Jump to content

Polygraph: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
m Effectiveness: Citation order
 
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Psuedoscientific device that attempts to infer lying}}
{{About|the forensic instrument|the automatic signing instrument|Autopen|the dual pen device that produces a simultaneous copy of an original while it is written in [[cursive writing]]|Polygraph (duplicating device)|an author that can write on a variety of different subjects|Polygraph (author)}}
{{other uses}}
{{Redirect|Lie detector test}}
{{redirect|Lie Detector}}
[[File:Leonarde Keeler 1937.jpg|thumb|American inventor [[Leonarde Keeler]] testing his improved polygraph on Kohler, a former witness for the prosecution at the 1935 trial of [[Richard Hauptmann]]]]
A '''polygraph''', often incorrectly referred to as a '''lie detector test''',<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.law.cornell.edu/wex/lie_detector_test|title=lie detector test|publisher=[[Legal Information Institute]]|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=June 2020|access-date=July 27, 2022|archivedate=March 5, 2022|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20220305085649/https://www.law.cornell.edu/wex/lie_detector_test}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.apa.org/topics/cognitive-neuroscience/polygraph|publisher=[[American Psychological Association]]|title=The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=2004|access-date=July 27, 2022|archivedate=March 5, 2022|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20220305085649/https://www.law.cornell.edu/wex/lie_detector_test}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=https://abcnews.go.com/US/story?id=92847&page=1|title=Polygraphs Accurate But Not Foolproof|author=Robinson, Bryan|publisher=[[ABC News (United States)|ABC News]]|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=July 14, 2001|access-date=July 27, 2022|archivedate=April 20, 2021|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20210420002433/https://abcnews.go.com/US/story?id=92847&page=1}}</ref> is a [[pseudoscientific]]<ref>{{Cite web |title=United States v. Scheffer: A U.S. Supreme Court Ruling on Polygraph Testing |url=https://sgp.fas.org/othergov/polygraph/scheffer.html |publisher=[[Federation of American Scientists]]|date=March 31, 1998|access-date=November 11, 2023|archive-date=November 11, 2023|archive-url=https://archive.today/20231111193602/https://sgp.fas.org/othergov/polygraph/scheffer.html}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Saxe |first=Leonard |date=July 1991 |title=Science and the CQT polygraph. |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02912514 |journal=Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science |volume=26 |issue=3 |pages=223–231|doi=10.1007/BF02912514 |pmid=1954162 }}</ref><ref name=NatResearchCouncil>{{cite book|title=The Polygraph and Lie Detection|author=Board on Behavioral, Cognitive, and Sensory Sciences and Education (BCSSE) and Committee on National Statistics (CNSTAT)|publisher=[[National Research Council (United States)|National Research Council]] |url=https://nap.nationalacademies.org/read/10420/chapter/1|doi= 10.17226/10420 |isbn= 978-0-309-26392-4|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=March 19, 2013|access-date=November 11, 2023|archive-date=November 11, 2023|archive-url=https://archive.today/20231111191525/https://nap.nationalacademies.org/catalog/10420/the-polygraph-and-lie-detection}}</ref> device or procedure that measures and records several physiological indicators such as [[blood pressure]], [[pulse]], [[Respiration (physiology)|respiration]], and [[Electrodermal activity|skin conductivity]] while a person is asked and answers a series of questions.<ref>{{cite journal|author=J P Rosenfeld|title=Alternative Views of Bashore and Rapp's (1993) alternatives to traditional polygraphy: a critique|year=1995|journal=Psychological Bulletin|doi=10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.159|volume=117|pages=159–166}}</ref> The belief underpinning the use of the polygraph is that deceptive answers will produce physiological responses that can be differentiated from those associated with non-deceptive answers; however, there are no specific physiological reactions associated with [[lying]], making it difficult to identify factors that separate those who are lying from those who are telling the truth.<ref name="apa1">{{cite web |title=The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests) |url=https://www.apa.org/research/action/polygraph |website=apa.org |publisher=American Psychological Association}}</ref>


In some countries, polygraphs are used as an [[interrogation]] tool with criminal suspects or candidates for sensitive public or private sector employment. Some [[United States law enforcement]] and federal government agencies,<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.vox.com/2014/8/14/5999119/polygraphs-lie-detectors-do-they-work|title=Lie detectors: Why they don't work, and why police use them anyway|publisher=[[Vox (magazine)|Vox]]|author=Stromberg, Joseph|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=December 15, 2014|access-date=September 13, 2022|archivedate=August 16, 2014|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20140816182851/https://www.vox.com/2014/8/14/5999119/polygraphs-lie-detectors-do-they-work}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.usnews.com/news/blogs/washington-whispers/2012/09/25/nsa-whistleblower-reveals-how-to-beat-a-polygraph-test|title=NSA Whistleblower Reveals How To Beat a Polygraph Test|author=Flock, Elizabeth|magazine=[[U.S. News & World Report]]|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=September 25, 2012|access-date=September 13, 2022|archivedate=August 13, 2022|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20220813195242/https://www.usnews.com/news/blogs/washington-whispers/2012/09/25/nsa-whistleblower-reveals-how-to-beat-a-polygraph-test}}</ref> as well as many police departments, use polygraph examinations to interrogate suspects and screen new employees. Within the [[US federal government]], a polygraph examination is also referred to as a psychophysiological detection of deception examination.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://antipolygraph.org/documents/federal-polygraph-handbook-02-10-2006.pdf|title=Federal Psychophysiological Detection of Deception Examiner Handbook|work=Antipolygraph.org|access-date=15 May 2019}}</ref>
[[File:Computerized Polygraph.jpg|thumb|300px|Limestone Technologies Inc. Polygraph Professional Suite computerized polygraph system.]]


Assessments of polygraphy by scientific and government bodies generally suggest that polygraphs are highly inaccurate, may easily be defeated by countermeasures, and are an imperfect or invalid means of assessing truthfulness.<ref name=faspol/><ref name=APA2004/><ref name=NatResearchCouncil/><ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.cnn.com/2018/03/21/health/lie-detector-facts-accuracy/index.html|title=The shaky science of lie detectors|publisher=[[CNN]]|author=LaMotte, Sandee|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=September 7, 2018|access-date=November 5, 2023|archive-date=November 5, 2023|archive-url=https://archive.today/20231105062126/https://www.cnn.com/2018/03/21/health/lie-detector-facts-accuracy/index.html}}</ref> A comprehensive 2003 review by the [[National Academy of Sciences]] of existing research concluded that there was "little basis for the expectation that a polygraph test could have extremely high accuracy."<ref name=NatResearchCouncil/> The [[American Psychological Association]] states that "most psychologists agree that there is little evidence that polygraph tests can accurately detect lies."<ref name="apa1"/>
A '''polygraph''' (popularly referred to as a '''lie detector''') is an instrument that measures and records several physiological indices such as [[blood pressure]], [[pulse]], [[Respiration (physiology)|respiration]], and [[Galvanic skin response|skin conductivity]] while the subject is asked and answers a series of questions, in the belief that deceptive answers will produce physiological responses that can be differentiated from those associated with non-deceptive answers.


==Testing procedure==
The polygraph was invented in 1921 by [[John Augustus Larson]], a medical student at the [[University of California at Berkeley]] and a police officer of the [[Berkeley Police Department]] in Berkeley, California. According to [[Encyclopædia Britannica]]; the polygraph -- along with Post-It Notes, the guillotine, and the atom bomb -- was on its 2003 list of the 325 greatest inventions, described by the company as inventions that "have had profound effects on human life for better or worse."<ref>{{cite web|title=Editors Name Greatest Inventions of All Time|url=http://corporate.britannica.com/press/releases/invention.html|work=Encyclopædia Britannica News Releases}}</ref>
{{see also|Lie detection#Questioning and testing techniques}}
The examiner typically begins polygraph test sessions with a pre-test interview to gain some preliminary information which will later be used to develop diagnostic questions. Then the tester will explain how the polygraph is supposed to work, emphasizing that it can detect lies and that it is important to answer truthfully. Then a "stim test" is often conducted: the subject is asked to deliberately lie and then the tester reports that he was able to detect this lie. Guilty subjects are likely to become more anxious when they are reminded of the test's validity. However, there are risks of innocent subjects being equally or more anxious than the guilty.<ref name="Lewis, J. A. 2009">{{cite journal | last1 = Lewis | first1 = J. A. | last2 = Cuppari | first2 = M. | year = 2009 | title = The polygraph: The truth lies within | journal = Journal of Psychiatry and Law | volume = 37 | issue = 1| pages = 85–92 | doi = 10.1177/009318530903700107 | s2cid = 152221286 }}</ref> Then the actual test starts. Some of the questions asked are "irrelevant" ("Is your name Fred?"), others are "diagnostic" questions, and the remainder are the "relevant questions" that the tester is really interested in. The different types of questions alternate. The test is passed if the physiological responses to the diagnostic questions are larger than those during the relevant questions.<ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.nap.edu/read/10420/chapter/12|title=The Polygraph and Lie Detection|publisher=The National Academies Press|year=2003|isbn=978-0-309-13313-5|location=Washington, DC|pages=253–254|language=en|doi=10.17226/10420}}</ref>


Criticisms have been given regarding the validity of the administration of the Control Question Technique. The CQT may be vulnerable to being conducted in an interrogation-like fashion. This kind of interrogation style would elicit a nervous response from innocent and guilty suspects alike. There are several other ways of administering the questions.<ref>{{Cite book|chapter-url=https://www.nap.edu/read/10420/chapter/12|title=The Polygraph and Lie Detection|publisher=The National Academies Press|year=2003|isbn=978-0-309-13313-5|pages=253–258|chapter=Appendix A: Polygraph Questioning Techniques|doi=10.17226/10420}}</ref>
Polygraphy is generally rejected by the scientific community as [[pseudoscience]] .<ref>Iacono, W.G. "Forensic 'lie detection': Procedures without scientific basis," ''Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice'', Vol. 1 (2001), No. 1, pp. 75-86.</ref> Despite the scientific consensus, polygraphs are in some countries used as an [[interrogation]] tool with criminal suspects or candidates for sensitive public or private sector employment. US federal government agencies such as the FBI and the CIA and many police departments such as the LAPD use polygraph examinations to interrogate suspects and screen new employees. Within the US federal government, a polygraph examination is also referred to as a '''psychophysiological detection of deception''' (PDD) examination.{{Citation needed|date=July 2010}}


An alternative is the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT), or the Concealed Information Test, which is used in [[Japan]].<ref>{{cite journal| url = http://digitool.haifa.ac.il/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=673186&local_base=GEN01| title = The Admissibility of Polygraph Evidence in Criminal Courts| author = Don Sosunov| date = October 14, 2010| journal = | access-date = November 1, 2012| archive-date = November 6, 2018| archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20181106072418/http://digitool.haifa.ac.il/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=673186&local_base=GEN01| url-status = dead}}</ref> The administration of this test is given to prevent potential errors that may arise from the questioning style. The test is usually conducted by a tester with no knowledge of the [[crime]] or circumstances in question. The administrator tests the participant on their knowledge of the crime that would not be known to an innocent person. For example: "Was the crime committed with a .45 or a 9 mm?" The questions are in multiple choice and the participant is rated on how they react to the correct answer. If they react strongly to the guilty information, then proponents of the test believe that it is likely that they know facts relevant to the case. This administration is considered more valid by supporters of the test because it contains many safeguards to avoid the risk of the administrator influencing the results.<ref>For more info on the Guilty Knowledge Test, see [http://websites.mscc.huji.ac.il/zerolab/GKTCHAP3.PDF The Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT) as an Application of Psychophysiology: Future Prospects and Obstacles] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080528204623/http://websites.mscc.huji.ac.il/zerolab/GKTCHAP3.PDF |date=2008-05-28 }}</ref>
==History==
Early devices for lie detection include an 1895 invention of [[Cesare Lombroso]] used to measure changes in blood pressure for police cases, a 1904 device by [[Vittorio Benussi]] used to measure breathing, and an abandoned project by American [[William Moulton Marston|William Marston]] which used blood pressure to examine German prisoners of war (POWs).<ref>[http://www.nitv1.com/History.htm Nitv Llc<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref>


==Effectiveness==
Marston wrote a second paper on the concept in 1915, when finishing his undergraduate studies. He entered [[Harvard Law School]] and graduated in 1918, re-publishing his earlier work in 1917.<ref>Marston, William M. "Systolic Blood Pressure Changes in Deception," ''Journal of Experimental Psychology,'' 2:117-163.</ref> According to their son, Marston's wife, [[Elizabeth Holloway Marston]], was also involved in the development of the systolic blood pressure test: "According to Marston’s son, it was his mother Elizabeth, Marston’s wife, who suggested to him that 'When she got mad or excited, her blood pressure seemed to climb' (Lamb, 2001). Although Elizabeth is not listed as Marston’s collaborator in his early work, Lamb, Matte (1996), and others refer directly and indirectly to Elizabeth’s work on her husband’s deception research. She also appears in a picture taken in his polygraph laboratory in the 1920s (reproduced in Marston, 1938)."<ref>[http://books.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10420&page=292 WILLIAM MOULTON MARSTON, THE NATIONAL RESEARCH COUNCIL, AND WONDER WOMAN]</ref><ref>[http://books.google.com/books?id=USg-j9esZagC&pg=PA292&lpg=PA292&dq=elizabeth+marston+%22wonder+woman%22&source=web&ots=eL7Vd4wTR4&sig=pzvRdGsniz6h9-dGgEXdlyzdem4 The Polygraph and Lie Detection]</ref> The comic book character, [[Wonder Woman]], by William Marston (and influenced by Elizabeth Marston<ref name=bu>[http://www.bu.edu/alumni/bostonia/2001/fall/wonderwoman/ Who Was Wonder Woman? Long-ago LAW alumna Elizabeth Marston was the muse who gave us a superheroine]</ref><ref name=outtowns>[http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9E0CE1DF1539F93BA25751C0A964958260&sec=&spon=&pagewanted=print OUR TOWNS; She's Behind the Match For That Man of Steel]</ref>) carries a [[Lasso of Truth|magic lasso]] which was modelled upon the pneumograph (breathing monitor) test.<ref name=bu /><ref>[http://books.google.com/books?id=USg-j9esZagC&pg=PA295&lpg=PA295&dq=wonder+woman+%22magic+lasso%22+marston+polygraph&source=web&ots=eL7W48wYVZ&sig=znyHik1Ucdr6yQUsZKn1jTpe4MI The Polygraph and Lie Detection, p.295]</ref>
Assessments of polygraphy by scientific and government bodies generally suggest that polygraphs are inaccurate, may be defeated by countermeasures, and are an imperfect or invalid means of assessing truthfulness.<ref name=NatResearchCouncil/><ref name=faspol>{{cite web |url=https://fas.org/sgp/othergov/polygraph/ota/index.html |access-date=2008-02-29 |title=Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation |publisher= Washington, DC: U.S. Congress Office of Technology Assessment |year=1983}}</ref><ref name=APA2004>{{cite web |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug04/polygraph.html |access-date=2008-02-29 |date=July 2004 |title=Monitor on Psychology – The polygraph in doubt |publisher=American Psychological Association}}</ref> Despite claims that polygraph tests are between 80% and 90% accurate by advocates,<ref>{{cite news|last=Evans|first=Gareth|date=October 4, 2018|title=How credible are lie detector tests?|work=BBC News|url=https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/world-us-canada-45736631|quote="If the examiner is well-trained, if the test is properly carried out, and if there's proper quality controls, the accuracy is estimated between 80%-90%".}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=https://theconversation.com/polygraph-lie-detector-tests-can-they-really-stop-criminals-reoffending-130477|title=Polygraph lie detector tests: can they really stop criminals reoffending?|last=Warmelink|first=Lara|date=January 24, 2020|website=The Conversation|quote=There have been several reviews of polygraph accuracy. They suggest that polygraphs are accurate between 80% and 90% of the time.}}</ref> the [[National Research Council (United States)|National Research Council]] has found no evidence of effectiveness.<ref name=APA2004/><ref name="BBCpol">{{cite news|date=July 16, 2009|title=A scanner to detect terrorists|work=BBC News|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/magazine/8153539.stm}}</ref> In particular, studies have indicated that the relevant–irrelevant questioning technique is not ideal, as many innocent subjects exert a heightened physiological reaction to the crime-relevant questions.<ref name="Iacono, W. G. 2008">{{cite journal |last1=Iacono |first1=W. G. |year=2008 |title=Effective policing: Understanding how polygraph tests work and are used |journal=Criminal Justice and Behavior |volume=35 |issue=10 |pages=1295–1308 |doi=10.1177/0093854808321529 |s2cid=143302792}}</ref> The [[American Psychological Association]] states "Most psychologists agree that there is little evidence that polygraph tests can accurately detect lies."<ref name="apa1"/>


In 2002, a review by the National Research Council found that, in populations "untrained in [[countermeasure]]s, specific-incident polygraph tests can discriminate lying from truth telling at rates well above chance, though well below perfection". The review also warns against generalization from these findings to justify the use of polygraphs—"polygraph accuracy for screening purposes is almost certainly lower than what can be achieved by specific-incident polygraph tests in the field"—and notes some examinees may be able to take countermeasures to produce deceptive results.<ref>National Research Council (2003); [http://www.nap.edu/openbook.php?isbn=0309084369 Pages 4 - 5]</ref>
Marston was the self-proclaimed “father of the polygraph” despite his predecessor's contributions.
Marston remained the device's primary advocate, lobbying for its use in the courts. In 1938 he published a book, ''The Lie Detector Test,'' wherein he documented the theory and use of the device.<ref>Marston, William Moulton. ''The Lie Detector''. New York: Richard R. Smith, 1938.</ref> In 1938 he appeared in advertising by the [[Gillette (brand)|Gillette]] company claiming that the polygraph showed Gillette razors were better than the competition.<ref>{{cite web|publisher=[[Reason (magazine)|Reason magazine]]|date=2001-05|title=William Marston's Secret Identity|url=http://www.reason.com/news/show/28014.html}}</ref><ref>[http://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/marston-razor-high-res.pdf Now! Lie Detector Charts Emotional Effects of Shaving - 1938 Gillette Advertisement]</ref><ref>[http://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/marston-fbi-file.pdf FBI File of William Moulton Marston] (including report on Gillette advertising campaign)</ref>


In the 1998 [[Supreme Court of the United States|US Supreme Court]] case ''[[United States v. Scheffer]]'', the majority stated that "There is simply no consensus that polygraph evidence is reliable [...] Unlike other expert witnesses who testify about factual matters outside the jurors' knowledge, such as the analysis of fingerprints, ballistics, or DNA found at a crime scene, a polygraph expert can supply the jury only with another opinion." The Supreme Court summarized their findings by stating that the use of polygraph was "little better than could be obtained by the toss of a coin."<ref name = "us">{{cite journal| date =March 31, 1998| title =United States v. Scheffer| url =https://www.law.cornell.edu/supct/html/96-1133.ZO.html}}</ref> In 2005, the 11th Circuit Court of Appeals stated that "polygraphy did not enjoy general acceptance from the scientific community".<ref name="pdf">{{cite journal| date =May 23, 2005| title =United States v. Henderson| url =http://www.ca11.uscourts.gov/opinions/ops/200411545.pdf| access-date =August 31, 2007| archive-url =https://web.archive.org/web/20071127173339/http://www.ca11.uscourts.gov/opinions/ops/200411545.pdf| archive-date =November 27, 2007| url-status =dead}} [PDF]</ref> In 2001, William Iacono, Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience at the [[University of Minnesota]], concluded:
A device recording both blood pressure and [[galvanic skin response]] was invented in 1921 by Dr. John A. Larson of the [[University of California]] and first applied in law enforcement work by the [[Berkeley, California|Berkeley]] Police Department under its nationally renowned police chief [[August Vollmer]]. Further work on this device was done by [[Leonarde Keeler]].<ref>[http://www.lie2me.net/thepolygraphmuseum/id12.html Leonarde Keeler and his Instruments<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref>
<blockquote>Although the CQT [Control Question Test] may be useful as an investigative aid and tool to induce confessions, it does not pass muster as a scientifically credible test. CQT theory is based on naive, implausible assumptions indicating (a) that it is biased against innocent individuals and (b) that it can be beaten simply by artificially augmenting responses to control questions. Although it is not possible to adequately assess the error rate of the CQT, both of these conclusions are supported by published research findings in the best social science journals (Honts et al., 1994; Horvath, 1977; Kleinmuntz & Szucko, 1984; Patrick & Iacono, 1991). Although defense attorneys often attempt to have the results of friendly CQTs admitted as evidence in court, there is no evidence supporting their validity and ample reason to doubt it. Members of scientific organizations who have the requisite background to evaluate the CQT are overwhelmingly skeptical of the claims made by polygraph proponents.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Iacono|first=William G.|year=2001|title=Forensic 'Lie Detection': Procedures Without Scientific Basis|url=https://antipolygraph.org/articles/article-018.shtml|journal=Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice|volume=1|issue=1|pages=75–86|doi=10.1300/J158v01n01_05|s2cid=143077241}}</ref>
</blockquote>


