gamson, Steven Thorne, David Baker. Intelligence 33 (2005) 93–106</ref> Researchers have warned that constantly greater exposure to industrial chemicals shown to damage the nervous system, especially in children, in industrialized nations may be responsible for a "silent pandemic" of brain development disorders.[http://www.cbsnews.com/stories/2006/11/07/health/webmd/main2161153.shtml] |
|||
===Heterosis=== |
|||
Also, if the Flynn effect has ended for the majority, it may still continue for minorities, especially for groups like immigrants where many may have received poor nutrition during early childhood.{{Fact|date=February 2007}} For example, William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn write in their 2006 paper that ''Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples'' that blacks have gained 5 or 6 IQ points on non-Hispanic whites between 1972 and 2002. Gains have been fairly uniform across the entire range of black cognitive ability.<ref>''[http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples]'' William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn. Oct. 2006</ref> |
|||
{{Further|Inbreeding depression#In humans}} |
|||
[[Heterosis]], or ''hybrid vigor'', associated with historical reductions of the levels of [[inbreeding]], has been proposed by Michael Mingroni as an alternative explanation of the Flynn effect.<ref>Mingroni, M. A. (2007). "[http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/mingroni2007.pdf Resolving the IQ paradox: Heterosis as a cause of the Flynn effect and other trends]" (PDF). ''Psychological Review'', 114(3), 806–829. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.806</ref><ref>Mingroni, M.A. (2004). "[https://www.mingroni.net/_files/ugd/970723_2f1c2ed3946246e0b69a9c1b1396c825.pdf The secular rise in IQ: Giving heterosis a second look]" (PDF). ''Intelligence'', 32, 65-83.</ref> However, James Flynn has pointed out that even if everyone mated with a sibling in 1900, subsequent increases in heterosis would not be a sufficient explanation of the observed IQ gains.<ref>[[Nicholas Mackintosh|Mackintosh, N.J.]] (2011). ''IQ and Human Intelligence''. Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 291.</ref> |
|||
===Reduction of lead in gasoline=== |
|||
== References == |
|||
{{See also|Lead abatement|Lead poisoning|Lead–crime hypothesis}} |
|||
<references/> |
|||
One study found the drop in blood lead levels in the United States from the 1970s to 2007 correlated with a 4-5 point increase in IQ.<ref>{{cite journal |title=The possible societal impact of the decrease in U.S. blood lead levels on adult IQ |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0013935114001066 |first1=Alan S. |last1=Kaufman |first2=Xiaobin |last2=Zhou |first3=Matthew R. |last3=Reynolds |first4=Nadeen L. |last4=Kaufman |first5=Garo P. |last5=Green |first6=Lawrence G. |last6=Weisse |doi=10.1016/j.envres.2014.04.015 |journal=Environmental Research |volume=132 |date=July 2014 |pages=413–420|pmid=24853978 |bibcode=2014ER....132..413K }}</ref> |
|||
== Further reading == |
|||
* Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 29-51. |
|||
==Possible end of progression== |
|||
* Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171-191. |
|||
[[File:Sundet et al 2004 fig 3.svg|thumb|right|Mean standing height and mean GA (both in z scores units+5) by year of testing, from Sundet et al. 2004 (figure 3)]] |
|||
* Flynn, J. R., What is Intelligence?: Beyond the Flynn Effect, Cambridge University Press (2007). |
|||
Jon Martin Sundet and colleagues (2004) examined scores on intelligence tests given to [[Norway|Norwegian]] [[Norwegian Armed Forces#Conscription|conscripts]] between the 1950s and 2002. They found that the increase of scores of general intelligence stopped after the mid-1990s and declined in numerical reasoning sub-tests.<ref name="doi10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.004"/> |
|||
* Ulric Neisser et al.: The Rising Curve: Long-Term Gains in IQ and Related Measures. American Psychological Association (APA), 1998, ISBN 1-55798-503-0. |
|||
Teasdale and Owen (2005) examined the results of IQ tests given to [[Denmark|Danish]] male conscripts. Between 1959 and 1979 the gains were 3 points per decade. Between 1979 and 1989 the increase approached 2 IQ points. Between 1989 and 1998 the gain was about 1.3 points. Between 1998 and 2004 IQ declined by about the same amount as it gained between 1989 and 1998. They speculate that "a contributing factor in this recent fall could be a simultaneous decline in proportions of students entering 3-year advanced-level school programs for 16–18-year-olds."<ref name="Teasdale2005">{{cite journal|last1=Teasdale|first1=Thomas W.|last2=Owen|first2=David R.|year=2005|title=A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse|journal=Personality and Individual Differences|volume=39|issue=4|pages=837–43|doi=10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.029}}</ref> The same authors in a more comprehensive 2008 study, again on Danish male conscripts, found that there was a 1.5-point increase between 1988 and 1998, but a 1.5-point decrease between 1998 and 2003/2004.<ref name = "reversal">{{cite journal |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2007.01.007 |vauthors=Teasdale TW, Owen DR |title=Secular declines in cognitive test scores: A reversal of the Flynn Effect |journal=Intelligence |volume=36 |issue=2 |pages=121–26 |year=2008 |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/teasdale2008.pdf |access-date=April 18, 2010 |archive-date=October 15, 2012 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20121015184832/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/teasdale2008.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> |
|||
In Australia, the IQ of 6–12-year-olds (as measured by [[Raven's Progressive Matrices|colored progressive matrices]]) has shown no increase from 1975 to 2003.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Cotton | first1= S.M.| last2= Kiely| first2= P.M.|last3= Crewther|first3= D.P.|last4= Thomson|first4= B.|last5= Laycock|first5= R.|last6= Crewther|first6= S.G. |year=2005|title= A normative and reliability study for the Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices for primary school aged children in Australia|journal= Personality and Individual Differences|volume= 39| issue= 3|pages= 647–60| doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.015}}</ref> |
|||
In the United Kingdom, a study by Flynn (2009) himself found that tests carried out in 1980 and again in 2008 show that the IQ score of an average 14-year-old dropped by more than two points over the period. For the upper half of the results, the performance was even worse. Average IQ scores declined by six points. However, children aged between five and 10 saw their IQs increase by up to half a point a year over the three decades. Flynn argues that the abnormal drop in British teenage IQ could be due to youth culture having "stagnated" or even dumbed down.<ref name="requiem">{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009 | pmid = 19251490| last1 = Flynn | first1 = J.R. | title = Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938–2008 | journal = Economics & Human Biology | volume = 7 | issue = 1 | pages = 18–27 | year = 2009 }}</ref> |
|||
Bratsberg & Rogeberg (2018) present evidence that the Flynn effect in Norway has reversed between the years 1962–1991, and that both the original rise in mean IQ scores and their subsequent decline within this period can be observed within families consisting of native-born parents and their children, indicating that environmental factors were the likely cause for these changes. Because IQ data was only available for male Norwegians, who were subject to military conscription, years of schooling were used as an approximation for female IQ to support this conclusion.<ref name="pnas2018" /> |
|||
