Jump to content

List of optical illusions: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Use Kanizsa triangle as an example of illusory contour
because
 
(146 intermediate revisions by 91 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|none}}
List of '''[[optical illusion]]s'''

{| class="wikitable sortable" style="width:100%; height:100px;" cellpadding="5"
{{dynamic list}}

This is a list of [[visual illusion]]s.

{| class="wikitable sortable" style="width:100%; height:100%;" cellpadding="5"
|-
|-
! style="width:21%;"|Name
! Name
! style="width:12%;"|Example 1
! Example
! Notes
! style="width:12%;"|Example 2
! style="width:55%;"|Description/Remarks
|-
|-
|[[Afterimage effect|Afterimage illusion]]
|[[Afterimage]] illusion
|[[File:Afterimagenpov.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Afterimagenpov.svg|120px]]
|An '''afterimage''' or ghost image is a visual illusion involving an image continuing to appear in one's vision after the exposure to the original image has ceased.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Afterimage on empty shape]] (also known as [[color dove illusion]])
|[[Afterimage on empty shape]] (also known as [[color dove illusion]])
|[[File:Afterimage.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Afterimage.svg|120px]]
|This type of illusion is designed to exploit graphical similarities.
|
|An afterimage or ghost image is an optical illusion that refers to an image continuing to appear in one's vision after the exposure to the original image has ceased.
|-
|-
|[[Ambiguous image]]
|[[Ambiguous image]]
|[[File:Duck-Rabbit illusion.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:Duck-Rabbit illusion.jpg|120px]]
|These are images that can form two separate pictures. For example, the image shown forms a rabbit and a duck.
|
|
|-
|[[Ambigram]]
|[[File:Ambigram of the word ambigram - rotation animation.gif|120px]]
|A calligraphic design that has multiple or symmetric interpretations.
|-
|-
|[[Ames room]] illusion
|[[Ames room]] illusion
|[[File:Ames room.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Ames room.svg|120px]]
|An '''Ames room''' is a distorted room that is used to create a visual illusion.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Ames trapezoid|Ames trapezoid window]] illusion
|[[Ames trapezoid|Ames trapezoid window]] illusion
|[[File:Ames window.png|120px]]
|
|A window is formed in the shape of a trapezium. It is often hung and spun around to provide the illusion that the window rotates through less than 180 degrees.
|
|An Ames room is a distorted room that is used to create an optical illusion.
|-
|[[ASCII stereogram]]
|
|
|
|-
|[[Autokinesis]] visual illusion
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Autokinetic effect]]
|[[Autokinetic effect]]
|
|
|The '''autokinetic effect''', or autokinesis, occurs when a stationary image appears to move.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Autostereogram]]
|[[Autostereogram]]
|[[File:Stereogram Tut Random Dot Shark.png|100px]]
|[[File:Stereogram Tut Random Dot Shark.png|120px]]<br>[[File:Stereogram Tut Eye Object Size.png|120px]]
|An '''autostereogram''' is a single-image stereogram (SIS), designed to create the visual illusion of a three-dimensional (3D) scene from a two-dimensional image in the human brain. An [[ASCII stereogram]] is an image that is formed using characters on a keyboard. ''[[Magic Eye]]'' is an autostereogram book series.
|[[File:Stereogram Tut Eye Object Size.png|100px]]
|An autostereogram is a single-image stereogram (SIS), designed to create the visual illusion of a three-dimensional (3D) scene from a two-dimensional image in the human brain.
|-
|-
|[[Barberpole illusion]]
|[[Barberpole illusion]]
|[[File:Barberpole illusion animated.gif|100px]]
|[[File:Barberpole illusion animated.gif|120px]]
|
|The [[barber pole]] illusion is a visual illusion that reveals biases in the processing of visual motion in the human brain.
|The [[barber pole]] illusion is a visual illusion that reveals biases in the processing of visual motion in the human brain.
|-
|-
|[[Benham's top]]
|[[Benham's top]]
|[[File:Benham's Disc.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Benham's Disc.svg|120px]]
|When a disk that has lines or colours on it is spun, it can form arcs of colour.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Beta movement]]
|[[Beta movement]]
|[[File:Phi phenomenom no watermark.gif|100px]]
|[[File:Phi phenomenom no watermark.gif|120px]]
|Movement that appears to occur when fixed pictures turn on and off.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Bezold Effect]]
|[[Bezold Effect]]
|[[File:Bezold Effect.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Bezold Effect.svg|120px]]
|An apparent change of tone of a colour due to the alteration of the colour of the background.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Blivet]]
|[[Blivet]]
|[[File:Blivet2.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Poiuyt.svg|120px]]
|Also known as "poiuyt" or "devil's fork", this illusion is an impossible image because in reality the shape cannot exist.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Café wall illusion]]
|[[Café wall illusion]]
|[[File:Café wall.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Café wall.svg|120px]]
|This illusion is a pattern in which the [[Mortar (masonry)|mortar]] or [[grout]] between different coloured bricks or tiles on a wall appear to form non-parallel, straight lines, despite the lines being parallel (and straight). Its name comes from a café wall that produced the illusion.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Catoptric cistula]]
|[[Catoptric cistula]]
|[[File:Catoptric theatre.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:Catoptric theatre.jpg|120px]]
|A catoptric cistula is a box with insides made of mirrors so as to distort images of objects put into the box.
|
|
|-
|[[Checker shadow illusion]]
|[[File:Checker shadow illusion.svg|120px]]<br>[[File:Grey square optical illusion proof2.svg|120px]]
|The '''checker shadow illusion''' shows that when a shadow is cast onto a checked board, the colours of squares A and B in the photos appear to be different, when in fact they are the same.
|-
|-
|[[Chubb illusion]]
|[[Chubb illusion]]
|[[File:Chubbillusion.gif|100px]]
|[[File:Chubbillusion.gif|120px]]
|The '''Chubb illusion''' is an optical illusion or error in visual perception in which the apparent contrast of an object varies substantially to most viewers depending on its relative contrast to the field on which it is displayed.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Color constancy]]
|[[Color constancy]]
|[[File:Grey square optical illusion.PNG|100px]]
|[[File:Hot air balloon - color constancy.jpg|120px]]
|'''Colour constancy''' is an example of subjective constancy and a feature of the human color perception system which ensures that the perceived color of objects remains relatively constant under varying illumination conditions. A green apple for instance looks green to us at midday, when the main illumination is white sunlight, and also at sunset, when the main illumination is red.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Color Phi phenomenon]]
|[[Color phi phenomenon]]
|
|
|
|
|The '''color phi phenomenon''' is a perceptual illusion in which a disembodied perception of motion is produced by a succession of still images.
|-
|-
|[[Contingent perceptual aftereffect]]
|[[Contingent perceptual aftereffect]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Convergence micropsia]]
|[[Convergence micropsia]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Cornsweet illusion]]
|[[Cornsweet illusion]]
|[[File:Cornsweet illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Cornsweet illusion.svg|120px]]
|An illusion where two colours can obviously be seen to be different when placed directly beside each other; however, when the two colours are separated by a thick black line, they appear to be of the same hue.
|
|
|-
|[[Crater illusion]]