Polygraphs measure [[arousal]], which can be affected by [[anxiety]], [[anxiety disorder]]s such as [[posttraumatic stress disorder]] (PTSD), nervousness, fear, confusion, hypoglycemia, psychosis, depression, substance induced states (nicotine, stimulants), substance withdrawal state (alcohol withdrawal) or other emotions; polygraphs do not measure "lies".<ref name="Lewis, J. A. 2009"/><ref name="dpca.state.ny.us">{{cite web |url=http://dpca.state.ny.us/pdfs/sopolygraphresearchbulletin3.pdf |title=Archived copy |access-date=2014-01-06 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110605202732/http://www.dpca.state.ny.us/pdfs/sopolygraphresearchbulletin3.pdf |archive-date=2011-06-05 }}</ref><ref>{{cite news| url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2006/04/30/AR2006043001006.html | newspaper=The Washington Post | first1=Dan | last1=Eggen | first2=Shankar | last2=Vedantam | title=Polygraph Results Often in Question | date=May 1, 2006}}</ref> A polygraph cannot differentiate anxiety caused by dishonesty and anxiety caused by something else.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.motherjones.com/politics/2002/11/lie-detector-roulette|title=Lie Detector Roulette|work=Mother Jones}}</ref>
Several devices similar to Keeler's polygraph version included the Berkeley Psychograph, a blood pressure-pulse-respiration recorder developed by C. D. Lee in 1936<ref name="ReferenceA">Inbau, Fred E. Lie Detection and Criminal Interrogation, The Williams & Wilkins Company, 1948</ref> and the Darrow Behavior Research Photopolygraph, which was developed and intended solely for behavior research experiments.<ref name="ReferenceA"/><ref>Troville, P. V. "A History of Lie Detection," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 29 (6)-848 (1939); 30 (1):104 (1939)</ref>


Since the polygraph does not measure lying, the [[Silent Talker Lie Detector]] inventors expected that adding a camera to film microexpressions would improve the accuracy of the evaluators. This did not happen in practice according to an article in the Intercept.<ref>{{cite web |last1=Gallagher |first1=Ryan |title=We Tested Europe's New Lie Detector for Travelors-and Immediately Triggered a False Positive |url=https://theintercept.com/2019/07/26/europe-border-control-ai-lie-detector/ |website=The Intercept |date=26 July 2019 |access-date=17 March 2023}}</ref>
A device which recorded muscular activity accompanying changes in blood pressure was developed in 1945 by John E. Reid, who claimed that greater accuracy could be obtained by making these recordings simultaneously with standard blood pressure-pulse-respiration recordings.<ref name="ReferenceA"/><ref>Reid, J. E. "Simulated Blood Pressure Responses in Lie-Detection Tests and a Method for Their Deception," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 36 (1):201-215 (1945)</ref>


===US Congress Office of Technology Assessment===
==Testing procedure==
In 1983, the US Congress Office of Technology Assessment published a review of the technology<ref>{{cite journal|title=Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation – A Technical Memorandum|journal=(Washington, DC: U.S. Congress, Office of Technology Assessment|date=November 1983|issue=OTA-TM-H-15|url=http://ota.fas.org/reports/8320.pdf|access-date=3 September 2016}}</ref> and found that
{{Globalize|section|date=April 2010}}
Today, polygraph examiners use two types of instrumentation: analog and computerized. In the [[United States]], most examiners now use [[computerized]] instrumentation.{{Citation needed|date=July 2010}}


{{quote|there is at present only limited scientific evidence for establishing the validity of polygraph testing. Even where the evidence seems to indicate that polygraph testing detects deceptive subjects better than chance, significant error rates are possible, and examiner and examinee differences and the use of countermeasures may further affect validity.<ref>{{Skeptoid|id=4422|number=422|title=Lie Detection|access-date=3 September 2016}}</ref>}}
A typical polygraph test starts with a pre-test [[interview]] to gain some preliminary information which will later be used for "control questions", or CQ. Then the tester will explain how the polygraph is supposed to work, emphasizing that it can detect lies and that it is important to answer truthfully. Then a "stim test" is often conducted: the subject is asked to deliberately lie and then the tester reports that he was able to detect this lie. Then the actual test starts. Some of the questions asked are "irrelevant" or IR ("Is your name Chris?"), others are "probable-lie" control questions that most people will lie about ("Have you ever stolen money?") and the remainder are the "relevant questions", or RQ, that the tester is really interested in. The different types of questions alternate. The test is passed if the physiological responses during the probable-lie control questions (CQ) are larger than those during the relevant questions (RQ). If this is not the case, the tester attempts to elicit admissions during a post-test interview, for example, "Your situation will only get worse if we don't clear this up".<ref>For details on the intricacies of various polygraph techniques, see the [http://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/federal-polygraph-handbook-02-10-2006.pdf Federal Psychophysiological Detection of Deception Examiner Handbook], the U.S. Government's official guide to the administration of polygraph examinations.</ref><ref>For interrogation techniques associated with polygraph testing, see the [http://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/dodpi-interrogation.pdf Department of Defense Polygraph Institute's Interview & Interrogation Handbook].</ref>


===National Academy of Sciences===
Criticisms have been given regarding the validity of the administration of the Comparative Questions test (CQT). The CQT may be vulnerable to being conducted in an interrogation-like fashion. This kind of interrogation style would elicit a nervous response from innocent and guilty suspects alike. There are several other ways of administrating the questions.
In 2003, the [[National Academy of Sciences]] (NAS) issued a report entitled "The Polygraph and Lie Detection". The NAS found that "overall, the evidence is scanty and scientifically weak", concluding that 57 of the approximately 80 research studies that the [[American Polygraph Association]] relied on to reach their conclusions were significantly flawed.<ref>[http://www.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10420&page=212 "Conclusions and Recommendations"]. ''The Polygraph and Lie Detection'' (2003), [[National Academies Press]]. p. 212</ref> These studies did show that specific-incident polygraph testing, in a person untrained in counter-measures, could discern the truth at "a level greater than chance, yet short of perfection". However, due to several flaws, the levels of accuracy shown in these studies "are almost certainly higher than actual polygraph accuracy of specific-incident testing in the field".<ref>National Research Council (2013); [http://www.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10420&page=212 Chapter 8: Conclusions and Recommendations, p. 212])</ref> By adding a camera, the [[Silent Talker Lie Detector]] attempted to give more data to the evaluator by providing information about microexpressions. However adding the Silent Talker camera did not improve lie detection and was very expensive and cumbersome to include according to an article in the Intercept.{{fact|date=November 2023}}


When polygraphs are used as a screening tool (in [[national security]] matters and for [[Law enforcement agency|law enforcement]] agencies for example) the level of accuracy drops to such a level that "Its accuracy in distinguishing actual or potential security violators from innocent test takers is insufficient to justify reliance on its use in employee security screening in [[List of federal agencies in the United States|federal agencies]]." The NAS concluded that the polygraph "may have some utility but that there is "little basis for the expectation that a polygraph test could have extremely high accuracy".<ref name=NatResearchCouncil/>
An alternative is the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT), or the Concealed Information Test (CIT). The administration of this test is given to prevent potential errors that may arise from the questioning style. The test is usually conducted by a tester with no knowledge of the [[crime]] or circumstances in question. The administrator tests the participant on their knowledge of the crime that would not be known to an innocent person. For example: "Was the crime committed with a .45 or a 9 mm?" The questions are in multiple choice and the participant is rated on how they react to the correct answer. If they react strongly to the guilty information, then proponents of the test believe that it is likely that they know facts relevant to the case. This administration is considered more valid by supporters of the test because it contains many safeguards to avoid the risk of the administrator influencing the results.<ref>For more info on the GKT, see the [http://websites.mscc.huji.ac.il/zerolab/GKTCHAP3.PDF The Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT) as an Application of Psychophysiology: Future Prospects and Obstacles].</ref>


The NAS conclusions paralleled those of the earlier [[United States Congress]] [[Office of Technology Assessment]] report "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation".<ref>{{cite web |title= Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation |author= Office of Technology Assessment |date= November 1983 |url=https://fas.org/sgp/othergov/polygraph/ota/ |author-link= Office of Technology Assessment }}</ref> Similarly, a report to Congress by the [[Moynihan Commission on Government Secrecy]] concluded that "The few Government-sponsored scientific research reports on polygraph validity (as opposed to its utility), especially those focusing on the screening of applicants for employment, indicate that the polygraph is neither scientifically valid nor especially effective beyond its ability to generate admissions".<ref name=moynihan>[http://www.gpo.gov/congress/commissions/secrecy/pdf/09ps.pdf IV Personnel Security: Protection Through Detection] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110612003618/http://www.gpo.gov/congress/commissions/secrecy/pdf/09ps.pdf |date=2011-06-12 }} quoting Ralph M. Carney, ''SSBI Source Yield: An Examination of Sources Contacted During the SSBI'' (Monterey: Defense Personnel Security Research Center, 1996), 6, affirming that in 81% of cases, the derogatory informations were obtained through questionnaire and/or interrogation.</ref>
==Validity==
<span id=validity> </span>
Polygraphy has little credibility among scientists.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.fas.org/sgp/othergov/polygraph/ota/index.html |accessdate=2008-02-29 |title=Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation |publisher= Washington, D. C.: U.S. Congress
Office of Technology Assessment |year=1983}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug04/polygraph.html |accessdate=2008-02-29 |date=07-2004 |title=Monitor on Psychology - The polygraph in doubt |publisher=American Psychological Association}}</ref> Despite claims of 90-95% validity by polygraph advocates, and 95-100% by businesses providing polygraph services,<ref>url=http://www.theftstopper.com/polygraph_employee_theft.htm</ref> critics maintain that rather than a "test", the method amounts to an inherently unstandardizable [[interrogation]] technique whose accuracy cannot be established. A 1997 survey of 421 psychologists estimated the test's average accuracy at about 61%, a little better than chance.<ref name = "usa">{{cite journal
| first =Dan
| last =Vergano
| year =2002
| month =September 9
| title = Telling the truth about lie detectors
| journal =USA Today
| url =http://www.usatoday.com/news/nation/2002-09-09-lie_x.htm
}}</ref> Critics also argue that even given high estimates of the polygraph's accuracy a significant number of subjects (e.g. 10% given a 90% accuracy) will appear to be lying, and would unfairly suffer the consequences of "failing" the polygraph. In the 1998 [[Supreme Court of the United States|Supreme Court]] case, ''[[United States v. Scheffer]]'', the majority stated that "There is simply no consensus that polygraph evidence is reliable" and "Unlike other expert witnesses who testify about factual matters outside the jurors' knowledge, such as the analysis of fingerprints, ballistics, or DNA found at a crime scene, a polygraph expert can supply the jury only with another opinion..."<ref name = "us">{{cite journal
| year =1998
| month =March 31
| title =United States v. Scheffer
| url =http://www.law.berkeley.edu/faculty/sklansky/evidence/evidence/cases/Cases%20for%20TOA/Scheffer,%20United%20States%20v.htm
}}</ref> Also, in 2005 the 11th Circuit Court of Appeals stated that “polygraphy did not enjoy general acceptance from the scientific community”.<ref name="pdf">{{cite journal
| year =2005
| month =May 23
| title =United States v. Henderson
| url =http://www.ca11.uscourts.gov/opinions/ops/200411545.pdf
|format=PDF}} [PDF]</ref> [[Charles Honts]], a psychology professor at [[Boise State University]], states that polygraph interrogations give a high rate of false positives on innocent people.<ref name="Hess, Pamela 2008">Hess, Pamela, "Pentagon's Intelligence Arm Steps Up Lie-Detector Efforts", ''[[Arizona Daily Star]]'', August 24, 2008. Also in Fox News via AP [http://www.foxnews.com/story/0,2933,409502,00.html]</ref> In 2001 William G. Iacono, Distinguished McKnight University Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience and Director, Clinical Science and Psychopathology Research Training Program at the University of Minnesota, published a paper titled “Forensic “Lie Detection": Procedures Without Scientific Basis” in the peer reviewed ''Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice''. He concluded that
<blockquote>Although the CQT [Control Question Test] may be useful as an investigative aid and tool to induce confessions, it does not pass muster as a scientifically credible test. CQT theory is based on naive, implausible assumptions indicating (a) that it is biased against innocent individuals and (b) that it can be beaten simply by artificially augmenting responses to control questions. Although it is not possible to adequately assess the error rate of the CQT, both of these conclusions are supported by published research findings in the best social science journals (Honts et al., 1994; Horvath, 1977; Kleinmuntz & Szucko, 1984; Patrick & Iacono, 1991). Although defense attorneys often attempt to have the results of friendly CQTs admitted as evidence in court, there is no evidence supporting their validity and ample reason to doubt it. Members of scientific organizations who have the requisite background to evaluate the CQT are overwhelmingly skeptical of the claims made by polygraph proponents.
<ref>{{cite web|author=William G. Iacono|title= Forensic “Lie Detection": Procedures Without Scientific Basis |publisher='' Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice''|year=2001|url=http://www.psych.umn.edu/faculty/iacono/2001%20forensic%20lie%20detection%20-%20procedures%20without%20scientific.pdf|format=PDF}}</ref>
</blockquote>


Despite the NAS finding of a "high rate of false positives," failures to expose individuals such as [[Aldrich Ames]] and [[Larry Wu-Tai Chin]], and other inabilities to show a scientific justification for the use of the polygraph, it continues to be employed.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Randi |first1=James |author-link1=James Randi |title=A Consistently Erroneous Technology |journal=[[Skeptical Inquirer]] |date=2017 |volume=41 |issue=5 |pages=16–19 |url=https://www.csicop.org/si/show/a_consistently_erroneous_technology |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180728234040/https://www.csicop.org/si/show/a_consistently_erroneous_technology |archive-date=2018-07-28 |access-date=28 July 2018}}</ref>
Summarizing the consensus in psychological research, professor David W. Martin, PhD, from North Carolina State University, states that people have tried to use the polygraph for measuring human emotions, but there is simply no royal road to (measuring) human emotions.<ref>[http://www.teach12.com/ttcx/CourseDescLong2.aspx?cid=1620 Psychology of Human Behavior] The Teaching Company, course No. 1620 taught by David W. Martin, lesson 19.</ref> Therefore, since one cannot reliably measure human emotions (especially when one has an interest in hiding his/her emotions), the idea of valid detection of truth or falsehood through measuring respiratory rate, blood volume, pulse rate and galvanic skin response is a mere pretense. Since psychologists cannot ascertain what emotions one has,<ref>Professor Martin considers an urban legend the idea that psychologists could read people's minds or look inside their heads and know their thoughts/emotions, cf. lesson 1 of the same course.</ref> polygraph professionals are not able to do that either.


==Countermeasures==
Polygraphy has also been faulted for failing to trap known [[spying|spies]] such as double-agent [[Aldrich Ames]], who passed two polygraph tests while spying for the Soviet Union.<ref name="Hess, Pamela 2008"/><ref>Ames provides personal insight into the U.S. Government's reliance on polygraphy in a 2000 letter to Steven Aftergood of the Federation of American Scientists.{{cite journal
Several proposed countermeasures designed to pass polygraph tests have been described. There are two major types of countermeasures: "general state" (intending to alter the physiological or psychological state of the subject during the test), and "specific point" (intending to alter the physiological or psychological state of the subject at specific periods during the examination, either to increase or decrease responses during critical examination periods).<ref name="dpca.state.ny.us"/>
| first =Aldrich
* '''General state''': asked how he passed the polygraph test, [[Central Intelligence Agency]] officer turned [[KGB]] mole [[Aldrich Ames]] explained that he sought advice from his [[Soviet Union|Soviet]] handler and received the simple instruction to: "Get a good night's sleep, and rest, and go into the test rested and relaxed. Be nice to the polygraph examiner, develop a rapport, and be cooperative and try to maintain your calm".<ref name="david">Weiner, Tim, David Johnston, and Neil A. Lewis, ''Betrayal: The Story of Aldrich Ames, an American Spy,'' 1995.</ref> Additionally, Ames explained, "There's no special magic... Confidence is what does it. Confidence and a friendly relationship with the examiner... rapport, where you smile and you make him think that you like him".<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.dailypress.com/1994/04/29/ames-separated-spy-agent-lives/|title=Ames: Separated Spy, Agent Lives|work=tribunedigital-dailypress|date=29 April 1994 |access-date=2012-06-01|archive-date=2013-12-12|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131212021732/http://articles.dailypress.com/1994-04-29/news/9404290219_1_aldrich-h-ames-soviet-defectors-spy|url-status=live}}</ref>
| last =Ames
* '''Specific point''': other suggestions for countermeasures include for the subject to mentally record the control and relevant questions as the examiner reviews them before the interrogation begins. During the interrogation the subject is supposed to carefully control their [[breathing]] while answering the relevant questions, and to try to artificially increase their [[heart rate]] during the control questions, for example by thinking of something scary or exciting, or by pricking themselves with a pointed object concealed somewhere on the body. In this way the results will not show a significant reaction to any of the relevant questions.<ref name="tremor">Lykken, David T. ''A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector,'' 2nd ed., New York: Plenum Trade, 1998, pp. 273–279.</ref>
| authorlink =Aldrich Ames
| year = 2000
| month =November 28
| title =A Letter from Aldrich Ames on Polygraph Testing
| journal = Federation of American Scientists
| url = http://www.fas.org/sgp/othergov/polygraph/ames.html
}}</ref> Other spies who passed the polygraph include [[Karl Koecher]],<ref name="karl">Kessler, Ron. "Moscow's Mole in the CIA: How a Swinging Czech Superspy Stole America's Most Sensitive Secrets," ''Washington Post,'' April 17, 1988, C1.</ref> [[Ana Montes|Ana Belen Montes]],<ref>{{cite web|last=Bachelet|first=Pablo|title=Book outlines how spy exposed U.S. intelligence secrets to Cuba|publisher=McClatchey Washington Bureau|date=October 13, 2006|url=http://www.realcities.com/mld/krwashington/15754464.htm}} "She first came under U.S. suspicion in 1994, when Cuba detected a highly secret electronic surveillance system. Montes took a polygraph test and passed it."</ref> and [[Leandro Aragoncillo]].<ref>{{cite web|author=Ross, Brian and Richard Esposito|title=Investigation Continues: Security Breach at the White House|publisher=[[ABC News]]|date=October 6, 2005|url=http://abcnews.go.com/WNT/Investigation/story?id=1190375}} "Officials say Aragoncillo passed several lie detector tests that are routinely given to individuals with top secret clearances."</ref> However, CIA spy Harold James Nicholson failed his polygraph examinations, which aroused suspicions that led to his eventual arrest.<ref>{{cite web
| title =Dept. of Energy, Office of Counterintelligence
| work =: Harold James Nicholson Dossier
| url =http://www.hanford.gov/oci/ci_spy.cfm?dossier=62#
| accessdate=2009-06-01
}}</ref> Polygraph examination and background checks failed to detect [[Nada Nadim Prouty]], who was not a spy but was convicted for improperly obtaining US citizenship and using it to obtain a restricted position at the FBI.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2007/11/13/AR2007111302033.html|title=Ex-FBI Employee's Case Raises New Security Concerns Sham Marriage Led to U.S. Citizenship|publisher=Washington Post|date=2007-11-14 | first1=Joby | last1=Warrick | first2=Dan | last2=Eggen | accessdate=May 22, 2010}}</ref> <!--- the relevance of this case, currently unstated here for lack of sourced discussion, is that a reader might think ordinary immigrants wouldn't have some special secret polygraph-breaking training that double agents above might have --->


==Use==
The polygraph also failed to catch [[Gary Ridgway]], the "Green River Killer". Ridgway passed a polygraph in 1984 and confessed almost 20 years later when confronted with DNA evidence.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.thenewstribune.com/2003/11/09/366408/what-made-ridgway-kill-still-a.html|title=What made Ridgway kill still a riddle|publisher=The News Tribune|date=2003-11-09}}</ref>
Law enforcement agencies and intelligence agencies in the United States are by far the biggest users of polygraph technology. In the United States alone most federal law enforcement agencies either employ their own polygraph examiners or use the services of examiners employed in other agencies.<ref>{{cite web|author=Van Aperen, Steven|date=February 29, 2000 |url=http://www.nettrace.com.au/content/nta10001.htm|title=The polygraph as an investigative tool in criminal and private investigations|publisher=Net-Trace}}</ref> In 1978 [[Richard Helms]], the eighth Director of Central Intelligence, stated:


{{quote|We discovered there were some Eastern Europeans who could defeat the polygraph at any time. Americans are not very good at it, because we are raised to tell the truth and when we lie it is easy to tell we are lying. But we find a lot of Europeans and Asiatics can handle that polygraph without a blip, and you know they are lying and you have evidence that they are lying.<ref>{{cite web|url= http://mcadams.posc.mu.edu/russ/jfkinfo2/jfk4/Hscahelm.htm |title= Testimony of Richard Helms, Former Director of Central Intelligence, Former Ambassador to Iran, and Presently a Business Consultant in Washington, D.C., and Represented by Gregory B. Craig, of Williams & Connelly |website= mcadams.posc.mu.edu|date= 2001-01-29 |access-date= 2015-04-19|archivedate=July 1, 2002|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20020701034017/http://mcadams.posc.mu.edu/russ/jfkinfo2/jfk4/Hscahelm.htm}}</ref>}}
Conversely, innocent people have been known to fail polygraph tests. In [[Wichita, Kansas]] in 1986, after failing two polygraph tests (one police administered, the other given by an expert that he had hired), Bill Wegerle had to live under a cloud of suspicion of murdering his wife Vicki Wegerle, even though he was neither arrested nor convicted of her death. In March 2004, a letter was sent to [[The Wichita Eagle]] reporter Hurst Laviana that contained Vicki's drivers license and what first appeared to be crime scene photographs of her body. The photos had actually been taken by her true murderer, [[Dennis Rader|BTK]],<ref>[http://www.harpercollins.com/authors/32230/Hurst_Laviana/index.aspx www.harpercollins.com ]</ref> the serial killer that had plagued the people of Wichita since 1974 and had recently resurfaced in February 2004 after an apparent 25 year period of dormancy (he had actually killed three women between 1985 and 1991, including Wegerle). That effectively cleared Bill Wegerle of the murder of his wife. In 2005 conclusive DNA evidence, including DNA retrieved from under the fingernails of Vicki Wegerle, demonstrated that the BTK Killer was [[Dennis Rader]].<ref>[http://www.cbsnews.com/stories/2005/09/29/48hours/main890980.shtml Oct. 1, 2005 ''48 Hours Mysteries:'' BTK: Out Of The Shadows]</ref>


