Pain: Difference between revisions
→Pain asymbolia and insensitivity: weasel word and euphemism fixed, but I'm not even sure if this is true |
Reverting edit(s) by 2001:56A:F888:1100:4962:1C57:CB8A:CFF7 (talk) to rev. 1259684194 by JenOttawa: Vandalism (RW 16.1) |
||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Short description|Type of distressing feeling}} |
|||
{{Use dmy dates|date=July 2012}} |
|||
{{About|physical pain |
{{About|physical pain|mental or emotional pain|Psychological pain|other uses|Pain (disambiguation)}} |
||
{{Use dmy dates|date=September 2022}} |
|||
{{SignSymptom infobox |
|||
{{Infobox medical condition (new) |
|||
| Image = OuchFlintGoodrichShot1941.jpg |
|||
| |
| image = Wrist pain.jpg |
||
| |
| caption = An illustration of [[wrist pain]] |
||
| specialty = [[Neurology]]<br />[[Pain medicine]] |
|||
| ICD9 = {{ICD9|338}} |
|||
| symptoms = Unpleasant sensory and emotional sensations<ref name="IASPdefinition">{{cite journal | vauthors = Raja SN, Carr DB, Cohen M, Finnerup NB, Flor H, Gibson S, Keefe FJ, Mogil JS, Ringkamp M, Sluka KA, Song XJ, Stevens B, Sullivan MD, Tutelman PR, Ushida T, Vader K | display-authors = 6 | title = The revised International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises | language = en-US | journal = Pain | volume = 161 | issue = 9 | pages = 1976–1982 | date = September 2020 | pmid = 32694387 | pmc = 7680716 | doi = 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001939 | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
| MedlinePlus = 002164 |
|||
| |
| complications = |
||
| |
| onset = |
||
| duration = Typically depends on the cause |
|||
| types = Physical, [[Psychological pain|psychological]], [[Psychogenic pain|psychogenic]] |
|||
| causes = |
|||
| risks = |
|||
| diagnosis = |
|||
| differential = |
|||
| prevention = |
|||
| treatment = |
|||
| medication = [[Analgesic]] |
|||
| prognosis = |
|||
| frequency = |
|||
| deaths = |
|||
}} |
}} |
||
'''Pain''' is |
'''Pain''' is a distressing feeling often caused by intense or damaging stimuli. The [[International Association for the Study of Pain]] defines pain as "an unpleasant [[sense|sensory]] and [[emotion]]al experience associated with, or resembling that associated with, actual or potential tissue damage."<ref name="IASPdefinition"/> |
||
Pain motivates |
Pain motivates [[organism]]s to withdraw from damaging situations, to protect a damaged body part while it heals, and to avoid similar experiences in the future.<ref>{{Cite book|title=Understanding Pain : Exploring the Perception of Pain| vauthors = Cervero F |date=2012 |publisher=MIT Press |isbn=9780262305433 |location=Cambridge, Mass. |pages=Chapter 1 |oclc=809043366 }}</ref> Most pain resolves once the [[noxious stimulus]] is removed and the body has healed, but it may persist despite removal of the stimulus and apparent healing of the body. Sometimes pain arises in the absence of any detectable stimulus, damage or disease.<ref name="Raj_2007">{{cite book |vauthors=Raj PP |title=In: The Handbook of Chronic Pain |publisher=Nova Biomedical Books |year=2007 |isbn=9781600210440 |chapter=Taxonomy and classification of pain |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZG4Svh_UL3UC&pg=PA41 |access-date=3 February 2016 |archive-date=30 March 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210330014627/https://books.google.com/books?id=ZG4Svh_UL3UC&pg=PA41 |url-status=live }}</ref> |
||
Pain is the most common reason for physician consultation in the |
Pain is the most common reason for physician consultation in most developed countries.<ref name="painreview">{{cite journal | vauthors = Debono DJ, Hoeksema LJ, Hobbs RD | title = Caring for patients with chronic pain: pearls and pitfalls | journal = The Journal of the American Osteopathic Association | volume = 113 | issue = 8 | pages = 620–7 | date = August 2013 | pmid = 23918913 | doi = 10.7556/jaoa.2013.023 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref name="Turk & Dennis 2004">{{cite journal | vauthors = Turk DC, Dworkin RH | title = What should be the core outcomes in chronic pain clinical trials? | journal = Arthritis Research & Therapy | volume = 6 | issue = 4 | pages = 151–4 | year = 2004 | pmid = 15225358 | pmc = 464897 | doi = 10.1186/ar1196 | doi-access = free }}</ref> It is a major symptom in many medical conditions, and can interfere with a person's [[quality of life]] and general functioning.<ref name= Breivik2008>{{cite journal | vauthors = Breivik H, Borchgrevink PC, Allen SM, Rosseland LA, Romundstad L, Hals EK, Kvarstein G, Stubhaug A | title = Assessment of pain | journal = British Journal of Anaesthesia | volume = 101 | issue = 1 | pages = 17–24 | date = July 2008 | pmid = 18487245 | doi = 10.1093/bja/aen103 | doi-access = free }}</ref> People in pain experience impaired concentration, [[working memory]], [[Cognitive flexibility|mental flexibility]], problem solving and information processing speed, and are more likely to experience irritability, depression, and anxiety. |
||
Simple pain medications are useful in 20% to 70% of cases.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Moore RA, Wiffen PJ, Derry S, Maguire T, Roy YM, Tyrrell L | title = Non-prescription (OTC) oral analgesics for acute pain – an overview of Cochrane reviews | journal = The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews | volume = 11 | issue = 11 | pages = CD010794 | date = November 2015 | pmid = 26544675 | pmc = 6485506 | doi = 10.1002/14651858.CD010794.pub2 }}</ref> Psychological factors such as [[social support]], [[cognitive behavioral therapy]], excitement, or distraction can affect pain's intensity or unpleasantness.<ref name="Eisenberger_2005">{{cite book |title=The Social Outcast: Ostracism, Social Exclusion, Rejection, & Bullying (Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology) |vauthors=Eisenberger NI, Lieberman M |publisher=Psychology Press |year=2005 |isbn=9781841694245 |veditors=Williams KD |location=East Sussex |pages=210 |chapter=Why it hurts to be left out: The neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain |chapter-url=https://www.academia.edu/621614}}</ref><ref name="Mind-Body Therapies for Opioid-Trea">{{cite journal | vauthors = Garland EL, Brintz CE, Hanley AW, Roseen EJ, Atchley RM, Gaylord SA, Faurot KR, Yaffe J, Fiander M, Keefe FJ | display-authors = 6 | title = Mind-Body Therapies for Opioid-Treated Pain: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis | journal = JAMA Internal Medicine | volume = 180 | issue = 1 | pages = 91–105 | date = January 2020 | pmid = 31682676 | pmc = 6830441 | doi = 10.1001/jamainternmed.2019.4917 }}</ref> |
|||
==Classification== |
|||
In 1994, responding to the need for a more useful system for describing chronic pain, the [[International Association for the Study of Pain]] (IASP) classified pain according to specific characteristics: (1) region of the body involved (e.g., abdomen, lower limbs), (2) system whose dysfunction may be causing the pain (e.g., nervous, gastrointestinal), (3) duration and pattern of occurrence, (4) intensity and time since onset, and (5) [[etiology]].<ref name= Merskey_Bogduk>{{vcite book| author = Merskey H & Bogduk N |title = Classification of Chronic Pain | edition = 2 | location = Seattle | publisher = International Association for the Study of Pain | year = 1994 | isbn = 0-931092-05-1 | pages = 3 & 4}}</ref> This system has been criticized by [[Clifford J. Woolf]] and others as inadequate for guiding research and treatment.<ref name=Woolf1998>{{vcite journal | author = Woolf CJ, Bennett GJ, Doherty M, Dubner R, Kidd B, Koltzenburg M, Lipton R, Loeser JD, Payne R, Torebjork E | title = Towards a mechanism-based classification of pain? | journal = Pain | volume = 77 | issue = 3 | pages = 227–9 | year = 1998 | month = September | pmid = 9808347 | doi = 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00099-2 | url = | issn = }}</ref> According to Woolf, there are three classes of pain : nociceptive pain (see hereunder), inflammatory pain which is associated with tissue damage and the infiltration of immune cells, and pathological pain which is a disease state caused by damage to the nervous system (neuropathic pain, see hereunder) or by its abnormal function (dysfunctional pain, like in fibromyalgia, irritable bowel syndrome, tension type headache, etc.).<ref>{{vcite journal |author=Woolf CJ |title=What is this thing called pain?|journal=Journal of Clinical Investigation | year=2010 | month=Nov | volume=120 | issue=11| pages=3742–4 | doi=10.1172/JCI45178 | pmc=2965006 | pmid=21041955}}</ref> |
|||
== |
== Etymology == |
||
First attested in English in 1297, the word ''peyn'' comes from the [[Old French]] ''peine'', in turn from [[Latin]] ''poena'' meaning "punishment, penalty"<ref>{{cite web | vauthors = Lewis CT, Short C | work = A Latin Dictionary | url = https://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0059%3Aentry%3Dpoena | title = Poena | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20110513202944/https://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0059%3Aentry%3Dpoena| archive-date = 13 May 2011 | via = Perseus Digital Library }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Lavoie |first1=Anne |last2=Toledo |first2=Paloma |date=2013-09-01 |title=Multimodal Postcesarean Delivery Analgesia |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0095510813000638 |journal=Clinics in Perinatology |series=Pain Management in the Peripartum Period |language=en |volume=40 |issue=3 |pages=443–455 |doi=10.1016/j.clp.2013.05.008 |pmid=23972750 |issn=0095-5108}}</ref> (also meaning "torment, hardship, suffering" in Late Latin) and that from [[Greek language|Greek]] ποινή (''poine''), generally meaning "price paid, penalty, punishment".<ref>{{cite web | url = https://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0057%3Aentry%3Dpoinh%2F | title = ποινή | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20110513202951/https://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0057%3Aentry%3Dpoinh%2F| archive-date = 13 May 2011 | vauthors = Liddell HG, Scott R | work = A Greek-English Lexicon | via = Perseus Digital Library }}</ref><ref>{{cite web | url = https://www.etymonline.com/index.php?term=pain | title = Pain | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20110728085534/https://www.etymonline.com/index.php?term=pain| archive-date=28 July 2011 | via = Online Etymology Dictionary }}</ref> |
|||
{{main|Chronic pain}} |
|||
Pain is usually transitory, lasting only until the noxious stimulus is removed or the underlying damage or pathology has healed, but some painful conditions, such as [[rheumatoid arthritis]], [[peripheral neuropathy]], cancer and [[idiopathic]] pain, may persist for years. Pain that lasts a long time is called ''[[Chronic pain|chronic]]'', and pain that resolves quickly is called ''[[Acute (medicine)|acute]]''. Traditionally, the distinction between ''acute'' and ''chronic'' pain has relied upon an arbitrary interval of time from onset; the two most commonly used markers being 3 months and 6 months since the onset of pain,<ref name = Turk&Okifuji2001>{{vcite book | author = Turk DC, Okifuji A | authorlink = | editor = Bonica JJ, Loeser JD, Chapman CR, Turk DC, Butler SH | others = | title = Bonica's management of pain | edition = | publisher = Lippincott Williams & Wilkins | location = Hagerstwon, MD | year = 2001 | isbn = 0-683-30462-3 | chapter = Pain terms and taxonomies of pain }}</ref> though some theorists and researchers have placed the transition from acute to chronic pain at 12 months.<ref name="isbn0-443-05683-8">{{vcite book | author = Spanswick CC, Main CJ | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach | edition = | publisher = Churchill Livingstone | location = Edinburgh | year = 2000 | pages = 93 | isbn = 0-443-05683-8 | chapter = | chapterurl = | url = http://books.google.com/books?id=wcEQPzTOEAoC&pg=PA93&#v=onepage&q&f=false | accessdate = }}</ref> Others apply ''acute'' to pain that lasts less than 30 days, ''chronic'' to pain of more than six months duration, and ''subacute'' to pain that lasts from one to six months.<ref name=Thienhaus1>{{vcite book | author = Thienhaus O, Cole BE | authorlink = | editor = Weiner R | others = | title = Pain management: a practical guide for clinicians | edition = | publisher = CRC Press | location = Boca Raton | year = 2002 | pages = 28 | isbn = 0-8493-0926-3 | chapter = Classification of pain | doi = | url =}}</ref> A popular alternative definition of ''chronic pain'', involving no arbitrarily fixed durations is "pain that extends beyond the expected period of healing."<ref name = Turk&Okifuji2001/> Chronic pain may be classified as [[cancer pain]] or benign.<ref name=Thienhaus1/> |