|[[File:Tycho LRO.png|120px]]
|A type of multistable illusion where an image of a concave object, rotated so that the light source is below, may sometimes appear convex, and vice versa. This phenomenon is because light sources tend to shine from above the subject.
|-
|-
|[[Delboeuf illusion]]
|[[Delboeuf illusion]]
|[[File:Delboeuf.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:Delboeuf.svg|120px]]
|An optical illusion of relative size perception. The two black circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the left seems larger.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Disappearing Model]]
|''[[Disappearing Model]]''
|[[File:Disappearing Model.jpg|100px]]
|
|
|
|A trompe-l'œil body painting by [[Joanne Gair]].
|-
| ''[[The dress]]''
|<br>[[File:Wikipe-tan wearing The Dress reduced.svg|120px|alt=Two ways in which the photograph of ''The dress'' may be perceived: {{blist|blue and black under a yellow-tinted illumination (left figure) or |white and gold under a blue-tinted illumination (right figure).}}]]
| An optical illusion resulting from the brain's attempt to discount coloured tinting from daylight and other sources.<ref>{{cite magazine |title = The Science of Why No One Agrees on the Colour of This Dress |url = https://www.wired.com/2015/02/science-one-agrees-color-dress/ |magazine = Wired |access-date = 27 February 2015 |date = 2015-02-27 |archive-date = 28 February 2015 |archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20150228232822/http://www.wired.com/2015/02/science-one-agrees-color-dress/ |url-status = live }}</ref> The dress was a viral phenomenon, which was either seen as blue and black or as white and gold. Its true colours were eventually confirmed to be blue and black.<ref>{{cite news|title=Optical illusion: Dress color debate goes global|work=BBC News|publisher=BBC|url=https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-scotland-highlands-islands-31656935|url-status=live|access-date=28 February 2015|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190713222129/https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-scotland-highlands-islands-31656935|archive-date=2019-07-13}}</ref>
|-
|-
|[[Ebbinghaus illusion]]
|[[Ebbinghaus illusion]]
|[[File:Mond-vergleich.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Mond-vergleich.svg|120px]]
|The '''Ebbinghaus illusion''', or Titchener circles, is an optical illusion of relative size perception. The two orange circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the right appears larger.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Ehrenstein illusion]]
|[[Ehrenstein illusion]]
|[[File:Ehrenstein.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Ehrenstein.svg|120px]]<br>[[File:Ehrenstein Illusion.svg|120px]]
|[[File:Ehrenstein2.gif|100px]]
|The '''Ehrenstein illusion''' is an optical illusion studied by the German psychologist [[Walter Ehrenstein]] in which the sides of a square placed inside a pattern of concentric circles take an apparent curved shape.
|The '''Ehrenstein illusion''' is an optical illusion studied by the German psychologist [[Walter Ehrenstein]] in which the sides of a square placed inside a pattern of concentric circles take an apparent curved shape.
|-
|[[Fata Morgana (mirage)]]
|[[File:Fata Morgana Example.jpg|120px]]
|Visible in a narrow band right above the horizon, Fata Morgana mirages significantly distort the object or objects on which they are based, often such that the object is completely unrecognizable. A Fata Morgana may be seen on land or at sea, in polar regions, or in deserts. It may involve almost any kind of distant object, including boats, islands, and the coastline.
|-
|-
|[[Fechner color]]
|[[Fechner color]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Figure-ground (perception)]]
|[[Figure-ground (perception)]]
|[[File:Cup or faces paradox.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Cup or faces paradox.svg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Filling-in]]
|[[Filling-in]]
|[[File:Troxler fading.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Troxler fading.svg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Flash lag illusion]]
|[[Flash lag illusion]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Forced perspective]]
|[[Forced perspective]]
|[[File:Europe 2007 Disk 1 340.jpg|120px]]
|
|Application used in film and architecture to create the illusion of larger, more distant objects.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Fraser spiral illusion]]
|[[Fraser spiral illusion]]
|[[File:Fraser spiral.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Fraser spiral.svg|120px]]
|The '''Fraser spiral illusion''', or false spiral, or the twisted cord illusion, was first described by the British psychologist Sir James Fraser in 1908. The overlapping black arc segments appear to form a spiral; however, the arcs are a series of concentric circles.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Gravity hill]]
|[[Gravity hill]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Grid illusion]]
|[[Grid illusion]]
|[[File:HermannGrid.gif|100px]]
|[[File:HermannGrid.svg|120px]]<br/>[[File:Grid illusion.svg|120px]]
|Any kind of grid that deceives a person's vision. The two most common types of grid illusions are the Hermann grid illusion (1870) and the scintillating grid illusion (1994). The first is characterized by "ghostlike" grey blobs perceived at the intersections of a white (or light-colored) grid on a black background. The grey blobs disappear when looking directly at an intersection. The second is constructed by superimposing white discs on the intersections of orthogonal gray bars on a black background. Dark dots seem to appear and disappear rapidly at random intersections, hence the label "scintillating". When a person keeps their eyes directly on a single intersection, the dark dot does not appear. The dark dots disappear if one is too close to or too far from the image.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Hering illusion]]
|[[Hering illusion]]
|[[File:Hering illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Hering illusion.svg|120px]]
|The '''Hering illusion''' (1861): When two straight and parallel lines are presented in front of radial background (like the spokes of a bicycle), the lines appear as if they were bowed outwards.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Hollow-Face illusion]]
|[[Hollow-Face illusion]]
|[[File:Bjorn Borg Hollow Face.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:Bjorn Borg Hollow Face.jpg|120px]]
|
|The '''Hollow-Face illusion''' is an optical illusion in which the perception of a concave mask of a face appears as a normal convex face.
|The '''Hollow-Face illusion''' is an optical illusion in which the perception of a concave mask of a face appears as a normal convex face.
|-
|-
|[[Hybrid image]]
|[[Hybrid image]]
|[[File:Hybrid image decomposition.jpg|120px]]
|
|A '''Hybrid image''' is an optical illusion developed at MIT in which an image can be interpreted in one of two different ways depending on viewing distance.
|
|A '''Hybrid Image''' is an optical illusion developed at MIT in which an image can be interpreted in one of two different ways depending on viewing distance.
|-
|-
|[[Illusory contours]]
|[[Illusory contours]]
|[[File:Kanizsa triangle.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Kanizsa triangle.svg|120px]]
|
|'''Illusory contours''' or '''subjective contours''' are a form of visual illusion where contours are perceived without a luminance or color change across the contour.
|'''Illusory contours''' or '''subjective contours''' are a form of visual illusion where contours are perceived without a luminance or color change across the contour.
|-
|[[Illusory motion]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Impossible object]]
|[[Impossible object]]
|[[File:Impossible cube illusion angle.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Impossible cube illusion angle.svg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Irradiation illusion]]