Susan McCarthy of ''[[Salon (website)|Salon]]'' said in 2000 that "The polygraph is an American phenomenon, with limited use in a few countries, such as Canada, Israel and Japan."<ref>McCarthy, Susan. "[http://www.salon.com/2000/03/02/polygraph/ The truth about the polygraph]." ''[[Salon (website)|Salon]]''. March 2, 2000. Retrieved on July 5, 2013.</ref>
Prolonged polygraph examinations are sometimes used as a tool by which [[confession (legal)|confession]]s are extracted from a defendant, as in the case of [[Richard Miller (agent)|Richard Miller]], who was persuaded to confess largely by polygraph results combined with appeals from a religious leader.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.afda.org/afda/news/Opinion_Miller.pdf|format=PDF|title=United States of America versus William Galbreth|date=1995-03-09}}</ref>


=== Armenia ===
Law enforcement agencies and intelligence agencies in the United States are by far the biggest users of polygraph technology. In the United States alone all federal law enforcement agencies either employ their own polygraph examiners or use the services of examiners employed in other agencies.<ref>url=http://www.nettrace.com.au/content/nta10001.htm</ref> This is despite persistent claims of unreliability. For example in 1978 [[Richard Helms]], the 8th Director of Central Intelligence, stated that:<br>
In [[Armenia]], government administered polygraphs are legal, at least for use in national security investigations. The [[National Security Service (Armenia)|National Security Service]] (NSS), Armenia's primary intelligence service, requires polygraph examinations of all new applicants.<ref>{{Cite web |title=How to become an employee of the NSS |url=https://www.sns.am/en/pages/show/officer |access-date=2023-02-15 |website=www.sns.am |language=en}}</ref>
:"We discovered there were some Eastern Europeans who could defeat the polygraph at any time. Americans are not very good at it, because we are raised to tell the truth and when we lie it is easy to tell [we] are lying. But we find a lot of Europeans and Asiatics [who] can handle that polygraph without a blip, and you know they are lying and you have evidence that they are lying."<ref>url=http://mcadams.posc.mu.edu/russ/jfkinfo2/jfk4/Hscahelm.htm</ref>


==Countermeasures==
===Australia===
Polygraph evidence is currently inadmissible in New South Wales courts under the Lie Detectors Act 1983. Under the same act, it is also illegal to use polygraphs for the purpose of granting employment, insurance, financial accommodation, and several other purposes for which polygraphs may be used in other jurisdictions.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.legislation.nsw.gov.au/maintop/view/inforce/act+62+1983+cd+0+N |title=''Lie Detectors Act 1983 (NSW)'' |publisher=Legislation.nsw.gov.au |access-date=2018-10-02 |archive-date=2018-10-03 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181003013735/https://www.legislation.nsw.gov.au/maintop/view/inforce/act+62+1983+cd+0+N |url-status=dead }}</ref>
Several countermeasures designed to pass polygraph tests have been described. Asked how he passed the polygraph test, Ames explained that he sought advice from his [[Soviet]] handler and received the simple instruction to: "Get a good night's sleep, and rest, and go into the test rested and relaxed. Be nice to the polygraph examiner, develop a rapport, and be cooperative and try to maintain your calm."<ref name="david">Weiner, Tim, David Johnston, and Neil A. Lewis, ''Betrayal: The Story of Aldrich Ames, an American Spy,'' 1995.</ref>


===Canada===
Other suggestions for countermeasures include for the subject to mentally record the control and relevant questions as the examiner reviews them prior to commencing the interrogation. Once the interrogation begins, the subject is then supposed to carefully control their [[breathing]] during the relevant questions, and to try to artificially increase their [[heart rate]] during the control questions, such as by thinking of something scary or exciting or by pricking themselves with a pointed object concealed somewhere on their body. In this way the results will not show a significant reaction to any of the relevant questions.<ref name="tremor">Lykken, David T. ''A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector,'' 2nd ed., New York: Plenum Trade, 1998, pp. 273-279.</ref><ref>[http://antipolygraph.org/ Antipolygraph.org].</ref>
In Canada, the 1987 decision of ''[[R v Béland]]'', the [[Supreme Court of Canada]] rejected the use of polygraph results as evidence in court, finding that they were inadmissible. The polygraph is still used as a tool in the investigation of criminal acts and sometimes employed in the screening of employees for government organizations.<ref>{{cite web|title=Pre-Employment Polygraph|website=[[Royal Canadian Mounted Police]]|url=http://www.rcmp-grc.gc.ca/en/pre-employment-polygraph|date=2016-05-10|access-date=2018-04-17|archive-date=2021-02-26|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210226205643/http://www.rcmp-grc.gc.ca/en/pre-employment-polygraph|url-status=dead}}</ref>


In the province of Ontario, the use of polygraphs by an employer is not permitted. A police force does have the authorization to use a polygraph in the course of the investigation of an offence.<ref>{{cite web|title=Lie Detector Tests |website= [[Ministry of Labour (Ontario)|Ontario Ministry of Labour]]|url=http://www.labour.gov.on.ca/english/es/pubs/guide/liedetectors.php}}</ref>
==2003 National Academy of Sciences report==


===Europe===
The accuracy of the polygraph has been contested almost since the introduction of the device. In 2003, the [[United States National Academy of Sciences|National Academy of Sciences]] (NAS) issued a report entitled "The Polygraph and Lie Detection". The NAS found that the majority of polygraph research was "Unreliable, Unscientific and Biased", concluding that 57 of the approximately 80 research studies that the APA relies on to come to their conclusions were significantly flawed. These studies did show that specific-incident polygraph testing, in a person untrained in counter-measures, could discern the truth at "a level greater than chance, yet short of perfection". However, due to several flaws, the levels of accuracy shown in these studies "are almost certainly higher than actual polygraph accuracy of specific-incident testing in the field".<ref name="nap.edu"/>
In a majority of [[Europe]]an jurisdictions, polygraphs are generally considered to be unreliable for gathering evidence, and are usually not used by local law enforcement agencies. Polygraph testing is widely seen in Europe to violate [[Right to silence|the right to remain silent]].<ref name=Koppen2017>{{cite book|last1=Meijer|first1=Ewout H|last2=van Koppen|first2=Peter J|editor1-last=Canter|editor1-first=David|editor2-last=Žukauskiene|editor2-first=Rita|title=Psychology and Law : Bridging the Gap|date=2017|publisher=Routledge|isbn=978-1351907873|chapter=Chapter 3. Lie Detectors and the Law: The Use of the Polygraph in Europe|chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=4VJBDgAAQBAJ&pg=PT62}}</ref>{{rp|62ff}}


In [[England and Wales]] a polygraph test can be taken, but the results cannot be used in a court of law to prove a case.<ref>{{cite news |title=How widely are lie detectors used in the UK? |work=BBC News |date=29 June 2019 |url=https://www.bbc.com/news/uk-48775614 |publisher=British Broadcasting Corporation |access-date=28 August 2020}}</ref> However, the [[Offender Management Act 2007]] put in place an option to use polygraph tests to monitor serious sex offenders on parole in England and Wales;<ref>{{cite web | author-link= Wendy M. Grossman |last1=Grossman |first1=Wendy |title=Letter to America: The Black Box that Wouldn't Die |date=25 August 2020 |url=https://skepticalinquirer.org/exclusive/letter-to-america-the-black-box-that-wouldnt-die/ |publisher=Skeptical Inquirer |access-date=28 August 2020}}</ref> these tests became compulsory in 2014 for high risk sexual offenders currently on parole in England and Wales.<ref>{{cite web |last1=Bowcott |first1=Owen |title=Lie detector tests introduced to monitor released sex offenders |url=https://www.theguardian.com/society/2014/aug/08/lie-detector-polygraph-tests-introduced-monitor-sex-offenders |work=The Guardian |date=8 August 2014 |access-date=28 August 2020}}</ref>
When polygraphs are used as a screening tool (in [[national security]] matters and for [[Law enforcement agency|law enforcement]] agencies for example) the level of accuracy drops to such a level that "Its accuracy in distinguishing actual or potential security violators from innocent test takers is insufficient to justify reliance on its use in employee security screening in [[Federal agency|federal agencies]]." In fact, the NAS extrapolated that if the test were sensitive enough to detect 80% of spies (a level of accuracy which it did not assume), this would hardly be sufficient anyway. Let us take for example a hypothetical polygraph screening of a body of 10,000 employees among which are 10 spies. With an 80% success rate, the polygraph test would show that 8 spies and 1,992 non-spies fail the test. Thus, roughly 99.6 percent of positives (those failing the test) would be ''false'' positives. The NAS concluded that the polygraph "...may have some utility"<ref name="nap.edu"/> but that there is "little basis for the expectation that a polygraph test could have extremely high accuracy."<ref name="nap.edu">{{citation |title= The Polygraph and Lie Detection |author= Board on Behavioral, Cognitive, and Sensory Sciences and Education (BCSSE) and Committee on National Statistics (CNSTAT) |publisher= [[United States National Research Council]] |year= 2003 |url= http://www.nap.edu/books/0309084369/html/ }} ([http://www.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10420&page=212 Chapter 8: Conclusions and Recommendations, page 212])</ref>{{rp|212}}


The [[Supreme Court of Poland]] declared on January 29, 2015, that the use of polygraph in interrogation of suspects is forbidden by the Polish Code of Criminal Procedure. Its use might be allowed though if the suspect has been already accused of a crime and if the interrogated person consents of the use of a polygraph. Even then, the use of polygraph can never be used as a substitute of actual evidence.<ref>{{Cite web|date=2015-01-30|title=SN: tylko bez wariografu w przesłuchaniu I KZP 25/14|url=https://czasopismo.legeartis.org/2015/01/sad-najwyzszy-przeciwko-wariografowi/|access-date=2021-07-09|website=Czasopismo Lege Artis|language=pl-PL}}</ref>
The NAS conclusions paralleled those of the earlier [[United States Congress]] [[Office of Technology Assessment]] report "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation”.<ref>{{citation |title= Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation |author= [[Office of Technology Assessment]] |date= November 1983 |url= http://www.fas.org/sgp/othergov/polygraph/ota/ }}</ref>


As of 2017, the justice ministry and Supreme Court of both of the [[Netherlands]] and [[Germany]] had rejected use of polygraphs.<ref name=Koppen2017/>{{rp|62ff}}<ref>Bundesgerichtshof: Entscheidungen vom 17.12.1998, 1 StR 156/98, 1 StR 258/98</ref>
==Admissibility of polygraphs in court==
===United States===
{{As of|2007|alt=In 2007}}, polygraph testimony was admitted by stipulation in 19 states, and was subject to the discretion of the trial judge in federal court. The use of polygraph in court testimony remains controversial, although it is used extensively in post-conviction supervision, particularly of sex offenders. In ''Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals'' (1993),<ref>[http://www.law.cornell.edu/supct/html/92-102.ZS.html]</ref> the old [[Frye standard]] was lifted and all forensic evidence, including polygraph, had to meet the new [[Daubert standard]] in which "underlying reasoning or methodology is scientifically valid and properly can be applied to the facts at issue." While polygraph tests are commonly used in police investigations in the US, no defendant or witness can be forced to undergo the test. In ''[[United States v. Scheffer]]'' (1998),<ref>[http://www.law.cornell.edu/supct/html/96-1133.ZS.html]</ref> the [[Supreme Court of the United States|U.S. Supreme Court]] left it up to individual jurisdictions whether polygraph results could be admitted as evidence in court cases. Nevertheless, it is used extensively by [[prosecutor]]s, [[defense attorney]]s, and [[Policing in the United States|law enforcement agencies]]. In the States of [[Massachusetts]], [[Maryland]], [[New Jersey]], [[Delaware]] and [[Iowa]] it is illegal for any employer to order a polygraph either as conditions to gain employment, or if an employee has been suspected of wrongdoing. The [[Employee Polygraph Protection Act]] of 1988 (EPPA) generally prevents employers from using lie detector tests, either for pre-employment screening or during the course of employment, with certain exemptions.<ref>[http://www.dol.gov/compliance/laws/comp-eppa.htm Compliance Assistance By Law - The Employee Polygraph Protection Act (EPPA)<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref>


According to the 2017 book ''Psychology and Law: Bridging the Gap'' by psychologists [[David Canter]] and Rita Zukauskiene [[Belgium]] was the European country with the most prevalent use of polygraph testing by police, with about 300 polygraphs carried out each year in the course of police investigations. The results are not considered viable evidence in bench trials, but have been used in jury trials.<ref name=Koppen2017/>{{rp|62ff}}
In the United States, the State of [[New Mexico]] admits polygraph testing in front of [[Jury|juries]] under certain circumstances. In many other states, polygraph examiners are permitted to testify in front of judges in various types of [[Hearing (law)|hearings]] ([[Motion (legal)|Motion]] to Revoke [[Probation]], Motion to Adjudicate Guilt).


In [[Lithuania]], "polygraphs have been in use since 1992",<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Saldžiūnas |first1=Vitas |last2=Kovalenko |first2=Aleksandras |date=2008| title=The Event Knowledge Test (EKT) |url=https://repozytorium.ka.edu.pl/bitstream/handle/11315/24938/SALDZIUNAS_The_event_knowledge_test_2008.pdf?sequence=6&isAllowed=y |journal=European Polygraph |volume=1 |issue=3 |page=21}}</ref> with law enforcement utilizing the Event Knowledge Test (a "modification"<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Saldžiūnas |first1=Vitas |last2=Kovalenko |first2=Aleksandras |date=2015 |title=Selecting the Most Optimal Conditions for the Polygraph Examination |url=https://www.polygraph.pl/vol/2015-2/european-polygraph-2015-no2-saldziunas-kovalenka.pdf |journal=European Polygraph |volume=9 |issue=2 (32) |page=70 |doi=10.1515/ep-2015-0003|s2cid=148000927 }}</ref> of the Concealed Information Test) in criminal investigations.
==== The American Polygraph Association ====
The American Polygraph Association (APA) is a non-profit United States organization, representing more than 3,200 experienced polygraph examiners in private business, law enforcement and government. The APA establishes standards of ethical practices, techniques, instrumentation and research, as well as provides advanced training and continuing education programs.<ref name="American Polygraph Association, National Website">[http://www.polygraph.org, "American Polygraph Association"]</ref>


===Europe===
===India===
In 2008, an Indian court adopted the [[Brain Electrical Oscillation Signature Profiling]] test as evidence to convict a woman who was accused of murdering her fiancé.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gaudet | first1 = Lyn M | title = Brain Fingerprinting, Scientific Evidence, and "Daubert": A Cautionary Lesson from India | year = 2011 | url = https://www.jstor.org/stable/41307131 | journal = Jurimetrics: The Journal of Law, Science & Technology | volume = 51 | issue = 3| pages = 293–318 | jstor = 41307131 }}</ref> It was the first time that the result of polygraph was used as evidence in court.<ref>{{cite news|last=Giridharadas |first=Anand|title = India's Novel Use of Brain Scans in Courts is Debated|work=The New York Times|url=https://www.nytimes.com/2008/09/15/world/asia/15brainscan.html|date=September 14, 2008|access-date = 2008-09-15}}</ref>
In most [[Europe]]an jurisdictions, polygraphs are not considered reliable evidence and are not generally used by police forces. Courts themselves do not order or pay for polygraph tests. In most cases, polygraph tests are voluntarily taken by a defendant in order to substantiate his or her claims.
On May 5, 2010, [[The Supreme Court of India]] declared use of [[narcoanalysis]], [[brain mapping]] and polygraph tests on suspects as illegal and against the constitution if consent is not obtained and forced.<ref>{{cite news|title = No narcoanalysis test without consent, says SC|work=The Times of India|url=http://timesofindia.indiatimes.com/India/No-narcoanalysis-test-without-consent-says-SC/articleshow/5892348.cms|date=May 5, 2010|access-date = 2010-05-05|first1=Dhananjay|last1=Mahapatra}}</ref> Article 20(3) of the Indian Constitution states: "No person accused of any offence shall be compelled to be a witness against himself."<ref>{{cite journal|url=https://www.ijlmh.com/paper/right-against-self-incrimination-a-detailed-study-analysis-of-laws-prevailing-in-india/|title=Right against Self-Incrimination: A Detailed Study & Analysis of Laws Prevailing in India|author=Mittal, Akshat; Mishra, Aakarsh|journal=International Journal of Law Management and Humanities|date=2021|accessdate=December 21, 2021|archivedate=April 11, 2021|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20210411160316/https://www.ijlmh.com/paper/right-against-self-incrimination-a-detailed-study-analysis-of-laws-prevailing-in-india/}}</ref> Polygraph tests are still legal if the defendant requests one.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.hindustantimes.com/india-news/polygraph-test-can-only-be-conducted-with-consent-of-the-accused-karnataka-hc-101615972372932.html|newspaper=[[Hindustan Times]]|author=Bose, Joydeep|title=Polygraph test can only be conducted with consent of the accused: Karnataka HC|date=March 17, 2021|accessdate=December 21, 2021|archivedate=March 17, 2021|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20210317101847/https://www.hindustantimes.com/india-news/polygraph-test-can-only-be-conducted-with-consent-of-the-accused-karnataka-hc-101615972372932.html}}</ref>


===Israel===
The [[Federal Court of Justice of Germany]] has ruled that polygraph evidence is not admissible in court.<ref>Bundesgerichtshof: Entscheidungen vom 17.12.1998, 1 StR 156/98, 1 StR 258/98</ref>
The [[Supreme Court of Israel]], in Civil Appeal 551/89 (''Menora Insurance v. Jacob Sdovnik''), ruled that the polygraph has not been recognized as a reliable device. In other decisions, polygraph results were ruled inadmissible in criminal trials. Polygraph results are only admissible in [[civil trial]]s if the person being tested agrees to it in advance.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.timesofisrael.com/when-a-job-interview-turns-into-an-interrogation/|title=When a job interview turns into an interrogation|magazine=[[Times of Israel]]|author=Weinglass, Simona|date=March 6, 2016|accessdate=November 16, 2021|archivedate=March 7, 2016|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20160307105056/https://www.timesofisrael.com/when-a-job-interview-turns-into-an-interrogation/}}</ref>


===Canada===
===Philippines===
The results of polygraph tests are inadmissible in court in the [[Philippines]].<ref>{{cite news |last1=Patag |first1=Kristine Joy |title=How forensic science is making a breakthrough in the Philippines |url=https://www.philstar.com/headlines/2018/03/27/1797029/how-forensic-science-making-breakthrough-philippines |access-date=9 September 2023 |work=The Philippine Star |date=27 May 2018}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |title=G.R. Nos. 116196-97: Philippines vs. Pablo Adoviso |url=https://elibrary.judiciary.gov.ph/thebookshelf/showdocs/1/37355 |website=Supreme Court E-Library |publisher=Supreme Court of the Philippines |access-date=9 September 2023}}</ref> The [[National Bureau of Investigation (Philippines)|National Bureau of Investigation]] do use polygraphs in aid of investigation.<ref>{{cite news |last1=Mayol |first1=Ador Vincent |last2=Bongcac |first2=Doris C. |last3=Pateña |first3=Patricia Andrea |title=Polygraph tests start for Ballesteros, center staff |url=https://newsinfo.inquirer.net/131345/polygraph-tests-start-for-ballesteros-center-staff |access-date=9 September 2023 |newspaper=Philippine Daily Inquirer |date=20 January 2012 |language=en}}</ref>
In Canada, the polygraph is regularly used as a forensic tool in the investigation of criminal acts and sometimes employed in the screening of employees for government organizations. In the 1987 decision of ''[[R. v. Béland]]'', the [[Supreme Court of Canada]] rejected the use of polygraph results as evidence in court. This decision did not however affect the use of the polygraph in criminal investigations. The polygraph continues to be used as an investigative tool.


===Australia===
===United States===
[[File:DOD polygraph brochure.pdf|thumb|Brochure of the [[Defense Security Service]] (DSS) about polygraph testing]] [[File:Administration of Polygraph.jpg|thumb|Demonstrating the administration of the polygraph, the polygrapher making notes on the readouts. 1970s]]
The [[High Court of Australia]] has not yet considered the admissibility of polygraph evidence. However, the [[New South Wales]] District Court rejected the use of the device in a criminal trial. In ''Raymond George Murray'' 1982 7A Crim R48 Sinclair DCJ refused to admit polygraph evidence tending to support the defence. The judge rejected the evidence because
[[File:NSApolygraphvideo.webm|thumb|"The Truth About the Polygraph" ([[National Security Agency]] (NSA)-produced video on the polygraph process)]]
#The veracity of the accused and the weight to be given to his evidence, and other witnesses called in the trial, was a matter for the jury.
In 2018, [[Wired (magazine)|''Wired'' magazine]] reported that an estimated 2.5 million polygraph tests were given each year in the United States, with the majority administered to [[paramedics]], [[police officers]], [[firefighters]], and [[State police|state troopers]]. The average cost to administer the test is more than $700 and is part of a $2 billion industry.<ref name=wiredcost>{{cite news |last=Harris |first=Mark |url=https://www.wired.com/story/inside-polygraph-job-screening-black-mirror/ |title=The Lie Generator: Inside the Black Mirror World of Polygraph Job Screenings |magazine=[[Wired (magazine)|Wired]] |date=October 1, 2018|access-date=October 2, 2018}}</ref>
#The polygraph "expert" sought to express an opinion as to ultimate facts in issue, which is peculiarly the province of the jury.
#The test purported to be expert evidence by the witness who was not qualified as an expert, he was merely an operator and assessor of a polygraph. The scientific premise upon which his assessment was based had not been proved in any Court in Australia.
#Devoid of any proved or accepted scientific basis, the evidence of the operator is [[hearsay]] which is inadmissible.
The Court cited, with approval, the Canadian case of ''Phillion v R'' 1978 1SCR 18.