|||
== Classification == |
|||
===Nociceptive=== |
|||
The [[International Association for the Study of Pain]] recommends using specific features to describe a patient's pain: |
|||
Nociceptive pain is caused by stimulation of [[Peripheral nervous system|peripheral nerve fibers]] that respond only to stimuli approaching or exceeding harmful intensity ([[nociceptors]]), and may be classified according to the mode of noxious stimulation; the most common categories being "thermal" (heat or cold), "mechanical" (crushing, tearing, etc.) and "chemical" (iodine in a cut, chili powder in the eyes). |
|||
# Region of the body involved (e.g. abdomen, lower limbs) |
|||
# System whose dysfunction may be causing the pain (e.g., nervous, gastrointestinal) |
|||
# Duration and pattern of occurrence |
|||
# Intensity |
|||
# Cause<ref name= Merskey_Bogduk>{{cite book |vauthors=Merskey H, Bogduk N |title=Classification of Chronic Pain |edition=2 nd |location=Seattle |publisher=International Association for the Study of Pain |year=1994 |isbn=978-0931092053 |pages=[https://archive.org/details/classificationof0000unse_o5f1/page/3 3 & 4] |url=https://archive.org/details/classificationof0000unse_o5f1/page/3 }}</ref> |
|||
=== Chronic versus acute === |
|||
Nociceptive pain may also be divided into "visceral," "deep somatic" and "superficial somatic" pain. [[Viscus|Visceral structures]] are highly sensitive to stretch, [[ischemia]] and [[inflammation]], but relatively insensitive to other stimuli that normally evoke pain in other structures, such as burning and cutting. [[Visceral pain]] is diffuse, difficult to locate and often [[referred pain|referred]] to a distant, usually superficial, structure. It may be accompanied by nausea and vomiting and may be described as sickening, deep, squeezing, and dull.<ref name = Urch>{{vcite book | author = Urch CE & Suzuki R | chapter = Pathophysiology of somatic, visceral, and neuropathic cancer pain | editor = Sykes N, Bennett MI & Yuan C-S | title = Clinical pain management: Cancer pain | edition = 2 | isbn = 978-0-340-94007-5 | publisher = Hodder Arnold | location = London | pages = 3–12 }}</ref> ''Deep somatic'' pain is initiated by stimulation of nociceptors in ligaments, tendons, bones, blood vessels, [[fasciae]] and muscles, and is dull, aching, poorly localized pain. Examples include [[sprain]]s and broken bones. ''Superficial'' pain is initiated by activation of nociceptors in the skin or other superficial tissue, and is sharp, well-defined and clearly located. Examples of injuries that produce superficial somatic pain include minor wounds and minor (first degree) burns.<ref name="isbn0-443-05683-8">{{vcite book | author = Coda BA, Bonica JJ | authorlink = | editor = Spanswick CC, Main CJ | others = | title = Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach | edition = | publisher = Churchill Livingstone | location = Edinburgh | year = 2000 | pages = | isbn = 0-443-05683-8 | chapter = General considerations of acute pain | doi = | url =}}</ref> |
|||
{{main|Chronic pain}} |
|||
Pain is usually transitory, lasting only until the noxious stimulus is removed or the underlying damage or pathology has healed. But some painful conditions, such as [[rheumatoid arthritis]], [[peripheral neuropathy]], [[Cancer pain|cancer]], and [[idiopathic]] pain, may persist for years. Pain that lasts a long time is called "[[Chronic pain|chronic]]" or "persistent", and pain that resolves quickly is called "[[Acute (medicine)|acute]]". Traditionally, the distinction between acute and chronic pain has relied upon an arbitrary interval of time between onset and resolution; the two most commonly used markers being 3 months and 6 months since the onset of pain,<ref name = Turk&Okifuji2001>{{cite book | vauthors = Turk DC, Okifuji A | veditors = Bonica JJ, Loeser JD, Chapman CR, Turk DC |title=Bonica's management of pain |publisher=Lippincott Williams & Wilkins |location=Hagerstwon, MD |year=2001 |isbn=978-0781768276 |chapter=Pain terms and taxonomies of pain}}</ref> though some theorists and researchers have placed the transition from acute to chronic pain at 12 months.<ref name="isbn0-443-05683-8"/>{{rp|93}} Others apply "acute" to pain that lasts less than 30 days, "chronic" to pain of more than six months' duration, and "subacute" to pain that lasts from one to six months.<ref name=Thienhaus1>{{cite book | vauthors = Thienhaus O, Cole BE | veditors = Weiner R |title=Pain management: a practical guide for clinicians | url = https://archive.org/details/painmanagementpr00wein | url-access = limited |publisher=CRC Press |location=Boca Raton |year=2002 |pages=[https://archive.org/details/painmanagementpr00wein/page/n60 28] |isbn=978-0849322624 |chapter=Classification of pain}}</ref> A popular alternative definition of "chronic pain", involving no arbitrarily fixed duration, is "pain that extends beyond the expected period of healing".<ref name = Turk&Okifuji2001/> Chronic pain may be classified as "[[cancer pain|cancer-related]]" or "benign."<ref name=Thienhaus1/> |
|||
===Neuropathic=== |
|||
{{Main|Neuropathic pain}} |
|||
Neuropathic pain is caused by damage or disease affecting any part of the nervous system involved in bodily feelings (the [[somatosensory system]]).<ref name= Treede2008>{{vcite journal | author = Treede RD, Jensen TS, Campbell JN, Cruccu G, Dostrovsky JO, Griffin JW, Hansson P, Hughes R, Nurmikko T, Serra J | title = Neuropathic pain: redefinition and a grading system for clinical and research purposes | journal = Neurology | volume = 70 | issue = 18 | pages = 1630–5 | year = 2008 | month = April | pmid = 18003941 | doi = 10.1212/01.wnl.0000282763.29778.59 | url = https://www.fpa-support.org/learning/Articles/2008/documents/Treed2008NeuropathicpainredefintionandgradingNeurology.pdf | issn = }}</ref> Peripheral neuropathic pain is often described as “burning,” “tingling,” “electrical,” “stabbing,” or “pins and needles.”<ref name= Paice2003>{{vcite journal | author = Paice JA | title = Mechanisms and management of neuropathic pain in cancer | journal = J. Support Oncol. | volume = 1 | issue = 2 | pages = 107–20 | year = 2003 | pmid = 15352654 | doi = | url = http://www.supportiveoncology.net/journal/articles/0102107.pdf | issn = }}</ref> Bumping the "[[funny bone]]" elicits acute peripheral neuropathic pain. |
|||
=== |
=== Allodynia === |
||
[[Allodynia]] is pain experienced in response to a normally painless stimulus.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal | vauthors = Jensen TS, Finnerup NB | title = Allodynia and hyperalgesia in neuropathic pain: clinical manifestations and mechanisms | journal = The Lancet. Neurology | volume = 13 | issue = 9 | pages = 924–935 | date = September 2014 | pmid = 25142459 | doi = 10.1016/s1474-4422(14)70102-4 | s2cid = 25011309 }}</ref> It has no biological function and is classified by characteristics of the stimuli as cold, heat, touch, pressure or a pinprick.<ref name=":0"/><ref name="Lolignier 133–139">{{cite journal | vauthors = Lolignier S, Eijkelkamp N, Wood JN | title = Mechanical allodynia | journal = Pflügers Archiv | volume = 467 | issue = 1 | pages = 133–139 | date = January 2015 | pmid = 24846747 | pmc = 4281368 | doi = 10.1007/s00424-014-1532-0 }}</ref> |
|||
=== Phantom === |
|||
{{Main|Phantom pain}} |
{{Main|Phantom pain}} |
||
[[Phantom pain]] is pain felt in a part of the body that has been |
[[Phantom pain]] is pain felt in a part of the body that has been [[amputated]], or from which the brain no longer receives signals. It is a type of neuropathic pain.<ref name="pmid10863043">{{cite journal | vauthors = Kooijman CM, Dijkstra PU, Geertzen JH, Elzinga A, van der Schans CP | title = Phantom pain and phantom sensations in upper limb amputees: an epidemiological study | journal = Pain | volume = 87 | issue = 1 | pages = 33–41 | date = July 2000 | pmid = 10863043 | doi = 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00264-5 | s2cid = 7565030 | url = https://research.rug.nl/en/publications/phantom-pain-and-phantom-sensations-in-upper-limb-amputees(8dd1fc12-cc0d-400b-aef1-72ed0443f9ad).html }}{{Dead link|date=October 2022 |bot=InternetArchiveBot |fix-attempted=yes }}</ref> |
||
The [[prevalence]] of phantom pain in upper limb amputees is nearly 82%, and in lower limb amputees is 54%.<ref name="pmid10863043" |
The [[prevalence]] of phantom pain in upper limb amputees is nearly 82%, and in lower limb amputees is 54%.<ref name="pmid10863043"/> One study found that eight days after amputation, 72% of patients had phantom limb pain, and six months later, 67% reported it.<ref name="pmid6657285">{{cite journal | vauthors = Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P | title = Phantom limb, phantom pain and stump pain in amputees during the first 6 months following limb amputation | journal = Pain | volume = 17 | issue = 3 | pages = 243–256 | date = November 1983 | pmid = 6657285 | doi = 10.1016/0304-3959(83)90097-0 | s2cid = 10304696 }}</ref><ref name="pmid3991231">{{cite journal | vauthors = Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P | title = Immediate and long-term phantom limb pain in amputees: incidence, clinical characteristics and relationship to pre-amputation limb pain | journal = Pain | volume = 21 | issue = 3 | pages = 267–278 | date = March 1985 | pmid = 3991231 | doi = 10.1016/0304-3959(85)90090-9 | s2cid = 24358789 }}</ref> Some amputees experience continuous pain that varies in intensity or quality; others experience several bouts of pain per day, or it may reoccur less often. It is often described as shooting, crushing, burning or cramping. If the pain is continuous for a long period, parts of the intact body may become sensitized, so that touching them evokes pain in the phantom limb. Phantom limb pain may accompany [[urination]] or [[defecation]].<ref name = "The challenge of pain">{{cite book | vauthors = Wall PD, Melzack R |title=The challenge of pain |edition=2nd |publisher=Penguin Books |location=New York |year=1996 |isbn=978-0140256703}}</ref>{{rp|61–69}} |
||
Local anesthetic injections into the nerves or sensitive areas of the stump may relieve pain for days, weeks or |
[[Local anesthetic]] injections into the nerves or sensitive areas of the stump may relieve pain for days, weeks, or sometimes permanently, despite the drug wearing off in a matter of hours; and small injections of [[:wikt:hypertonic|hypertonic]] saline into the soft tissue between vertebrae produces local pain that radiates into the phantom limb for ten minutes or so and may be followed by hours, weeks, or even longer of partial or total relief from phantom pain. Vigorous vibration or electrical stimulation of the stump, or current from electrodes surgically implanted onto the spinal cord, all produce relief in some patients.<ref name = "The challenge of pain"/>{{rp|61–69}} |
||
[[Mirror box]] therapy produces the illusion of movement and touch in a phantom limb which in turn may cause a reduction in pain.<ref name="pmid8637922">{{cite journal | vauthors = Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D | title = Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors | journal = Proceedings. Biological Sciences | volume = 263 | issue = 1369 | pages = 377–386 | date = April 1996 | pmid = 8637922 | doi = 10.1098/rspb.1996.0058 | bibcode = 1996RSPSB.263..377R | s2cid = 4819370 }}</ref> |
|||
[[Paraplegia]], the loss of sensation and voluntary motor control after serious spinal cord damage, may be accompanied by [[girdle pain]] at the level of the spinal cord damage, [[visceral pain]] evoked by a filling bladder or bowel, or, in five to ten per cent of paraplegics, phantom body pain in areas of complete sensory loss. This phantom body pain is initially described as burning or tingling but may evolve into severe crushing or pinching pain, fire running down the legs, or a knife twisting in the flesh. Onset may be immediate or may not occur until years after the disabling injury. Surgical treatment rarely provides lasting relief.<ref name = "Melzack&Wall61–69"/> |
|||
===Psychogenic=== |
|||
{{Main|Psychogenic pain}} |
|||