|[[Irradiation illusion]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Isometric illusion]]
|[[Isometric illusion]]
|[[File:Cubes at Heureka, optical illusion.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:Cubes at Heureka, optical illusion.jpg|120px]]
|
|An '''isometric illusion''' (also called an '''ambiguous figure''' or '''inside/outside illusion''') is a type of optical illusion, specifically one due to [[multistable perception]].
|An '''isometric illusion''' (also called an '''ambiguous figure''' or '''inside/outside illusion''') is a type of optical illusion, specifically one due to [[multistable perception]].
|-
|-
|[[Jastrow illusion]]
|[[Jastrow illusion]]
|[[File:Jastrow illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Jastrow illusion.svg|120px]]
|
|The '''Jastrow illusion''' is an [[optical illusion]] discovered by the American psychologist [[Joseph Jastrow]] in 1889.
|The '''Jastrow illusion''' is an [[optical illusion]] discovered by the American psychologist [[Joseph Jastrow]] in 1889.
|-
|-
|[[Kanizsa triangle]]
|[[Kanizsa triangle]]
|[[File:Kanizsa triangle.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Kanizsa triangle.svg|120px]]
|The '''Kanizsa triangle''' is an optical illusion first described by the Italian psychologist [[Gaetano Kanizsa]] in 1955. It is a triangle formed of illusory contours.
|
|-
|The '''Kanizsa triangle''' is an optical illusion first described by the Italian psychologist [[Gaetano Kanizsa]] in 1955. It is a triangle formed of [[illusory contours]].
|[[Kinetic Depth Effect]]
|<!-- Deleted image removed: [[File:An Example of The Kinetic Depth Effect.gif|120px]] -->
|The '''Kinetic depth effect''' is the phenomenon whereby the three-dimensional structural form of a silhouette can be perceived when the object is moving. In the absence of other visual depth cues, this might be the only perception mechanism available to infer the object's shape. Additionally the direction of motion can reverse due to the existence of multiple 3D visual solutions.
|-
|-
|-
|[[Leaning tower illusion]]
|[[Leaning tower illusion]]
|
|
|The '''Leaning tower illusion''' is an optical illusion that presents two identical images of the [[Leaning Tower of Pisa]] side by side.
|
|The '''Leaning Tower Illusion''' is an optical illusion that presents two identical images of the [[Leaning Tower of Pisa]] side by side.
|-
|-
|[[Lilac chaser]]
|[[Lilac chaser]]
|[[File:Lilac-Chaser.gif|100px]]
|[[File:Lilac-Chaser.gif|120px]]
|
|'''Lilac chaser''' is a [[visual illusion]], also known as the '''[[Pac-Man]] illusion'''.
|'''Lilac chaser''' is a [[visual illusion]], also known as the '''[[Pac-Man]] illusion'''.
|-
|-
Line 240: Line 211:
|
|
|
|
|-
|[[Lunar terminator#Lunar terminator illusion|Lunar terminator illusion]]
|
|
|'''Lunar terminator illusion''' is an optical illusion where the apparent source of sunlight illuminating the moon does not corresponding with the actual position of the sun.
|-
|-
|[[Mach bands]]
|[[Mach bands]]
|[[File:Mach band.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Mach band.svg|120px]]
|
|'''Mach bands''' is an optical illusion named after the physicist [[Ernst Mach]].
|'''Mach bands''' is an optical illusion named after the physicist [[Ernst Mach]].
|-
|[[Magic Eye]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[McCollough effect]]
|[[McCollough effect]]
|[[File:Grid for McCollough effect.png|100px]]
|[[File:Grid for McCollough effect.svg|120px]]
|The '''McCollough effect''' (1965) is a phenomenon of human visual perception in which colorless gratings appear colored contingent on the orientation of the gratings. It is an aftereffect requiring a period of induction to produce it.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Missing square puzzle]]
|[[Missing square puzzle]]
|[[File:Missing square puzzle.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Missing square puzzle.svg|120px]]<br>[[File:Missing square edit.gif|120px]]
|[[File:Missing square edit.gif|100px]]
|The '''missing square puzzle''' is an optical illusion used in [[mathematics]] classes to help students reason about geometrical figures.
|The '''missing square puzzle''' is an optical illusion used in [[mathematics]] classes to help students reason about geometrical figures.
|-
|-
|[[Moon illusion]]
|[[Moon illusion]]
|[[File:Moonillusion.png|100px]]
|[[File:Moon size illusion.png|120px]]
|
|The '''Moon illusion''' is an optical illusion in which the [[Moon]] appears larger near the [[horizon]] than it does while higher up in the [[sky]].
|The '''Moon illusion''' is an optical illusion in which the [[Moon]] appears larger near the [[horizon]] than it does while higher up in the [[sky]].
|-
|-
|[[Motion aftereffect]]
|[[Motion aftereffect]]
|[[File:Illusion movie.ogg|100px]]
|[[File:Illusion movie.ogg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Motion illusion]]
|[[Motion illusion]]
|[[File:Anomalous motion illusion1.png|100px]]
|[[File:Anomalous motion illusion1.svg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Müller-Lyer illusion]]
|[[Müller-Lyer illusion]]
|[[File:Müller-Lyer illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Müller-Lyer illusion.svg|120px]]
|
|The '''Müller-Lyer illusion''' is an optical illusion consisting of a stylized arrow.
|The '''Müller-Lyer illusion''' is an optical illusion consisting of a stylized arrow.
|-
|-
|[[Multistable perception]]
|[[Multistability]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Musion Eyeliner]]
|[[Necker cube]]
|[[File:Necker cube.svg|120px]]
|
|The '''Necker cube''' is an optical illusion first published in 1832 by [[Switzerland|Swiss]] [[crystallography|crystallographer]] [[Louis Albert Necker]].
|
|
|-
|[[Necker cube illusion]]
|[[File:Necker cube and impossible cube.svg|100px]]
|
|The '''Necker Cube''' is an optical illusion first published in 1832 by [[Switzerland|Swiss]] [[crystallography|crystallographer]] [[Louis Albert Necker]].
|-
|[[Necker Cube]]
|[[File:Necker cube.svg|100px]]
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Numerosity adaptation effect]]
|[[Numerosity adaptation effect]]
|[[File:Numerosityadaptation.png|100px]]
|[[File:Numerosityadaptation.png|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Orbison illusion]]
|[[Orbison illusion]]
|[[File:Orbison illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Orbison illusion.svg|120px]]
|The '''Orbison illusion''' is an optical illusion that was first described by the psychologist [[William Orbison]] in 1939.
|
|-
|The '''Orbison illusion''' is an optical illusion that was first described by the psychologist Roy Orbison in 1939.
|[[Oppel-Kundt illusion]]
|[[File:Fig OK.tif|120px]]
|The '''Oppel-Kundt illusion''' is an optical illusion named after German physicists {{ill|Johann Joseph Oppel|de}} (first mentioned this phenomenon in 1860) and [[August Kundt]] (first performed a systematic study of the illusion in 1863).
|-
|-
|[[Penrose stairs]]
|[[Penrose stairs]]
|[[File:Impossible staircase.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Impossible staircase.svg|120px]]
|The '''Penrose stairs''' were created by [[Lionel Penrose]] and his son [[Roger Penrose]].<ref name=p1958>{{cite journal |first1=LS |last1=Penrose |first2=R. |last2=Penrose |title=Impossible objects: A special type of optical illusion |journal=British Journal of Psychology |volume=49 |issue=1 |year=1958 |pages=31–33 |doi=10.1111/j.2044-8295.1958.tb00634.x |pmid=13536303}}</ref> A variation on the [[Penrose triangle]], it is a two-dimensional depiction of a staircase in which the stairs make four 90-degree turns as they ascend or descend yet form a continuous loop, so that a person could climb them forever and never get any higher.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Penrose triangle]]
|[[Penrose triangle]]
|[[File:Penrose triangle.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Penrose triangle.svg|120px]]