{{As of|2007|alt=In 2007}}, polygraph testimony was admitted by stipulation in 19 states, and was subject to the discretion of the trial judge in federal court. The use of polygraph in court testimony remains controversial, although it is used extensively in post-conviction supervision, particularly of sex offenders. In ''[[Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc.]]'' (1993),<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.law.cornell.edu/supct/html/92-102.ZS.html|title=Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U.S. 579 (1993).|publisher=[[Cornell University]]}}</ref> the old [[Frye standard]] was lifted and all forensic evidence, including polygraph, had to meet the new [[Daubert standard]] in which "underlying reasoning or methodology is scientifically valid and properly can be applied to the facts at issue." While polygraph tests are commonly used in police investigations in the US, no defendant or witness can be forced to undergo the test unless they are under the supervision of the courts.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Vance |first1=Stephen |title=Looking at the Law: An Updated Look at the Privilege Against Self-Incrimination in PostConviction Supervision |journal=Federal Probation |number=1 |volume=75 |url=https://www.uscourts.gov/sites/default/files/75_1_6_0.pdf}}</ref> In ''[[United States v. Scheffer]]'' (1998),<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.law.cornell.edu/supct/html/96-1133.ZS.html|title=United States v. Scheffer, 523 U.S. 303 (1998)|publisher=[[Cornell University]]}}</ref> the US Supreme Court left it up to individual jurisdictions whether polygraph results could be admitted as evidence in court cases. Nevertheless, it is used extensively by [[prosecutor]]s, [[defense attorney]]s, and [[Policing in the United States|law enforcement agencies]]. In the states of [[Rhode Island]], [[Massachusetts]], [[Maryland]], [[New Jersey]], [[Oregon]], [[Delaware]] and [[Iowa]] it is illegal for any employer to order a polygraph either as conditions to gain employment, or if an employee has been suspected of wrongdoing.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://malegislature.gov/Laws/GeneralLaws/PartI/TitleXXI/Chapter149/Section19B|title=General Law – Part I, Title XXI, Chapter 149, Section 19B|website=malegislature.gov|access-date=2019-04-03}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=https://law.justia.com/codes/maryland/2013/article-gle/section-3-702/|title=2013 Maryland Code :: Labor and Employment :: § 3-702 – Lie detector tests|website=Justia Law|language=en|access-date=2019-04-03}}</ref> The [[Employee Polygraph Protection Act]] of 1988 (EPPA) generally prevents employers from using lie detector tests, either for [[pre-employment screening]] or during the course of employment, with certain exemptions.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.dol.gov/compliance/laws/comp-eppa.htm|title=Compliance Assistance By Law – The Employee Polygraph Protection Act (EPPA)|work=dol.gov|url-status=dead|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20050923221110/http://www.dol.gov/compliance/laws/comp-eppa.htm|archive-date=2005-09-23}}</ref> As of 2013, about 70,000 job applicants are polygraphed by the federal government on an annual basis.<ref name=TaylorWootsonPolyFeds>Taylor, Marisa and Cleve R. Wootson Jr. "[http://www.mcclatchydc.com/2013/08/16/199590/seeing-threats-feds-target-instructors.html#.UiIeOn9fuSp Seeing threats, feds target instructors of polygraph-beating methods]". ''[[The McClatchy Company|McClatchy Newspapers]]''. August 16, 2013. Retrieved August 31, 2013.</ref> In the United States, the State of [[New Mexico]] admits polygraph testing in front of [[Jury|juries]] under certain circumstances.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://swrtc.nmsu.edu/files/2014/12/New-Mexico-Rules-of-Evidence.pdf|title=New Mexico Rules of Evidence|website=New Mexico State University, Southwest Regional Training Centre|access-date=30 March 2019|archive-date=24 September 2020|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200924184043/https://swrtc.nmsu.edu/files/2014/12/New-Mexico-Rules-of-Evidence.pdf|url-status=dead}}</ref>
Lie detector evidence is currently inadmissible in New South Wales courts under the ''Lie Detectors Act'' <ref>http://www.legislation.nsw.gov.au/maintop/view/inforce/act+62+1983+cd+0+N ''Lie Detectors Act 1983 (NSW)'' </ref>.


In 2010 the NSA produced a video explaining its polygraph process.<ref name=Nageshvideo>{{cite news|author=Nagesh, Gautham|url=https://thehill.com/policy/technology/163354-nsa-video-tries-to-dispel-fear-about-polygraph-use-during-job-interviews/|title=NSA video tries to dispel fear about polygraph use during job interviews|work=[[The Hill (newspaper)|The Hill]]|date=June 14, 2010|access-date=June 15, 2013}}</ref> The video, ten minutes long, is titled "The Truth About the Polygraph" and was posted to the website of the [[Defense Security Service]]. Jeff Stein of ''[[The Washington Post]]'' said that the video portrays "various applicants, or actors playing them—it’s not clear—describing everything bad they had heard about the test, the implication being that none of it is true."<ref name=Steinpolygraph>Stein, Jeff. "[https://archive.today/20130705215730/http://voices.washingtonpost.com/spy-talk/2010/06/facing_nsas_lie_detector_relax.html NSA lie detectors no sweat, video says]". ''[[The Washington Post]]''. June 14, 2010. Retrieved on July 5, 2013.</ref> AntiPolygraph.org argues that the NSA-produced video omits some information about the polygraph process; it produced a video responding to the NSA video.<ref name=Nageshvideo/> George Maschke, the founder of the website, accused the NSA polygraph video of being "[[Orwellian]]".<ref name=Steinpolygraph/>
===Israel===
The High Court of Israel, in Civil Appeal 551/89 (Menora Insurance Vs. Jacob Sdovnik), ruled that as the polygraph has not been recognized as a reliable device, polygraph results are inadmissible as evidence in a [[civil trial]]. In other decisions, polygraph results were ruled inadmissible in criminal trials. However, some insurance companies attempt to include a clause in insurance contracts, in which the beneficiary agrees that polygraph results be admissible as evidence. In such cases, where the beneficiary has willingly agreed to such a clause, signed the contract, and taken the test, the courts will honor the contract, and take the polygraph results into consideration. Interestingly, it is common practice for [[lawyer]]s to advise people who signed such contracts to refuse to take the test. Depending on whether or not the beneficiary signed an agreements clause, and whether the test was already taken or not, such a refusal usually has no ill effects; at worst, the court will simply order the person to take the test as agreed. At best, the court will cancel the clause and release the person from taking the test, or rule the evidence inadmissible.


The polygraph was invented in 1921 by [[John Augustus Larson]], a medical student at the [[University of California, Berkeley]] and a police officer of the [[Berkeley Police Department]] in Berkeley, California.<ref name=PolygraphFAQ>{{cite web|title=Polygraph/Lie Detector FAQs|publisher=International League of Polygraph Examiners|url=http://www.theilpe.com/faq_eng.html}}</ref> The polygraph was on the ''[[Encyclopædia Britannica]]'' 2003 list of greatest inventions, described as inventions that "have had profound effects on human life for better or worse."<ref>{{cite encyclopedia|url=http://corporate.britannica.com/press/inventions.html |title=Encyclopædia Britannica's Great Inventions |encyclopedia=Encyclopædia Britannica Almanac 2003, via Wayback Machine |access-date=5 August 2014 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120519003729/http://corporate.britannica.com/press/inventions.html |archive-date=May 19, 2012}}</ref> In 2013, the US federal government had begun indicting individuals who stated that they were teaching methods on how to defeat a polygraph test.<ref name=TaylorWootsonPolyFeds/><ref>"[https://www.justice.gov/opa/pr/owner-polygraph-indicted-allegedly-training-customers-lir-during-federally-administered "Owner of 'Polygraph.com' Indicted for Allegedly Training Customers to Lie During Federally Administered Polygraph Examinations"]. Department of Justice, November 14, 2014. Accessed March 19, 2018.</ref><ref>Taylor, Marisa. "[http://seattletimes.com/html/nationworld/2021772209_lyingpenaltyxml.html Indiana man gets 8 months for lie-detector fraud]". ''[[The Seattle Times]]''. [[The McClatchy Company|McClatchy Newspapers]]. September 6, 2013. Retrieved on September 8, 2013.</ref> During one of those investigations, upwards of 30 federal agencies were involved in investigations of almost 5000 people who had various degrees of contact with those being prosecuted or who had purchased books or DVDs on the topic of beating polygraph tests.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://arstechnica.com/tech-policy/2013/09/coach-who-taught-people-how-to-beat-lie-detectors-headed-to-prison/|title=Coach who taught people how to beat lie detectors headed to prison|work=Ars Technica|date=2013-09-09}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.mcclatchydc.com/2013/11/14/208438/americans-personal-data-shared.html|title=Washington: Americans' personal data shared with CIA, IRS, others in security probe|work=McClatchy DC}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://seattletimes.com/html/nationworld/2021772209_lyingpenaltyxml.html|title=Indiana man gets 8 months for lie-detector fraud|work=The Seattle Times}}</ref>
===India===
Recently an Indian court adopted the [[brain electrical oscillations signature test]] as evidence to convict a woman, who was accused of murdering her fiance. It is the first time that the result of polygraph was used as evidence in court.<ref>{{cite news|last=GIRIDHARADAS|first=Anand|title = India's Novel Use of Brain Scans in Courts is Debated|publisher=''The New York Times''|url=http://www.nytimes.com/2008/09/15/world/asia/15brainscan.html|date=September 14, 2008|accessdate = 2008-09-15}}</ref>
On May 5, 2010, [[The Supreme Court of India]] declared use of [[narcoanalysis]], [[brain mapping]] and polygraph tests on suspects as illegal and as against constitution.<ref>{{cite news|title = No narcoanalysis test without consent, says SC|publisher=''The Times Of India''|url=http://timesofindia.indiatimes.com/India/No-narcoanalysis-test-without-consent-says-SC/articleshow/5892348.cms|date=May 5, 2010|accessdate = 2010-05-05|first1=Dhananjay|last1=Mahapatra}}</ref> Article 20(3) of the Indian Constitution-"No person accused of any offence shall be compelled to be a witness against himself." Polygraph tests are still legal if the defendant requests one, however.


==Use with espionage and security clearances==
==Security clearances==
In 1995, [[Harold James Nicholson]], a CIA employee later convicted of spying for [[Russia]], had undergone his periodic five-year reinvestigation, in which he showed a strong probability of deception on questions regarding relationships with a foreign intelligence unit. This polygraph test later led to an investigation which resulted in his eventual arrest and conviction. In most cases, however, polygraphs are more of a tool to "scare straight" those who would consider espionage. [[Jonathan Pollard]] was advised by his Israeli handlers that he was to resign his job from American intelligence if he was ever told he was subject to a polygraph test.{{cn|date= September 2023}} Likewise, [[John Anthony Walker]] was advised by his handlers not to engage in espionage until he had been promoted to the highest position for which a polygraph test was not required, to refuse promotion to higher positions for which polygraph tests were required, and to retire when promotion was mandated.<ref name=NatResearchCouncil/>
{{Globalize|section|date=April 2010}}
In the American military and intelligence communities, polygraphs have been administered both as terms of qualifying for a security clearance and as part of a periodic reinvestigation to retain a clearance. There is no uniform standard for whether the polygraph is needed, as some methods of adjudication do not demand a successful polygraph test to earn a clearance. Other agencies, particularly certain military units, actually prohibit polygraph testing on their members.


In 1983, CIA employee [[Edward Lee Howard]] was dismissed when, during a polygraph screening, he truthfully answered a series of questions admitting to minor crimes such as petty theft and drug abuse. In retaliation for his perceived unjust punishment for minor offenses, he later sold his knowledge of CIA operations to the Soviet Union.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://peacecorpsonline.org/messages/messages/2629/1008570.html|title=RPCV and CIA defector Edward Howard dies in Moscow|date=2002-07-22|publisher=Peace Corps Online}}</ref>
It is difficult to precisely determine the effectiveness of polygraph results for the detection or deterrence of spying. Failure of a polygraph test could cause revocation of a security clearance, but it is inadmissible evidence in most federal courts and military courts martial. The polygraph is more often used as a deterrent to espionage rather than detection. One exception to this was the case of [[Harold James Nicholson]], a CIA employee later convicted of spying for [[Russia]]. In 1995, Nicholson had undergone his periodic five year reinvestigation where he showed a strong probability of deception on questions regarding relationships with a foreign intelligence unit. This polygraph test later launched an investigation which resulted in his eventual arrest and conviction. In most cases, however, polygraphs are more of a tool to "scare straight" those who would consider espionage. [[Jonathan Pollard]] was advised by his Israeli handlers that he was to resign his job from American intelligence if he was ever told he was subject to a polygraph test. Likewise, [[John Anthony Walker]] was advised to by his handlers not to engage in espionage until he had been promoted to the highest position for which a polygraph test was not required, to refuse promotion to higher positions for which polygraph tests were required, and to retire when promotion was mandated.<ref>{{cite book|url=http://books.google.com/?id=USg-j9esZagC|title=The polygraph and lie detection|author=Committee to Review the Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph (National Research Council (US)), Mark Harrison Moore|year=2002 | isbn=9780309084369 | publisher=National Academies Press}}</ref> As part of his [[plea bargain]] agreement for his case of espionage for the [[Soviet Union]], [[Robert Hanssen]] would be made to undergo a polygraph at any time as part of damage assessment. In Hanssen's 25-year career with the [[FBI]], not once was he made to undergo a polygraph. He later said that if he had been ordered to, he may have thought twice about espionage.{{Citation needed|date=July 2010}}


Polygraph tests may not deter espionage. From 1945 to the present, at least six Americans have committed espionage while successfully passing polygraph tests. Notable cases of two men who created a false negative result with the polygraphs were [[Larry Wu-Tai Chin]], who spied for China, and [[Aldrich Ames]], who was given two polygraph examinations while with the CIA, the first in 1986 and the second in 1991, while spying for the Soviet Union/Russia. The CIA reported that he passed both examinations after experiencing initial indications of deception.<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.loyola.edu/dept/politics/intel/hitzrept.html|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20090705120623/http://www.loyola.edu/dept/politics/intel/hitzrept.html|url-status=dead|title=The Adrich H. Ames Case: An Assessment of CIA's Role, Oct. 21, 1994 Memorandum for Heads of Agency Offices from Director of Central Intelligence|archivedate=July 5, 2009}}</ref> According to a Senate investigation, an FBI review of the first examination concluded that the indications of deception were never resolved.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://fas.org/irp/congress/1994_rpt/ssci_ames.htm|title=An Assessment of the Aldrich H. Ames Espionage Case and Its Implications for U.S. Intelligence – Senate Select Committee on Intelligence – 01 November 1994 – Part One|work=fas.org}}</ref>
Alternatively, the use of polygraph testing, where it causes desperation over dismissal for past dishonesty, may encourage spying. For example, [[Edward Lee Howard]] was dismissed from the CIA when, during a polygraph screening, he truthfully answered a series of questions admitting to minor crimes such as petty theft and drug abuse. The CIA failed to see that the firing was an action that would logically anger Howard, and in retaliation for his perceived unjust punishment for minor offenses, he later sold his knowledge of CIA operations to the Soviet Union.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://peacecorpsonline.org/messages/messages/2629/1008570.html|title=RPCV and CIA defector Edward Howard dies in Moscow|date=2002-07-22|publisher=Peace Corps Online}}</ref>


[[Ana Belen Montes]], a Cuban spy, passed a counterintelligence scope polygraph test administered by DIA in 1994.<ref>{{cite news| url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/sf/feature/wp/2013/04/18/ana-montes-did-much-harm-spying-for-cuba-chances-are-you-havent-heard-of-her/ | newspaper=The Washington Post | title=Magazine}}</ref>
It is also worth noting that polygraph tests may not deter espionage. From 1945 to the present, at least six Americans had been committing espionage while they successfully passed polygraph tests. Two of the most notable cases of two men who created a false negative result with the polygraphs were [[Larry Wu-Tai Chin]] and [[Aldrich Ames]]. Ames was given two polygraph examinations while with the CIA, the first in 1986 and the second in 1991. The CIA reported that he passed both examinations after experiencing initial indications of deception.<ref>[http://www.loyola.edu/dept/politics/intel/hitzrept.html The Adrich H. Ames Case: An Assessment of CIA's Role, Oct. 21, 1994 Memorandum for Heads of Agency Offices from Director of Central Intelligence]</ref> According to a Senate investigation, an FBI review of the first examination concluded that the indications of deception were never resolved.<ref>[http://www.fas.org/irp/congress/1994_rpt/ssci_ames.htm An Assessment of the Aldrich H. Ames Espionage Case and Its Implications for U.S. Intelligence, Senate Select Committee on Intelligence, November 1, 1994]</ref> The Senate committee reported that the second examination, at a time when Ames was under suspicion, resulted in indications of deception and a retest a few days later with a different examiner. The second examiner concluded that there were no further indications of deception. In the CIA's analysis of the second exam, they were critical of their own failure to convey to their examiner the existing suspicions that were not addressed in the examination.


Despite these errors, in August 2008, the US [[Defense Intelligence Agency]] (DIA) announced that it would subject each of its 5,700 prospective and current employees to polygraph testing at least once annually.<ref name="Hess, Pamela 2008">Hess, Pamela, "Pentagon's Intelligence Arm Steps Up Lie-Detector Efforts", ''[[Arizona Daily Star]]'', August 24, 2008. Also in Fox News via AP [http://www.foxnews.com/story/0,2933,409502,00.html]</ref> This expansion of polygraph screening at DIA occurred while DIA polygraph managers ignored documented technical problems discovered in the Lafayette computerized polygraph system.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.mcclatchydc.com/2013/05/20/191542/glitch-in-widely-used-polygraph.html|title=Glitch in widely used polygraph can skew results|work=McClatchy DC|access-date=2013-05-26|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130527014932/http://www.mcclatchydc.com/2013/05/20/191542/glitch-in-widely-used-polygraph.html|archive-date=2013-05-27|url-status=dead}}</ref> The DIA uses computerized Lafayette polygraph systems for routine counterintelligence testing. The impact of the technical flaws within the Lafayette system on the analysis of recorded physiology and on the final polygraph test evaluation is currently unknown.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.mcclatchydc.com/polygraph/|title=McClatchy – The Polygraph Files|work=mcclatchydc.com|access-date=2013-05-26|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130527013422/http://www.mcclatchydc.com/polygraph/|archive-date=2013-05-27|url-status=dead}}</ref>
In August 2008, the US [[Defense Intelligence Agency]] announced that it would subject each of its 5,700 prospective and current employees to a polygraph interrogation at least once annually.<ref name="Hess, Pamela 2008"/>


In 2012, a [[The McClatchy Company|McClatchy]] investigation found that the [[National Reconnaissance Office]] was possibly breaching ethical and legal boundaries by encouraging its polygraph examiners to extract personal and private information from [[United States Department of Defense|US Department of Defense]] personnel during polygraph tests that purported to be limited in scope to counterintelligence matters.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.mcclatchydc.com/2012/07/10/155644/the-ig-complaint-of-mark-phillips.html|title=The IG complaint of Mark Phillips concerning the NRO|work=McClatchy DC}}</ref> Allegations of abusive polygraph practices were brought forward by former NRO polygraph examiners.<ref>Taylor, Marisa, "[http://www.mcclatchydc.com/2012/07/27/158078/sen-charles-grassley-seeks-probe.html Sen. Charles Grassley Seeks Probe Of Polygraph Techniques At National Reconnaissance Office]", ''[[The McClatchy Company]]'', 27 July 2012</ref>
==Hand-held lie detector for U.S. military==
A hand-held lie detector is being deployed by the U.S. Department of Defense according to a report in 2008 by investigative reporter [[Bill Dedman]] of [[msnbc.com]]. The Preliminary Credibility Assessment Screening System, or PCASS, captures less physiological information than a polygraph, and uses an algorithm, not the judgment of a polygraph examiner, to render a decision whether it believes the person is being deceptive or not. The device will be used first in Afghanistan by U.S. Army troops. The Department of Defense ordered to limit its use to non-U.S. persons.<ref>{{cite journal
| first =Bill
| last =Dedman
| year =2008
| month =April 9
| title = New anti-terror weapon: Hand-held lie detector
| journal =Msnbc.com
| url =http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/23926278
}}</ref>