One possible explanation of a worldwide decline in intelligence is an increase in air pollution; coal burning emits mercury, and intelligence has continued to climb in areas, like the southern United States, where coal burning has declined.<ref>{{cite journal|first1=Ivica|last1=Pesovski|first2=Andrea|last2=Kulakov|first3=Vladimir|last3=Trajkovikj|title=Differences in cognitive ability assessment results between Millennial and Generation Z cohorts|url=https://repository.ukim.mk/handle/20.500.12188/25674|date=2022|journal=The 19th International Conference on Informatics and Information Technologies – CIIT 2022}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|first1=Bernt|last1=Bratsberg|first2=Ole|last2=Rogeberg|title=Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|date=26 June 2018|issn=0027-8424|pages=6674–6678|volume=115|issue=26|pmid=29891660|pmc=6042097|doi=10.1073/pnas.1718793115|doi-access=free |bibcode=2018PNAS..115.6674B }}</ref> |
|||
Winter et al. (2024) when comparing two [[Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale|WAIS-5]] validity studies found a reduced Flynn effect of an increase of 1.2 IQ points per decade rather than the expected 3 IQ point increase per decade. The authors identified various novel factors including [[social media]] dependency and the [[Impact of COVID-19 on neurological, psychological and other mental health outcomes|COVID-19 pandemic]] which may have contributed to a reduced Flynn effect.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Winter |first1=Emily L. |last2=Trudel |first2=Sierra M. |last3=Kaufman |first3=Alan S. |date=2024-11-15 |title=Wait, Where's the Flynn Effect on the WAIS-5? |journal=Journal of Intelligence |language=en |volume=12 |issue=11 |pages=118 |doi=10.3390/jintelligence12110118 |doi-access=free |issn=2079-3200|pmc=11595985 }}</ref> |
|||
==IQ group differences== |
|||
[[File:Flynn-–-World-Regions.png|thumb|Gains in IQ that different world regions have made since the first year for which data is available for a particular region.]] |
|||
{{see also|Nations and intelligence|Race and intelligence|Sex differences in intelligence}} |
|||
If the Flynn effect has ended in developed nations but continues in less developed ones, this would tend to diminish [[Nations and intelligence|national differences in IQ scores]].<ref name = "reversal" /> |
|||
Also, if the Flynn effect has ended for the majority in developed nations, it may still continue for minorities, especially for groups like immigrants where many may have received poor nutrition during early childhood or have had other disadvantages. A study in the [[Netherlands]] found that children of non-Western immigrants had improvements for [[g factor (psychometrics)|g]], educational achievements, and work proficiency compared to their parents, although there were still remaining differences compared to ethnic Dutch.<ref name=Nijenhuis04/> |
|||
In the United States, the IQ gap between black and white people was gradually closing over the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black–white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Vincent |first=Ken R. |date=March 1991 |title=Black/white IQ differences: Does age make the difference? |journal=Journal of Clinical Psychology |volume=47 |issue=2 |pages=266–270 |doi=10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2<266::aid-jclp2270470213>3.0.co;2-s |pmid=2030133 }}</ref> Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of black people and white people closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002,{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished.<ref>Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association</ref> Reviews by Flynn and Dickens,<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Dickens |first1=William T |last2=Flynn |first2=James R |year=2006 |title=Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples |url=http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_iq.pdf |url-status=dead |journal=Psychological Science |volume=17 |issue=10 |pages=913–20 |citeseerx=10.1.1.186.2540 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x |pmid=17100793 |s2cid=6593169 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20091009095003/http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf |archive-date=2009-10-09 }}</ref> [[Nicholas Mackintosh]],<ref>{{Cite book |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |author-link=Nicholas Mackintosh|title=IQ and Human Intelligence |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-19-958559-5 |edition=second |location=Oxford}}</ref>{{page needed|date=July 2024}} and Nisbett et al.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |year=2012a |title=Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/nisbett2012int.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |pages=130–159 |doi=10.1037/a0026699 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=22233090 |access-date=22 July 2013 |number=2 |archive-date=November 8, 2020 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201108132004/http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/nisbett2012int.pdf}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |first=Dan |last=Willingham |title=The latest on intelligence |url=http://www.danielwillingham.com/1/post/2012/05/the-latest-on-intelligence.html |website=Daniel Willingham—Science & Education |date=10 May 2012}}</ref> all concluded that the gradual closing of the gap was a real phenomenon. |
|||
Flynn has commented that he never claimed that the Flynn effect has the same causes as observed differences in average IQ test performance between blacks and whites, but that it shows that environmental factors can create IQ differences of a magnitude similar to that gap.<ref>{{Cite journal |doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001 |last1 = Flynn |first1 = J.R. |title = The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate |journal = Intelligence |volume = 38 |issue = 4 |pages = 363–66 |year = 2010 }}</ref> Wicherts ''et al''. had previously suggested a similar interpretation in a 2004 paper.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=Hessen |first3=David J. |last4=Oosterveld |first4=Paul |last5=van Baal |first5=G. Caroline M. |last6=Boomsma |first6=Dorret I. |last7=Span |first7=Mark M. |date=2004 |title=Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect |journal=Intelligence |volume=32 |issue=5 |pages=509–537 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002 |quote=It appears therefore that the nature of the Flynn effect is qualitatively different from the nature of B–W differences in the United States. Each comparison of groups should be investigated separately. IQ gaps between cohorts do not teach us anything about IQ gaps between contemporary groups, except that each IQ gap should not be confused with real (i.e., latent) differences in intelligence.}}</ref> Flynn also argued that his findings undermine the so-called [[Spearman's hypothesis]], which hypothesized that differences in g factor are the major driver of the blacks-whites IQ gap.<ref>{{Cite journal |doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5 |title = Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time |year = 1999 |last1 = Flynn |first1 = J.R. |journal = American Psychologist |volume=54| pages = 5–9 |url = http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/stuff_for_blog/flynn.pdf |access-date = 26 October 2017 |archive-date = 25 June 2010 |archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20100625085640/http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/stuff_for_blog/flynn.pdf |url-status = live}}</ref> |
|||
==See also== |
==See also== |
||
{{portal|Biology}} |
|||
* [[Intelligence (trait)|Intelligence]] |
|||
* [[ |
* [[Academic inflation]] |
||
* [[Environment and intelligence]] |
|||
* [[Heterosis]] |
|||
* [[Gene–environment correlation]] |
|||
== |
==References== |
||
{{Reflist|30em}} |
|||
* [http://www.indiana.edu/%7Eintell/flynneffect.shtml The Flynn Effect] by Indiana University. |
|||
* Marguerite Holloway, ''Flynn's effect'', [[Scientific American]], January 1999; [http://www.sciam.com/article.cfm?articleID=00037F65-D9C0-1C6A-84A9809EC588EF21&ref=sciam online edition] |
|||
* [http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/FLYNNEFF.html Increasing intelligence: the Flynn effect] |