Psychogenic pain, also called ''psychalgia'' or ''somatoform pain'', is pain caused, increased, or prolonged by mental, emotional, or behavioral factors.<ref>[http://my.clevelandclinic.org/services/Pain_Management/hic_Psychogenic_Pain.aspx Cleveland Clinic, Health information] |
|||
<p>'''^''' [http://www.biology-online.org/dictionary/Psychogenic_pain "Psychogenic pain - definition from Biology-Online.org"] Biology-online.org. Retrieved 2008-11-05.</ref> Headache, back pain, and stomach pain are sometimes diagnosed as psychogenic.<ref name="cleveland">[http://my.clevelandclinic.org/services/Pain_Management/hic_Psychogenic_Pain.aspx Cleveland Clinic, Health information]</ref> Sufferers are often stigmatized, because both medical professionals and the general public tend to think that pain from a psychological source is not "real". However, specialists consider that it is no less actual or hurtful than pain from any other source.<ref name=IASP_definitions>[http://web.archive.org/web/20080512061229/http%3A//www.iasp-pain.org/AM/Template.cfm%3FSection%3DGeneral_Resource_Links%26Template%3D/CM/HTMLDisplay.cfm%26ContentID%3D3058#Pain "International Association for the Study of Pain | Pain Definitions".]. Retrieved 12 October 2010.</ref> |
|||
[[Paraplegia]], the loss of sensation and voluntary motor control after serious spinal cord damage, may be accompanied by [[girdle pain]] at the level of the spinal cord damage, [[visceral pain]] evoked by a filling bladder or bowel, or, in five to ten percent of paraplegics, phantom body pain in areas of complete sensory loss. This phantom body pain is initially described as burning or tingling but may evolve into severe crushing or pinching pain, or the sensation of fire running down the legs or of a knife twisting in the flesh. Onset may be immediate or may not occur until years after the disabling injury. Surgical treatment rarely provides lasting relief.<ref name = "The challenge of pain"/>{{rp|61–69}} |
|||
People with long term pain frequently display psychological disturbance, with elevated scores on the [[Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory]] scales of [[hysteria]], [[depression (mood)|depression]] and [[hypochondriasis]] (the "[[neurotic triad]]"). Some investigators have argued that it is this neuroticism that causes acute injuries to turn chronic, but clinical evidence points the other way, to chronic pain causing [[neuroticism]]. When long term pain is relieved by therapeutic intervention, scores on the neurotic triad and [[anxiety]] fall, often to normal levels. Self-esteem, often low in chronic pain patients, also shows improvement once pain has resolved.<ref name=Melzack_Wall_1996_31-32>{{vcite book | author = Wall PD, Melzack R | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = The challenge of pain | edition = | publisher = Penguin Books | location = New York | year = 1996 | pages = | isbn = 0-14-025670-9 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> |
|||
=== Breakthrough{{anchor|Breakthrough_pain}} === |
|||
{{quote|“The term 'psychogenic' assumes that medical diagnosis is so perfect that all organic causes of pain can be detected; regrettably, we are far from such infallibility... All too often, the diagnosis of neurosis as the cause of pain hides our ignorance of many aspects of pain medicine.”| [[Ronald Melzack]], 1996.<ref name=Melzack_Wall_1996_31-32/>}} |
|||
Breakthrough pain is transitory pain that comes on suddenly and is not alleviated by the patient's regular [[pain management]]. It is common in cancer patients who often have background pain that is generally well-controlled by medications, but who also sometimes experience bouts of severe pain that from time to time "breaks through" the medication. The characteristics of breakthrough [[cancer pain]] vary from person to person and according to the cause. Management of breakthrough pain can entail intensive use of [[opioid]]s, including [[fentanyl]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Mishra S, Bhatnagar S, Chaudhary P, Rana SP | title = Breakthrough cancer pain: review of prevalence, characteristics and management | journal = Indian Journal of Palliative Care | volume = 15 | issue = 1 | pages = 14–18 | date = January 2009 | pmid = 20606850 | pmc = 2886208 | doi = 10.4103/0973-1075.53506 | df = dmy-all | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Caraceni A, Hanks G, Kaasa S, Bennett MI, Brunelli C, Cherny N, Dale O, De Conno F, Fallon M, Hanna M, Haugen DF, Juhl G, King S, Klepstad P, Laugsand EA, Maltoni M, Mercadante S, Nabal M, Pigni A, Radbruch L, Reid C, Sjogren P, Stone PC, Tassinari D, Zeppetella G | title = Use of opioid analgesics in the treatment of cancer pain: evidence-based recommendations from the EAPC | journal = The Lancet. Oncology | volume = 13 | issue = 2 | pages = e58–68 | date = February 2012 | pmid = 22300860 | doi = 10.1016/S1470-2045(12)70040-2 | url = https://www.fadin.org/Documenti/164/Recommendations_EAPC_2012.pdf | url-status = dead | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20141019205001/https://www.fadin.org/Documenti/164/Recommendations_EAPC_2012.pdf | archive-date = 19 October 2014 | accessdate = 7 March 2022 }}</ref> |
|||
=== Asymbolia and insensitivity === |
|||
===Breakthrough pain=== |
|||
Breakthrough pain is pain that comes on suddenly for short periods of time {{Citation needed|date=August 2012}} and is not alleviated by the patients' normal pain management. It is common in [[cancer]] patients who often have a background level of pain controlled by [[medication]]s, but whose pain periodically "breaks through" the medication. The characteristics of breakthrough [[cancer pain]] vary from person to person and according to the cause. |
|||
===Incident pain=== |
|||
Incident pain is pain that arises as a result of activity, such as movement of an arthritic joint, stretching a wound, etc. |
|||
===Pain asymbolia and insensitivity=== |
|||
{{Main|Pain asymbolia|Congenital insensitivity to pain}} |
{{Main|Pain asymbolia|Congenital insensitivity to pain}} |
||
{{Redirect|Painless}} |
|||
[[File:No pain. Science Museum Painless Exhibition Series.webm|thumb|A patient and doctor discuss congenital insensitivity to pain.]] |
|||
The ability to experience pain is essential for protection from injury, and recognition of the presence of injury. Episodic [[analgesia]] may occur under special circumstances, such as in the excitement of sport or war: a soldier on the battlefield may feel no pain for many hours from a traumatic amputation or other severe injury.<ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Beecher HK | date = 1959 | title = Measurement of subjective responses | url = https://archive.org/details/measurementofsub0000beec | url-access = registration | location = New York | publisher = Oxford University Press}} cited in {{cite book | vauthors = Melzack R, Wall PD | date = 1996 | title = The challenge of pain | edition = 2nd | location = London | publisher = Penguin | page = 7 | isbn = 978-0140256703}}</ref> |
|||
Although unpleasantness is an essential part of the [[International Association for the Study of Pain|IASP]] definition of pain,<ref name="IASPdef">{{cite web|title=International Association for the Study of Pain: Pain Definitions|url=https://www.iasp-pain.org/Taxonomy|url-status=dead|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150113000208/https://www.iasp-pain.org/Taxonomy|archive-date=13 January 2015|access-date=12 January 2015|quote=Pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage}} [https://etc.ch/ZTKs Alt URL]{{Dead link|date=February 2022 |bot=InternetArchiveBot |fix-attempted=yes }} Derived from {{cite journal|vauthors=Bonica JJ|date=June 1979|title=The need of a taxonomy|journal=Pain|volume=6|issue=3|pages=247–248|doi=10.1016/0304-3959(79)90046-0|pmid=460931|s2cid=53161389}}</ref> it is possible in some patients to induce a state known as pain asymbolia, described as intense pain devoid of unpleasantness, with [[morphine]] injection or [[psychosurgery]].<ref name=IASP_definitions/> Such patients report that they have pain but are not bothered by it; they recognize the sensation of pain but suffer little, or not at all.<ref name="asymbolia">{{cite book | vauthors = Grahek N |title=Feeling pain and being in pain |date=2007 |publisher=MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass. |isbn=978-0262517324 |edition=2nd | url = https://docserver.bis.uni-oldenburg.de/publikationen/bisverlag/2001/grafee01/grafee01.html | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20080927042509/https://docserver.bis.uni-oldenburg.de/publikationen/bisverlag/2001/grafee01/grafee01.html | archive-date = 27 September 2008 }}</ref> Indifference to pain can also rarely be present from birth; these people have normal nerves on medical investigations, and find pain unpleasant, but do not avoid repetition of the pain stimulus.<ref name="pmid12583863">{{cite journal | vauthors = Nagasako EM, Oaklander AL, Dworkin RH | title = Congenital insensitivity to pain: an update | journal = Pain | volume = 101 | issue = 3 | pages = 213–219 | date = February 2003 | pmid = 12583863 | doi = 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00482-7 | s2cid = 206055264 }}</ref> |
|||
The ability to experience pain is essential for protection from injury, and recognition of the presence of injury. Episodic analgesia may occur under special circumstances, such as in the excitement of sport or war: a soldier on the battlefield may feel no pain for many hours from a traumatic amputation or other severe injury.<ref>Beecher, HK (1959). ''Measurement of subjective responses''. New York: Oxford University Press. cited in Melzack, R; Wall, PD (1996). ''The challenge of pain'' (2 ed.). London: Penguin. p. 7. ISBN 978-0-14-025670-3.</ref> |
|||
Insensitivity to pain may also result from abnormalities in the [[nervous system]]. This is usually the result of [[acquired disorder|acquired]] damage to the nerves, such as [[spinal cord injury]], [[diabetes mellitus]] ([[diabetic neuropathy]]), or [[leprosy]] in countries where that disease is prevalent.<ref name=Brand_1997>{{cite book | vauthors = Brand PW, Yancey P |title=The gift of pain: why we hurt & what we can do about it |publisher=Zondervan Publ |location=Grand Rapids, Mich |year=1997 |isbn=978-0310221449}}</ref> These individuals are at risk of tissue damage and infection due to undiscovered injuries. People with diabetes-related nerve damage, for instance, sustain poorly-healing foot ulcers as a result of decreased sensation.<ref name="pmid15472838">{{cite journal | vauthors = Lipsky BA, Berendt AR, Deery HG, Embil JM, Joseph WS, Karchmer AW, LeFrock JL, Lew DP, Mader JT, Norden C, Tan JS | title = Diagnosis and treatment of diabetic foot infections | journal = Clinical Infectious Diseases | volume = 39 | issue = 7 | pages = 885–910 | date = October 2004 | pmid = 15472838 | doi = 10.1086/424846 | first11 = JS | first10 = C | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
Although unpleasantness is an essential part of the [[International Association for the Study of Pain|IASP]] definition of pain,<ref name = IASPdef>{{vcite web | quote = Pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage | url = http://www.iasp-pain.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section=Pain_Defi...isplay.cfm&ContentID=1728 | title = International Association for the Study of Pain: Pain Definitions | accessdate =10 Sep 2011 }} Derived from {{vcite journal | author = Bonica JJ | title = The need of a taxonomy | journal = Pain | volume = 6 | issue = 3 | pages = 247–8 | year = 1979 | month = June | pmid = 460931 | doi = 10.1016/0304-3959(79)90046-0 | url = | issn = }}</ref> it is possible to induce a state described as intense pain devoid of [[unpleasantness]] in some patients, with morphine injection or psychosurgery.<ref name=IASP_definitions/> Such patients report that they have pain but are not bothered by it, they recognize the sensation of pain but suffer little, or not at all.<ref name="asymbolia">Nikola Grahek, [http://docserver.bis.uni-oldenburg.de/publikationen/bisverlag/2001/grafee01/grafee01.html Feeling pain and being in pain], Oldenburg, 2001. ISBN 3-8142-0780-7.</ref> Indifference to pain can also rarely be present from birth; these people have normal nerves on medical investigations, and find pain unpleasant, but do not avoid repetition of the pain stimulus.<ref name="pmid12583863">{{vcite journal | author = Nagasako EM, Oaklander AL, Dworkin RH | title = Congenital insensitivity to pain: an update | journal = Pain | volume = 101 | issue = 3 | pages = 213–9 | year = 2003 | month = February | pmid = 12583863 | doi = 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00482-7 | url = | issn = }}</ref> |