==Alternative tests==
==Use with sex offenders==
Most polygraph researchers have focused more on the exam's predictive value on a subject's guilt. However, there have been no empirical theories established to explain how a polygraph measures deception. A 2010 study indicated that [[functional magnetic resonance imaging]] (fMRI) may benefit in explaining the psychological correlations of polygraph exams. It could also explain which parts of the brain are active when subjects use artificial memories.{{clarify |date=October 2016 |reason=What is an artificial memory?}}<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Bell | first1 = B. G. | last2 = Grubin | first2 = D. | year = 2010 | title = Functional magnetic resonance imaging may promote theoretical understanding of the polygraph test | journal = Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology | volume = 21 | issue = 1| pages = 52–65 | doi=10.1080/14789940903220676| s2cid = 144428132 }}</ref> Most brain activity occurs in both sides of the [[prefrontal cortex]], which is linked to response inhibition. This indicates that deception may involve inhibition of truthful responses.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Langleben | first1 = DD | last2 = Schroeder | first2 = L | last3 = Maldjian | first3 = JA | last4 = Gur | first4 = RC | last5 = McDonald | first5 = S | last6 = Ragland | first6 = JD | last7 = O'Brien | first7 = CP | last8 = Childress | first8 = AR | year = 2002 | title = Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study | journal = NeuroImage | volume = 15 | issue = 3| pages = 727–732 | doi = 10.1006/nimg.2001.1003 | pmid = 11848716 | s2cid = 14676750 }}</ref> Some researchers believe that [[mental chronometry|reaction time]] (RT) based tests may replace polygraphs in concealed information detection. RT based tests differ from polygraphs in stimulus presentation duration and can be conducted without physiological recording as subject response time is measured via computer. However, researchers have found limitations to these tests as subjects voluntarily control their reaction time, deception can still occur within the response deadline, and the test itself lacks physiological recording.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Verschuere | first1 = B. | last2 = Crombez | first2 = G. | last3 = Degrootte | first3 = T. | last4 = Rosseel | first4 = Y. | year = 2010 | title = Detecting concealed information with reaction times: Validity and comparison with the polygraph | journal = Applied Cognitive Psychology | volume = 24 | issue = 7| pages = 991–1002| doi=10.1002/acp.1601}}</ref>
[[sex offender|Sexual offender]]s are now routinely polygraphed in many states of the United States and it is often a mandatory condition of [[probation]] or [[parole]]. In Texas, a state [[appellate court]] has upheld the testing of sex offenders under community supervision and has also upheld written statements given by sex offenders if they have committed a further offense with new victims. These statements are then used when a motion is filed to revoke probation and the probationer may then be sentenced to prison for having violated his or her probation.{{Citation needed|date=July 2010}}


==History==
Regular polygraph testing is sometimes also used during the rehabilitation of convicted sex offenders. Questioning the offender specifically about their inner thoughts, [[Desire (emotion)|desires]], and [[Impulse (psychology)|impulses]] is intended to give a general indication of their treatment progress and likelihood of future offenses. Similarly, predatory or violent offenders at some facilities may also undergo [[Penile plethysmograph|testing]] for involuntary physical arousal when shown provocative images relating to their past crimes. Perhaps the most well-known example of this rehabilitation technique is practiced at [[Coalinga State Hospital]] in California.<ref>[http://www.dmh.ca.gov/Services_and_Programs/State_Hospitals/Coalinga/Treatment.asp] Coalinga State Hospital Treatment Description</ref>
Earlier societies utilized elaborate methods of [[lie detection]] which mainly involved [[torture]]. For instance, in the Middle Ages, boiling water was used to detect liars, as it was believed honest men would withstand it better than liars.<ref name="Grubin, D. 2005">{{cite journal | last1 = Grubin | first1 = D. | last2 = Madsen | first2 = L. | year = 2005 | title = Lie detection and the polygraph: A historical review | journal = Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology | volume = 16 | issue = 2| pages = 357–369 | doi=10.1080/14789940412331337353| s2cid = 144898901 }}</ref> Early devices for lie detection include an 1895 invention of [[Cesare Lombroso]] used to measure changes in blood pressure for police cases, a 1904 device by [[Vittorio Benussi]] used to measure breathing, the Mackenzie-Lewis Polygraph first developed by [[James Mackenzie (cardiologist)|James Mackenzie]] in 1906 and an abandoned project by American [[William Moulton Marston]] which used blood pressure to examine German prisoners of war (POWs).<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.nitv1.com/History.htm|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20071203185847/http://www.nitv1.com/History.htm|url-status=dead|archive-date=2007-12-03|title=History of CVSA|work=nitv1.com}}</ref> Marston said he found a strong positive [[#correlation and dependence|correlation]] between systolic blood pressure and lying.<ref name="Lewis, J. A. 2009"/>


Marston wrote a second paper on the concept in 1915, when finishing his undergraduate studies. He entered [[Harvard Law School]] and graduated in 1918, re-publishing his earlier work in 1917.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Marston|first1 = William M|year=1917|title = Systolic Blood Pressure Changes in Deception|url=https://zenodo.org/record/1429147|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology|volume=2|issue=2|pages=117–163|doi=10.1037/h0073583}}</ref> Marston's main inspiration for the device was his wife, [[Elizabeth Holloway Marston]].<ref name="Grubin, D. 2005"/> "According to Marston’s son, it was his mother Elizabeth, Marston's wife, who suggested to him that 'When she got mad or excited, her blood pressure seemed to climb{{'"}} (Lamb, 2001). Although Elizabeth is not listed as Marston’s collaborator in his early work, Lamb, Matte (1996), and others refer directly and indirectly to Elizabeth's work on her husband's deception research. She also appears in a picture taken in his polygraph laboratory in the 1920s (reproduced in Marston, 1938).<ref>National Research Council (2013); [http://books.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10420&page=292 Page 292]</ref>
A significant number of Federal appeals courts have upheld polygraph testing for Federal probationers as well.{{Citation needed|date=July 2010}} The most recent decision was by the [[Second Circuit Court of Appeals]] regarding a New York sex offender.{{Citation needed|date=July 2010}}


Despite his predecessors' contributions, Marston styled himself the "father of the polygraph". (Today he is often equally or more noted as the creator of the comic book character [[Wonder Woman]] and her [[Lasso of Truth]], which can force people to tell the truth.)<ref name="Lepore, Jill 2014, pages 183-209">Lepore, Jill. The Secret History of Wonder Woman, New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 2014, {{ISBN|978-0385354042}}, pp. 183–209.</ref> Marston remained the device's primary advocate, lobbying for its use in the courts. In 1938 he published a book, ''The Lie Detector Test'', wherein he documented the theory and use of the device.<ref>Marston, William Moulton. ''The Lie Detector Test''. New York: Richard R. Smith, 1938.</ref> In 1938 he appeared in advertising by the [[Gillette (brand)|Gillette]] company claiming that the polygraph showed Gillette razors were better than the competition.<ref>{{cite magazine|magazine=[[Reason (magazine)|Reason magazine]]|date=May 2001|title=William Marston's Secret Identity|url=http://www.reason.com/news/show/28014.html}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/marston-razor-high-res.pdf|title=Lie Detector Charts Emotional Effects of Shaving – 1938 Gillette Advertisement|access-date=2007-01-10|archive-date=2018-10-10|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181010213555/https://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/marston-razor-high-res.pdf|url-status=dead}}</ref><ref>[http://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/marston-fbi-file.pdf FBI File of William Moulton Marston] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070621154436/http://www.antipolygraph.org/documents/marston-fbi-file.pdf |date=2007-06-21 }}(including report on Gillette advertising campaign)</ref>
The UK will soon allow compulsory polygraph tests for convicted sex offenders released on licence.<ref name="bbc">{{cite journal
| month =1 December
| title =Lie-test plan for sex offenders
| journal =BBC
| url = http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/uk_politics/6197458.stm
| date=December 1, 2006 | accessdate=January 6, 2010
| work=BBC News}}</ref><ref name = "UK">{{cite journal
| title =Polygraph conditions for certain offenders released on licence
| journal = Office of Public Sector Information
| url =http://www.opsi.gov.uk/acts/acts2007/ukpga_20070021_en_4#pt3
| format = – <sup>[http://scholar.google.co.uk/scholar?hl=en&lr=&q=intitle%3APolygraph+conditions+for+certain+offenders+released+on+licence&as_publication=Office+of+Public+Sector+Information&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=Search Scholar search]</sup>
}} {{Dead link|date=June 2008}}</ref>


A device recording both blood pressure and breathing was invented in 1921 by [[John Augustus Larson]] of the [[University of California]] and first applied in law enforcement work by the [[Berkeley, California|Berkeley]] Police Department under its nationally renowned police chief [[August Vollmer]].<ref name=Illumin>Katherine To for Illumin. Fall 2003. [https://illumin.usc.edu/43/lie-detection-the-science-and-development-of-the-polygraph/ Lie Detection: The Science and Development of the Polygraph]</ref> Further work on this device was done by [[Leonarde Keeler]].<ref name=PolyMuseum>{{cite web|url=http://www.lie2me.net/thepolygraphmuseum/id12.html|title=Leonarde Keeler and his Instruments|work=lie2me.net}}</ref> As Larson's protege, Keeler updated the device by making it portable and added the [[galvanic skin response]] to it in 1939. His device was then purchased by the [[Federal Bureau of Investigation|FBI]], and served as the prototype of the modern polygraph.<ref name="Grubin, D. 2005"/><ref name=Illumin/>
==Polygraphy in popular culture==
Lie detection has a long history in mythology and fairy tales; the polygraph has allowed modern fiction to use a device more easily seen as scientific and plausible. Notable instances of polygraph usage include uses in crime and espionage themed television shows and some [[daytime television]] talk shows, cartoons and films. The most notable polygraph TV show is ''[[Lie Detector (TV series)|Lie Detector]]'', which first aired in the 1950s created and hosted by [[Ralph Andrews]]. Then in the 1960s Ralph produced a series of specials hosted by [[Melvin Belli]], then in the 1970s hosted by Jack Anderson. In 1998 TV producer [[Mark Phillips (producer)|Mark Phillips]] with his Mark Phillips Philms & Telephision put ''Lie Detector'' back on the air on the FOX Network—on that program Dr. Ed Gelb with host [[Marcia Clark]] cleared [[Mark Fuhrman]] from the allegation that he "planted the bloody glove." Later Phillips produced ''Lie Detector'' as a series for PAX/ION—some of the guests included [[Paula Jones]], Reverend [[Paul Crouch]] accuser Lonny Ford, Ben Rowling, [[Jeff Gannon]] and [[Swift Vets and POWs for Truth|Swift Boat Vet]], Steve Garner.


Several devices similar to Keeler's polygraph version included the Berkeley Psychograph, a blood pressure-pulse-respiration recorder developed by C. D. Lee in 1936<ref name="ReferenceA">Inbau, Fred E. Lie Detection and Criminal Interrogation, The Williams & Wilkins Company, 1948</ref> and the Darrow Behavior Research Photopolygraph, which was developed and intended solely for behavior research experiments.<ref name="ReferenceA"/><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Troville | first1 = P.V. | year = 1939 | title = A History of Lie Detection | journal = Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology | volume = 29 | issue = 6| page = 848 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Troville | first1 = P.V. | year = 1939 | title = A History of Lie Detection | journal = Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology | volume = 30 | issue = 1| page = 104 }}</ref>
FOX has taken this one step further with their game show ''[[The Moment of Truth (US game show)|The Moment of Truth]]'' which pits people's honesty against their own sense of modesty, propriety, etc. Contestants are given a polygraph test administered by a polygraph expert in a pre-screening session answering over 50 questions. Later they must sit in front of a studio audience including their friends & family for the televised portion of the show. There they need only answer 21 answers truthfully "as determined by the polygraph" to win $500,000. The questions get more personal and/or more revealing as they advance. Most polygraph experts caution that the polygraph techniques used on Moment of Truth do not conform to any known or accepted methods of polygraphy.{{Citation needed|date=September 2008}}


A device which recorded muscular activity accompanying changes in blood pressure was developed in 1945 by John E. Reid, who claimed that greater accuracy could be obtained by making these recordings simultaneously with standard blood pressure-pulse-respiration recordings.<ref name="ReferenceA"/><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Reid | first1 = J. E. | year = 1945 | title = Simulated Blood Pressure Responses in Lie-Detection Tests and a Method for Their Deception | journal = Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology | volume = 36 | issue = 1| pages = 201–215 }}</ref>
Daytime talk shows, such as [[Maury Povich]] and [[Steve Wilkos]], frequently use lie detectors to tell if someone is cheating on their significant other.


==Society and culture==
In one ''[[MacGyver]]'' episode 'Slow Death', MacGyver assists the Indian tribesmen by improvising a polygraph to weed out the crooked doctor. This is made possible by using an analog [[sphygmomanometer]] to monitor blood pressure change, and an electronic alarm clock to detect sweat. To test its reliability, MacGyver asked a passenger on the train a few 'placebo' questions. The culprit was only discovered when he was trying to hide his crime, thus his sweat triggered the alarm clock and blood pressure climbed up.
===Portrayals in media===
Lie detection has a long history in mythology and fairy tales; the polygraph has allowed modern fiction to use a device more easily seen as scientific and plausible. Notable instances of polygraph usage include uses in crime and espionage themed television shows and some [[daytime television]] talk shows, cartoons and films. Numerous TV shows have been called ''Lie Detector'' or featured the device. The first ''Lie Detector'' TV show aired in the 1950s, created and hosted by [[Ralph Andrews]]. In the 1960s Andrews produced a series of specials hosted by [[Melvin Belli]]. In the 1970s the show was hosted by Jack Anderson. In early 1983 [[Columbia Pictures Television]] put on a syndicated series hosted by [[F. Lee Bailey]].<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.impac-systems.com/world/americas/ct.asp|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20081024193643/http://www.impac-systems.com/world/americas/ct.asp|url-status=dead|title=IMPAC: F. Lee Bailey bio|archivedate=October 24, 2008}}</ref> In 1998 TV producer Mark Phillips with his Mark Phillips Philms & Telephision put ''Lie Detector'' back on the air on the FOX Network—on that program Ed Gelb with host [[Marcia Clark]] questioned [[Mark Fuhrman]] about the allegation that he "planted [[O. J. Simpson murder case|the bloody glove]]". In 2005 Phillips produced ''[[Lie Detector (TV series)|Lie Detector]]'' as a series for PAX/ION; some of the guests included [[Paula Jones]], Reverend [[Paul Crouch]] accuser Lonny Ford, Ben Rowling, [[Jeff Gannon]] and [[Swift Vets and POWs for Truth|Swift Boat Vet]], Steve Garner.<ref>{{Cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Kr3xmUi61goC&dq=Lie+Detector+F.+Lee+Bailey&pg=PA151|title=Lie Detectors: A Social History|first=Kerry|last=Segrave|date=November 18, 2014|publisher=McFarland|isbn=9780786481613 |via=Google Books}}</ref>


In the UK, shows such as ''[[The Jeremy Kyle Show]]'' used polygraph tests extensively. The show was ultimately canceled when a participant committed suicide shortly after being polygraphed. The guest was slated by Kyle on the show for failing the polygraph, but no other evidence has come forward to prove any guilt. Producers later admitted in the inquiry that they were unsure on how accurate the tests performed were.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/jeremy-kyle-show-cancelled-itv-lie-detector-accuracy-producer-tom-mclennan-a8973756.html|title=Jeremy Kyle producer unable to say how accurate lie detector tests were|newspaper=[[The Independent]]|author=Barr, Sabrina|language=en|url-status=live|date=June 25, 2019|access-date=December 15, 2022|archivedate=June 26, 2019|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20190626061810/https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/jeremy-kyle-show-cancelled-itv-lie-detector-accuracy-producer-tom-mclennan-a8973756.html}}</ref>
In the movie ''[[Harsh Times]]'' the protagonist, played by actor [[Christian Bale]], is caught trying to "beat" a polygraph test during a pre-employment screening for a federal law enforcement job. He stores a tack in the toe of his shoe and uses the pain sensation to mask his true apprehension of certain questions. The polygrapher is immediately suspicious and the examination is ended.


In the Fox game show ''[[The Moment of Truth (US game show)|The Moment of Truth]]'', contestants are privately asked personal questions a few days before the show while hooked to a polygraph. On the show they asked the same questions in front of a studio audience and members of their family. In order to advance in the game they must give a "truthful" answer as determined by the previous polygraph exam.<ref name="TVWeek">{{cite magazine|url=http://www.tvweek.com/news/2007/11/darnell_in_defense_of_the_trut.php|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080221113425/http://www.tvweek.com/news/2007/11/darnell_in_defense_of_the_trut.php|archive-date=2008-02-21|title=Darnell in Defense of the 'Truth': Fox Executive Talks About the Network's Controversial Lie Detector Show|magazine=TV Week|author=James Hibberd|date=2007-11-25|access-date=2008-03-11}}</ref>
In the movie ''[[Ocean's 13]]'', one of the characters beats a polygraph test by stepping on a tack when answering truthfully, which supposedly raises the polygraph's readings for the truthful answers so they equal the deceptive ones.


Daytime talk shows, such as [[Maury Povich]] and [[Steve Wilkos]], have used polygraphs to supposedly detect deception in interview subjects on their programs that pertain to [[Cheating (sex)|cheating]], [[child abuse]], and [[theft]].<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.popmatters.com/post/176031-thetruth-about-maury/|title=The Tested Truth About 'Maury Povich'|last=O'Dell|first=Cary|date=November 12, 2013|work=Pop Matters|access-date=9 December 2014}}</ref>
In the television series ''[[Profit (TV series)|Profit]]'', there is a memorable sequence at the end of episode "Healing" where the eponymous character, Jim Profit, manages to fool a polygraph. He does that by putting a nail through the sole of his shoe and pushing it inside of his heel while answering every question in order to even out the readings. This scene is very graphic, especially for its time, 1996. During a voice over, Profit explains the theory behind the polygraph and the flaws he intends to exploit in it.


In episode 93 of the USA popular science show [[Mythbusters]], they attempted to fool the polygraph by using pain to try to increase the readings when answering truthfully (so the machine will supposedly interpret the truthful and non-truthful answers as the same.) They also attempted to fool the polygraph by thinking happy thoughts when lying and thinking stressful thoughts when telling the truth to try to confuse the machine. However, neither technique was successful for a number of reasons. Michael Martin correctly identified each guilty and innocent subject. The show also noted the opinion that, when done properly, polygraphs are correct 80-99% of the time.<ref>For critical commentary on this episode, see {{cite web|last=Maschke|first=George|title=Mythbusters Beat the Lie Detector Episode featuring Michael Martin|publisher=AntiPolygraph.org|date=7 December 2007|url=https://antipolygraph.org/cgi-bin/forums/YaBB.pl?num=1197009999}}</ref>
In episode 93 of the US science show ''[[MythBusters]]'', the hosts attempted to fool the polygraph by using pain when answering truthfully, in order to test the notion that polygraphs interpret truthful and non-truthful answers as the same. They also attempted to fool the polygraph by thinking pleasant thoughts when lying and thinking stressful thoughts when telling the truth, to try to confuse the machine. However, neither technique was successful for a number of reasons. Michael Martin correctly identified each guilty and innocent subject. Martin suggested that when conducted properly, polygraphs are correct 98% of the time, but no scientific evidence has been offered for this.<ref>For critical commentary on this episode, see {{cite web | last=Maschke|first=George|title=Mythbusters Beat the Lie Detector Episode featuring Michael Martin|publisher=AntiPolygraph.org|date=December 7, 2007|url=https://antipolygraph.org/cgi-bin/forums/YaBB.pl?num=1197009999}}</ref>


The history of the polygraph is the subject of the [[documentary film]] ''The Lie Detector'', which first aired on ''[[American Experience]]'' on January 3, 2023.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.kpbs.org/news/2022/12/20/american-experience-the-lie-detector|author=Robinson, Jennifer|title=American Experience: The Lie Detector|publisher=[[KPBS (TV)|KPBS]]|language=en-US|url-status=live|date=December 20, 2022|access-date=January 4, 2023|archivedate=January 2, 2023|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20230102080908/https://www.kpbs.org/news/2022/12/20/american-experience-the-lie-detector}}</ref>
In season 7, episode 5 of Penn & Teller's Showtime series [[Bullshit!]], it was claimed and appeared to have been demonstrated that polygraphs can be confused or beaten by tightening up one's anal [[sphincter]]. Doug Williams, a former Oklahoma City police polygraph examiner, explained that many large arteries exist around the anal sphincter and that by tightening the muscles, the arteries will constrict, raising one's blood pressure, and registering a lie. It was then demonstrated by having a woman hooked up to a polygraph, having her write a number from 1-10 on a piece of paper (she chose 7), deny that she chose each number as asked by the examiner but tighten up her anal sphincter on the number 6. Doing this on the number 6 caused it to register as a lie, even though she was telling the truth. This episode also touched on people who have lost their security clearances, and subsequently their jobs, due to failing a polygraph even though they claimed to have told the complete and honest truth.