|||
* [http://www.psych.usyd.edu.au/spearman/bios/flynn.html Flynn biography] |
|||
* [http://home.adelphia.net/~rdfuerle/Flynn.html "An Explanation for the Flynn Effect"] |
|||
* [http://www.apa.org/journals/features/rev1082346.pdf "Heritability Estimates Versus Large Environmental Effects: The IQ Paradox Resolved"] - article by Dickens and Flynn |
|||
*[http://socserv.mcmaster.ca/anthro/course.und/3L/105-1_gravleeetal.pdf Heredity, Environment, and Cranial Form: A Reanalysis of Boas’s Immigrant Data] |
|||
* [http://lance.qualquant.net/gravlee03b.pdf Did Boas get it right or wrong?] |
|||
* [http://www.wired.com/wired/archive/13.05/flynn_pr.html "Dome Improvement"] (''[[Wired (magazine)|Wired]]'' article) |
|||
* [http://www.newyorker.com/arts/critics/books/2007/12/17/071217crbo_books_gladwell Malcolm Gladwell on race, I.Q., and the Flynn effect] |
|||
==Further reading== |
|||
{{Race and intelligence}} |
|||
* [[te Nijenhuis, Jan]], & van der Flier, H. (2013). "Is the Flynn effect on g?: A meta-analysis." ''Intelligence'', 41(6), 802–807. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.001 |
|||
* [[Rodgers, Joseph Lee]] (1998). "A critique of the Flynn Effect: Massive IQ gains, methodological artifacts, or both?" ''Intelligence'', 26(4), 337–356. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00004-5 |
|||
* {{Cite journal |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.29 |author=Flynn, James R. |title=The Mean IQ of Americans: Massive Gains 1932 to 1978 |journal=[[Psychological Bulletin]] |volume=95 |pages=29–51 |year=1984 |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn1984b.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120310210720/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn1984b.pdf |archive-date=2012-03-10 |url-status=live |access-date=May 16, 2013 }} |
|||
* {{Cite journal |author=Flynn, James R. |title=Massive IQ Gains in 14 Nations: What IQ Tests Really Measure |journal=[[Psychological Bulletin]] |volume=101 |issue=2 |pages=171–91 |date= March 1987 |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn1987.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110124091833/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn1987.pdf |archive-date=2011-01-24 |url-status=live |access-date=May 13, 2013 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171 }} |
|||
* {{Cite book |author=Flynn, James R. |title=Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-first century |location=Cambridge |publisher=[[Cambridge University Press]] |isbn=978-1-107-60917-4 |year=2012 |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/arewegettingsmar0000flyn }} |
|||
* {{cite journal |first=Lea |last=Winerman |title=Smarter than ever? |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/2013/03/smarter.aspx |journal=Monitor on Psychology |volume=44 |issue=3 |page=30|date=March 2013}} |
|||
* {{Cite book |title=The Rising Curve: Long-Term Gains in IQ and Related Measures |editor-last=Neisser |editor-first=Ulric |author=Ulric Neisser |author2=James R. Flynn |author3=Carmi Schooler |author4=Patricia M. Greenfield |author5=Wendy M. Williams |author6=Marian Sigman |author7=Shannon E. Whaley |author8=Reynaldo Martorell |author9=Richard Lynn |author10=Robert M. Hauser |author11=David W. Grissmer |author12=Stephanie Williamson |author13=Sheila Nataraj Kirby |author14=Mark Berends |author15=Stephen J. Ceci |author16=Tina B. Rosenblum |author17=Matthew Kumpf |author18=Min-Hsiung Huang |author19=Irwin D. Waldman |author20=Samuel H. Preston |author21=John C. Loehlin |year=1998 |publisher=American Psychological Association |location=Washington DC |isbn=978-1-55798-503-3 |series=APA Science Volume Series |url=https://archive.org/details/risingcurvelongt00neis |url-access=registration}} |
|||
* {{citation |last=Tuddenham |first=Read D. |title=Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II |journal=American Psychologist |volume=3 |issue=2 |pages=54–56 |date=1948 |doi=10.1037/h0054962 |pmid=18911933 }} |
|||
==External links== |
|||
* {{Cite web |last=Flynn |first=James Robert |author-link=James Flynn (academic) |date=2006-12-15 |title=Beyond the Flynn Effect |url=https://www.psychometrics.cam.ac.uk/about-us/directory/beyond-the-flynn-effect |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20171011193938/https://www.psychometrics.cam.ac.uk/about-us/directory/beyond-the-flynn-effect |archive-date=2017-10-11 |access-date=2024-05-30 |website=[[University of Cambridge]]}} |
|||
* {{Cite web |last=Graham |first=Charles |date=2001 |title=The Flynn Effect |url=http://www.indiana.edu/%7Eintell/flynneffect.shtml |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20051025230525/http://www.indiana.edu/%7Eintell/flynneffect.shtml |archive-date=2005-10-25 |access-date=2024-05-30 |website=[[Indiana University]]}} |
|||
* {{Cite journal |last=Holloway |first=Marguerite |date=1999-01-01 |title=Flynn's Effect |url=http://www.sciam.com/article.cfm?articleID=00037F65-D9C0-1C6A-84A9809EC588EF21 |url-status=dead |journal=[[Scientific American]] |volume=280 |issue=1 |pages=37–38 |doi=10.1038/scientificamerican0199-37 |bibcode=1999SciAm.280a..37H |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20050910211047/http://www.sciam.com/article.cfm?articleID=00037F65-D9C0-1C6A-84A9809EC588EF21 |archive-date=2005-09-10 }} |
|||
* {{Cite journal |last1=Dickens |first1=William Theodore |author-link=William Dickens |last2=Flynn |first2=James Robert |author-link2=James Flynn (academic) |date=2001 |title=Heritability Estimates Versus Large Environmental Effects: The IQ Paradox Resolved |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2F0033-295X.108.2.346 |journal=[[Psychological Review]] |language=en |volume=108 |issue=2 |pages=346–369 |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346 |issn=0033-295X |eissn=1939-1471 |via=[[American Psychological Association]]}} |
|||
* {{Cite magazine |last=Johnson |first=Steven |date=2005-05-01 |title=Dome Improvement |url=https://www.wired.com/2005/05/flynn-2/ |url-status=live |magazine=[[Wired (magazine)|Wired]] |issn=1059-1028 |eissn=1078-3148 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161019222711/https://www.wired.com/2005/05/flynn-2/ |archive-date=2016-10-19 }} |
|||
* {{Cite web |last=Heylighen |first=Francis Paul |author-link=Francis Heylighen |date=2000-08-22 |title=Increasing intelligence: the Flynn effect |url=http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/FLYNNEFF.html |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070704094614/http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/FLYNNEFF.html |archive-date=2007-07-04 |access-date=2024-05-30 |website=[[Principia Cybernetica]]}} |
|||
{{Human intelligence topics}} |
|||
[[Category:Psychometrics]] |
|||
{{Evolutionary psychology}} |
|||
[[Category:Futurology]] |
|||
[[Category:Psychological theories]] |
|||
[[Category:Race and intelligence controversy| ]] |
|||
[[Category:Intelligence]] |
|||
[[Category:Intelligence quotient]] |
|||
[[ca:Efecte Flynn]] |
|||
[[Category:Race and intelligence controversy]] |
|||
[[de:Flynn-Effekt]] |
|||
[[es:Efecto Flynn]] |
|||
[[it:Effetto Flynn]] |
|||
[[hu:Flynn-effektus]] |
|||
[[nl:Flynn-effect]] |
|||
[[no:Flynneffekten]] |
|||
[[pl:Efekt Flynna]] |
|||
[[fi:Flynnin ilmiö]] |
|||
[[sv:Flynneffekten]] |
|||
[[zh:弗林效应]] |
Latest revision as of 00:40, 15 December 2024
The Flynn effect is the substantial and long-sustained increase in both fluid and crystallized intelligence test scores that were measured in many parts of the world over the 20th century, named after researcher James Flynn (1934–2020).[1][2] When intelligence quotient (IQ) tests are initially standardized using a sample of test-takers, by convention the average of the test results is set to 100 and their standard deviation is set to 15 or 16 IQ points. When IQ tests are revised, they are again standardized using a new sample of test-takers, usually born more recently than the first; the average result is set to 100. When the new test subjects take the older tests, in almost every case their average scores are significantly above 100.