|The '''Penrose triangle''' was first created by the Swedish artist [[Oscar Reutersvärd]] in 1934. The [[mathematician]] [[Roger Penrose]] independently devised and popularised it in the 1950s, describing it as "impossibility in its purest form".
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Pepper's ghost]]
|[[Pepper's ghost]]
|[[File:ToT PeppersGhost.jpg|100px]]
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Perceived visual angle]]
|[[Perceived visual angle]]
|[[File:VisualAngleAboveHorizonDefs.svg|100px]]
|[[File:VisualAngleAboveHorizonDefs.svg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Peripheral drift illusion]]
|[[Peripheral drift illusion]]
|[[File:PDIFaubertHerbert.png|100px]]
|[[File:PDIFaubertHerbert.png|120px]]
|A motion illusion (1979/1999) generated by the presentation of a sawtooth luminance grating in the visual periphery.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Phantogram]]
|[[Phantogram (optical illusion)|Phantogram]]
|[[File:Phantogram projection diagram.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Phantogram projection diagram.svg|120px]]
|'''Phantograms''', also known as Phantaglyphs, Op-Ups, free-standing anaglyphs, levitated images, and book anaglyphs, are a form of optical illusion.
|
|Phantograms, also known as Phantaglyphs, Op-Ups, free-standing anaglyphs, levitated images, and book anaglyphs, are a form of optical illusion.
|-
|-
|[[Phi phenomenon]]
|[[Phi phenomenon]]
|
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Poggendorff illusion]]
|[[Poggendorff illusion]]
|[[File:Poggendorff illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Poggendorff illusion.svg|120px]]
|The '''Poggendorff illusion''' (1860) involves the misperception of the position of one segment of a transverse line that has been interrupted by the contour of an intervening structure (here a rectangle).
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Poiuyt]]
|[[Ponzo illusion]]
|[[File:Ponzo illusion.gif|120px]]
|
|In the '''Ponzo illusion''' (1911) two identical lines across a pair of converging lines, similar to railway tracks, are drawn. The upper line looks longer because we interpret the converging sides according to linear perspective as parallel lines receding into the distance. In this context, we interpret the upper line as though it were farther away, so we see it as longer – a farther object would have to be longer than a nearer one for both to produce retinal images of the same size.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Ponzo illusion]]
|[[Pulfrich effect]]
|[[File:Ponzo illusion.gif|100px]]
|
|
|
|The '''Pulfrich effect''' is the effect that covering one eye with transparent but darkened glass can cause purely lateral motion to appear to have a depth component even though in reality it doesn't; even a completely flat scene such as one shown on a television screen can appear to exhibit some three-dimensional motion, but this is an illusion because darkening the scene for one eye causes the photoreceptors in that eye to respond more slowly.
|-
|-
|[[Rubin vase]]
|[[Rubin vase]]
|[[File:Rubin2.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:Face or vase ata 01.svg|120px]]
|'''Rubin vase''' (1915): an ambiguous or bi-stable (i.e., reversing) two-dimensional form.
|
|
|-
|[[Same color]]
|[[File:Grey square optical illusion.PNG|100px]]
|[[File:Same color illusion proof2.png|100px]]
|
|-
|-
|[[Sander illusion]]
|[[Sander illusion]]
|[[File:Sander Illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Sander Illusion.svg|120px]]
|In '''Sander's parallelogram''' (1926) the diagonal line bisecting the larger, left-hand parallelogram appears to be considerably longer than the diagonal line bisecting the smaller, right-hand parallelogram, but is in fact the same length.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Silencing]]
|[[Motion silencing illusion|Silencing]]
|[[File:silencinghue.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:silencinghue.jpg|120px]]
|'''Silencing''' is an illusion in which a set of objects that change in luminance, hue, size, or shape appears to stop changing when it moves.
|
|Silencing is an illusion in which a set of objects that change in luminance, hue, size, or shape appears to stop changing when it moves.
|-
|-
|[[Size-weight illusion]]
|[[Size–weight illusion]]
|
|
|
|The size-weight illusion is also known as the Charpentier illusion (or Charpentier-Koseleff illusion).
|The '''size–weight illusion''' is also known as the Charpentier illusion or Charpentier–Koseleff illusion.
|-
|[[Stepping feet illusion]]
|[[File:Stepping-Feet-Motion-Illusion.gif|120px]]
|The '''stepping feet illusion''' is influenced by the contrast between moving objects and their background.
|-
|-
|[[Stroboscopic effect]]
|[[Stroboscopic effect]]
|[[File:Strobe 2.gif|100px]]
|[[File:Strobe 2.gif|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Swept-plane display]]
|[[Swept-plane display]]
|
|
|
|
|
Line 399: Line 334:
|[[Ternus illusion]]
|[[Ternus illusion]]
|
|
|The '''Ternus illusion''' (1926/1938) is based upon apparent motion.
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Thaumatrope]]
|[[Thaumatrope]]
|[[File:Taumatropio fiori e vaso, 1825.gif|100px]]
|[[File:Taumatropio fiori e vaso, 1825.gif|120px]]
|A '''thaumatrope''' is a toy that was popular in Victorian times.
|
|A thaumatrope is a toy that was popular in Victorian times.
|-
|[[The Spinning Dancer]]
|[[File:Spinning Dancer.gif|100px]]
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Trompe-l'œil]]
|[[Trompe-l'œil]]
|
|
|
|
|
Line 419: Line 346:
|[[Troxler's fading]]
|[[Troxler's fading]]
|
|
|'''Troxler's fading''': When one fixates on a particular point for even a short period of time, an unchanging stimulus away from the fixation point will fade away and disappear.
|
|
|-
|[[Vanishing puzzle]]
|[[File:the_disappearing_bicyclist_vanishing_puzzle.svg|120px]]
|A '''vanishing puzzle''' is a mechanical optical illusion showing different numbers of a certain object when parts of the puzzle are moved around.<ref>The Guardian, [http://www.theguardian.com/science/alexs-adventures-in-numberland/gallery/2014/apr/01/vanishing-leprechaun-disappearing-dwarf-puzzles-pictures ''Vanishing Leprechaun, Disappearing Dwarf and Swinging Sixties Pin-up Girls – puzzles in pictures'']</ref>
|-
|-
|[[Vertical–horizontal illusion]]
|[[Vertical–horizontal illusion]]
|[[File:Vertical–horizontal illusion.png|100px]]
|[[File:Vertical–horizontal illusion.png|120px]]
|The '''Vertical-horizontal illusion''' is the tendency for observers to overestimate the length of a vertical line relative to a horizontal line of the same length.
|
|-
|[[Vista paradox]]
|[[File:Effetto Cannocchiale.jpg|120px]]
|-
|[[Tilt illusion|Visual tilt effects]]
|[[File:TiltIllusion.jpg|120px]]<br />[[File:TiltAfterEffect.jpg|120px]]
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Wagon-wheel effect]]
|[[Wagon-wheel effect]]
|[[File:WagonWheelEffect.gif|100px]]
|[[File:WagonWheelEffect.gif|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[White's illusion]]
|[[White's illusion]]
|[[File:White illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:White illusion.svg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Wundt illusion]]
|[[Wundt illusion]]
|[[File:Wundt illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Wundt illusion.svg|120px]]
|The two red vertical lines are both straight, but they may look as if they are bowed inwards to some observers. The distortion is induced by the crooked lines on the background
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Zoetrope]]
|[[Zoetrope]]
|[[File:Zoetrope.jpg|100px]]
|[[File:Zoetrope.jpg|120px]]
|
|
|
|-
|-
|[[Zöllner illusion]]
|[[Zöllner illusion]]
|[[File:Zollner illusion.svg|100px]]
|[[File:Zollner illusion.svg|120px]]
|The '''Zöllner illusion''' is a classic optical illusion named after its discoverer, German astrophysicist Johann Karl Friedrich Zöllner.
|
|The Zöllner illusion is a classic optical illusion named after its discoverer, German astrophysicist Johann Karl Friedrich Zöllner.
|}
|}