===Hand-held lie detector for US military===
==Usage==
A hand-held lie detector is being deployed by the US Department of Defense according to a report in 2008 by investigative reporter [[Bill Dedman]] of [[NBC News]]. The Preliminary Credibility Assessment Screening System, or PCASS, captures less physiological information than a polygraph, and uses an algorithm, not the judgment of a polygraph examiner, to render a decision whether it believes the person is being deceptive or not. The device was first used in Afghanistan by US Army troops. The Department of Defense ordered its use be limited to non-US persons, in overseas locations only.<ref>{{cite journal|first=Bill|last=Dedman|date=April 9, 2008|title=New anti-terror weapon: Hand-held lie detector|publisher=NBC News|url=https://www.nbcnews.com/id/wbna23926278}}</ref>
Lie-detector has been using to scrutinized 6 South Korea football teams in line with match-fixing scandal. All players suspected of wrongdoing will now have to take compulsory of a polygraph test. Up to July 11, 2011 ten players have been given lifetime bans and who are clean will get double annual minimum wages.<ref>[http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/world-asia-pacific-14104009 Match-fixing scandal]</ref>

===Notable cases===
Polygraphy has been faulted for failing to trap known [[Espionage|spies]] such as double-agent [[Aldrich Ames]], who passed two polygraph tests while spying for the Soviet Union.<ref name="Hess, Pamela 2008"/><ref>Ames provides personal insight into the U.S. Government's reliance on polygraphy in a 2000 letter to Steven Aftergood of the Federation of American Scientists at {{cite journal|first=Aldrich|last=Ames|author-link=Aldrich Ames|date=November 28, 2000|title=A Letter from Aldrich Ames on Polygraph Testing|journal=Federation of American Scientists|url=https://fas.org/sgp/othergov/polygraph/ames.html}}</ref> Ames failed several tests while at the CIA that were never acted on.<ref>{{cite web|author=Ronald Kessler|url=https://www.nytimes.com/1994/03/08/opinion/spies-lies-averted-eyes.html|title=Spies, Lies, Averted Eyes|work=The New York Times|date=March 8, 1994|access-date=March 19, 2018}}</ref> Other spies who passed the polygraph include [[Karl Koecher]],<ref name="karl">{{cite news|last=Kessler|first=Ron|title=Moscow's Mole in the CIA: How a Swinging Czech Superspy Stole America's Most Sensitive Secrets|newspaper=Washington Post|date=April 17, 1988|page=C1}}</ref> [[Ana Montes]],<ref>{{cite web|last=Bachelet|first=Pablo|title=Book outlines how spy exposed U.S. intelligence secrets to Cuba|publisher=McClatchy Washington Bureau|date=October 13, 2006|url=http://www.realcities.com/mld/krwashington/15754464.htm|quote=She first came under U.S. suspicion in 1994, when Cuba detected a highly secret electronic surveillance system. Montes took a polygraph test and passed it.}}</ref> and [[Leandro Aragoncillo]].<ref>{{cite web|author1=Ross, Brian |author2=Richard Esposito |name-list-style=amp |title=Investigation Continues: Security Breach at the White House|publisher=[[ABC News (United States)|ABC News]]|date=October 6, 2005|url=https://abcnews.go.com/WNT/Investigation/story?id=1190375|quote=Officials say Aragoncillo passed several lie detector tests that are routinely given to individuals with top secret clearances.}}</ref> CIA spy [[Harold James Nicholson]] failed his polygraph examinations, which aroused suspicions that led to his eventual arrest.<ref>{{cite web|title=Dept. of Energy, Office of Counterintelligence|work=Harold James Nicholson Dossier|url=http://www.hanford.gov/oci/ci_spy.cfm?dossier=62#|access-date=2009-06-01|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080921234938/http://www.hanford.gov/oci/ci_spy.cfm?dossier=62|archive-date=2008-09-21|url-status=dead}}</ref> Polygraph examination and background checks failed to detect [[Nada Nadim Prouty]], who was not a spy but was convicted for improperly obtaining US citizenship and using it to obtain a restricted position at the FBI.<ref>{{cite news|url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2007/11/13/AR2007111302033.html|title=Ex-FBI Employee's Case Raises New Security Concerns Sham Marriage Led to U.S. Citizenship|newspaper=Washington Post|date=2007-11-14 |first1=Joby |last1=Warrick |first2=Dan |last2=Eggen |access-date=May 22, 2010}}</ref><!--- the relevance of this case, currently unstated here for lack of sourced discussion, is that a reader might think ordinary immigrants wouldn't have some special secret polygraph-breaking training that double agents above might have --->

The polygraph also failed to catch [[Gary Ridgway]], the "Green River Killer". Another suspect allegedly failed a given lie detector test, whereas Ridgway passed.<ref name="Lewis, J. A. 2009"/> Ridgway passed a polygraph in 1984; he confessed almost 20 years later when confronted with DNA evidence.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.thenewstribune.com/news/special-reports/article25855150.html|title=What made Ridgway kill still a riddle|work=The News Tribune|date=2003-11-09}}</ref> Conversely, innocent people have been known to fail polygraph tests. In [[Wichita, Kansas]] in 1986, Bill Wegerle was suspected of murdering his wife Vicki Wegerle because he failed two polygraph tests (one administered by the police, the other conducted by an expert that Wegerle had hired), although he was neither arrested nor convicted of her death. In March 2004, evidence surfaced connecting her death to the serial killer known as BTK, and in 2005 DNA evidence from the Wegerle murder confirmed that BTK was [[Dennis Rader]], exonerating Wegerle.<ref>{{cite episode|url=https://www.cbsnews.com/news/btk-out-of-the-shadows/|title=BTK: Out Of The Shadows|series=48 Hours Mysteries|network=CBS News|date=October 1, 2005}}</ref>

Prolonged polygraph examinations are sometimes used as a tool by which [[Confession (law)|confessions]] are extracted from a defendant, as in the case of [[Richard Miller (agent)|Richard Miller]], who was persuaded to confess largely by polygraph results combined with appeals from a religious leader.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.afda.org/afda/news/Opinion_Miller.pdf|title=United States of America versus William Galbreth|date=1995-03-09|access-date=2008-02-24|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080227114423/http://www.afda.org/afda/news/Opinion_Miller.pdf|archive-date=2008-02-27|url-status=dead}}</ref> In the [[Watts family murders]], Christopher Watts failed one such polygraph test and subsequently confessed to murdering his wife.<ref>{{Cite news|url=https://abc13.com/chris-watts-wife-killed-our-girls-so-i-strangled-her/4009246/|title=Chris Watts: Wife killed our girls, so I strangled her|date=August 20, 2018|publisher=[[KTRK-TV]]|location=[[Houston]]|access-date=February 2, 2019}}</ref> In the 2002 disappearance of seven-year-old [[Danielle van Dam]] of [[San Diego]], police suspected neighbor David Westerfield; he became the prime suspect when he allegedly failed a polygraph test.<ref>{{cite web|last=Roth|first=Alex|url=http://legacy.utsandiego.com/news/metro/danielle/20030109-9999_1m9david.html|title=Westerfield failed polygraph test badly: 'Greater than 99%' chance he was lying, examiner says on tape|work=San Diego Union-Tribune|date=January 9, 2003|url-status=dead|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120929224315/http://legacy.utsandiego.com/news/metro/danielle/20030109-9999_1m9david.html|archive-date=September 29, 2012}}</ref>


==See also==
==See also==
{{Portal|United States}}<!--A polygraph test if of American origin-->
* [[Brain fingerprinting]]
* [[Bogus pipeline]]
* [[Cleve Backster]]
* [[Cleve Backster]]
* [[Doug Williams (polygraph critic)]]
* [[Ecological fallacy]]
* [[Ecological fallacy]]
* [[Lie detection]]
* [[Voice stress analysis]]
* [[Ronald Pelton]]
* [[Ronald Pelton]]
* [[Google "Quadri-Track ZCT"]]
* [[Silent Talker Lie Detector]]
* [[Voice stress analysis]]
* [[P300 (neuroscience)#Applications]]


==Notes==
==References==
{{Reflist|2}}
{{Reflist}}


==Further reading==
==Further reading==
* {{cite journal | last1 = Aftergood | first1 = Steven | year = 2000| title = Essays on Science and Society: Polygraph Testing and the DOE National Laboratories| journal = [[Science (magazine)|Science]] | volume = 290 | issue = 5493| pages = 939–940 | doi = 10.1126/science.290.5493.939 | pmid = 17749189 | s2cid = 153185280 }}
* {{Cite book | last = Alder | first = Ken | title = The Lie Detectors | publisher = Free Press | location = New York | year = 2007 | isbn = 0743259882 }}
* {{Cite book | last = Alder | first = Ken | title = The Lie Detectors | publisher = Free Press | location = New York | year = 2007 | isbn = 978-0-7432-5988-0 | url-access = registration | url = https://archive.org/details/liedetectorshist0000alde }}
*[[Steve Blinkhorn|Blinkhorn, S.]] (1988) "Lie Detection as a psychometric procedure" In "The Polygraph Test" (Gale, A. ed. 1988) 29-39.
* Bunn, Geoffrey C. ''The Truth Machine: A Social History of the Lie Detector'' (Johns Hopkins University Press; 2012) 256 pages
* {{Cite book | last = Jones | first = Ishmael | title = The Human Factor: Inside the CIA's Dysfunctional Intelligence Culture | publisher = Encounter Books | location = New York | year = 2008 | isbn = 978-1594033827 }}
* [[Steve Blinkhorn|Blinkhorn, S.]] (1988) "Lie Detection as a psychometric procedure" In "The Polygraph Test" (Gale, A. ed. 1988) 29–39.
*Maschke, G.W. & Scalabrini, G.J. (2005) ''The Lie Behind the Lie Detector.'' 3rd ed. Available on-line at http://antipolygraph.org.
* Cumming, Alfred (Specialist in Intelligence and National Security). "[https://www.fas.org/sgp/crs/intel/RL31988.pdf Polygraph Use by the Department of Energy: Issues for Congress]." ([https://web.archive.org/web/20140328190935/http://www.fas.org/sgp/crs/intel/RL31988.pdf Archive]) ''[[Congressional Research Service]]''. February 9, 2009.
* {{Cite book | last = Lykken | first = David | title = A Tremor in the Blood | publisher = Plenum Trade | location = New York | year = 1998 | isbn = 0306457822 }}
* {{Cite magazine|last=Harris|first=Mark|date=October 1, 2018|title=The Lie Generator: Inside the Black Mirror World of Polygraph Job Screenings |url=https://www.wired.com/story/inside-polygraph-job-screening-black-mirror/?mbid=nl_100518_backchannel_list_p|magazine=Wired}}
* {{Cite doi|10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.363}}
* {{Cite book | last = Sullivan | first = John | title = Gatekeeper | publisher = Potomac Books Inc | year = 2007 | isbn = 159797045X }}
* {{Cite book | last = Jones | first = Ishmael | title = The Human Factor: Inside the CIA's Dysfunctional Intelligence Culture | publisher = Encounter Books | location = New York | year = 2008 | isbn = 978-1-59403-382-7 }}
* {{Cite book | last = Lykken | first = David | title = A Tremor in the Blood | publisher = Plenum Trade | location = New York | year = 1998 | isbn = 978-0-306-45782-1 }}
* Maschke, G.W. & Scalabrini, G.J. (2018) ''The Lie Behind the Lie Detector.'' 5th ed. Available on-line at [https://antipolygraph.org Learn How to Pass (or Beat) a Polygraph Test].
* McCarthy, Susan. "[http://www.salon.com/2000/03/02/lie_detection/ Passing the polygraph]." ''[[Salon (website)|Salon]]''. March 2, 2000.
* {{Cite journal| first1 = N. J.| first2 = D. W. | title = Twenty years of bogus pipeline research: A critical review and meta-analysis | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 114| issue = 2 | pages = 363–375 | year = 1993| last1 = Roese | doi = 10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.363| last2 = Jamieson}}
* {{Cite book | last = Sullivan | first = John | title = Gatekeeper | publisher = Potomac Books Inc | year = 2007 | isbn = 978-1-59797-045-7 }}
* Taylor, Marisa (Tish Wells contributed). "[http://www.mcclatchydc.com/2012/12/06/176313/feds-expand-polygraph-screening.html Feds expand polygraph screening, often seeking intimate facts]." ''[[The McClatchy Company|McClatchy]]''. December 6, 2012.
* Woodrow, Michael J. "The Truth about the Psychophysiological Detection of Deception Examination 3rd Edition" Lulu Press. New York {{ISBN|978-1-105-89546-3}}


==External links==
==External links==
{{commons category|Polygraphs}}
* [http://www.policepolygraph.org American Association of Police Polygraphists]
* [http://www.antipolygraph.org AntiPolygraph.org] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190905164151/https://antipolygraph.org/ |date=2019-09-05 }}, a website critical of polygraph
* [http://www.polygraph.org American Polygraph Association]
* [http://www.antipolygraph.org AntiPolygraph.org], a website critical of polygraphy
* [http://jimfisher.edinboro.edu/forensics/polywar1.html History of the Polygraph] by Jim Fisher
* [http://www.fas.org/irp/eprint/nsa-interview.pdf Interviewing with an Intelligence Agency] First person account of NSA interview (including polygraph)
* [http://www.limestonetech.com/index.cfm/product-lines/credibility-assessment/polygraph-professional-suite/instrumentation/ Limestone Technologies Inc.] Polygraph Software and Instrumentation.
* [http://www.lie2me.net/thepolygraphmuseum/ The Polygraph Museum] Historical photographs and descriptions of polygraph instruments.
* [http://www.lie2me.net/thepolygraphmuseum/ The Polygraph Museum] Historical photographs and descriptions of polygraph instruments.
* [https://web.archive.org/web/20070610165851/http://psych.utoronto.ca/~furedy/napoly.htm The North American Polygraph and Psychophysiology: Disinterested, Uninterested, and Interested Perspectives] by John J. Furedy, ''International Journal of Psychophysiology,'' Spring/Summer 1996
* [http://www.nzzfolio.ch/www/d80bd71b-b264-4db4-afd0-277884b93470/showarticle/f213a4d9-ea9f-4ca1-ae24-96284b2e1861.aspx Technology of Truth] by Ken Alder. Magazine article about the history of the lie detector.
* [http://www.murdoch.edu.au/elaw/issues/v7n1/clarke71_text.html ''Trial By Ordeal? Polygraph Testing In Australia'']
* [http://www.psych.utoronto.ca/~furedy/napoly.htm The North American Polygraph and Psychophysiology: Disinterested, Uninterested, and Interested Perspectives] by John J. Furedy, ''International Journal of Psychophysiology,'' Spring/Summer 1996
* [https://books.google.com/books?id=rNoDAAAAMBAJ&dq=Popular+Science+1935+plane+%22Popular+Mechanics%22&pg=PA414 "Thought Wave Lie Detector Measures Current in Nerves" ''Popular Mechanics'', July 1937]
*[http://www.murdoch.edu.au/elaw/issues/v7n1/clarke71_text.html Trial By Ordeal? Polygraph Testing In Australia]
* {{cite web| first =David| last =Mikkelson| date = July 11, 2011| title =Next case on the Legal Colander| website=[[Snopes]]|
* [http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/23926278 New U.S. weapon Hand-held lie detector] [[MSNBC]] April 9, 2008.
url = http://www.snopes.com/legal/colander.asp}}
*{{Cite journal

| first =B
{{Pseudoscience|state=autocollapse}}
| last =Mikkelson
{{Authority control}}
| authorlink =
| coauthors =
| year = 2000
| month =
| title =Next case on the Legal Colander
| journal = Urban Legends Reference Page, Snopes.com
| volume =
| issue =
| pages =
| id =
| url = http://www.snopes.com/legal/colander.asp
}}


[[Category:Pseudoscience]]
[[Category:Pseudoscience]]
[[Category:American inventions]]
[[Category:Physiological instruments]]
[[Category:Physiological instruments]]
[[Category:Forensic equipment]]
[[Category:Forensic equipment]]
[[Category:1902 introductions]]
[[Category:1902 introductions]]
[[Category:Lie detection]]
[[Category:Polygraph]]
[[Category:Articles containing video clips]]

[[ar:جهاز كشف الكذب]]
[[cs:Polygraf]]
[[de:Lügendetektor]]
[[et:Valedetektor]]
[[es:Detector de mentiras]]
[[fr:Détecteur de mensonge]]
[[ko:거짓말 탐지기]]
[[hi:पॉलीग्राफ]]
[[id:Uji kebohongan]]
[[it:Macchina della verità]]
[[he:פוליגרף]]
[[lt:Poligrafas]]
[[hu:Poligráf]]
[[nl:Polygraaf]]
[[ja:ポリグラフ]]
[[no:Polygraf]]
[[pl:Wariograf]]
[[pt:Polígrafo (detector de mentiras)]]
[[ro:Poligraf]]
[[ru:Полиграф]]
[[sr:Полиграф]]
[[fi:Valheenpaljastin]]
[[sv:Lögndetektor]]
[[tr:Yalan makinesi]]
[[uk:Поліграф]]
[[vi:Máy phát hiện nói dối]]
[[zh:測謊機]]

Latest revision as of 18:50, 2 January 2025

American inventor Leonarde Keeler testing his improved polygraph on Kohler, a former witness for the prosecution at the 1935 trial of Richard Hauptmann

A polygraph, often incorrectly referred to as a lie detector test,[1][2][3] is a pseudoscientific[4][5][6] device or procedure that measures and records several physiological indicators such as blood pressure, pulse, respiration, and skin conductivity while a person is asked and answers a series of questions.[7] The belief underpinning the use of the polygraph is that deceptive answers will produce physiological responses that can be differentiated from those associated with non-deceptive answers; however, there are no specific physiological reactions associated with lying, making it difficult to identify factors that separate those who are lying from those who are telling the truth.[8]

In some countries, polygraphs are used as an interrogation tool with criminal suspects or candidates for sensitive public or private sector employment. Some United States law enforcement and federal government agencies,[9][10] as well as many police departments, use polygraph examinations to interrogate suspects and screen new employees. Within the US federal government, a polygraph examination is also referred to as a psychophysiological detection of deception examination.[11]

Assessments of polygraphy by scientific and government bodies generally suggest that polygraphs are highly inaccurate, may easily be defeated by countermeasures, and are an imperfect or invalid means of assessing truthfulness.[12][13][6][14] A comprehensive 2003 review by the National Academy of Sciences of existing research concluded that there was "little basis for the expectation that a polygraph test could have extremely high accuracy."[6] The American Psychological Association states that "most psychologists agree that there is little evidence that polygraph tests can accurately detect lies."[8]

Testing procedure

[edit]

The examiner typically begins polygraph test sessions with a pre-test interview to gain some preliminary information which will later be used to develop diagnostic questions. Then the tester will explain how the polygraph is supposed to work, emphasizing that it can detect lies and that it is important to answer truthfully. Then a "stim test" is often conducted: the subject is asked to deliberately lie and then the tester reports that he was able to detect this lie. Guilty subjects are likely to become more anxious when they are reminded of the test's validity. However, there are risks of innocent subjects being equally or more anxious than the guilty.[15] Then the actual test starts. Some of the questions asked are "irrelevant" ("Is your name Fred?"), others are "diagnostic" questions, and the remainder are the "relevant questions" that the tester is really interested in. The different types of questions alternate. The test is passed if the physiological responses to the diagnostic questions are larger than those during the relevant questions.[16]

Criticisms have been given regarding the validity of the administration of the Control Question Technique. The CQT may be vulnerable to being conducted in an interrogation-like fashion. This kind of interrogation style would elicit a nervous response from innocent and guilty suspects alike. There are several other ways of administering the questions.[17]

An alternative is the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT), or the Concealed Information Test, which is used in Japan.[18] The administration of this test is given to prevent potential errors that may arise from the questioning style. The test is usually conducted by a tester with no knowledge of the crime or circumstances in question. The administrator tests the participant on their knowledge of the crime that would not be known to an innocent person. For example: "Was the crime committed with a .45 or a 9 mm?" The questions are in multiple choice and the participant is rated on how they react to the correct answer. If they react strongly to the guilty information, then proponents of the test believe that it is likely that they know facts relevant to the case. This administration is considered more valid by supporters of the test because it contains many safeguards to avoid the risk of the administrator influencing the results.[19]

Effectiveness

[edit]

Assessments of polygraphy by scientific and government bodies generally suggest that polygraphs are inaccurate, may be defeated by countermeasures, and are an imperfect or invalid means of assessing truthfulness.[6][12][13] Despite claims that polygraph tests are between 80% and 90% accurate by advocates,[20][21] the National Research Council has found no evidence of effectiveness.[13][22] In particular, studies have indicated that the relevant–irrelevant questioning technique is not ideal, as many innocent subjects exert a heightened physiological reaction to the crime-relevant questions.[23] The American Psychological Association states "Most psychologists agree that there is little evidence that polygraph tests can accurately detect lies."[8]

In 2002, a review by the National Research Council found that, in populations "untrained in countermeasures, specific-incident polygraph tests can discriminate lying from truth telling at rates well above chance, though well below perfection". The review also warns against generalization from these findings to justify the use of polygraphs—"polygraph accuracy for screening purposes is almost certainly lower than what can be achieved by specific-incident polygraph tests in the field"—and notes some examinees may be able to take countermeasures to produce deceptive results.[24]

In the 1998 US Supreme Court case United States v. Scheffer, the majority stated that "There is simply no consensus that polygraph evidence is reliable [...] Unlike other expert witnesses who testify about factual matters outside the jurors' knowledge, such as the analysis of fingerprints, ballistics, or DNA found at a crime scene, a polygraph expert can supply the jury only with another opinion." The Supreme Court summarized their findings by stating that the use of polygraph was "little better than could be obtained by the toss of a coin."[25] In 2005, the 11th Circuit Court of Appeals stated that "polygraphy did not enjoy general acceptance from the scientific community".[26] In 2001, William Iacono, Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of Minnesota, concluded:

Although the CQT [Control Question Test] may be useful as an investigative aid and tool to induce confessions, it does not pass muster as a scientifically credible test. CQT theory is based on naive, implausible assumptions indicating (a) that it is biased against innocent individuals and (b) that it can be beaten simply by artificially augmenting responses to control questions. Although it is not possible to adequately assess the error rate of the CQT, both of these conclusions are supported by published research findings in the best social science journals (Honts et al., 1994; Horvath, 1977; Kleinmuntz & Szucko, 1984; Patrick & Iacono, 1991). Although defense attorneys often attempt to have the results of friendly CQTs admitted as evidence in court, there is no evidence supporting their validity and ample reason to doubt it. Members of scientific organizations who have the requisite background to evaluate the CQT are overwhelmingly skeptical of the claims made by polygraph proponents.[27]

Polygraphs measure arousal, which can be affected by anxiety, anxiety disorders such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), nervousness, fear, confusion, hypoglycemia, psychosis, depression, substance induced states (nicotine, stimulants), substance withdrawal state (alcohol withdrawal) or other emotions; polygraphs do not measure "lies".[15][28][29] A polygraph cannot differentiate anxiety caused by dishonesty and anxiety caused by something else.[30]

Since the polygraph does not measure lying, the Silent Talker Lie Detector inventors expected that adding a camera to film microexpressions would improve the accuracy of the evaluators. This did not happen in practice according to an article in the Intercept.[31]

US Congress Office of Technology Assessment

[edit]