Test score increases have been continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to the present. For example, a study published in the year 2009 found that British children's average scores on the Raven's Progressive Matrices test rose by 14 IQ points from 1942 to 2008.[3] Similar gains have been observed in many other countries in which IQ testing has long been widely used, including other Western European countries, as well as Japan and South Korea.[2] Improvements have also been reported for semantic and episodic memory.[4]
There are numerous proposed explanations of the Flynn effect, such as the rise in efficiency of education, along with skepticism concerning its implications. Some researchers have suggested the possibility of a mild reversal in the Flynn effect (i.e., a decline in IQ scores) in developed countries, beginning in the 1990s.[5][6][7][8] In certain cases, this apparent reversal may be due to cultural changes rendering parts of intelligence tests obsolete.[9] Meta-analyses indicate that, overall, the Flynn effect continues, either at the same rate,[10] or at a slower rate in developed countries.[11][12]
Origin of term
External videos | |
---|---|
James Flynn: Why our IQ levels are higher than our grandparents', (18:41), TED talks |
The Flynn effect is named for James Robert Flynn, who did much to document it and promote awareness of its implications. The term was coined by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray in their 1994 book The Bell Curve.[13][14][15] Flynn stated that, if asked, he would have named the effect after Read D. Tuddenham[16] who "was the first to present convincing evidence of massive gains on mental tests using a nationwide sample"[17] in a 1948 article.[18]
Although the general term for the phenomenon—referring to no researcher in particular—continues to be "secular rise in IQ scores", many textbooks on psychology and IQ testing have now followed the lead of Herrnstein and Murray in calling the phenomenon the Flynn effect.[19]
Rise in IQ
IQ tests are updated periodically. For example, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), originally developed in 1949, was updated in 1974, 1991, 2003, and again in 2014. The revised versions are standardized based on the performance of test-takers in standardization samples. A standard score of IQ 100 is defined as the mean performance of the standardization sample. Thus one way to see changes in norms over time is to conduct a study in which the same test-takers take both an old and new version of the same test. Doing so confirms IQ gains over time. Some IQ tests - for example, tests used for military draftees in NATO countries in Europe - report raw scores, and those also confirm a trend of rising scores over time. The average rate of increase seems to be about three IQ points per decade in the United States, as scaled by the Wechsler tests. The increasing test performance over time appears on every major test, in every age range, at every ability level, and in every modern industrialized country, although not necessarily at the same rate as in the United States. The increase was continuous and roughly linear from the earliest days of testing to the mid-1990s.[20][better source needed] Though the effect is most associated with IQ increases, a similar effect has been found with increases in attention and of semantic and episodic memory.[4]
Ulric Neisser estimated that using the IQ values of 1997, the average IQ of the United States in 1932, according to the first Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales standardization sample, was 80. Neisser states that "Hardly any of them would have scored 'very superior', but nearly one-quarter would have appeared to be 'deficient.'" He also wrote that "Test scores are certainly going up all over the world, but whether intelligence itself has risen remains controversial."[20] Quantitative psychologist, Joseph Lee Rodgers argues that the effect occurs outside of families in any case.[21]
Trahan et al. (2014) found that the effect was about 2.93 points per decade,[clarification needed] based on both Stanford–Binet and Wechsler tests; they also found no evidence the effect was diminishing.[22] In contrast, Pietschnig and Voracek (2015) reported, in their meta-analysis of studies involving nearly 4 million participants, that the Flynn effect had decreased in recent decades. They also reported that the magnitude of the effect was different for different types of intelligence ("0.41, 0.30, 0.28, and 0.21 IQ points annually for fluid, spatial, full-scale, and crystallized IQ test performance, respectively"), and that the effect was stronger for adults than for children.[23]
Raven (2000) found that, as Flynn suggested, data interpreted as showing a decrease in many abilities with increasing age must be re-interpreted as showing that there has been a dramatic increase of these abilities with the date of birth. On many tests this occurs at all levels of ability.[24]
Some studies have found the gains of the Flynn effect to be particularly concentrated at the lower end of the distribution. Teasdale and Owen (1989), for example, found the effect primarily reduced the number of low-end scores, resulting in an increased number of moderately high scores, with no increase in very high scores.[25] In another study, two large samples of Spanish children were assessed with a 30-year gap. Comparison of the IQ distributions indicated that the mean IQ scores on the test had increased by 9.7 points (the Flynn effect), the gains were concentrated in the lower half of the distribution and negligible in the top half, and the gains gradually decreased as the IQ of the individuals increased.[26] Some studies have found a reverse Flynn effect with declining scores for those with high IQ.[23]
In 1987, Flynn took the position that the very large increase indicates that IQ tests do not measure intelligence but only a minor sort of "abstract problem-solving ability" with little practical significance. He argued that if IQ gains did reflect intelligence increases, there would have been consequent changes of our society that have not been observed (a presumed non-occurrence of a "cultural renaissance").[20] By 2012 Flynn no longer endorsed this view of intelligence, having elaborated and refined his view of what rising IQ scores meant.[27]
Precursors to Flynn's publications
Earlier investigators had discovered rises in raw IQ test scores in some study populations, but had not published general investigations of that issue in particular. Historian Daniel C. Calhoun cited earlier psychology literature on IQ score trends in his book The Intelligence of a People (1973).[28] Robert L. Thorndike – not to be confused with his famous father Edward – drew attention to rises in Stanford-Binet scores in a 1975 review of the history of intelligence testing.[29] In 1982, Richard Lynn recorded an increase in average IQ among the population of Japan.[30]
Intelligence
There is debate about whether the rise in IQ scores also corresponds to a rise in general intelligence, or only a rise in special skills related to taking IQ tests. Because children attend school longer now and have become much more familiar with the testing of school-related material, one might expect the greatest gains to occur on such school content-related tests as vocabulary, arithmetic or general information. Just the opposite is the case: abilities such as these have experienced relatively small gains and even occasional decreases over the years. Meta-analytic findings indicate that Flynn effects occur for tests assessing both fluid and crystallized abilities. For example, Dutch conscripts gained 21 points during only 30 years, or 7 points per decade, between 1952 and 1982.[20] This rise in IQ test scores is not wholly explained by an increase in general intelligence. Studies have shown that while test scores have improved over time, the improvement is not fully correlated with latent factors related to intelligence.[31] Other researchers argue that the IQ gains described by the Flynn effect are due in part to increasing intelligence, and in part to increases in test-specific skills.[32][33][34] One study suggested that the IQ gains reflected changes in modes of thinking that better reflected cognitive skills assessed by IQ tests rather than raw intelligence itself.[35]
Proposed explanations
Schooling and test familiarity
The duration of average schooling has increased steadily. However, a criticism of this explanation is that if (in the United States) older and younger subjects, with similar educational levels, are compared together, then the IQ gains appear almost undiminished in each group compared to when they are considered individually.[20]
Many studies find that children who do not attend school score drastically lower on the tests than their regularly attending peers. During the 1960s, when some Virginia counties closed their public schools to avoid racial integration, compensatory private schooling was available only for White children. On average, the scores of African-American children who received no formal education during that period decreased at a rate of about six IQ points per year.[20]
Another explanation is an increased familiarity of the general population with tests and testing. For example, children who take the very same IQ test a second time usually gain five or six points. However, this seems to set an upper limit on the effects of test sophistication. One problem with this explanation and others related to schooling is that in the US, the groups with greater test familiarity show smaller IQ increases.[20]
Early intervention programs have shown mixed results. Some preschool (ages 3–4) intervention programs like "Head Start" do not produce lasting changes of IQ, although they may confer other benefits.[which?] The "Abecedarian Early Intervention Project", an all-day program that provided various forms of environmental enrichment to children from infancy onward, showed IQ gains that did not diminish over time. The IQ gains in the experimental group compared to the control group was 4.4 points. These gains persisted until at least age 21.[36]
Citing a high correlation between rising literacy rates and gains in IQ, David Marks has argued that the Flynn effect is caused by changes in literacy rates.[37]
Nutrition
Improved nutrition is another possible explanation. Today's average adult from an industrialized nation is taller than a comparable adult of a century ago. That increase of stature, likely the result of general improvements in nutrition and health, has been at a rate of more than a centimeter per decade. Available data suggest that these gains have been accompanied by analogous increases in head size, and by an increase in the average size of the brain.[20][38] This argument had been thought to suffer the difficulty that groups who tend to be of smaller overall body size (e.g. women, or people of Asian ancestry) do not have lower average IQs.[25]
A 2005 study presented data supporting the nutrition hypothesis, which predicts that gains will occur predominantly at the low end of the IQ distribution, where nutritional deprivation is probably most severe.[26] An alternative interpretation of skewed IQ gains could be that improved education has been particularly important for this group.[25]
A century ago, nutritional deficiencies may have limited body and organ functionality, including skull volume. The first two years of life are a critical time for nutrition. The consequences of malnutrition can be irreversible and may include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity.[39] On the other hand, Flynn has pointed to 20-point gains on Dutch military (Raven's type) IQ tests between 1952, 1962, 1972, and 1982. In 1962 he observed that Dutch 18-year-olds had a major nutritional handicap. They were either in the womb or were recently born, during the great Dutch famine of 1944—when German troops monopolized food and 18,000 people died of starvation.[40] Yet, concludes Flynn, "they do not show up even as a blip in the pattern of Dutch IQ gains. It is as if the famine had never occurred."[41][42] It appears that the effects of diet are gradual, taking effect over decades (affecting mother as well as the child) rather than a few months.[citation needed]
In support of the nutritional hypothesis, it is known that, in the United States, the average height before 1900 was about 10 cm (~4 inches) shorter than it is today.[43] Possibly related to the Flynn effect is a similar change of skull size and shape during the last 150 years. A Norwegian study found that height gains were strongly correlated with intelligence gains until the cessation of height gains in military conscript cohorts towards the end of the 1980s.[44] Both height and skull size increases probably result from a combination of phenotypic plasticity and genetic selection over this period.[45] With only five or six human generations in 150 years, time for natural selection has been very limited, suggesting that increased skeletal size resulting from changes in population phenotypes is more likely than recent genetic evolution.