|||
A much smaller number of people are insensitive to pain due to an inborn abnormality of the nervous system, known as "[[congenital insensitivity to pain]]".<ref name="pmid12583863"/> Children with this condition incur carelessly-repeated damage to their tongues, eyes, joints, skin, and muscles. Some die before adulthood, and others have a reduced life expectancy.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Raina* |first=Triveni |last2=Dash |first2=Bhagya Ranjan |title=AN INTRODUCTORY APPROACH TO PAIN MANAGEMENT THROUGH AYURVEDA WITH BRIEF HOLISTIC REVIEW |url=https://core.ac.uk/outputs/333809971/?utm_source=pdf&utm_medium=banner&utm_campaign=pdf-decoration-v1}}</ref> Most people with congenital insensitivity to pain have one of five [[hereditary sensory and autonomic neuropathy|hereditary sensory and autonomic neuropathies]] (which includes [[familial dysautonomia]] and [[congenital insensitivity to pain with anhidrosis]]).<ref name=Axelrod>{{cite journal | vauthors = Axelrod FB, Hilz MJ | title = Inherited autonomic neuropathies | journal = Seminars in Neurology | volume = 23 | issue = 4 | pages = 381–390 | date = December 2003 | pmid = 15088259 | doi = 10.1055/s-2004-817722 | s2cid = 260317729 }}</ref> These conditions feature decreased sensitivity to pain together with other neurological abnormalities, particularly of the [[autonomic nervous system]].<ref name="pmid12583863"/><ref name=Axelrod/> A very rare syndrome with isolated congenital insensitivity to pain has been linked with mutations in the ''[[SCN9A]]'' gene, which codes for a sodium channel ([[Nav1.7|Na<sub>v</sub>1.7]]) necessary in conducting pain nerve stimuli.<ref name="pmid21041956">{{cite journal | vauthors = Raouf R, Quick K, Wood JN | title = Pain as a channelopathy | journal = The Journal of Clinical Investigation | volume = 120 | issue = 11 | pages = 3745–3752 | date = November 2010 | pmid = 21041956 | pmc = 2965577 | doi = 10.1172/JCI43158 }}</ref> |
|||
Insensitivity to pain may also result from abnormalities in the nervous system. This is usually the result of [[acquired disorder|acquired]] damage to the nerves, such as [[spinal cord injury]], [[diabetes mellitus]] ([[diabetic neuropathy]]), or [[leprosy]] in countries where this is prevalent.<ref name=Brand_1997>{{vcite book | author = Brand PW, Yancey P | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = The gift of pain: why we hurt & what we can do about it | edition = | publisher = Zondervan Publ | location = Grand Rapids, Mich | year = 1997 | pages = | isbn = 0-310-22144-7 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> These individuals are at risk of tissue damage due to undiscovered injury. People with diabetes-related nerve damage, for instance, sustain poorly healing foot ulcers as a result of decreased sensation.<ref name="pmid15472838">{{vcite journal | author = Lipsky BA, Berendt AR, Deery HG, Embil JM, Joseph WS, Karchmer AW, LeFrock JL, Lew DP, Mader JT, Norden C, Tan JS | title = Diagnosis and treatment of diabetic foot infections | journal = Clin. Infect. Dis. | volume = 39 | issue = 7 | pages = 885–910 | year = 2004 | month = October | pmid = 15472838 | doi = 10.1086/424846 | url = | issn = }}</ref> |
|||
== Functional effects == |
|||
A much smaller number of people are insensitive to pain due to an inborn abnormality of the nervous system, known as "[[congenital insensitivity to pain]]".<ref name="pmid12583863"/> Children with this condition incur carelessly repeated damage to their tongue, eyes, joints, skin, and muscles. Some die before adulthood, and others have a reduced life expectancy.{{cn}} Most people with congenital insensitivity to pain have one of five [[hereditary sensory and autonomic neuropathy|hereditary sensory and autonomic neuropathies]] (which includes [[familial dysautonomia]] and [[congenital insensitivity to pain with anhidrosis]]).<ref name=Axelrod>{{vcite journal | author = Axelrod FB, Hilz MJ | title = Inherited autonomic neuropathies | journal = Semin Neurol | volume = 23 | issue = 4 | pages = 381–90 | year = 2003 | month = December | pmid = 15088259 | doi = 10.1055/s-2004-817722 | url = | issn = }}</ref> These conditions feature decreased sensitivity to pain together with other neurological abnormalties, particularly of the [[autonomic nervous system]].<ref name="pmid12583863"/><ref name=Axelrod/> A very rare syndrome with isolated congenital insensitivity to pain has been linked with mutations in the ''[[SCN9A]]'' gene, which codes for a sodium channel (Na<sub>v</sub>1.7) necessary in conducting pain nerve stimuli.<ref name="pmid21041956">{{vcite journal | author = Raouf R, Quick K, Wood JN | title = Pain as a channelopathy | journal = J. Clin. Invest. | volume = 120 | issue = 11 | pages = 3745–52 | year = 2010 | month = November | pmid = 21041956 | pmc = 2965577 | doi = 10.1172/JCI43158}}</ref> |
|||
Experimental subjects challenged by acute pain and patients in chronic pain experience impairments in attention control, [[Working memory|working memory capacity]], [[Cognitive flexibility|mental flexibility]], problem solving, and information processing speed.<ref name=Hart2003>{{cite journal | vauthors = Hart RP, Wade JB, Martelli MF | title = Cognitive impairment in patients with chronic pain: the significance of stress | journal = Current Pain and Headache Reports | volume = 7 | issue = 2 | pages = 116–126 | date = April 2003 | pmid = 12628053 | doi = 10.1007/s11916-003-0021-5 | s2cid = 14104974 }}</ref> Pain is also associated with increased depression, anxiety, fear, and anger.<ref name="pmid19146872">{{cite journal | vauthors = Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Chont M | title = Pain-related effects of trait anger expression: neural substrates and the role of endogenous opioid mechanisms | journal = Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews | volume = 33 | issue = 3 | pages = 475–491 | date = March 2009 | pmid = 19146872 | pmc = 2756489 | doi = 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.12.003 }}</ref> |
|||
{{blockquote|If I have matters right, the consequences of pain will include direct physical distress, unemployment, financial difficulties, marital disharmony, and difficulties in concentration and attention…|[[Harold Merskey]] 2000<ref name="isbn1-55798-646-0">{{cite book |vauthors=Merskey H |veditors=Weisberg JN, Gatchel RJ |title=Personality Characteristics of Patients With Pain |publisher=American Psychological Association (APA) |year=2000 |isbn=978-1557986467 |chapter=The History of Psychoanalytic Ideas Concerning Pain |chapter-url-access=registration |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/personalitychara0000unse }}</ref>}} |
|||
==Effect on functioning== |
|||
Experimental subjects challenged by acute pain and patients in chronic pain experience impairments in attention control, [[working memory]], [[Cognitive flexibility|mental flexibility]], problem solving, and information processing speed.<ref name=Hart2003>{{vcite journal | author = Hart RP, Wade JB, Martelli MF | title = Cognitive impairment in patients with chronic pain: the significance of stress | journal = Curr Pain Headache Rep | volume = 7 | issue = 2 | pages = 116–26 | year = 2003 | month = April | pmid = 12628053 | doi = 10.1007/s11916-003-0021-5 | url = | issn = }}</ref> Acute and chronic pain are also associated with increased [[Depression (mood)|depression]], anxiety, fear, and anger.<ref name="pmid19146872">{{vcite journal | author = Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Chont M | title = Pain-related effects of trait anger expression: neural substrates and the role of endogenous opioid mechanisms | journal = Neurosci Biobehav Rev | volume = 33 | issue = 3 | pages = 475–91 | year = 2009 | month = March | pmid = 19146872 | pmc = 2756489 | doi = 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.12.003 | url = | issn = }}</ref> |
|||
=== On subsequent negative emotion === |
|||
{{quote|"If I have matters right, the consequences of pain will include direct physical distress, unemployment, financial difficulties, marital disharmony, and difficulties in concentration and attention…" |Harold Merskey 2000<ref name="isbn1-55798-646-0">{{vcite book | author = Merskey H | authorlink = | editor = Weisberg JN, Gatchel RJ | others = | title = Personality Characteristics of Patients With Pain | edition = | publisher = American Psychological Association (APA) | location = | year = 2000 | pages = | isbn = 1-55798-646-0 | chapter = The History of Psychoanalitic Ideas Concerning Pain | doi = | url =}}</ref>}} |
|||
Although pain is considered to be aversive and unpleasant and is therefore usually avoided, a [[meta-analysis]] which summarized and evaluated numerous studies from various psychological disciplines, found a reduction in [[negative affect]]. Across studies, participants that were subjected to acute physical pain in the laboratory subsequently reported feeling better than those in non-painful control conditions, a finding which was also reflected in physiological parameters.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors= Bresin K, Kling L, Verona E | year = 2018 | title = The effect of acute physical pain on subsequent negative emotional affect: A meta-analysis | journal = Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment | volume = 9 | issue = 3| pages = 273–283 | doi = 10.1037/per0000248 | pmid = 28368146 | pmc = 5624817 }}</ref> A potential mechanism to explain this effect is provided by the [[opponent-process theory]]. |
|||
==Theory== |
==Theory== |
||
=== Historical theories === |
|||
{{see also|History of pain theory}} |
|||
Before the relatively recent discovery of [[neuron]]s and their role in pain, various different body functions were proposed to account for pain. There were several competing early theories of pain among the ancient Greeks: [[Aristotle]] believed that pain was due to evil spirits entering the body through injury, and [[Hippocrates]] believed that it was due to an imbalance in vital fluids.<ref name=Linton>Linton. Models of Pain Perception. Elsevier Health, 2005. Print.</ref> In the 11th century, [[Avicenna]] theorized that there were a number of feeling senses including touch, pain and titillation,<ref name = Dallenbach>{{vcite journal| author = Dallenbach KM | title = Pain: History and present status | journal = American Journal of Psychology | date = July 1939 | volume = 52 | pages = 331–347 }}</ref> but prior to the scientific [[Renaissance]] in Europe pain was not well-understood, and it was thought that pain originated outside the body, perhaps as a punishment from God.<ref name=Meldrum>Meldrum, Marcia. "A History of Pain Management." Opioids : Past, Present and Future. Journal of the American Medical Association. Web. 8 Nov 2011. <http://opioids.com/pain-management/history.html.></ref> |
|||
=== Historical<!--linked from 'Patrick David Wall'--> === |
|||
In 1644, René Descartes theorized that pain was a disturbance that passed down along nerve fibers until the disturbance reached the brain,<ref name=Linton /><ref name=MelzackKatz>{{vcite book | author = Melzack R, Katz J | authorlink = | editor = Craig KD, Hadjistavropoulos T | others = | title = Pain: psychological perspectives | edition = | publisher = Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers | location = Mahwah, N.J | year = 2004 | chapter = The Gate Control Theory: Reaching for the Brain | pages = | isbn = 0-8058-4299-3 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> a development that transformed the perception of pain from a spiritual, mystical experience to a physical, mechanical sensation. Descartes's work, along with Avicenna's, prefigured the 19th-century development of specificity theory. Specificity theory saw pain as "a specific sensation, with its own sensory apparatus independent of touch and other senses".<ref name = Bonica1>{{vcite book | author = Bonica JJ | chapter = History of pain concepts and therapies | title = The management of pain | volume = 1 | edition = 2 | year = 1990 | publisher = Lea & Febiger | location = London | page = 7}}</ref> Another theory that came to prominence in the 18th and 19th centuries was intensive theory, which conceived of pain not as a unique sensory modality, but an emotional state produced by stronger than normal stimuli such as intense light, pressure or temperature.<ref name = Finger>{{vcite book | author = Finger S | title = Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function | page 149 | publisher = Oxford University Press | isbn = 0-19-514694-8 | year = 2001 | location = USA }}</ref> By the mid-1890s, specificity was backed mostly by physiologists and physicians, and the intensive theory was mostly backed by psychologists. However, after a series of clinical observations by [[Henry Head]] and experiments by [[Max von Frey]], the psychologists migrated to specificity almost en masse, and by century's end, most textbooks on physiology and psychology were presenting pain specificity as fact.<ref name = Dallenbach/><ref name = Bonica1/> |