In 1983, the US Congress Office of Technology Assessment published a review of the technology[32] and found that

there is at present only limited scientific evidence for establishing the validity of polygraph testing. Even where the evidence seems to indicate that polygraph testing detects deceptive subjects better than chance, significant error rates are possible, and examiner and examinee differences and the use of countermeasures may further affect validity.[33]

National Academy of Sciences

[edit]

In 2003, the National Academy of Sciences (NAS) issued a report entitled "The Polygraph and Lie Detection". The NAS found that "overall, the evidence is scanty and scientifically weak", concluding that 57 of the approximately 80 research studies that the American Polygraph Association relied on to reach their conclusions were significantly flawed.[34] These studies did show that specific-incident polygraph testing, in a person untrained in counter-measures, could discern the truth at "a level greater than chance, yet short of perfection". However, due to several flaws, the levels of accuracy shown in these studies "are almost certainly higher than actual polygraph accuracy of specific-incident testing in the field".[35] By adding a camera, the Silent Talker Lie Detector attempted to give more data to the evaluator by providing information about microexpressions. However adding the Silent Talker camera did not improve lie detection and was very expensive and cumbersome to include according to an article in the Intercept.[citation needed]

When polygraphs are used as a screening tool (in national security matters and for law enforcement agencies for example) the level of accuracy drops to such a level that "Its accuracy in distinguishing actual or potential security violators from innocent test takers is insufficient to justify reliance on its use in employee security screening in federal agencies." The NAS concluded that the polygraph "may have some utility but that there is "little basis for the expectation that a polygraph test could have extremely high accuracy".[6]

The NAS conclusions paralleled those of the earlier United States Congress Office of Technology Assessment report "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation".[36] Similarly, a report to Congress by the Moynihan Commission on Government Secrecy concluded that "The few Government-sponsored scientific research reports on polygraph validity (as opposed to its utility), especially those focusing on the screening of applicants for employment, indicate that the polygraph is neither scientifically valid nor especially effective beyond its ability to generate admissions".[37]

Despite the NAS finding of a "high rate of false positives," failures to expose individuals such as Aldrich Ames and Larry Wu-Tai Chin, and other inabilities to show a scientific justification for the use of the polygraph, it continues to be employed.[38]

Countermeasures

[edit]

Several proposed countermeasures designed to pass polygraph tests have been described. There are two major types of countermeasures: "general state" (intending to alter the physiological or psychological state of the subject during the test), and "specific point" (intending to alter the physiological or psychological state of the subject at specific periods during the examination, either to increase or decrease responses during critical examination periods).[28]

  • General state: asked how he passed the polygraph test, Central Intelligence Agency officer turned KGB mole Aldrich Ames explained that he sought advice from his Soviet handler and received the simple instruction to: "Get a good night's sleep, and rest, and go into the test rested and relaxed. Be nice to the polygraph examiner, develop a rapport, and be cooperative and try to maintain your calm".[39] Additionally, Ames explained, "There's no special magic... Confidence is what does it. Confidence and a friendly relationship with the examiner... rapport, where you smile and you make him think that you like him".[40]
  • Specific point: other suggestions for countermeasures include for the subject to mentally record the control and relevant questions as the examiner reviews them before the interrogation begins. During the interrogation the subject is supposed to carefully control their breathing while answering the relevant questions, and to try to artificially increase their heart rate during the control questions, for example by thinking of something scary or exciting, or by pricking themselves with a pointed object concealed somewhere on the body. In this way the results will not show a significant reaction to any of the relevant questions.[41]

Use

[edit]

Law enforcement agencies and intelligence agencies in the United States are by far the biggest users of polygraph technology. In the United States alone most federal law enforcement agencies either employ their own polygraph examiners or use the services of examiners employed in other agencies.[42] In 1978 Richard Helms, the eighth Director of Central Intelligence, stated:

We discovered there were some Eastern Europeans who could defeat the polygraph at any time. Americans are not very good at it, because we are raised to tell the truth and when we lie it is easy to tell we are lying. But we find a lot of Europeans and Asiatics can handle that polygraph without a blip, and you know they are lying and you have evidence that they are lying.[43]

Susan McCarthy of Salon said in 2000 that "The polygraph is an American phenomenon, with limited use in a few countries, such as Canada, Israel and Japan."[44]

Armenia

[edit]

In Armenia, government administered polygraphs are legal, at least for use in national security investigations. The National Security Service (NSS), Armenia's primary intelligence service, requires polygraph examinations of all new applicants.[45]

Australia

[edit]

Polygraph evidence is currently inadmissible in New South Wales courts under the Lie Detectors Act 1983. Under the same act, it is also illegal to use polygraphs for the purpose of granting employment, insurance, financial accommodation, and several other purposes for which polygraphs may be used in other jurisdictions.[46]

Canada

[edit]

In Canada, the 1987 decision of R v Béland, the Supreme Court of Canada rejected the use of polygraph results as evidence in court, finding that they were inadmissible. The polygraph is still used as a tool in the investigation of criminal acts and sometimes employed in the screening of employees for government organizations.[47]

In the province of Ontario, the use of polygraphs by an employer is not permitted. A police force does have the authorization to use a polygraph in the course of the investigation of an offence.[48]

Europe

[edit]

In a majority of European jurisdictions, polygraphs are generally considered to be unreliable for gathering evidence, and are usually not used by local law enforcement agencies. Polygraph testing is widely seen in Europe to violate the right to remain silent.[49]: 62ff 

In England and Wales a polygraph test can be taken, but the results cannot be used in a court of law to prove a case.[50] However, the Offender Management Act 2007 put in place an option to use polygraph tests to monitor serious sex offenders on parole in England and Wales;[51] these tests became compulsory in 2014 for high risk sexual offenders currently on parole in England and Wales.[52]

The Supreme Court of Poland declared on January 29, 2015, that the use of polygraph in interrogation of suspects is forbidden by the Polish Code of Criminal Procedure. Its use might be allowed though if the suspect has been already accused of a crime and if the interrogated person consents of the use of a polygraph. Even then, the use of polygraph can never be used as a substitute of actual evidence.[53]

As of 2017, the justice ministry and Supreme Court of both of the Netherlands and Germany had rejected use of polygraphs.[49]: 62ff [54]

According to the 2017 book Psychology and Law: Bridging the Gap by psychologists David Canter and Rita Zukauskiene Belgium was the European country with the most prevalent use of polygraph testing by police, with about 300 polygraphs carried out each year in the course of police investigations. The results are not considered viable evidence in bench trials, but have been used in jury trials.[49]: 62ff 

In Lithuania, "polygraphs have been in use since 1992",[55] with law enforcement utilizing the Event Knowledge Test (a "modification"[56] of the Concealed Information Test) in criminal investigations.

India

[edit]

In 2008, an Indian court adopted the Brain Electrical Oscillation Signature Profiling test as evidence to convict a woman who was accused of murdering her fiancé.[57] It was the first time that the result of polygraph was used as evidence in court.[58] On May 5, 2010, The Supreme Court of India declared use of narcoanalysis, brain mapping and polygraph tests on suspects as illegal and against the constitution if consent is not obtained and forced.[59] Article 20(3) of the Indian Constitution states: "No person accused of any offence shall be compelled to be a witness against himself."[60] Polygraph tests are still legal if the defendant requests one.[61]

Israel

[edit]

The Supreme Court of Israel, in Civil Appeal 551/89 (Menora Insurance v. Jacob Sdovnik), ruled that the polygraph has not been recognized as a reliable device. In other decisions, polygraph results were ruled inadmissible in criminal trials. Polygraph results are only admissible in civil trials if the person being tested agrees to it in advance.[62]

Philippines

[edit]

The results of polygraph tests are inadmissible in court in the Philippines.[63][64] The National Bureau of Investigation do use polygraphs in aid of investigation.[65]

United States

[edit]
Brochure of the Defense Security Service (DSS) about polygraph testing
Demonstrating the administration of the polygraph, the polygrapher making notes on the readouts. 1970s
"The Truth About the Polygraph" (National Security Agency (NSA)-produced video on the polygraph process)

In 2018, Wired magazine reported that an estimated 2.5 million polygraph tests were given each year in the United States, with the majority administered to paramedics, police officers, firefighters, and state troopers. The average cost to administer the test is more than $700 and is part of a $2 billion industry.[66]

In 2007, polygraph testimony was admitted by stipulation in 19 states, and was subject to the discretion of the trial judge in federal court. The use of polygraph in court testimony remains controversial, although it is used extensively in post-conviction supervision, particularly of sex offenders. In Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (1993),[67] the old Frye standard was lifted and all forensic evidence, including polygraph, had to meet the new Daubert standard in which "underlying reasoning or methodology is scientifically valid and properly can be applied to the facts at issue." While polygraph tests are commonly used in police investigations in the US, no defendant or witness can be forced to undergo the test unless they are under the supervision of the courts.[68] In United States v. Scheffer (1998),[69] the US Supreme Court left it up to individual jurisdictions whether polygraph results could be admitted as evidence in court cases. Nevertheless, it is used extensively by prosecutors, defense attorneys, and law enforcement agencies. In the states of Rhode Island, Massachusetts, Maryland, New Jersey, Oregon, Delaware and Iowa it is illegal for any employer to order a polygraph either as conditions to gain employment, or if an employee has been suspected of wrongdoing.[70][71] The Employee Polygraph Protection Act of 1988 (EPPA) generally prevents employers from using lie detector tests, either for pre-employment screening or during the course of employment, with certain exemptions.[72] As of 2013, about 70,000 job applicants are polygraphed by the federal government on an annual basis.[73] In the United States, the State of New Mexico admits polygraph testing in front of juries under certain circumstances.[74]

In 2010 the NSA produced a video explaining its polygraph process.[75] The video, ten minutes long, is titled "The Truth About the Polygraph" and was posted to the website of the Defense Security Service. Jeff Stein of The Washington Post said that the video portrays "various applicants, or actors playing them—it’s not clear—describing everything bad they had heard about the test, the implication being that none of it is true."[76] AntiPolygraph.org argues that the NSA-produced video omits some information about the polygraph process; it produced a video responding to the NSA video.[75] George Maschke, the founder of the website, accused the NSA polygraph video of being "Orwellian".[76]

The polygraph was invented in 1921 by John Augustus Larson, a medical student at the University of California, Berkeley and a police officer of the Berkeley Police Department in Berkeley, California.[77] The polygraph was on the Encyclopædia Britannica 2003 list of greatest inventions, described as inventions that "have had profound effects on human life for better or worse."[78] In 2013, the US federal government had begun indicting individuals who stated that they were teaching methods on how to defeat a polygraph test.[73][79][80] During one of those investigations, upwards of 30 federal agencies were involved in investigations of almost 5000 people who had various degrees of contact with those being prosecuted or who had purchased books or DVDs on the topic of beating polygraph tests.[81][82][83]

Security clearances

[edit]

In 1995, Harold James Nicholson, a CIA employee later convicted of spying for Russia, had undergone his periodic five-year reinvestigation, in which he showed a strong probability of deception on questions regarding relationships with a foreign intelligence unit. This polygraph test later led to an investigation which resulted in his eventual arrest and conviction. In most cases, however, polygraphs are more of a tool to "scare straight" those who would consider espionage. Jonathan Pollard was advised by his Israeli handlers that he was to resign his job from American intelligence if he was ever told he was subject to a polygraph test.[citation needed] Likewise, John Anthony Walker was advised by his handlers not to engage in espionage until he had been promoted to the highest position for which a polygraph test was not required, to refuse promotion to higher positions for which polygraph tests were required, and to retire when promotion was mandated.[6]

In 1983, CIA employee Edward Lee Howard was dismissed when, during a polygraph screening, he truthfully answered a series of questions admitting to minor crimes such as petty theft and drug abuse. In retaliation for his perceived unjust punishment for minor offenses, he later sold his knowledge of CIA operations to the Soviet Union.[84]

Polygraph tests may not deter espionage. From 1945 to the present, at least six Americans have committed espionage while successfully passing polygraph tests. Notable cases of two men who created a false negative result with the polygraphs were Larry Wu-Tai Chin, who spied for China, and Aldrich Ames, who was given two polygraph examinations while with the CIA, the first in 1986 and the second in 1991, while spying for the Soviet Union/Russia. The CIA reported that he passed both examinations after experiencing initial indications of deception.[85] According to a Senate investigation, an FBI review of the first examination concluded that the indications of deception were never resolved.[86]

Ana Belen Montes, a Cuban spy, passed a counterintelligence scope polygraph test administered by DIA in 1994.[87]

Despite these errors, in August 2008, the US Defense Intelligence Agency (DIA) announced that it would subject each of its 5,700 prospective and current employees to polygraph testing at least once annually.[88] This expansion of polygraph screening at DIA occurred while DIA polygraph managers ignored documented technical problems discovered in the Lafayette computerized polygraph system.[89] The DIA uses computerized Lafayette polygraph systems for routine counterintelligence testing. The impact of the technical flaws within the Lafayette system on the analysis of recorded physiology and on the final polygraph test evaluation is currently unknown.[90]

In 2012, a McClatchy investigation found that the National Reconnaissance Office was possibly breaching ethical and legal boundaries by encouraging its polygraph examiners to extract personal and private information from US Department of Defense personnel during polygraph tests that purported to be limited in scope to counterintelligence matters.[91] Allegations of abusive polygraph practices were brought forward by former NRO polygraph examiners.[92]

Alternative tests

[edit]

Most polygraph researchers have focused more on the exam's predictive value on a subject's guilt. However, there have been no empirical theories established to explain how a polygraph measures deception. A 2010 study indicated that functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) may benefit in explaining the psychological correlations of polygraph exams. It could also explain which parts of the brain are active when subjects use artificial memories.[clarification needed][93] Most brain activity occurs in both sides of the prefrontal cortex, which is linked to response inhibition. This indicates that deception may involve inhibition of truthful responses.[94] Some researchers believe that reaction time (RT) based tests may replace polygraphs in concealed information detection. RT based tests differ from polygraphs in stimulus presentation duration and can be conducted without physiological recording as subject response time is measured via computer. However, researchers have found limitations to these tests as subjects voluntarily control their reaction time, deception can still occur within the response deadline, and the test itself lacks physiological recording.[95]

History

[edit]

Earlier societies utilized elaborate methods of lie detection which mainly involved torture. For instance, in the Middle Ages, boiling water was used to detect liars, as it was believed honest men would withstand it better than liars.[96] Early devices for lie detection include an 1895 invention of Cesare Lombroso used to measure changes in blood pressure for police cases, a 1904 device by Vittorio Benussi used to measure breathing, the Mackenzie-Lewis Polygraph first developed by James Mackenzie in 1906 and an abandoned project by American William Moulton Marston which used blood pressure to examine German prisoners of war (POWs).[97] Marston said he found a strong positive correlation between systolic blood pressure and lying.[15]

Marston wrote a second paper on the concept in 1915, when finishing his undergraduate studies. He entered Harvard Law School and graduated in 1918, re-publishing his earlier work in 1917.[98] Marston's main inspiration for the device was his wife, Elizabeth Holloway Marston.[96] "According to Marston’s son, it was his mother Elizabeth, Marston's wife, who suggested to him that 'When she got mad or excited, her blood pressure seemed to climb'" (Lamb, 2001). Although Elizabeth is not listed as Marston’s collaborator in his early work, Lamb, Matte (1996), and others refer directly and indirectly to Elizabeth's work on her husband's deception research. She also appears in a picture taken in his polygraph laboratory in the 1920s (reproduced in Marston, 1938).[99]

Despite his predecessors' contributions, Marston styled himself the "father of the polygraph". (Today he is often equally or more noted as the creator of the comic book character Wonder Woman and her Lasso of Truth, which can force people to tell the truth.)[100] Marston remained the device's primary advocate, lobbying for its use in the courts. In 1938 he published a book, The Lie Detector Test, wherein he documented the theory and use of the device.[101] In 1938 he appeared in advertising by the Gillette company claiming that the polygraph showed Gillette razors were better than the competition.[102][103][104]

A device recording both blood pressure and breathing was invented in 1921 by John Augustus Larson of the University of California and first applied in law enforcement work by the Berkeley Police Department under its nationally renowned police chief August Vollmer.[105] Further work on this device was done by Leonarde Keeler.[106] As Larson's protege, Keeler updated the device by making it portable and added the galvanic skin response to it in 1939. His device was then purchased by the FBI, and served as the prototype of the modern polygraph.[96][105]

Several devices similar to Keeler's polygraph version included the Berkeley Psychograph, a blood pressure-pulse-respiration recorder developed by C. D. Lee in 1936[107] and the Darrow Behavior Research Photopolygraph, which was developed and intended solely for behavior research experiments.[107][108][109]

A device which recorded muscular activity accompanying changes in blood pressure was developed in 1945 by John E. Reid, who claimed that greater accuracy could be obtained by making these recordings simultaneously with standard blood pressure-pulse-respiration recordings.[107][110]

Society and culture

[edit]

Portrayals in media

[edit]

Lie detection has a long history in mythology and fairy tales; the polygraph has allowed modern fiction to use a device more easily seen as scientific and plausible. Notable instances of polygraph usage include uses in crime and espionage themed television shows and some daytime television talk shows, cartoons and films. Numerous TV shows have been called Lie Detector or featured the device. The first Lie Detector TV show aired in the 1950s, created and hosted by Ralph Andrews. In the 1960s Andrews produced a series of specials hosted by Melvin Belli. In the 1970s the show was hosted by Jack Anderson. In early 1983 Columbia Pictures Television put on a syndicated series hosted by F. Lee Bailey.[111] In 1998 TV producer Mark Phillips with his Mark Phillips Philms & Telephision put Lie Detector back on the air on the FOX Network—on that program Ed Gelb with host Marcia Clark questioned Mark Fuhrman about the allegation that he "planted the bloody glove". In 2005 Phillips produced Lie Detector as a series for PAX/ION; some of the guests included Paula Jones, Reverend Paul Crouch accuser Lonny Ford, Ben Rowling, Jeff Gannon and Swift Boat Vet, Steve Garner.[112]

In the UK, shows such as The Jeremy Kyle Show used polygraph tests extensively. The show was ultimately canceled when a participant committed suicide shortly after being polygraphed. The guest was slated by Kyle on the show for failing the polygraph, but no other evidence has come forward to prove any guilt. Producers later admitted in the inquiry that they were unsure on how accurate the tests performed were.[113]

In the Fox game show The Moment of Truth, contestants are privately asked personal questions a few days before the show while hooked to a polygraph. On the show they asked the same questions in front of a studio audience and members of their family. In order to advance in the game they must give a "truthful" answer as determined by the previous polygraph exam.[114]

Daytime talk shows, such as Maury Povich and Steve Wilkos, have used polygraphs to supposedly detect deception in interview subjects on their programs that pertain to cheating, child abuse, and theft.[115]

In episode 93 of the US science show MythBusters, the hosts attempted to fool the polygraph by using pain when answering truthfully, in order to test the notion that polygraphs interpret truthful and non-truthful answers as the same. They also attempted to fool the polygraph by thinking pleasant thoughts when lying and thinking stressful thoughts when telling the truth, to try to confuse the machine. However, neither technique was successful for a number of reasons. Michael Martin correctly identified each guilty and innocent subject. Martin suggested that when conducted properly, polygraphs are correct 98% of the time, but no scientific evidence has been offered for this.[116]

The history of the polygraph is the subject of the documentary film The Lie Detector, which first aired on American Experience on January 3, 2023.[117]

Hand-held lie detector for US military

[edit]

A hand-held lie detector is being deployed by the US Department of Defense according to a report in 2008 by investigative reporter Bill Dedman of NBC News. The Preliminary Credibility Assessment Screening System, or PCASS, captures less physiological information than a polygraph, and uses an algorithm, not the judgment of a polygraph examiner, to render a decision whether it believes the person is being deceptive or not. The device was first used in Afghanistan by US Army troops. The Department of Defense ordered its use be limited to non-US persons, in overseas locations only.[118]

Notable cases

[edit]

Polygraphy has been faulted for failing to trap known spies such as double-agent Aldrich Ames, who passed two polygraph tests while spying for the Soviet Union.[88][119] Ames failed several tests while at the CIA that were never acted on.[120] Other spies who passed the polygraph include Karl Koecher,[121] Ana Montes,[122] and Leandro Aragoncillo.[123] CIA spy Harold James Nicholson failed his polygraph examinations, which aroused suspicions that led to his eventual arrest.[124] Polygraph examination and background checks failed to detect Nada Nadim Prouty, who was not a spy but was convicted for improperly obtaining US citizenship and using it to obtain a restricted position at the FBI.[125]

The polygraph also failed to catch Gary Ridgway, the "Green River Killer". Another suspect allegedly failed a given lie detector test, whereas Ridgway passed.[15] Ridgway passed a polygraph in 1984; he confessed almost 20 years later when confronted with DNA evidence.[126] Conversely, innocent people have been known to fail polygraph tests. In Wichita, Kansas in 1986, Bill Wegerle was suspected of murdering his wife Vicki Wegerle because he failed two polygraph tests (one administered by the police, the other conducted by an expert that Wegerle had hired), although he was neither arrested nor convicted of her death. In March 2004, evidence surfaced connecting her death to the serial killer known as BTK, and in 2005 DNA evidence from the Wegerle murder confirmed that BTK was Dennis Rader, exonerating Wegerle.[127]