==See also==
==See also==
{{colbegin|colwidth=25em}}
<div style="column-count:3;-moz-column-count:3;-webkit-column-count:3">
* [[Adaptation (eye)]]
* [[Adaptation (eye)]]
* [[Alice in Wonderland syndrome]]
* [[Alice in Wonderland syndrome]]
* [[Auditory illusion]]
* [[Auditory illusion]]
* [[Barber's pole]]
* [[Camouflage]]
* [[Camouflage]]
* [[Contingent perceptual aftereffect]]
* [[Contingent perceptual aftereffect]]
Line 465: Line 395:
* [[Emmert's law]]
* [[Emmert's law]]
* [[Entoptic phenomenon]]
* [[Entoptic phenomenon]]
* [[Forced perspective]] - application used in film and architecture to create the illusion of larger, more distant objects.
* [[Gestalt psychology]]
* [[Gestalt psychology]]
* [[Gravity hill]]
* [[Infinity pool]]
* [[Infinity pool]]
* [[Kinetic depth effect]]
* [[Kinetic depth effect]]
Line 473: Line 401:
* [[Multistable perception]]
* [[Multistable perception]]
* [[Op Art]]
* [[Op Art]]
{{colend}}
* [[Trompe l'oeil]]
* [[Visual reorientation illusions]]
</div>