It is well known that micronutrient deficiencies change the development of intelligence. For instance, one study has found that iodine deficiency causes a fall, on average, of 12 IQ points in China.[46]
Scientists James Feyrer, Dimitra Politi, and David N. Weil have found in the U.S. that the proliferation of iodized salt increased IQ by 15 points in some areas. Journalist Max Nisen has stated that with this type of salt becoming popular, that "the aggregate effect has been extremely positive."[47]
Daley et al. (2003) found a significant Flynn effect among children in rural Kenya, and concluded that nutrition was one of the hypothesized explanations that best explained their results (the others were parental literacy and family structure).[48]
Generally more stimulating environment
Still, another theory is that the general environment today is much more complex and stimulating. One of the most striking 20th-century changes in the human intellectual environment has come from the increase of exposure to many types of visual media. From pictures on the wall to movies to television to video games to computers, each successive generation has been exposed to richer optical displays than the one before and may have become more adept at visual analysis. This would explain why visual tests like the Raven's have shown the greatest increases. An increase only of particular forms of intelligence would explain why the Flynn effect has not caused a "cultural renaissance too great to be overlooked."[20]
In 2001, William Dickens and James Flynn presented a model for resolving several contradictory findings regarding IQ. They argue that the measure "heritability" includes both a direct effect of the genotype on IQ and also indirect effects such that the genotype changes the environment, thereby affecting IQ. That is, those with a greater IQ tend to seek stimulating environments that further increase IQ. These reciprocal effects result in gene environment correlation. The direct effect could initially have been very small, but feedback can create large differences in IQ. In their model, an environmental stimulus can have a very great effect on IQ, even for adults, but this effect also decays over time unless the stimulus continues (the model could be adapted to include possible factors, like nutrition during early childhood, that may cause permanent effects). The Flynn effect can be explained by a generally more stimulating environment for all people. The authors suggest that any program designed to increase IQ may produce long-term IQ gains if that program teaches children how to replicate the types of cognitively demanding experiences that produce IQ gains outside the program. To maximize lifetime IQ, the programs should also motivate them to continue searching for cognitively demanding experiences after they have left the program.[49][50]
Flynn, in his 2007 book What Is Intelligence?, further expanded on this theory. Environmental changes resulting from modernization—such as more intellectually demanding work, greater use of technology, and smaller families—have meant that a much larger proportion of people are more accustomed to manipulating abstract concepts such as hypotheses and categories than a century ago. Substantial portions of IQ tests deal with these abilities. Flynn gives, as an example, the question 'What do a dog and a rabbit have in common?' A modern respondent might say they are both mammals (an abstract, or a priori answer, which depends only on the meanings of the words dog and rabbit), whereas someone a century ago might have said that humans catch rabbits with dogs (a concrete, or a posteriori answer, which depended on what happened to be the case at that time).[51][52][better source needed]
Infectious diseases
Eppig, Fincher, and Thornhill (2011) conducted a study looking at different US states found that states with a higher prevalence of infectious diseases had lower average IQ. The effect remained after controlling for the effects of wealth and educational variation.[53]
Atheendar Venkataramani (2010) studied the effect of malaria on IQ in a sample of Mexicans. Malaria eradication during the birth year was associated with increases in IQ. It also increased the probability of employment in a skilled occupation. The author suggests that this may be one explanation for the Flynn effect and that this may be an important explanation for the link between national malaria burden and economic development.[54] A literature review of 44 papers states that cognitive abilities and school performance were shown to be impaired in sub-groups of patients (with either cerebral malaria or uncomplicated malaria) when compared with healthy controls. Studies comparing cognitive functions before and after treatment for acute malarial illness continued to show significantly impaired school performance and cognitive abilities even after recovery. Malaria prophylaxis was shown to improve cognitive function and school performance in clinical trials when compared to placebo groups.[55]
Heterosis
Heterosis, or hybrid vigor, associated with historical reductions of the levels of inbreeding, has been proposed by Michael Mingroni as an alternative explanation of the Flynn effect.[56][57] However, James Flynn has pointed out that even if everyone mated with a sibling in 1900, subsequent increases in heterosis would not be a sufficient explanation of the observed IQ gains.[58]
Reduction of lead in gasoline
One study found the drop in blood lead levels in the United States from the 1970s to 2007 correlated with a 4-5 point increase in IQ.[59]
Possible end of progression
Jon Martin Sundet and colleagues (2004) examined scores on intelligence tests given to Norwegian conscripts between the 1950s and 2002. They found that the increase of scores of general intelligence stopped after the mid-1990s and declined in numerical reasoning sub-tests.[44]
Teasdale and Owen (2005) examined the results of IQ tests given to Danish male conscripts. Between 1959 and 1979 the gains were 3 points per decade. Between 1979 and 1989 the increase approached 2 IQ points. Between 1989 and 1998 the gain was about 1.3 points. Between 1998 and 2004 IQ declined by about the same amount as it gained between 1989 and 1998. They speculate that "a contributing factor in this recent fall could be a simultaneous decline in proportions of students entering 3-year advanced-level school programs for 16–18-year-olds."[60] The same authors in a more comprehensive 2008 study, again on Danish male conscripts, found that there was a 1.5-point increase between 1988 and 1998, but a 1.5-point decrease between 1998 and 2003/2004.[61]
In Australia, the IQ of 6–12-year-olds (as measured by colored progressive matrices) has shown no increase from 1975 to 2003.[62]
In the United Kingdom, a study by Flynn (2009) himself found that tests carried out in 1980 and again in 2008 show that the IQ score of an average 14-year-old dropped by more than two points over the period. For the upper half of the results, the performance was even worse. Average IQ scores declined by six points. However, children aged between five and 10 saw their IQs increase by up to half a point a year over the three decades. Flynn argues that the abnormal drop in British teenage IQ could be due to youth culture having "stagnated" or even dumbed down.[63]
Bratsberg & Rogeberg (2018) present evidence that the Flynn effect in Norway has reversed between the years 1962–1991, and that both the original rise in mean IQ scores and their subsequent decline within this period can be observed within families consisting of native-born parents and their children, indicating that environmental factors were the likely cause for these changes. Because IQ data was only available for male Norwegians, who were subject to military conscription, years of schooling were used as an approximation for female IQ to support this conclusion.[8]
One possible explanation of a worldwide decline in intelligence is an increase in air pollution; coal burning emits mercury, and intelligence has continued to climb in areas, like the southern United States, where coal burning has declined.[64][65]
Winter et al. (2024) when comparing two WAIS-5 validity studies found a reduced Flynn effect of an increase of 1.2 IQ points per decade rather than the expected 3 IQ point increase per decade. The authors identified various novel factors including social media dependency and the COVID-19 pandemic which may have contributed to a reduced Flynn effect.[66]
IQ group differences
If the Flynn effect has ended in developed nations but continues in less developed ones, this would tend to diminish national differences in IQ scores.[61]
Also, if the Flynn effect has ended for the majority in developed nations, it may still continue for minorities, especially for groups like immigrants where many may have received poor nutrition during early childhood or have had other disadvantages. A study in the Netherlands found that children of non-Western immigrants had improvements for g, educational achievements, and work proficiency compared to their parents, although there were still remaining differences compared to ethnic Dutch.[33]
In the United States, the IQ gap between black and white people was gradually closing over the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black–white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults.[67] Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of black people and white people closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002,[68] a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished.[69] Reviews by Flynn and Dickens,[70] Nicholas Mackintosh,[71][page needed] and Nisbett et al.[72][73] all concluded that the gradual closing of the gap was a real phenomenon.