Prolonged polygraph examinations are sometimes used as a tool by which confessions are extracted from a defendant, as in the case of Richard Miller, who was persuaded to confess largely by polygraph results combined with appeals from a religious leader.[128] In the Watts family murders, Christopher Watts failed one such polygraph test and subsequently confessed to murdering his wife.[129] In the 2002 disappearance of seven-year-old Danielle van Dam of San Diego, police suspected neighbor David Westerfield; he became the prime suspect when he allegedly failed a polygraph test.[130]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ "lie detector test". Legal Information Institute. June 2020. Archived from the original on March 5, 2022. Retrieved July 27, 2022.
  2. ^ "The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)". American Psychological Association. 2004. Archived from the original on March 5, 2022. Retrieved July 27, 2022.
  3. ^ Robinson, Bryan (July 14, 2001). "Polygraphs Accurate But Not Foolproof". ABC News. Archived from the original on April 20, 2021. Retrieved July 27, 2022.
  4. ^ "United States v. Scheffer: A U.S. Supreme Court Ruling on Polygraph Testing". Federation of American Scientists. March 31, 1998. Archived from the original on November 11, 2023. Retrieved November 11, 2023.
  5. ^ Saxe, Leonard (July 1991). "Science and the CQT polygraph". Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science. 26 (3): 223–231. doi:10.1007/BF02912514. PMID 1954162.
  6. ^ a b c d e f Board on Behavioral, Cognitive, and Sensory Sciences and Education (BCSSE) and Committee on National Statistics (CNSTAT) (March 19, 2013). The Polygraph and Lie Detection. National Research Council. doi:10.17226/10420. ISBN 978-0-309-26392-4. Archived from the original on November 11, 2023. Retrieved November 11, 2023.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ J P Rosenfeld (1995). "Alternative Views of Bashore and Rapp's (1993) alternatives to traditional polygraphy: a critique". Psychological Bulletin. 117: 159–166. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.159.
  8. ^ a b c "The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)". apa.org. American Psychological Association.
  9. ^ Stromberg, Joseph (December 15, 2014). "Lie detectors: Why they don't work, and why police use them anyway". Vox. Archived from the original on August 16, 2014. Retrieved September 13, 2022.
  10. ^ Flock, Elizabeth (September 25, 2012). "NSA Whistleblower Reveals How To Beat a Polygraph Test". U.S. News & World Report. Archived from the original on August 13, 2022. Retrieved September 13, 2022.
  11. ^ "Federal Psychophysiological Detection of Deception Examiner Handbook" (PDF). Antipolygraph.org. Retrieved 15 May 2019.
  12. ^ a b "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation". Washington, DC: U.S. Congress Office of Technology Assessment. 1983. Retrieved 2008-02-29.
  13. ^ a b c "Monitor on Psychology – The polygraph in doubt". American Psychological Association. July 2004. Retrieved 2008-02-29.
  14. ^ LaMotte, Sandee (September 7, 2018). "The shaky science of lie detectors". CNN. Archived from the original on November 5, 2023. Retrieved November 5, 2023.
  15. ^ a b c d Lewis, J. A.; Cuppari, M. (2009). "The polygraph: The truth lies within". Journal of Psychiatry and Law. 37 (1): 85–92. doi:10.1177/009318530903700107. S2CID 152221286.
  16. ^ The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. 2003. pp. 253–254. doi:10.17226/10420. ISBN 978-0-309-13313-5.
  17. ^ "Appendix A: Polygraph Questioning Techniques". The Polygraph and Lie Detection. The National Academies Press. 2003. pp. 253–258. doi:10.17226/10420. ISBN 978-0-309-13313-5.
  18. ^ Don Sosunov (October 14, 2010). "The Admissibility of Polygraph Evidence in Criminal Courts". Archived from the original on November 6, 2018. Retrieved November 1, 2012. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  19. ^ For more info on the Guilty Knowledge Test, see The Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT) as an Application of Psychophysiology: Future Prospects and Obstacles Archived 2008-05-28 at the Wayback Machine
  20. ^ Evans, Gareth (October 4, 2018). "How credible are lie detector tests?". BBC News. "If the examiner is well-trained, if the test is properly carried out, and if there's proper quality controls, the accuracy is estimated between 80%-90%".
  21. ^ Warmelink, Lara (January 24, 2020). "Polygraph lie detector tests: can they really stop criminals reoffending?". The Conversation. There have been several reviews of polygraph accuracy. They suggest that polygraphs are accurate between 80% and 90% of the time.
  22. ^ "A scanner to detect terrorists". BBC News. July 16, 2009.
  23. ^ Iacono, W. G. (2008). "Effective policing: Understanding how polygraph tests work and are used". Criminal Justice and Behavior. 35 (10): 1295–1308. doi:10.1177/0093854808321529. S2CID 143302792.
  24. ^ National Research Council (2003); Pages 4 - 5
  25. ^ "United States v. Scheffer". March 31, 1998. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  26. ^ "United States v. Henderson" (PDF). May 23, 2005. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 27, 2007. Retrieved August 31, 2007. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help) [PDF]
  27. ^ Iacono, William G. (2001). "Forensic 'Lie Detection': Procedures Without Scientific Basis". Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice. 1 (1): 75–86. doi:10.1300/J158v01n01_05. S2CID 143077241.
  28. ^ a b "Archived copy" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-06-05. Retrieved 2014-01-06.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link)
  29. ^ Eggen, Dan; Vedantam, Shankar (May 1, 2006). "Polygraph Results Often in Question". The Washington Post.
  30. ^ "Lie Detector Roulette". Mother Jones.
  31. ^ Gallagher, Ryan (26 July 2019). "We Tested Europe's New Lie Detector for Travelors-and Immediately Triggered a False Positive". The Intercept. Retrieved 17 March 2023.
  32. ^ "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation – A Technical Memorandum" (PDF). (Washington, DC: U.S. Congress, Office of Technology Assessment (OTA-TM-H-15). November 1983. Retrieved 3 September 2016.
  33. ^ Dunning, Brian. "Skeptoid #422: Lie Detection". Skeptoid. Retrieved 3 September 2016.
  34. ^ "Conclusions and Recommendations". The Polygraph and Lie Detection (2003), National Academies Press. p. 212
  35. ^ National Research Council (2013); Chapter 8: Conclusions and Recommendations, p. 212)
  36. ^ Office of Technology Assessment (November 1983). "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation".
  37. ^ IV Personnel Security: Protection Through Detection Archived 2011-06-12 at the Wayback Machine quoting Ralph M. Carney, SSBI Source Yield: An Examination of Sources Contacted During the SSBI (Monterey: Defense Personnel Security Research Center, 1996), 6, affirming that in 81% of cases, the derogatory informations were obtained through questionnaire and/or interrogation.
  38. ^ Randi, James (2017). "A Consistently Erroneous Technology". Skeptical Inquirer. 41 (5): 16–19. Archived from the original on 2018-07-28. Retrieved 28 July 2018.
  39. ^ Weiner, Tim, David Johnston, and Neil A. Lewis, Betrayal: The Story of Aldrich Ames, an American Spy, 1995.
  40. ^ "Ames: Separated Spy, Agent Lives". tribunedigital-dailypress. 29 April 1994. Archived from the original on 2013-12-12. Retrieved 2012-06-01.
  41. ^ Lykken, David T. A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector, 2nd ed., New York: Plenum Trade, 1998, pp. 273–279.
  42. ^ Van Aperen, Steven (February 29, 2000). "The polygraph as an investigative tool in criminal and private investigations". Net-Trace.
  43. ^ "Testimony of Richard Helms, Former Director of Central Intelligence, Former Ambassador to Iran, and Presently a Business Consultant in Washington, D.C., and Represented by Gregory B. Craig, of Williams & Connelly". mcadams.posc.mu.edu. 2001-01-29. Archived from the original on July 1, 2002. Retrieved 2015-04-19.
  44. ^ McCarthy, Susan. "The truth about the polygraph." Salon. March 2, 2000. Retrieved on July 5, 2013.
  45. ^ "How to become an employee of the NSS". www.sns.am. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  46. ^ "Lie Detectors Act 1983 (NSW)". Legislation.nsw.gov.au. Archived from the original on 2018-10-03. Retrieved 2018-10-02.
  47. ^ "Pre-Employment Polygraph". Royal Canadian Mounted Police. 2016-05-10. Archived from the original on 2021-02-26. Retrieved 2018-04-17.
  48. ^ "Lie Detector Tests". Ontario Ministry of Labour.
  49. ^ a b c Meijer, Ewout H; van Koppen, Peter J (2017). "Chapter 3. Lie Detectors and the Law: The Use of the Polygraph in Europe". In Canter, David; Žukauskiene, Rita (eds.). Psychology and Law : Bridging the Gap. Routledge. ISBN 978-1351907873.
  50. ^ "How widely are lie detectors used in the UK?". BBC News. British Broadcasting Corporation. 29 June 2019. Retrieved 28 August 2020.
  51. ^ Grossman, Wendy (25 August 2020). "Letter to America: The Black Box that Wouldn't Die". Skeptical Inquirer. Retrieved 28 August 2020.
  52. ^ Bowcott, Owen (8 August 2014). "Lie detector tests introduced to monitor released sex offenders". The Guardian. Retrieved 28 August 2020.
  53. ^ "SN: tylko bez wariografu w przesłuchaniu I KZP 25/14". Czasopismo Lege Artis (in Polish). 2015-01-30. Retrieved 2021-07-09.
  54. ^ Bundesgerichtshof: Entscheidungen vom 17.12.1998, 1 StR 156/98, 1 StR 258/98
  55. ^ Saldžiūnas, Vitas; Kovalenko, Aleksandras (2008). "The Event Knowledge Test (EKT)" (PDF). European Polygraph. 1 (3): 21.
  56. ^ Saldžiūnas, Vitas; Kovalenko, Aleksandras (2015). "Selecting the Most Optimal Conditions for the Polygraph Examination" (PDF). European Polygraph. 9 (2 (32)): 70. doi:10.1515/ep-2015-0003. S2CID 148000927.
  57. ^ Gaudet, Lyn M (2011). "Brain Fingerprinting, Scientific Evidence, and "Daubert": A Cautionary Lesson from India". Jurimetrics: The Journal of Law, Science & Technology. 51 (3): 293–318. JSTOR 41307131.
  58. ^ Giridharadas, Anand (September 14, 2008). "India's Novel Use of Brain Scans in Courts is Debated". The New York Times. Retrieved 2008-09-15.
  59. ^ Mahapatra, Dhananjay (May 5, 2010). "No narcoanalysis test without consent, says SC". The Times of India. Retrieved 2010-05-05.
  60. ^ Mittal, Akshat; Mishra, Aakarsh (2021). "Right against Self-Incrimination: A Detailed Study & Analysis of Laws Prevailing in India". International Journal of Law Management and Humanities. Archived from the original on April 11, 2021. Retrieved December 21, 2021.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  61. ^ Bose, Joydeep (March 17, 2021). "Polygraph test can only be conducted with consent of the accused: Karnataka HC". Hindustan Times. Archived from the original on March 17, 2021. Retrieved December 21, 2021.
  62. ^ Weinglass, Simona (March 6, 2016). "When a job interview turns into an interrogation". Times of Israel. Archived from the original on March 7, 2016. Retrieved November 16, 2021.
  63. ^ Patag, Kristine Joy (27 May 2018). "How forensic science is making a breakthrough in the Philippines". The Philippine Star. Retrieved 9 September 2023.
  64. ^ "G.R. Nos. 116196-97: Philippines vs. Pablo Adoviso". Supreme Court E-Library. Supreme Court of the Philippines. Retrieved 9 September 2023.
  65. ^ Mayol, Ador Vincent; Bongcac, Doris C.; Pateña, Patricia Andrea (20 January 2012). "Polygraph tests start for Ballesteros, center staff". Philippine Daily Inquirer. Retrieved 9 September 2023.
  66. ^ Harris, Mark (October 1, 2018). "The Lie Generator: Inside the Black Mirror World of Polygraph Job Screenings". Wired. Retrieved October 2, 2018.
  67. ^ "Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U.S. 579 (1993)". Cornell University.
  68. ^ Vance, Stephen. "Looking at the Law: An Updated Look at the Privilege Against Self-Incrimination in PostConviction Supervision" (PDF). Federal Probation. 75 (1).
  69. ^ "United States v. Scheffer, 523 U.S. 303 (1998)". Cornell University.
  70. ^ "General Law – Part I, Title XXI, Chapter 149, Section 19B". malegislature.gov. Retrieved 2019-04-03.
  71. ^ "2013 Maryland Code :: Labor and Employment :: § 3-702 – Lie detector tests". Justia Law. Retrieved 2019-04-03.
  72. ^ "Compliance Assistance By Law – The Employee Polygraph Protection Act (EPPA)". dol.gov. Archived from the original on 2005-09-23.
  73. ^ a b Taylor, Marisa and Cleve R. Wootson Jr. "Seeing threats, feds target instructors of polygraph-beating methods". McClatchy Newspapers. August 16, 2013. Retrieved August 31, 2013.
  74. ^ "New Mexico Rules of Evidence" (PDF). New Mexico State University, Southwest Regional Training Centre. Archived from the original (PDF) on 24 September 2020. Retrieved 30 March 2019.
  75. ^ a b Nagesh, Gautham (June 14, 2010). "NSA video tries to dispel fear about polygraph use during job interviews". The Hill. Retrieved June 15, 2013.
  76. ^ a b Stein, Jeff. "NSA lie detectors no sweat, video says". The Washington Post. June 14, 2010. Retrieved on July 5, 2013.
  77. ^ "Polygraph/Lie Detector FAQs". International League of Polygraph Examiners.
  78. ^ "Encyclopædia Britannica's Great Inventions". Encyclopædia Britannica Almanac 2003, via Wayback Machine. Archived from the original on May 19, 2012. Retrieved 5 August 2014.
  79. ^ ""Owner of 'Polygraph.com' Indicted for Allegedly Training Customers to Lie During Federally Administered Polygraph Examinations". Department of Justice, November 14, 2014. Accessed March 19, 2018.
  80. ^ Taylor, Marisa. "Indiana man gets 8 months for lie-detector fraud". The Seattle Times. McClatchy Newspapers. September 6, 2013. Retrieved on September 8, 2013.
  81. ^ "Coach who taught people how to beat lie detectors headed to prison". Ars Technica. 2013-09-09.
  82. ^ "Washington: Americans' personal data shared with CIA, IRS, others in security probe". McClatchy DC.
  83. ^ "Indiana man gets 8 months for lie-detector fraud". The Seattle Times.
  84. ^ "RPCV and CIA defector Edward Howard dies in Moscow". Peace Corps Online. 2002-07-22.
  85. ^ "The Adrich H. Ames Case: An Assessment of CIA's Role, Oct. 21, 1994 Memorandum for Heads of Agency Offices from Director of Central Intelligence". Archived from the original on July 5, 2009.
  86. ^ "An Assessment of the Aldrich H. Ames Espionage Case and Its Implications for U.S. Intelligence – Senate Select Committee on Intelligence – 01 November 1994 – Part One". fas.org.
  87. ^ "Magazine". The Washington Post.
  88. ^ a b Hess, Pamela, "Pentagon's Intelligence Arm Steps Up Lie-Detector Efforts", Arizona Daily Star, August 24, 2008. Also in Fox News via AP [1]
  89. ^ "Glitch in widely used polygraph can skew results". McClatchy DC. Archived from the original on 2013-05-27. Retrieved 2013-05-26.
  90. ^ "McClatchy – The Polygraph Files". mcclatchydc.com. Archived from the original on 2013-05-27. Retrieved 2013-05-26.
  91. ^ "The IG complaint of Mark Phillips concerning the NRO". McClatchy DC.
  92. ^ Taylor, Marisa, "Sen. Charles Grassley Seeks Probe Of Polygraph Techniques At National Reconnaissance Office", The McClatchy Company, 27 July 2012
  93. ^ Bell, B. G.; Grubin, D. (2010). "Functional magnetic resonance imaging may promote theoretical understanding of the polygraph test". Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology. 21 (1): 52–65. doi:10.1080/14789940903220676. S2CID 144428132.
  94. ^ Langleben, DD; Schroeder, L; Maldjian, JA; Gur, RC; McDonald, S; Ragland, JD; O'Brien, CP; Childress, AR (2002). "Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study". NeuroImage. 15 (3): 727–732. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1003. PMID 11848716. S2CID 14676750.
  95. ^ Verschuere, B.; Crombez, G.; Degrootte, T.; Rosseel, Y. (2010). "Detecting concealed information with reaction times: Validity and comparison with the polygraph". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 24 (7): 991–1002. doi:10.1002/acp.1601.
  96. ^ a b c Grubin, D.; Madsen, L. (2005). "Lie detection and the polygraph: A historical review". Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology. 16 (2): 357–369. doi:10.1080/14789940412331337353. S2CID 144898901.
  97. ^ "History of CVSA". nitv1.com. Archived from the original on 2007-12-03.
  98. ^ Marston, William M (1917). "Systolic Blood Pressure Changes in Deception". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2 (2): 117–163. doi:10.1037/h0073583.
  99. ^ National Research Council (2013); Page 292
  100. ^ Lepore, Jill. The Secret History of Wonder Woman, New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 2014, ISBN 978-0385354042, pp. 183–209.
  101. ^ Marston, William Moulton. The Lie Detector Test. New York: Richard R. Smith, 1938.
  102. ^ "William Marston's Secret Identity". Reason magazine. May 2001.
  103. ^ "Lie Detector Charts Emotional Effects of Shaving – 1938 Gillette Advertisement" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-10-10. Retrieved 2007-01-10.
  104. ^ FBI File of William Moulton Marston Archived 2007-06-21 at the Wayback Machine(including report on Gillette advertising campaign)
  105. ^ a b Katherine To for Illumin. Fall 2003. Lie Detection: The Science and Development of the Polygraph
  106. ^ "Leonarde Keeler and his Instruments". lie2me.net.
  107. ^ a b c Inbau, Fred E. Lie Detection and Criminal Interrogation, The Williams & Wilkins Company, 1948
  108. ^ Troville, P.V. (1939). "A History of Lie Detection". Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology. 29 (6): 848.
  109. ^ Troville, P.V. (1939). "A History of Lie Detection". Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology. 30 (1): 104.
  110. ^ Reid, J. E. (1945). "Simulated Blood Pressure Responses in Lie-Detection Tests and a Method for Their Deception". Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology. 36 (1): 201–215.
  111. ^ "IMPAC: F. Lee Bailey bio". Archived from the original on October 24, 2008.
  112. ^ Segrave, Kerry (November 18, 2014). Lie Detectors: A Social History. McFarland. ISBN 9780786481613 – via Google Books.
  113. ^ Barr, Sabrina (June 25, 2019). "Jeremy Kyle producer unable to say how accurate lie detector tests were". The Independent. Archived from the original on June 26, 2019. Retrieved December 15, 2022.
  114. ^ James Hibberd (2007-11-25). "Darnell in Defense of the 'Truth': Fox Executive Talks About the Network's Controversial Lie Detector Show". TV Week. Archived from the original on 2008-02-21. Retrieved 2008-03-11.
  115. ^ O'Dell, Cary (November 12, 2013). "The Tested Truth About 'Maury Povich'". Pop Matters. Retrieved 9 December 2014.
  116. ^ For critical commentary on this episode, see Maschke, George (December 7, 2007). "Mythbusters Beat the Lie Detector Episode featuring Michael Martin". AntiPolygraph.org.
  117. ^ Robinson, Jennifer (December 20, 2022). "American Experience: The Lie Detector". KPBS. Archived from the original on January 2, 2023. Retrieved January 4, 2023.
  118. ^ Dedman, Bill (April 9, 2008). "New anti-terror weapon: Hand-held lie detector". NBC News. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  119. ^ Ames provides personal insight into the U.S. Government's reliance on polygraphy in a 2000 letter to Steven Aftergood of the Federation of American Scientists at Ames, Aldrich (November 28, 2000). "A Letter from Aldrich Ames on Polygraph Testing". Federation of American Scientists.
  120. ^ Ronald Kessler (March 8, 1994). "Spies, Lies, Averted Eyes". The New York Times. Retrieved March 19, 2018.
  121. ^ Kessler, Ron (April 17, 1988). "Moscow's Mole in the CIA: How a Swinging Czech Superspy Stole America's Most Sensitive Secrets". Washington Post. p. C1.
  122. ^ Bachelet, Pablo (October 13, 2006). "Book outlines how spy exposed U.S. intelligence secrets to Cuba". McClatchy Washington Bureau. She first came under U.S. suspicion in 1994, when Cuba detected a highly secret electronic surveillance system. Montes took a polygraph test and passed it.
  123. ^ Ross, Brian & Richard Esposito (October 6, 2005). "Investigation Continues: Security Breach at the White House". ABC News. Officials say Aragoncillo passed several lie detector tests that are routinely given to individuals with top secret clearances.
  124. ^ "Dept. of Energy, Office of Counterintelligence". Harold James Nicholson Dossier. Archived from the original on 2008-09-21. Retrieved 2009-06-01.
  125. ^ Warrick, Joby; Eggen, Dan (2007-11-14). "Ex-FBI Employee's Case Raises New Security Concerns Sham Marriage Led to U.S. Citizenship". Washington Post. Retrieved May 22, 2010.
  126. ^ "What made Ridgway kill still a riddle". The News Tribune. 2003-11-09.
  127. ^ "BTK: Out Of The Shadows". 48 Hours Mysteries. October 1, 2005. CBS News.
  128. ^ "United States of America versus William Galbreth" (PDF). 1995-03-09. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-02-27. Retrieved 2008-02-24.
  129. ^ "Chris Watts: Wife killed our girls, so I strangled her". Houston: KTRK-TV. August 20, 2018. Retrieved February 2, 2019.
  130. ^ Roth, Alex (January 9, 2003). "Westerfield failed polygraph test badly: 'Greater than 99%' chance he was lying, examiner says on tape". San Diego Union-Tribune. Archived from the original on September 29, 2012.

Further reading

[edit]
[edit]