==Notes==
==Notes==
Line 482: Line 408:
==External links==
==External links==
{{Commons|Optical illusion}}
{{Commons|Optical illusion}}

<!-- -->
<!-- -->
<!-- Please do not add advertising links to this article. -->
<!-- Please do not add advertising links to this article. -->
* [http://www.goillusions.com/ Optical Illusion Examples] by Great Optical Illusions
<!-- -->
* [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/ Optical Illusions & Visual Phenomena] by [[Michael Bach (vision scientist)|Michael Bach]]
<!-- -->
* [http://opticalillusion4u.webs.com/ Optical Illusions ] Types & Related Images
* [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/ Optical Illusions & Visual Phenomena] by Michael Bach
* [http://www.moillusions.com/ Optical Illusions Database] by Mighty Optical Illusions
* [http://www.moillusions.com/ Optical Illusions Database] by Mighty Optical Illusions
* [http://www.archimedes-lab.org/index_optical.html Optical illusions and perception paradoxes] by Archimedes Lab
* [http://www.archimedes-lab.org/index_optical.html Optical illusions and perception paradoxes] by Archimedes Lab
* [http://ilusaodeotica.com http://ilusaodeotica.com] hundreds of optical illusions
* [https://web.archive.org/web/20100419004856/http://ilusaodeotica.com/ https://web.archive.org/web/20100419004856/http://ilusaodeotica.com/] hundreds of optical illusions
* [http://lite.bu.edu/vision/applets/lite/lite/lite.html Project LITE Atlas of Visual Phenomena]
* [http://lite.bu.edu/vision/applets/lite/lite/lite.html Project LITE Atlas of Visual Phenomena] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100329231919/http://lite.bu.edu/vision/applets/lite/lite/lite.html |date=2010-03-29 }}
* [http://www.ritsumei.ac.jp/~akitaoka/index-e.html Akiyoshi's illusion pages] Professor Akiyoshi KITAOKA's anomalous motion illusions
* [http://www.ritsumei.ac.jp/~akitaoka/index-e.html Akiyoshi's illusion pages] Professor Akiyoshi KITAOKA's anomalous motion illusions
* [http://demonstrations.wolfram.com/SpiralOrNot/ Spiral Or Not?] by Enrique Zeleny, [[Wolfram Demonstrations Project]]
* [http://demonstrations.wolfram.com/SpiralOrNot/ Spiral Or Not?] by Enrique Zeleny, [[Wolfram Demonstrations Project]]
* [http://rangki.com/optical/ Magical Optical Illusions] by Rangki
* [https://web.archive.org/web/20100218030035/http://rangki.com/optical/ Magical Optical Illusions] by Rangki
* [http://hunch.com/optical-illusions/ Hunch Optical Illusions] by Hunch
* [http://hunch.com/optical-illusions/ Hunch Optical Illusions] by Hunch
* [https://web.archive.org/web/20170706144659/http://www.oohmybrain.com/ Optical Illusions] by Ooh, My Brain!