|||
{{see also|History of pain theory}} |
|||
Before the relatively recent discovery of [[neuron]]s and their role in pain, various body functions were proposed to account for pain. There were several competing early theories of pain among the ancient Greeks: [[Hippocrates]] believed that it was due to an imbalance in [[vital fluid]]s.<ref name=Linton>Linton. Models of Pain Perception. Elsevier Health, 2005. Print.</ref> In the 11th century, [[Avicenna]] theorized that there were a number of feeling senses, including touch, pain, and titillation.<ref name = Dallenbach>{{cite journal | vauthors = Dallenbach KM |title=Pain: History and present status |journal=American Journal of Psychology |date=July 1939 |volume=52 |issue=3 |pages=331–347 |doi=10.2307/1416740|jstor=1416740 }}</ref> |
|||
[[File:Jan Baptist Weenix - Portrait of René Descartes.jpg|thumb|right|Portrait of [[René Descartes]] by [[Jan Baptist Weenix]], 1647–1649]] |
|||
In 1955, DC Sinclair and G Weddell developed "peripheral pattern theory", based on a 1934 suggestion by [[John Paul Nafe]]. They proposed that all skin fiber endings (with the exception of those innervating hair cells) are identical, and that pain is produced by intense stimulation of these fibers.<ref name = Bonica1/> Another 20th-century theory was "gate control" theory, introduced by [[Ronald Melzack]] and [[Patrick David Wall|Patrick Wall]] in the 1965 [[Science (journal)|''Science'']] article "Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory".<ref name="pmid5320816">{{vcite journal | author = [[Ronald Melzack|Melzack R]], [[Patrick David Wall|Wall PD]] | title = Pain mechanisms: a new theory | journal = Science | volume = 150 | issue = 3699 | pages = 971–9 | year = 1965 | month = November | pmid = 5320816 | doi = 10.1126/science.150.3699.971| url = http://www.hnehealth.nsw.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0012/70122/pain_mechanisms_20100315013844.pdf | issn = }}</ref> The authors proposed that both thin (pain) and large diameter (touch, pressure, vibration) nerve fibers carry information from the site of injury to two destinations in the [[posterior horn of spinal cord|dorsal horn]] of the spinal cord, and that the more large fiber activity relative to thin fiber activity at the inhibitory cell, the less pain is felt.<ref name="MelzackKatz"/> Both peripheral pattern theory and gate control theory have been superseded by more modern theories of pain. |
|||
In 1644, [[René Descartes]] theorized that pain was a disturbance that passed along nerve fibers until the disturbance reached the brain.<ref name=Linton /><ref name=MelzackKatz>{{cite book | vauthors = Melzack R, Katz J | veditors = Craig KD, Hadjistavropoulos T |title=Pain: psychological perspectives |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers |location=Mahwah, N.J |year=2004 |chapter=The Gate Control Theory: Reaching for the Brain |isbn=978-0415650618}}</ref> The work of Descartes and Avicenna prefigured the 19th-century development of ''specificity theory''. Specificity theory saw pain as "a specific sensation, with its own sensory apparatus independent of touch and other senses".<ref name = Bonica1>{{cite book | vauthors = Bonica JJ |chapter=History of pain concepts and therapies |title=The management of pain |volume=1 |edition=2 |year=1990 |publisher=Lea & Febiger |location=London |page=7 |isbn=978-0812111224}}</ref> Another theory that came to prominence in the 18th and 19th centuries was ''intensive theory'', which conceived of pain not as a unique sensory modality, but an emotional state produced by stronger than normal stimuli such as intense light, pressure or temperature.<ref name = Finger>{{cite book | vauthors = Finger S |title=Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function |page = 149 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0195146943|year=2001 |location=US}}</ref> By the mid-1890s, ''specificity'' was backed primarily by physiologists and physicians, and psychologists mostly backed the ''intensive theory''. However, after a series of clinical observations by [[Henry Head]] and experiments by [[Max von Frey]], the psychologists migrated to ''specificity'' almost en masse. By the century's end, most physiology and psychology textbooks presented pain ''specificity'' as fact.<ref name = Dallenbach/><ref name = Bonica1/> |
|||
===Modern=== |
|||
===Three dimensions of pain=== |
|||
[[File:Schematic of cortical areas involved with pain processing and fMRI.jpg|thumb|right|Regions of the cerebral cortex associated with pain]] |
|||
In 1968 Ronald Melzack and [[Kenneth L. Casey|Kenneth Casey]] described pain in terms of its three dimensions: "sensory-discriminative" (sense of the intensity, location, quality and duration of the pain), "affective-motivational" (unpleasantness and urge to escape the unpleasantness), and "cognitive-evaluative" (cognitions such as appraisal, cultural values, distraction and hypnotic suggestion).<ref name="Melzack_1968"/> They theorized that pain intensity (the sensory discriminative dimension) and unpleasantness (the affective-motivational dimension) are not simply determined by the magnitude of the painful stimulus, but “higher” cognitive activities can influence perceived intensity and unpleasantness. Cognitive activities "may affect both sensory and affective experience or they may modify primarily the affective-motivational dimension. Thus, excitement in games or war appears to block both dimensions of pain, while suggestion and placebos may modulate the affective-motivational dimension and leave the sensory-discriminative dimension relatively undisturbed." (p. 432) The paper ends with a call to action: "Pain can be treated not only by trying to cut down the sensory input by anesthetic block, surgical intervention and the like, but also by influencing the motivational-affective and cognitive factors as well." (p. 435) |
|||
Some sensory fibers do not differentiate between [[Noxious stimulus|noxious]] and non-noxious stimuli, while others (i.e., [[nociceptor]]s) respond only to noxious, high-intensity stimuli. At the peripheral end of the nociceptor, noxious stimuli generate currents that, above a given threshold, send [[action potential|signals]] along the nerve fiber to the spinal cord. The "specificity" (whether it responds to thermal, chemical, or mechanical features of its environment) of a nociceptor is determined by which [[ion channel]]s it expresses at its peripheral end. So far, dozens of types of nociceptor ion channels have been identified, and their exact functions are still being determined.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Woolf CJ, Ma Q | title = Nociceptors{{snd}}noxious stimulus detectors | journal = Neuron | volume = 55 | issue = 3 | pages = 353–364 | date = August 2007 | pmid = 17678850 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.016 | s2cid = 13576368 | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
The pain signal travels from the periphery to the spinal cord along [[A delta fiber|A-delta]] and [[C fiber|C]] fibers. Because the A-delta fiber is thicker than the C fiber, and is thinly sheathed in an electrically insulating material ([[myelin]]), it carries its signal faster (5–30 [[Metre per second|m/s]]) than the unmyelinated C fiber (0.5–2 m/s).<ref name="Marchand">{{cite book | vauthors = Marchand S | veditors = Beaulieu P, Lussier D, Porreca F, Dickenson A |title=Pharmacology of pain |chapter=Applied pain neurophysiology |publisher=International Association for the Study of Pain Press |location=Seattle |year=2010 |pages=3–26 |isbn=978-0931092787}}</ref> Pain evoked by the A-delta fibers is described as sharp and is felt first. This is followed by a duller pain—often described as burning—carried by the C fibers.<ref name="Skevington9">{{cite book |vauthors=Skevington S |title=Psychology of pain |publisher=Wiley |location=New York |year=1995 |page=[https://archive.org/details/psychologyofpain0000skev/page/9 9] |isbn=978-0471957737 |url=https://archive.org/details/psychologyofpain0000skev/page/9 }}</ref> These A-delta and C fibers enter the spinal cord via [[Lissauer's tract]] and connect with spinal cord nerve fibers in the [[central gelatinous substance of the spinal cord]]. These spinal cord fibers then cross the cord via the [[anterior white commissure]] and ascend in the [[spinothalamic tract]]. Before reaching the brain, the spinothalamic tract splits into the [[lateral (anatomy)|lateral]], [[neospinothalamic tract]] and the [[medial (anatomy)|medial]], [[paleospinothalamic tract]]. The neospinothalamic tract carries the fast, sharp A-delta signal to the ventral posterolateral nucleus of the [[thalamus]]. The paleospinothalamic tract carries the slow, dull C fiber pain signal. Some of the paleospinothalamic fibers peel off in the brain stem—connecting with the reticular formation or midbrain periaqueductal gray—and the remainder terminate in the intralaminar nuclei of the thalamus.<ref name=Skevington1995>{{cite book |vauthors=Skevington SM |year=1995 |title=Psychology of pain |location=Chichester, UK |publisher=Wiley |page=[https://archive.org/details/psychologyofpain0000skev/page/18 18] |isbn=978-0471957737 |url=https://archive.org/details/psychologyofpain0000skev/page/18 }}</ref> |
|||
===Theory today=== |
|||
[[File:Schematic of cortical areas involved with pain processing and fMRI.jpg|thumb|right|Regions of the cerebral cortex associated with pain.]]Wilhelm Erb's (1874) "intensive" theory, that a pain signal can be generated by intense enough stimulation of ''any'' sensory receptor, has been soundly disproved. Some sensory fibers do not differentiate between noxious and non-noxious stimuli, while others, [[nociceptors]], respond only to noxious, high intensity stimuli. At the peripheral end of the nociceptor, noxious stimuli are [[Transduction|transduced]] into currents that, above a given threshold, begin to generate [[action potential]]s that travel along the nerve fiber to the spinal cord. The "specificity" (whether it responds to thermal, chemical or mechanical features of its environment) of a nociceptor is determined by which [[ion channel]]s it expresses at its peripheral end. Dozens of nociceptor [[Transient receptor potential channel|transient receptor potential]], [[Acid-sensing ion channel|acid sensing]], [[Potassium channel|potassium]] and [[Ligand-gated ion channel|ligand-gated]] ion channels have so far been identified, and their exact functions are still being determined.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Woolf CJ, Ma Q |title=Nociceptors--noxious stimulus detectors |journal=Neuron |volume=55 |issue=3 |pages=353–64 |year=2007 |month=August |pmid=17678850 |doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.016 |url=}}</ref> |
|||
Pain-related activity in the thalamus spreads to the [[insular cortex]] (thought to embody, among other things, the feeling that distinguishes pain from other [[homeostatic emotion]]s such as itch and nausea) and [[anterior cingulate cortex]] (thought to embody, among other things, the affective/motivational element, the unpleasantness of pain),<ref name= Craig2003a>{{cite journal | vauthors = Craig AD | title = Pain mechanisms: labeled lines versus convergence in central processing | journal = Annual Review of Neuroscience | volume = 26 | pages = 1–30 | year = 2003 | pmid = 12651967 | doi = 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131022 | s2cid = 12387848 }}</ref> and pain that is distinctly located also activates the [[primary somatosensory cortex|primary]] and [[secondary somatosensory cortex]].<ref name="Romanelli P, Esposito V.">{{cite journal | vauthors = Romanelli P, Esposito V | title = The functional anatomy of neuropathic pain | journal = Neurosurgery Clinics of North America | volume = 15 | issue = 3 | pages = 257–268 | date = July 2004 | pmid = 15246335 | doi = 10.1016/j.nec.2004.02.010 }}</ref> |