Flynn has commented that he never claimed that the Flynn effect has the same causes as observed differences in average IQ test performance between blacks and whites, but that it shows that environmental factors can create IQ differences of a magnitude similar to that gap.[74] Wicherts et al. had previously suggested a similar interpretation in a 2004 paper.[75] Flynn also argued that his findings undermine the so-called Spearman's hypothesis, which hypothesized that differences in g factor are the major driver of the blacks-whites IQ gap.[76]
See also
References
- ^ Trahan, Lisa H.; Stuebing, Karla K.; Fletcher, Jack M.; Hiscock, Merrill (2014). "The Flynn effect: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1332–1360. doi:10.1037/a0037173. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 4152423. PMID 24979188.
- ^ a b Baker, David P.; Eslinger, Paul J.; Benavides, Martin; Peters, Ellen; Dieckmann, Nathan F.; Leon, Juan (March 2015). "The cognitive impact of the education revolution: A possible cause of the Flynn Effect on population IQ". Intelligence. 49: 144–58. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.01.003. ISSN 0160-2896.
- ^ Flynn, James R. (March 2009). "Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938–2008". Economics and Human Biology. 7 (1): 18–27. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009. ISSN 1873-6130. PMID 19251490.
- ^ a b Rönnlund M, Nilsson LG (September 2009). "Flynn effects on sub-factors of episodic and semantic memory: parallel gains over time and the same set of determining factors". Neuropsychologia. 47 (11): 2174–80. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.007. PMID 19056409. S2CID 15706086.
- ^ Al-Shahomee; et al. (2018). "An increase of intelligence in Libya from 2008 to 2017". Personality and Individual Differences. 130: 147–149. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.004. S2CID 149095461.
- ^ Teasdale, Thomas W; Owen, David R (2005). "A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (4): 837–43. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.029.
- ^ Pietschnig, Jakob; Gittler, Georg (2015). "A reversal of the Flynn effect for spatial perception in German-speaking countries: Evidence from a cross-temporal IRT-based meta-analysis (1977–2014)". Intelligence. 53: 145–53. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.10.004.
- ^ a b Bratsberg, Bernt; Rogeberg, Ole (June 6, 2018). "Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (26): 6674–78. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.6674B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1718793115. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 6042097. PMID 29891660.
- ^ Gonthier, Corentin; Grégoire, Jacques; Besançon, Maud (January 2021). "No negative Flynn effect in France: Why variations of intelligence should not be assessed using tests based on cultural knowledge". Intelligence. 84: 101512. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2020.101512. S2CID 230538271.
- ^ Trahan, Lisa H.; Stuebing, Karla K.; Fletcher, Jack M.; Hiscock, Merrill (2014). "The Flynn effect: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1332–1360. doi:10.1037/a0037173. PMC 4152423. PMID 24979188.
- ^ Pietschnig, Jakob; Voracek, Martin (May 2015). "One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-Analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (3): 282–306. doi:10.1177/1745691615577701. PMID 25987509. S2CID 12604392.
- ^ Wongupparaj, Peera; Kumari, Veena; Morris, Robin G. (March 2015). "A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Raven's Progressive Matrices: Age groups and developing versus developed countries". Intelligence. 49: 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.008.
- ^ Flynn, James R. (2009). What Is Intelligence: Beyond the Flynn Effect (expanded paperback ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 1–2. ISBN 978-0-521-74147-7.
The 'Flynn effect' is the name that has become attached to an exciting development, namely, that the twentieth century saw massive IQ gains from one generation to another. To forestall a diagnosis of megalomania, the label was coined by Herrnstein and Murray, the authors of The Bell Curve, and not by myself.
- ^ Shalizi, Cosma (April 27, 2009). "The Domestication of the Savage Mind". University of Michigan (Review). Archived from the original on July 19, 2012. Retrieved August 13, 2010.
- ^ Herrnstein, Richard J.; Murray, Charles (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: The Free Press. p. 307. ISBN 0-02-914673-9.
- ^ Haig, Brian D. (July 1, 2013). "Detecting Psychological Phenomena: Taking Bottom-Up Research Seriously". The American Journal of Psychology. 126 (2): 135–153. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.2.0135. ISSN 0002-9556. PMID 23858950.
- ^ Flynn, James R. (2011), "Secular Changes in Intelligence" (PDF), The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, pp. 647–665, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511977244.033, ISBN 978-0-511-97724-4, retrieved March 27, 2023
- ^ Tuddenham, R. D. (1948). "Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II". The American Psychologist. 3 (2): 54–56. doi:10.1037/h0054962. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 18911933.
- ^ Fletcher, Richard B.; Hattie, John (2011). Intelligence and Intelligence Testing. Taylor & Francis. p. 26. ISBN 978-1-136-82321-3. Retrieved August 31, 2013.
Indeed, this effect, now called the 'Flynn effect', is well established. Nations, almost without exception, have shown gains of about 20 IQ points per generation (30 years). These gains are highest for IQ tests that are most related to reasoning and the capacity to figure out novel problems (this is often called 'fluid intelligence', see Chapter 5); and least related to knowledge, which arises from better educational opportunity, a history of persistence and good motivation for learning (this is often called 'crystallized intelligence', see Chapter 5).
- Freeman, Joan (2010). Gifted Lives: What Happens when Gifted Children Grow Up. London: Routledge. pp. 290–91. ISBN 978-0-415-47009-4.
A strange new phenomenon has been growing since about 1950, called the 'Flynn Effect' after Professor James Flynn of the University of Otago, New Zealand. In his book What is Intelligence ?, Flynn describes a year-on-year rise in measured intelligence, about three IQ points a decade.
- Barbieri, Annalisa (October 8, 2010). "Young, gifted and likely to suffer for it". The Guardian. Archived from the original on November 11, 2020. Retrieved December 14, 2016.
- Urbina, Susana (2004). Essentials of Psychological Testing. John Wiley & Sons. p. 103. ISBN 978-0-471-41978-5.
A puzzling longitudinal trend in the opposite direction, known as the 'Flynn effect', has been well documented in successive revisions of major intelligence tests (like the S-B and the Wechsler scales) that invariably involve the administration of both the old and new versions to a segment of the newer standardization sample, for comparative purposes. Data from revisions of various intelligence tests in the United States as well as in other countries—extensively analyzed by J.R. Flynn (1984, 1987)—show a pronounced, long-term upward trend in the level of performance required to obtain any given IQ score. The Flynn effect presumably reflects population gains over time in the kinds of cognitive performance that intelligence tests sample.
Wasserman, John D. (2012). "Chapter 18: Assessment of Intellectual Functioning". In Weiner, Irving B.; Graham, John R.; Naglieri, Jack A. (eds.). Handbook of Psychology. Vol. 10: Assessment Psychology. John Wiley & Sons. p. 486. ISBN 978-0-470-89127-8.Both definitions also specify that the intellectual functioning criterion for a diagnosis of intellectual disability is approximately 2 SDs or more below the normative mean, but factors such as test score statistical error (standard error of measurement), test fairness, normative expectations for the population of interest, the Flynn effect, and practice effects from previous testing need to be considered before arriving at any diagnosis.
- Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (2011). Personality and Individual Differences. Wiley. p. 221. ISBN 978-1-4051-9927-8.
Flynn effect The finding by sociologist James Flynn that there are generational increases in IQ across nations.
- Freeman, Joan (2010). Gifted Lives: What Happens when Gifted Children Grow Up. London: Routledge. pp. 290–91. ISBN 978-0-415-47009-4.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i Neisser U (1997). "Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests". American Scientist. 85 (5): 440–47. Bibcode:1997AmSci..85..440N. Archived from the original on November 4, 2016.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link) - ^ Joseph Lee Rodgers (2014). "http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/rodgers2014.pdf Intelligence Are birth order effects on intelligence really Flynn Effects? Reinterpreting Belmont and Marolla 40 years later" (PDF). Intelligence 42: 128-133. "No within-family data exist that document an increase in intelligence over birth order, suggesting that its source derives from outside the family and will only manifest in data and analyses that account for between-family variance (such as cross-sectional data)." (p. 130)
- ^ Trahan, LH; Stuebing, KK; Fletcher, JM; Hiscock, M (September 2014). "The Flynn effect: a meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1332–60. doi:10.1037/a0037173. PMC 4152423. PMID 24979188.
- ^ a b Jakob Pietschnig; Martin Voracek (May 1, 2015). "One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-Analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (3): 282–306. doi:10.1177/1745691615577701. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 25987509. S2CID 12604392.
- ^ Raven John (2000). "The Raven's Progressive Matrices: Change and Stability over Culture and Time" (PDF). Cognitive Psychology. 41 (1): 1–48. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0735. PMID 10945921. S2CID 26363133. Archived (PDF) from the original on April 28, 2019. Retrieved July 9, 2011.
- ^ a b c Teasdale, T. (1989). "Continuing secular increases in intelligence and a stable prevalence of high intelligence levels". Intelligence. 13 (3): 255–62. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(89)90021-4.
- ^ a b Colom, R.; Lluis-Font, J.M. & Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2005). "The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: Supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis" (PDF). Intelligence. 33 (1): 83–91. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010. Archived (PDF) from the original on August 13, 2012. Retrieved October 4, 2012.
- ^ Tucker, Ian (September 22, 2012). "James Flynn: IQ may go up as well as down". The Guardian. ISSN 0261-3077. Archived from the original on January 21, 2019. Retrieved January 21, 2019.
- ^ Calhoun, Daniel (1973). The Intelligence of a People. Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-04619-8.
- ^ Thorndike, Robert L. (1975). "Mr. Binet's Test 70 Years Later". Educational Researcher. 4 (5): 3–7. doi:10.3102/0013189X004005003. ISSN 0013-189X. JSTOR 1174855. S2CID 145355731.
- ^ Lynn, Richard (May 1982). "IQ in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity". Nature. 297 (5863): 222–223. Bibcode:1982Natur.297..222L. doi:10.1038/297222a0. ISSN 1476-4687. S2CID 4331657.
- ^ Must O, Must A, Raudik V (2003). "The secular rise in IQs: In Estonia, the Flynn effect is not a Jensen effect" (PDF). Intelligence. 31 (5): 461–71. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00013-8. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 11, 2017. Retrieved September 13, 2011.
- ^ Wicherts, J.M.; Dolan, C.V.; Hessen, D.J.; Oosterveld, P.; Baal, G.C.M. van; Boomsma, D.I. & Span, M.M. (2004). "Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect" (PDF). Intelligence. 32 (5): 509537. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.207.4350. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002. Archived from the original (PDF) on May 29, 2005.
The overall conclusion of the present paper is that factorial invariance with respect to cohorts is not tenable . . . . The fact that the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure, should not sit well with explanations that appeal solely to changes at the level of the latent variables.
- ^ a b Te Nijenhuis J, De Jong MJ, Evers A, Van Der Flier H (2004). "Are cognitive differences between immigrant and majority groups diminishing?" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 18 (5): 405–34. doi:10.1002/per.511. S2CID 4806581. Archived (PDF) from the original on February 21, 2021. Retrieved November 10, 2019.
- ^ Colom R, Garcia-Lopez O (2003). "Secular Gains in Fluid Intelligence: Evidence from the Culture-Fair Intelligence Test". Journal of Biosocial Science. 35 (1): 33–39. doi:10.1017/S0021932003000336. PMID 12537154. S2CID 24493926.
- ^ Flynn, James Robert; Te Nijenhuis, Jan; Metzen, Daniel (May–June 2014). "The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses". Intelligence. 44: 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.009. Retrieved July 2, 2023.
- ^ Plomin R.; DeFries J.C.; Craig I.W.; McGuffin P. (2003). Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era (4th ed.).
- ^ Marks, David Francis (June 1, 2010). "IQ Variations across Time, Race, and Nationality: An Artifact of Differences in Literacy Skills". Psychological Reports. 106 (3): 643–664. doi:10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664. ISSN 0033-2941. PMID 20712152 – via Sage Publishing.
- ^ Jantz, R.; Meadows Jantz, L. (2000). "Secular change in craniofacial morphology". American Journal of Human Biology. 12 (3): 327–38. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(200005/06)12:3<327::AID-AJHB3>3.0.CO;2-1. PMID 11534023. S2CID 22059721.
- ^ "The Lancet Series on Maternal and Child Undernutrition". 2008. Archived from the original on July 17, 2011. Retrieved February 11, 2011.
- ^ C. Banning (1946). "Food Shortage and Public Health, First Half of 1945". Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science Vol. 245, The Netherlands during German Occupation (May 1946), pp. 93–110
- ^ Flynn J.R. (1987). "Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure". Psychological Bulletin. 101 (2): 171–91. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171.
- ^ Flynn, James R. (2009). What Is Intelligence? (p. 103). Cambridge University Press. Kindle Edition.
- ^ Samaras, Thomas T.; Elrick, Harold (May 2002). "Group Height, body size, and longevity: is smaller better for the human body?". West J Med. 176 (3): 206–08. doi:10.1136/ewjm.176.3.206. PMC 1071721. PMID 12016250.
- ^ a b Sundet, J.; Barlaug, D.; Torjussen, T. (2004). "The end of the Flynn effect?: A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century". Intelligence. 32 (4): 349–62. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.004.
- ^ Jantz RL, Meadows Jantz L (May 2000). "Secular change in craniofacial morphology". Am. J. Hum. Biol. 12 (3): 327–38. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(200005/06)12:3<327::AID-AJHB3>3.0.CO;2-1. PMID 11534023. S2CID 22059721.
Jantz RL (July 2001). "Cranial change in Americans: 1850–1975". J. Forensic Sci. 46 (4): 784–87. doi:10.1520/JFS15047J. PMID 11451056. - ^ Qian M; Wang D; Watkins WE; et al. (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 14 (1): 32–42. PMID 15734706.
- ^ Nisen, Max (July 22, 2013). "How Adding Iodine To Salt Resulted In A Decade's Worth Of IQ Gains For The United States". Business Insider. Retrieved January 23, 2023.
- ^ Daley, TC; Whaley, SE; Sigman, MD; Espinosa, MP; Neumann, C (May 2003). "IQ on the rise: the Flynn effect in rural Kenyan children". Psychological Science. 14 (3): 215–19. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02434. PMID 12741743. S2CID 12315212.
- ^ Dickens WT, Flynn JR (2001). "Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved" (PDF). Psychological Review. 108 (2): 346–369. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.139.2436. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346. PMID 11381833.
- ^ Dickens WT, Flynn JR (2002). "The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved" (PDF). Psychological Review. 109 (4): 764–71. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.764. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 19, 2007.
- ^ Flynn, James R. (August 2012). What Is Intelligence? Beyond the Flynn Effect. Cambridge University Press. pp. 24–29. ISBN 9780511605253.