{{Optical illusions}}


{{DEFAULTSORT:Optical illusions}}

[[Category:Optical illusions|*]]

[[Category:Optical illusions| ]]
[[Category:Optical phenomena]]
[[Category:Optical phenomena]]
[[Category:Consciousness studies]]
[[Category:Articles containing video clips]]
[[Category:Physics-related lists]]

Latest revision as of 03:59, 28 December 2024

This is a list of visual illusions.

Name Example Notes
Afterimage illusion An afterimage or ghost image is a visual illusion involving an image continuing to appear in one's vision after the exposure to the original image has ceased.
Afterimage on empty shape (also known as color dove illusion) This type of illusion is designed to exploit graphical similarities.
Ambiguous image These are images that can form two separate pictures. For example, the image shown forms a rabbit and a duck.
Ambigram A calligraphic design that has multiple or symmetric interpretations.
Ames room illusion An Ames room is a distorted room that is used to create a visual illusion.
Ames trapezoid window illusion A window is formed in the shape of a trapezium. It is often hung and spun around to provide the illusion that the window rotates through less than 180 degrees.
Autokinetic effect The autokinetic effect, or autokinesis, occurs when a stationary image appears to move.
Autostereogram
An autostereogram is a single-image stereogram (SIS), designed to create the visual illusion of a three-dimensional (3D) scene from a two-dimensional image in the human brain. An ASCII stereogram is an image that is formed using characters on a keyboard. Magic Eye is an autostereogram book series.
Barberpole illusion The barber pole illusion is a visual illusion that reveals biases in the processing of visual motion in the human brain.
Benham's top When a disk that has lines or colours on it is spun, it can form arcs of colour.
Beta movement Movement that appears to occur when fixed pictures turn on and off.
Bezold Effect An apparent change of tone of a colour due to the alteration of the colour of the background.
Blivet Also known as "poiuyt" or "devil's fork", this illusion is an impossible image because in reality the shape cannot exist.
Café wall illusion This illusion is a pattern in which the mortar or grout between different coloured bricks or tiles on a wall appear to form non-parallel, straight lines, despite the lines being parallel (and straight). Its name comes from a café wall that produced the illusion.
Catoptric cistula A catoptric cistula is a box with insides made of mirrors so as to distort images of objects put into the box.
Checker shadow illusion
The checker shadow illusion shows that when a shadow is cast onto a checked board, the colours of squares A and B in the photos appear to be different, when in fact they are the same.
Chubb illusion The Chubb illusion is an optical illusion or error in visual perception in which the apparent contrast of an object varies substantially to most viewers depending on its relative contrast to the field on which it is displayed.
Color constancy Colour constancy is an example of subjective constancy and a feature of the human color perception system which ensures that the perceived color of objects remains relatively constant under varying illumination conditions. A green apple for instance looks green to us at midday, when the main illumination is white sunlight, and also at sunset, when the main illumination is red.
Color phi phenomenon The color phi phenomenon is a perceptual illusion in which a disembodied perception of motion is produced by a succession of still images.
Contingent perceptual aftereffect
Convergence micropsia
Cornsweet illusion An illusion where two colours can obviously be seen to be different when placed directly beside each other; however, when the two colours are separated by a thick black line, they appear to be of the same hue.
Crater illusion A type of multistable illusion where an image of a concave object, rotated so that the light source is below, may sometimes appear convex, and vice versa. This phenomenon is because light sources tend to shine from above the subject.
Delboeuf illusion An optical illusion of relative size perception. The two black circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the left seems larger.
Disappearing Model A trompe-l'œil body painting by Joanne Gair.
The dress
Two ways in which the photograph of The dress may be perceived: blue and black under a yellow-tinted illumination (left figure) or white and gold under a blue-tinted illumination (right figure).
An optical illusion resulting from the brain's attempt to discount coloured tinting from daylight and other sources.[1] The dress was a viral phenomenon, which was either seen as blue and black or as white and gold. Its true colours were eventually confirmed to be blue and black.[2]
Ebbinghaus illusion The Ebbinghaus illusion, or Titchener circles, is an optical illusion of relative size perception. The two orange circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the right appears larger.
Ehrenstein illusion
The Ehrenstein illusion is an optical illusion studied by the German psychologist Walter Ehrenstein in which the sides of a square placed inside a pattern of concentric circles take an apparent curved shape.
Fata Morgana (mirage) Visible in a narrow band right above the horizon, Fata Morgana mirages significantly distort the object or objects on which they are based, often such that the object is completely unrecognizable. A Fata Morgana may be seen on land or at sea, in polar regions, or in deserts. It may involve almost any kind of distant object, including boats, islands, and the coastline.
Fechner color
Figure-ground (perception)
Filling-in
Flash lag illusion
Forced perspective Application used in film and architecture to create the illusion of larger, more distant objects.
Fraser spiral illusion The Fraser spiral illusion, or false spiral, or the twisted cord illusion, was first described by the British psychologist Sir James Fraser in 1908. The overlapping black arc segments appear to form a spiral; however, the arcs are a series of concentric circles.
Gravity hill
Grid illusion
Any kind of grid that deceives a person's vision. The two most common types of grid illusions are the Hermann grid illusion (1870) and the scintillating grid illusion (1994). The first is characterized by "ghostlike" grey blobs perceived at the intersections of a white (or light-colored) grid on a black background. The grey blobs disappear when looking directly at an intersection. The second is constructed by superimposing white discs on the intersections of orthogonal gray bars on a black background. Dark dots seem to appear and disappear rapidly at random intersections, hence the label "scintillating". When a person keeps their eyes directly on a single intersection, the dark dot does not appear. The dark dots disappear if one is too close to or too far from the image.
Hering illusion The Hering illusion (1861): When two straight and parallel lines are presented in front of radial background (like the spokes of a bicycle), the lines appear as if they were bowed outwards.
Hollow-Face illusion The Hollow-Face illusion is an optical illusion in which the perception of a concave mask of a face appears as a normal convex face.
Hybrid image A Hybrid image is an optical illusion developed at MIT in which an image can be interpreted in one of two different ways depending on viewing distance.