|||
The pain signal travels from the periphery to the spinal cord along an [[A delta fiber|A-delta]] or [[C fiber|C]] fiber. Because the A-delta fiber is thicker than the C fiber, and is thinly sheathed in an electrically insulating material ([[myelin]]), it carries its signal faster (5–30 [[m/s]]) than the unmyelinated C fiber (0.5–2 m/s).<ref name=Marchand>{{vcite book | author = Marchand S | authorlink = | editor = Beaulieu P, Lussier D, Porreca F & Dickenson A| others = | title = Pharmacology of pain | chapter = Applied pain neurophysiology | publisher = International Association for the Study of Pain Press | location = Seattle | year = 2010 | pages = 3–26 | isbn = 978-0-931092-78-7 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> Pain evoked by the (faster) A-delta fibers is described as sharp and is felt first. This is followed by a duller pain, often described as burning, carried by the C fibers.<ref name=Skevington9>{{vcite book | author = Skevington S | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = Psychology of pain | edition = | publisher = Wiley | location = New York | year = 1995 | page = 9 | isbn = 0-471-95771-2 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> These first order neurons enter the spinal cord via [[Lissauer's tract]]. |
|||
Spinal cord fibers dedicated to carrying A-delta fiber pain signals and others that carry both A-delta and C fiber pain signals to the [[thalamus]] have been identified. Other spinal cord fibers, known as [[wide dynamic range neuron]]s, respond to A-delta and C fibers and the much larger, more heavily myelinated A-beta fibers that carry touch, pressure, and vibration signals.<ref name=Marchand/> |
|||
A-delta and C fibers synapse on second order neurons in [[Central gelatinous substance of spinal cord |substantia gelatinosa]] (laminae II and III of the dorsal horns). These second order neurons ([[spinothalamic tract]]) then [[decussation|decussate]], crossing via the [[anterior white commissure]] before ascending contralaterally. Before reaching the brain, the spinothalamic tract splits into the lateral [[neospinothalamic tract]] and the medial [[paleospinothalamic tract]].<ref name=Skevington1995>{{cite book |last=Skevington |first=S. M. |year=1995 |title=Psychology of pain |location=Chichester, UK |publisher=Wiley |page=18 |isbn=0-471-95771-2 }}</ref> |
|||
[[Ronald Melzack]] and [[Patrick David Wall|Patrick Wall]] introduced their [[gate control theory]] in the 1965 [[Science (journal)|''Science'']] article "Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory".<ref name="pmid5320816">{{cite journal | vauthors = Melzack R, Wall PD | title = Pain mechanisms: a new theory | journal = Science | volume = 150 | issue = 3699 | pages = 971–979 | date = November 1965 | pmid = 5320816 | doi = 10.1126/science.150.3699.971 | url = https://www.hnehealth.nsw.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0012/70122/pain_mechanisms_20100315013844.pdf | author-link = Ronald Melzack | url-status = dead | bibcode = 1965Sci...150..971M | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20120114141747/https://www.hnehealth.nsw.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0012/70122/pain_mechanisms_20100315013844.pdf | archive-date = 14 January 2012 | author-link2 = Patrick David Wall | accessdate = 7 March 2022 }}</ref> The authors proposed that the thin C and A-delta (pain) and large diameter A-beta (touch, pressure, vibration) nerve fibers carry information from the site of injury to two destinations in the [[posterior horn of spinal cord|dorsal horn]] of the spinal cord, and that A-beta fiber signals acting on inhibitory cells in the dorsal horn can reduce the intensity of pain signals sent to the brain.<ref name="MelzackKatz"/> |
|||
Second order neospinothalamic tract neurons carry information from A-delta fibers and terminate at the [[ventral posterolateral nucleus]] of the [[thalamus]], where they synapse on third order neurons (dendrites of the [[somatosensory cortex]]). Paleospinothalamic neurons carry information from C fibers and terminate throughout the [[brain stem]], a tenth of them in the [[thalamus]] and the rest in the [[medulla oblongata|medulla]], [[pons]] and [[periaqueductal grey]] matter.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://webcache.googleusercontent.com/search?q=cache:Zqzzi81MgKQJ:joson.rey.tripod.com/skinsofttissues/painpathway.rtf+slow+pain+Paleospinothalamic+aching,+throbbing+or+burning+pain&cd=8&hl=en&ct=clnk&gl=us&client=firefox-a |title=Pain Pathway |publisher=Webcache.googleusercontent.com |date= |accessdate=24 July 2010}}</ref> |
|||
====Three dimensions of pain==== |
|||
Spinal cord fibers dedicated to carrying A-delta fiber pain signals, and others that carry both A-delta and C fiber pain signals up the spinal cord to the [[thalamus]] in the brain have been identified. Other spinal cord fibers, known as [[wide dynamic range neuron]]s, respond to A-delta and C fibers, but also to the large A-beta fibers that carry touch, pressure and vibration signals.<ref name=Marchand/> Pain-related activity in the thalamus spreads to the [[insular cortex]] (thought to embody, among other things, the feeling that distinguishes pain from other [[homeostatic emotion]]s such as itch and nausea) and [[anterior cingulate cortex]] (thought to embody, among other things, the motivational element of pain);<ref name= Craig2003a>{{vcite journal | author = Craig AD | title = Pain mechanisms: labeled lines versus convergence in central processing | journal = Annu. Rev. Neurosci. | volume = 26 | issue = | pages = 1–30 | year = 2003 | pmid = 12651967 | doi = 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131022 | url = | issn = }}</ref> and pain that is distinctly located also activates the [[primary somatosensory cortex|primary]] and [[secondary somatosensory cortex|secondary]] somatosensory cortices.<ref name="Romanelli P, Esposito V.">{{vcite journal | author = Romanelli P, Esposito V | title = The functional anatomy of neuropathic pain | journal = Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am. | volume = 15 | issue = 3 | pages = 257–68 | year = 2004 | month = July | pmid = 15246335 | doi = 10.1016/j.nec.2004.02.010 | url = | issn = }}</ref> Melzack and Casey's 1968 picture of the dimensions of pain is as influential today as ever, firmly framing theory and guiding research in the [[Brain#Functional subsystems|functional neuroanatomy]] and psychology of pain. |
|||
In 1968, [[Ronald Melzack]] and [[Kenneth L. Casey|Kenneth Casey]] described chronic pain in terms of its three dimensions: |
|||
* "sensory-discriminative" (sense of the intensity, location, quality, and duration of the pain), |
|||
* "affective-motivational" (unpleasantness and urge to escape the unpleasantness) and |
|||
* "cognitive-evaluative" (cognitions such as appraisal, cultural values, distraction, and hypnotic suggestion). |
|||
They theorized that pain intensity (the sensory discriminative dimension) and unpleasantness (the affective-motivational dimension) are not simply determined by the magnitude of the painful stimulus, but "higher" cognitive activities can influence perceived intensity and unpleasantness. Cognitive activities may affect both sensory and affective experience, or they may modify primarily the affective-motivational dimension. Thus, excitement in games or war appears to block both the sensory-discriminative and affective-motivational dimensions of pain, while suggestion and placebos may modulate only the affective-motivational dimension and leave the sensory-discriminative dimension relatively undisturbed.<ref name=M&C>{{cite book|last1=Melzack|first1=Ronald|author-link1=Ronald Melzack|last2=Casey|first2=Kenneth|author-link2=Kenneth L. Casey|editor-last=Kenshalo|editor-first=Dan|year=1968|chapter=Sensory, Motivational, and Central Control Determinants of Pain|title=The Skin Senses|publisher=Charles C Thomas|publication-place=Springfield, Illinois|url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/233801589}}</ref> (p. 432) |
|||
A. D. (Bud) Craig and [[Derek Denton]] include pain in a class of feelings they name, respectively, "[[Human homeostasis|homeostatic]]" or "primordial" emotions. These are feelings such as hunger, thirst and fatigue, evoked by internal body states, communicated to the central nervous system by [[interoceptors]], which motivate behavior aimed at maintaining the internal milieu at its ideal state. Craig and Denton distinguish these feelings from the "classical emotions" such as love, fear and anger, which are elicited by environmental stimuli sensed through the nose, eyes and ears.<ref name="Denton2006p10">{{vcite book |author=Derek A. Denton |title=The primordial emotions: the dawning of consciousness |date=8 June 2006 |page=10 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-920314-7}}</ref><ref name=Craig2003b>{{vcite journal |author = Craig AD (Bud) | year = 2003 | title = Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body | journal=Current Opinion in Neurobiology | volume=13 |pages=500–505 |pmid=12965300 |doi=10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4 |url= http://www.jsmf.org/meetings/2007/oct-nov/CONB%20Craig%202003.pdf |issue=4}}</ref> |
|||
The paper ends with a call to action: "Pain can be treated not only by trying to cut down the sensory input by anesthetic block, surgical intervention and the like, but also by influencing the motivational-affective and cognitive factors as well."<ref name=M&C/> (p. 435) |
|||
===Evolutionary and behavioral role=== |
|||
Pain is part of the body's defense system, producing a [[Nervous system#Reflexes and other stimulus-response circuits|reflexive]] retraction from the painful stimulus, and tendencies to protect the affected body part while it heals, and avoid that harmful situation in the future.<ref name= Lynn1984/><ref name= Bernston2008>{{vcite book | author = Bernston GG, Cacioppo JT | authorlink = | editor = Gardner WL, Shah JY | others = | title = Handbook of Motivation Science | edition = | publisher = The Guilford Press | location = New York | year = 2007 | page = 191 | isbn = 1-59385-568-0 | oclc = | chapter = The neuroevolution of motivation | chapterurl = http://books.google.com.au/books?id=iCxpZkZtDG8C&pg=PT209&dq=%22One+general+class+of+spinal+reflexes+consists+of+the+flexor+(pain)+withdrawal%22&cd=1#v=onepage&q=%22One%20general%20class%20of%20spinal%20reflexes%20consists%20of%20the%20flexor%20(pain)%20withdrawal%22&f=true | accessdate = }}</ref> It is an important part of animal life, vital to healthy survival. People with [[congenital insensitivity to pain]] have reduced [[life expectancy]].<ref name="pmid12583863"/> |
|||
==Evolutionary and behavioral role== |
|||
In his book, ''[[The Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution]]'', biologist [[Richard Dawkins]] grapples with the question of why pain has to be so very painful. He describes the alternative as a simple, mental raising of a "red flag". To argue why that red flag might be insufficient, Dawkins explains that drives must compete with each other within living beings. The most fit creature would be the one whose pains are well balanced. Those pains which mean certain death when ignored will become the most powerfully felt. The relative intensities of pain, then, may resemble the relative importance of that risk to our ancestors (lack of food, too much cold, or serious injuries are felt as agony, whereas minor damage is felt as mere discomfort). This resemblance will not be perfect, however, because natural selection can be a [[Argument from poor design|poor designer]]. The result is often glitches in animals, including [[supernormal stimuli]]. Such glitches help explain pains which are not, or at least no longer directly adaptive (e.g. perhaps some forms of [[toothache]], or injury to fingernails).<ref name=Dawkins2009>{{vcite book | author = Dawkins, Richard |year=2009 | title = The Greatest Show on Earth |publisher = Free Press | Pages = 392-395}}</ref> |
|||