- ^ Gladwell, Malcolm (December 10, 2007). "None of the Above". The New Yorker. Retrieved July 6, 2024.
- ^ Eppig C, Fincher CL, Thornhill R (2011). "Parasite prevalence and the distribution of intelligence among the states of the USA". Intelligence. 39 (2–3): 155–60. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.02.008.
- ^ Venkataramani A (September 18, 2010). "Early Life Exposure to Malaria and Cognition and Skills in Adulthood: Evidence from Mexico". SSRN 1679164.
- ^ Fernando SD, Rodrigo C, Rajapakse S (2010). "The 'hidden' burden of malaria: cognitive impairment following infection". Malar. J. 9 (1): 366. doi:10.1186/1475-2875-9-366. PMC 3018393. PMID 21171998.
- ^ Mingroni, M. A. (2007). "Resolving the IQ paradox: Heterosis as a cause of the Flynn effect and other trends" (PDF). Psychological Review, 114(3), 806–829. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.806
- ^ Mingroni, M.A. (2004). "The secular rise in IQ: Giving heterosis a second look" (PDF). Intelligence, 32, 65-83.
- ^ Mackintosh, N.J. (2011). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 291.
- ^ Kaufman, Alan S.; Zhou, Xiaobin; Reynolds, Matthew R.; Kaufman, Nadeen L.; Green, Garo P.; Weisse, Lawrence G. (July 2014). "The possible societal impact of the decrease in U.S. blood lead levels on adult IQ". Environmental Research. 132: 413–420. Bibcode:2014ER....132..413K. doi:10.1016/j.envres.2014.04.015. PMID 24853978.
- ^ Teasdale, Thomas W.; Owen, David R. (2005). "A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (4): 837–43. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.029.
- ^ a b Teasdale TW, Owen DR (2008). "Secular declines in cognitive test scores: A reversal of the Flynn Effect" (PDF). Intelligence. 36 (2): 121–26. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.01.007. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 15, 2012. Retrieved April 18, 2010.
- ^ Cotton, S.M.; Kiely, P.M.; Crewther, D.P.; Thomson, B.; Laycock, R.; Crewther, S.G. (2005). "A normative and reliability study for the Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices for primary school aged children in Australia". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (3): 647–60. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.015.
- ^ Flynn, J.R. (2009). "Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938–2008". Economics & Human Biology. 7 (1): 18–27. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009. PMID 19251490.
- ^ Pesovski, Ivica; Kulakov, Andrea; Trajkovikj, Vladimir (2022). "Differences in cognitive ability assessment results between Millennial and Generation Z cohorts". The 19th International Conference on Informatics and Information Technologies – CIIT 2022.
- ^ Bratsberg, Bernt; Rogeberg, Ole (June 26, 2018). "Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (26): 6674–6678. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.6674B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1718793115. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 6042097. PMID 29891660.
- ^ Winter, Emily L.; Trudel, Sierra M.; Kaufman, Alan S. (November 15, 2024). "Wait, Where's the Flynn Effect on the WAIS-5?". Journal of Intelligence. 12 (11): 118. doi:10.3390/jintelligence12110118. ISSN 2079-3200. PMC 11595985.
- ^ Vincent, Ken R. (March 1991). "Black/white IQ differences: Does age make the difference?". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 47 (2): 266–270. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2<266::aid-jclp2270470213>3.0.co;2-s. PMID 2030133.
- ^ Dickens & Flynn 2006.
- ^ Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association
- ^ Dickens, William T; Flynn, James R (2006). "Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples" (PDF). Psychological Science. 17 (10): 913–20. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.186.2540. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x. PMID 17100793. S2CID 6593169. Archived from the original (PDF) on October 9, 2009.
- ^ Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and Human Intelligence (second ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-958559-5.
- ^ Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012a). "Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments" (PDF). American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22233090. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 8, 2020. Retrieved July 22, 2013.
- ^ Willingham, Dan (May 10, 2012). "The latest on intelligence". Daniel Willingham—Science & Education.
- ^ Flynn, J.R. (2010). "The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate". Intelligence. 38 (4): 363–66. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001.
- ^ Wicherts, Jelte M.; Dolan, Conor V.; Hessen, David J.; Oosterveld, Paul; van Baal, G. Caroline M.; Boomsma, Dorret I.; Span, Mark M. (2004). "Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect". Intelligence. 32 (5): 509–537. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002.
It appears therefore that the nature of the Flynn effect is qualitatively different from the nature of B–W differences in the United States. Each comparison of groups should be investigated separately. IQ gaps between cohorts do not teach us anything about IQ gaps between contemporary groups, except that each IQ gap should not be confused with real (i.e., latent) differences in intelligence.
- ^ Flynn, J.R. (1999). "Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time" (PDF). American Psychologist. 54: 5–9. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5. Archived (PDF) from the original on June 25, 2010. Retrieved October 26, 2017.
Further reading
- te Nijenhuis, Jan, & van der Flier, H. (2013). "Is the Flynn effect on g?: A meta-analysis." Intelligence, 41(6), 802–807. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.001
- Rodgers, Joseph Lee (1998). "A critique of the Flynn Effect: Massive IQ gains, methodological artifacts, or both?" Intelligence, 26(4), 337–356. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00004-5
- Flynn, James R. (1984). "The Mean IQ of Americans: Massive Gains 1932 to 1978" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 95: 29–51. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.29. Archived (PDF) from the original on March 10, 2012. Retrieved May 16, 2013.
- Flynn, James R. (March 1987). "Massive IQ Gains in 14 Nations: What IQ Tests Really Measure" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 101 (2): 171–91. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171. Archived (PDF) from the original on January 24, 2011. Retrieved May 13, 2013.
- Flynn, James R. (2012). Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-first century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1-107-60917-4.
- Winerman, Lea (March 2013). "Smarter than ever?". Monitor on Psychology. 44 (3): 30.
- Ulric Neisser; James R. Flynn; Carmi Schooler; Patricia M. Greenfield; Wendy M. Williams; Marian Sigman; Shannon E. Whaley; Reynaldo Martorell; Richard Lynn; Robert M. Hauser; David W. Grissmer; Stephanie Williamson; Sheila Nataraj Kirby; Mark Berends; Stephen J. Ceci; Tina B. Rosenblum; Matthew Kumpf; Min-Hsiung Huang; Irwin D. Waldman; Samuel H. Preston; John C. Loehlin (1998). Neisser, Ulric (ed.). The Rising Curve: Long-Term Gains in IQ and Related Measures. APA Science Volume Series. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1-55798-503-3.
- Tuddenham, Read D. (1948), "Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II", American Psychologist, 3 (2): 54–56, doi:10.1037/h0054962, PMID 18911933
External links
- Flynn, James Robert (December 15, 2006). "Beyond the Flynn Effect". University of Cambridge. Archived from the original on October 11, 2017. Retrieved May 30, 2024.
- Graham, Charles (2001). "The Flynn Effect". Indiana University. Archived from the original on October 25, 2005. Retrieved May 30, 2024.
- Holloway, Marguerite (January 1, 1999). "Flynn's Effect". Scientific American. 280 (1): 37–38. Bibcode:1999SciAm.280a..37H. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0199-37. Archived from the original on September 10, 2005.
- Dickens, William Theodore; Flynn, James Robert (2001). "Heritability Estimates Versus Large Environmental Effects: The IQ Paradox Resolved". Psychological Review. 108 (2): 346–369. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346. eISSN 1939-1471. ISSN 0033-295X – via American Psychological Association.
- Johnson, Steven (May 1, 2005). "Dome Improvement". Wired. eISSN 1078-3148. ISSN 1059-1028. Archived from the original on October 19, 2016.
- Heylighen, Francis Paul (August 22, 2000). "Increasing intelligence: the Flynn effect". Principia Cybernetica. Archived from the original on July 4, 2007. Retrieved May 30, 2024.