Illusory contours Illusory contours or subjective contours are a form of visual illusion where contours are perceived without a luminance or color change across the contour.
Impossible object
Irradiation illusion
Isometric illusion An isometric illusion (also called an ambiguous figure or inside/outside illusion) is a type of optical illusion, specifically one due to multistable perception.
Jastrow illusion The Jastrow illusion is an optical illusion discovered by the American psychologist Joseph Jastrow in 1889.
Kanizsa triangle The Kanizsa triangle is an optical illusion first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955. It is a triangle formed of illusory contours.
Kinetic Depth Effect The Kinetic depth effect is the phenomenon whereby the three-dimensional structural form of a silhouette can be perceived when the object is moving. In the absence of other visual depth cues, this might be the only perception mechanism available to infer the object's shape. Additionally the direction of motion can reverse due to the existence of multiple 3D visual solutions.
Leaning tower illusion The Leaning tower illusion is an optical illusion that presents two identical images of the Leaning Tower of Pisa side by side.
Lilac chaser Lilac chaser is a visual illusion, also known as the Pac-Man illusion.
Liquid crystal shutter glasses
Lunar terminator illusion Lunar terminator illusion is an optical illusion where the apparent source of sunlight illuminating the moon does not corresponding with the actual position of the sun.
Mach bands Mach bands is an optical illusion named after the physicist Ernst Mach.
McCollough effect The McCollough effect (1965) is a phenomenon of human visual perception in which colorless gratings appear colored contingent on the orientation of the gratings. It is an aftereffect requiring a period of induction to produce it.
Missing square puzzle
The missing square puzzle is an optical illusion used in mathematics classes to help students reason about geometrical figures.
Moon illusion The Moon illusion is an optical illusion in which the Moon appears larger near the horizon than it does while higher up in the sky.
Motion aftereffect
Motion illusion
Müller-Lyer illusion The Müller-Lyer illusion is an optical illusion consisting of a stylized arrow.
Multistable perception
Necker cube The Necker cube is an optical illusion first published in 1832 by Swiss crystallographer Louis Albert Necker.
Numerosity adaptation effect
Orbison illusion The Orbison illusion is an optical illusion that was first described by the psychologist William Orbison in 1939.
Oppel-Kundt illusion The Oppel-Kundt illusion is an optical illusion named after German physicists Johann Joseph Oppel [de] (first mentioned this phenomenon in 1860) and August Kundt (first performed a systematic study of the illusion in 1863).
Penrose stairs The Penrose stairs were created by Lionel Penrose and his son Roger Penrose.[3] A variation on the Penrose triangle, it is a two-dimensional depiction of a staircase in which the stairs make four 90-degree turns as they ascend or descend yet form a continuous loop, so that a person could climb them forever and never get any higher.
Penrose triangle The Penrose triangle was first created by the Swedish artist Oscar Reutersvärd in 1934. The mathematician Roger Penrose independently devised and popularised it in the 1950s, describing it as "impossibility in its purest form".
Pepper's ghost
Perceived visual angle
Peripheral drift illusion A motion illusion (1979/1999) generated by the presentation of a sawtooth luminance grating in the visual periphery.
Phantogram Phantograms, also known as Phantaglyphs, Op-Ups, free-standing anaglyphs, levitated images, and book anaglyphs, are a form of optical illusion.
Phi phenomenon
Poggendorff illusion The Poggendorff illusion (1860) involves the misperception of the position of one segment of a transverse line that has been interrupted by the contour of an intervening structure (here a rectangle).
Ponzo illusion In the Ponzo illusion (1911) two identical lines across a pair of converging lines, similar to railway tracks, are drawn. The upper line looks longer because we interpret the converging sides according to linear perspective as parallel lines receding into the distance. In this context, we interpret the upper line as though it were farther away, so we see it as longer – a farther object would have to be longer than a nearer one for both to produce retinal images of the same size.
Pulfrich effect The Pulfrich effect is the effect that covering one eye with transparent but darkened glass can cause purely lateral motion to appear to have a depth component even though in reality it doesn't; even a completely flat scene such as one shown on a television screen can appear to exhibit some three-dimensional motion, but this is an illusion because darkening the scene for one eye causes the photoreceptors in that eye to respond more slowly.
Rubin vase Rubin vase (1915): an ambiguous or bi-stable (i.e., reversing) two-dimensional form.
Sander illusion In Sander's parallelogram (1926) the diagonal line bisecting the larger, left-hand parallelogram appears to be considerably longer than the diagonal line bisecting the smaller, right-hand parallelogram, but is in fact the same length.
Silencing Silencing is an illusion in which a set of objects that change in luminance, hue, size, or shape appears to stop changing when it moves.
Size–weight illusion The size–weight illusion is also known as the Charpentier illusion or Charpentier–Koseleff illusion.
Stepping feet illusion The stepping feet illusion is influenced by the contrast between moving objects and their background.
Stroboscopic effect
Swept-plane display
Ternus illusion The Ternus illusion (1926/1938) is based upon apparent motion.
Thaumatrope A thaumatrope is a toy that was popular in Victorian times.
Trompe-l'œil
Troxler's fading Troxler's fading: When one fixates on a particular point for even a short period of time, an unchanging stimulus away from the fixation point will fade away and disappear.
Vanishing puzzle A vanishing puzzle is a mechanical optical illusion showing different numbers of a certain object when parts of the puzzle are moved around.[4]
Vertical–horizontal illusion The Vertical-horizontal illusion is the tendency for observers to overestimate the length of a vertical line relative to a horizontal line of the same length.
Vista paradox
Visual tilt effects
Wagon-wheel effect
White's illusion
Wundt illusion The two red vertical lines are both straight, but they may look as if they are bowed inwards to some observers. The distortion is induced by the crooked lines on the background
Zoetrope
Zöllner illusion The Zöllner illusion is a classic optical illusion named after its discoverer, German astrophysicist Johann Karl Friedrich Zöllner.

See also

[edit]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ "The Science of Why No One Agrees on the Colour of This Dress". Wired. 2015-02-27. Archived from the original on 28 February 2015. Retrieved 27 February 2015.
  2. ^ "Optical illusion: Dress color debate goes global". BBC News. BBC. Archived from the original on 2019-07-13. Retrieved 28 February 2015.
  3. ^ Penrose, LS; Penrose, R. (1958). "Impossible objects: A special type of optical illusion". British Journal of Psychology. 49 (1): 31–33. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1958.tb00634.x. PMID 13536303.
  4. ^ The Guardian, Vanishing Leprechaun, Disappearing Dwarf and Swinging Sixties Pin-up Girls – puzzles in pictures
[edit]