Pain is part of the body's defense system, producing a [[Nervous system#Reflexes and other stimulus-response circuits|reflexive]] retraction from the painful stimulus, and tendencies to protect the affected body part while it heals, and avoid that harmful situation in the future.<ref name="Lynn1984">{{cite book|title=The neurobiology of pain: Symposium of the Northern Neurobiology Group, held at Leeds on 18 April 1983|vauthors=Lynn B|publisher=Manchester University Press|year=1984|isbn=978-0719009969|veditors=Winlow W, Holden AV|location=Manchester|page=106|chapter=Cutaneous nociceptors|chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=S7rnAAAAIAAJ&pg=PA106|access-date=3 February 2016|archive-date=30 March 2021|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210330014628/https://books.google.com/books?id=S7rnAAAAIAAJ&pg=PA106|url-status=live}}</ref><ref name= Bernston2008>{{cite book |vauthors=Bernston GG, Cacioppo JT |veditors=Gardner WL, Shah JY |title=Handbook of Motivation Science |publisher=The Guilford Press |location=New York |year=2007 |page=191 |isbn=978-1593855680 |chapter=The neuroevolution of motivation |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=iCxpZkZtDG8C&q=%22One+general+class+of+spinal+reflexes+consists+of+the+flexor+(pain)+withdrawal%22&pg=PT209 |access-date=18 November 2020 |archive-date=30 March 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210330014656/https://books.google.com/books?id=iCxpZkZtDG8C&q=%22One+general+class+of+spinal+reflexes+consists+of+the+flexor+%28pain%29+withdrawal%22&pg=PT209 |url-status=live }}</ref> It is an important part of animal life, vital to healthy survival. People with [[congenital insensitivity to pain]] have reduced [[life expectancy]].<ref name="pmid12583863"/> |
|||
In ''[[The Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution]]'', biologist [[Richard Dawkins]] addresses the question of why pain should have the quality of being painful. He describes the alternative as a mental raising of a "red flag". To argue why that red flag might be insufficient, Dawkins argues that drives must compete with one another within living beings. The most "fit" creature would be the one whose pains are well balanced. Those pains which mean certain death when ignored will become the most powerfully felt. The relative intensities of pain, then, may resemble the relative importance of that risk to our ancestors.{{efn|For example, lack of food, extreme cold, or serious injuries are felt as exceptionally painful, whereas minor damage is felt as mere discomfort.}} This resemblance will not be perfect, however, because natural selection can be a [[Argument from poor design|poor designer]]. This may have maladaptive results such as [[Supernormal stimulus|supernormal stimuli]].<ref name=Dawkins2009>{{cite book | vauthors = Dawkins R |year=2009 |title=The Greatest Show on Earth |url=https://archive.org/details/greatestshowonea00dawk |url-access=registration |publisher=Free Press |pages=[https://archive.org/details/greatestshowonea00dawk/page/392 392–395]|isbn=978-1416594789 }}</ref> |
|||
[[Idiopathic]] pain (pain that persists after the trauma or pathology has healed, or that arises without any apparent cause), may be an exception to the idea that pain is helpful to survival, although some [[psychodynamic]] psychologists argue that such pain is psychogenic, enlisted as a protective distraction to keep dangerous emotions unconscious.<ref name="Sarno-DividedMind">{{vcite book | author = Sarno JE | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = The divided mind: the epidemic of mindbody disorders | edition = | publisher = ReganBooks | location = New York | year = 2006 | pages = | isbn = 0-06-085178-3 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> |
|||
Pain, however, does not only wave a "red flag" within living beings but may also act as a warning sign and a call for help to other living beings. Especially in humans who readily helped each other in case of sickness or injury throughout their evolutionary history, pain might be shaped by natural selection to be a credible and convincing signal of the need for relief, help, and care.<ref name = Steinkopf>{{cite journal |vauthors=Steinkopf L | date = June 2016 | title = An Evolutionary Perspective on Pain Communication | journal = Evolutionary Psychology | volume = 14 | issue = 2| page = 100 | doi = 10.1177/1474704916653964 | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
===Thresholds=== |
|||
In pain science, thresholds are measured by gradually increasing the intensity of a stimulus such as electric current or heat applied to the body. The [[Threshold of pain|pain perception threshold]] is the point at which the stimulus begins to hurt, and the [[pain tolerance|pain tolerance threshold]] is reached when the subject acts to stop the pain. |
|||
Idiopathic pain (pain that persists after the trauma or pathology has healed, or that arises without any apparent cause) may be an exception to the idea that pain is helpful to survival, although some [[psychodynamic]] psychologists argue that such pain is psychogenic, enlisted as a protective distraction to keep dangerous emotions unconscious.<ref name="Sarno-DividedMind">{{cite book | vauthors = Sarno JE |title=The divided mind: the epidemic of mindbody disorders | url = https://archive.org/details/dividedmindep00sarn | url-access = registration |publisher=ReganBooks |location=New York |year=2006 |isbn=978-0061174308}}</ref> |
|||
Differences in pain perception and tolerance thresholds are associated with, among other factors, ethnicity, genetics, and sex. People of Mediterranean origin report as painful some radiant heat intensities that northern Europeans describe as nonpainful, and Italian women tolerate less intense electric shock than Jewish or Native American women. Some individuals in all cultures have significantly higher than normal pain perception and tolerance thresholds. For instance, patients who experience painless heart attacks have higher pain thresholds for electric shock, muscle cramp and heat.<ref name=Melzack_Wall_1996>{{vcite book | author = [[Ronald Melzack|Melzack R]], [[Patrick David Wall|Wall PD]] | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = The challenge of pain | edition = 2nd | publisher = Penguin Books | location = New York | year = 1996 | pages = 17–19 | isbn = 0-14-025670-9 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> Women have lower pain perception and tolerance thresholds than men, and this sex difference appears to apply to all ages, including newborn infants.<ref name="pmid10692611">{{vcite journal | author = Guinsburg R, de Araújo Peres C, Branco de Almeida MF, de Cássia Xavier Balda R, Cássia Berenguel R, Tonelotto J, Kopelman BI | title = Differences in pain expression between male and female newborn infants | journal = Pain | volume = 85 | issue = 1-2 | pages = 127–33 | year = 2000 | month = March | pmid = 10692611 | doi = 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00258-4 | url = | issn = }}</ref> |
|||
==Thresholds== |
|||
In pain science, thresholds are measured by gradually increasing the intensity of a stimulus in a procedure called [[quantitative sensory testing]] which involves such stimuli as [[electric current]], thermal (heat or cold), mechanical (pressure, touch, vibration), [[ischemia|ischemic]], or chemical stimuli applied to the subject to evoke a response.<ref name="Fillingim">{{cite journal | vauthors = Fillingim RB, Loeser JD, Baron R, Edwards RR | title = Assessment of Chronic Pain: Domains, Methods, and Mechanisms | journal = The Journal of Pain | volume = 17 | issue = 9 Suppl | pages = T10–20 | date = September 2016 | pmid = 27586827 | pmc = 5010652 | doi = 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.08.010 }}</ref> The "[[Threshold of pain|pain perception threshold]]" is the point at which the subject begins to feel pain, and the "pain threshold intensity" is the stimulus intensity at which the stimulus begins to hurt. The "[[pain tolerance]] threshold" is reached when the subject acts to stop the pain.<ref name=Fillingim/> |
|||
==Assessment== <!-- linked from [[Pain in babies]]--> |
==Assessment== <!-- linked from [[Pain in babies]]--> |
||
{{See also|Pain scales|Pain ladder}} |
{{See also|Pain assessment|Pain scales|Pain ladder}} |
||
A person's self |
A person's self-report is the most reliable measure of pain.<ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Amico D |title=Health & physical assessment in nursing |publisher=Pearson |location=Boston |year=2016 |page=173 |isbn=978-0133876406}}</ref><ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Taylor C |title= Fundamentals of nursing : the art and science of person-centered nursing care |publisher=Wolters Kluwer Health |location=Philadelphia |year=2015 |isbn=978-1451185614 |page=241}}</ref><ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Venes D |title=Taber's cyclopedic medical dictionary |publisher=F.A. Davis |location=Philadelphia |year=2013 |isbn=978-0803629776 |page=1716}}</ref> Some health care professionals may underestimate pain severity.<ref name= Prkachin2007>{{cite journal | vauthors = Prkachin KM, Solomon PE, Ross J | title = Underestimation of pain by health-care providers: towards a model of the process of inferring pain in others | journal = The Canadian Journal of Nursing Research | volume = 39 | issue = 2 | pages = 88–106 | date = June 2007 | pmid = 17679587 }}</ref> A definition of pain widely employed in nursing, emphasizing its subjective nature and the importance of believing patient reports, was introduced by [[Margo McCaffery]] in 1968: "Pain is whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever he says it does".<ref>McCaffery M. (1968). ''Nursing practice theories related to cognition, bodily pain, and man-environment interactions''. Los Angeles: UCLA Students Store.<br />More recently, McCaffery defined pain as "whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever the experiencing person says it does." {{cite book | vauthors = Pasero C, McCaffery M |title=Pain: clinical manual |publisher=Mosby |location=St. Louis |year=1999 |isbn=978-0815156093}}</ref> To assess intensity, the patient may be asked to locate their pain on a scale of 0 to 10, with 0 being no pain at all, and 10 the worst pain they have ever felt. Quality can be established by having the patient complete the [[McGill Pain Questionnaire]] indicating which words best describe their pain.<ref name= Breivik2008/> |
||
===Visual analogue scale=== |
|||
{{Main|Visual analogue scale}} |
|||
The visual analogue scale is a common, reproducible tool in the assessment of pain and pain relief.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Kelly AM | title = The minimum clinically significant difference in visual analogue scale pain score does not differ with severity of pain | journal = Emergency Medicine Journal | volume = 18 | issue = 3 | pages = 205–207 | date = May 2001 | pmid = 11354213 | pmc = 1725574 | doi = 10.1136/emj.18.3.205 | df = dmy-all }}</ref> The scale is a continuous line anchored by verbal descriptors, one for each extreme of pain where a higher score indicates greater pain intensity. It is usually 10 cm in length with no intermediate descriptors as to avoid marking of scores around a preferred numeric value. When applied as a pain descriptor, these anchors are often 'no pain' and 'worst imaginable pain". Cut-offs for pain classification have been recommended as no pain (0–4mm), mild pain (5–44mm), moderate pain (45–74mm) and severe pain (75–100mm).<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Hawker GA, Mian S, Kendzerska T, French M | title = Measures of adult pain: Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS Pain), Numeric Rating Scale for Pain (NRS Pain), McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (SF-MPQ), Chronic Pain Grade Scale (CPGS), Short Form-36 Bodily Pain Scale (SF-36 BPS), and Measure of Intermittent and Constant Osteoarthritis Pain (ICOAP) | journal = Arthritis Care & Research | volume = 63 | issue = S11 | pages = S240–252 | date = November 2011 | pmid = 22588748 | doi = 10.1002/acr.20543 }}</ref>{{check quotation}} |
|||
===Multidimensional pain inventory=== |
===Multidimensional pain inventory=== |
||
The Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI) is a questionnaire designed to assess the [[psychosocial]] state of a person with chronic pain. |
The Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI) is a questionnaire designed to assess the [[psychosocial]] state of a person with chronic pain. Combining the MPI characterization of the person with their [[#Classification|IASP five-category pain profile]] is recommended for deriving the most useful case description.<ref name = Turk&Okifuji2001/> |
||
===Assessment in non-verbal people=== |
|||
===In nonverbal patients=== |
|||
{{See also|Pain and dementia|Pain in babies}} |
{{See also|Pain and dementia|Pain in babies}} |
||
[[Nonverbal communication|Non-verbal]] people cannot use words to tell others that they are experiencing pain. However, they may be able to communicate through other means, such as blinking, pointing, or nodding.<ref name=":1">{{Cite book|title=Medical-surgical nursing: Assessment and management of clinical problems| vauthors = Lewis SM, Bucher L, Heitkemper MM, Harding M |publisher= Elsevier |year=2017 |isbn=978-0323328524 |edition=10th|location=St. Louis, Missouri|pages=126|oclc=944472408}}</ref> |
|||
When a person is [[Nonverbal communication|non-verbal]] and cannot self report pain, observation becomes critical, and specific behaviors can be monitored as pain indicators. Behaviors such as facial grimacing and guarding indicate pain, as well as an increase or decrease in vocalizations, changes in routine behavior patterns and mental status changes. Patients experiencing pain may exhibit withdrawn [[social behavior]] and possibly experience a [[Anorexia (symptom)|decreased appetite]] and decreased nutritional intake. A change in condition that deviates from baseline such as moaning with movement or when manipulating a body part, and limited [[range of motion]] are also potential pain indicators. In patients who possess language but are incapable of expressing themselves effectively, such as those with dementia, an increase in confusion or display of aggressive behaviors or agitation, may signal that discomfort exists, and further assessment is necessary. |
|||
With a non-communicative person, observation becomes critical, and specific behaviors can be monitored as pain indicators. Behaviors such as facial grimacing and guarding (trying to protect part of the body from being bumped or touched) indicate pain, as well as an increase or decrease in vocalizations, changes in routine behavior patterns and mental status changes. Patients experiencing pain may exhibit withdrawn [[social behavior]] and possibly experience a [[Anorexia (symptom)|decreased appetite]] and decreased nutritional intake. A change in condition that deviates from baseline, such as moaning with movement or when manipulating a body part, and limited [[range of motion]] are also potential pain indicators. In patients who possess language but are incapable of expressing themselves effectively, such as those with dementia, an increase in confusion or display of aggressive behaviors or agitation may signal that discomfort exists, and further assessment is necessary. Changes in behavior may be noticed by caregivers who are familiar with the person's normal behavior.<ref name=":1" /> |
|||
[[Pain in babies|Infants feel pain]] but they lack the language needed to report it, so communicate distress by crying. A non-verbal pain assessment should be conducted involving the parents, who will notice changes in the infant not obvious to the health care provider. Pre-term babies are more sensitive to painful stimuli than full term babies.<ref name= Jarvis2004>{{vcite book | author = Jarvis C | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = Physical examination & health assessment | edition = | publisher = Elsevier Saunders | location = St. Louis, Mo | year = 2007 | pages = 180–192 | isbn = 1-4160-3243-6 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> |
|||
[[Pain in babies|Infants do feel pain]], but lack the language needed to report it, and so communicate distress by crying. A non-verbal pain assessment should be conducted involving the parents, who will notice changes in the infant which may not be obvious to the health care provider. [[Preterm birth|Pre-term babies]] are more sensitive to painful stimuli than those carried to full term.<ref name= Jarvis2004>{{cite book | vauthors = Jarvis C |title=Physical examination & health assessment |publisher=Elsevier Saunders |location=St. Louis, Mo |year=2007 |pages=180–192 |isbn=978-1455728107}}</ref> |
|||
===Other barriers to reporting=== |
|||
An aging adult may not respond to pain in the way that a younger person would. Their ability to recognize pain may be blunted by illness or the use of multiple [[prescription drug]]s. Depression may also keep the older adult from reporting they are in pain. The older adult may also quit doing activities they love because it hurts too much. Decline in self-care activities (dressing, grooming, walking, etc.) may also be indicators that the older adult is experiencing pain. The older adult may refrain from reporting pain because they are afraid they will have to have surgery or will be put on a drug they become addicted to. They may not want others to see them as weak, or may feel there is something impolite or shameful in complaining about pain, or they may feel the pain is deserved punishment for past transgressions.<ref>lawhorne, L; Passerini, J (1999). ''Chronic Pain Management in the Long Term Care Setting: Clinical Practice Guidelines.'' Baltimore, Maryland: American Medical Directors Association. pp. 1–27.</ref> |
|||
Another approach, when pain is suspected, is to give the person treatment for pain, and then watch to see whether the suspected indicators of pain subside.<ref name=":1" /> |
|||
Cultural barriers can also keep a person from telling someone they are in pain. Religious beliefs may prevent the individual from seeking help. They may feel certain pain treatment is against their religion. They may not report pain because they feel it is a sign that death is near. Many people fear the stigma of addiction and avoid pain treatment so as not to be prescribed addicting drugs. Many Asians do not want to lose respect in society by admitting they are in pain and need help, believing the pain should be borne in silence, while other cultures (e.g. Jewish) feel they should report pain right away and get immediate relief.<ref name= Jarvis2004/> Gender can also be a factor in reporting pain. [[Gender differences]] are usually the result of social and cultural expectations, with women expected to be emotional and show pain and men stoic, keeping pain to themselves.<ref name= Jarvis2004/> |
|||
===Other reporting barriers=== |
|||
The way in which one experiences and responds to pain is related to sociocultural characteristics, such as gender, ethnicity, and age.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Encandela JA | title = Social science and the study of pain since Zborowski: a need for a new agenda | journal = Social Science & Medicine | volume = 36 | issue = 6 | pages = 783–791 | date = March 1993 | pmid = 8480223 | doi = 10.1016/0277-9536(93)90039-7 }}</ref><ref>Zborowski M. ''People in Pain''. 1969, San Francisco, CA:Josey-Bass{{ISBN?}}{{page needed|date=January 2023}}</ref> An aging adult may not respond to pain in the same way that a younger person might. Their ability to recognize pain may be blunted by illness or the use of [[prescription drug|medication]]. Depression may also keep older adult from reporting they are in pain. Decline in [[self-care]] may also indicate the older adult is experiencing pain. They may be reluctant to report pain because they do not want to be perceived as weak, or may feel it is impolite or shameful to complain, or they may feel the pain is a form of deserved punishment.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Encandela JA |year=1997 |title=Social Construction of pain and aging: Individual artfulness within interpretive structures |journal= Symbolic Interaction|volume=20 |issue=3 |pages=251–273 |doi=10.1525/si.1997.20.3.251}}</ref><ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Lawhorne L, Passerini J | date = 1999 | title = Chronic Pain Management in the Long Term Care Setting: Clinical Practice Guidelines. | location = Baltimore, Maryland | publisher = American Medical Directors Association | pages = 1–27 }}</ref> |
|||
Cultural barriers may also affect the likelihood of reporting pain. Patients may feel that certain treatments go against their religious beliefs. They may not report pain because they feel it is a sign that death is near. Many people fear the stigma of addiction, and avoid pain treatment so as not to be prescribed potentially addicting drugs. Many Asians do not want to lose respect in society by admitting they are in pain and need help, believing the pain should be borne in silence, while other cultures feel they should report pain immediately to receive immediate relief.<ref name= Jarvis2004/> |
|||
Gender can also be a perceived factor in reporting pain. [[Gender differences]] can be the result of social and cultural expectations, with, in some cultures, women expected to be more emotional and show pain, and men to be more stoic.<ref name= Jarvis2004/> As a result, female pain may be at a higher risk of being stigmatized, leading to less urgent treatment of women based on social expectations of their ability to accurately report it.<ref>{{cite news | vauthors = Epstein R |date=19 March 2018 |title=When Doctors Don't Listen to Women |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2018/03/19/books/review/abby-norman-ask-me-about-my-uterus.html |work=The New York Times |access-date=20 July 2019 |archive-date=9 May 2019 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190509110006/https://www.nytimes.com/2018/03/19/books/review/abby-norman-ask-me-about-my-uterus.html |url-status=live }}</ref> This has been postulated to lead to extended emergency room wait times for women and frequent dismissal of their ability to accurately report pain.<ref>{{cite news | vauthors = Fasslet J |date=15 October 2015 |title=How Doctors Take Women's Pain Less Seriously |url=https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2015/10/emergency-room-wait-times-sexism/410515/ |work=The Atlantic |access-date=20 July 2019 |archive-date=17 July 2019 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20190717161732/https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2015/10/emergency-room-wait-times-sexism/410515/ |url-status=live }}</ref><ref>{{cite news |author=<!--Staff writer(s); no by-line.--> |title=Stories of Misunderstanding Women's Pain |url=https://www.theatlantic.com/notes/all/2015/10/stories-of-misunderstood-womens-pain/411793/ |work=The Atlantic |date=15 March 2016 |access-date=20 July 2019 |archive-date=15 April 2019 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190415114017/https://www.theatlantic.com/notes/all/2015/10/stories-of-misunderstood-womens-pain/411793/ |url-status=live }}</ref> |
|||
===Diagnostic aid=== |
|||
Pain is a symptom of many medical conditions. Knowing the time of onset, location, intensity, pattern of occurrence (continuous, intermittent, etc.), exacerbating and relieving factors, and quality (burning, sharp, etc.) of the pain will help the examining physician to accurately diagnose the problem. For example, chest pain described as extreme heaviness may indicate [[myocardial infarction]], while chest pain described as tearing may indicate [[aortic dissection]].<ref name="pmid9786377">{{cite journal | vauthors = Panju AA, Hemmelgarn BR, Guyatt GH, Simel DL | title = The rational clinical examination. Is this patient having a myocardial infarction? | journal = JAMA | volume = 280 | issue = 14 | pages = 1256–1263 | date = October 1998 | pmid = 9786377 | doi = 10.1001/jama.280.14.1256 }}</ref><ref name="pmid1020750">{{cite journal | vauthors = Slater EE, DeSanctis RW | title = The clinical recognition of dissecting aortic aneurysm | journal = The American Journal of Medicine | volume = 60 | issue = 5 | pages = 625–633 | date = May 1976 | pmid = 1020750 | doi = 10.1016/0002-9343(76)90496-4 }}</ref> |
|||
===Physiological measurement=== |
|||
[[Functional magnetic resonance imaging]] brain scanning has been used to measure pain, and correlates well with self-reported pain.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Brown JE, Chatterjee N, Younger J, Mackey S | title = Towards a physiology-based measure of pain: patterns of human brain activity distinguish painful from non-painful thermal stimulation | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 6 | issue = 9 | pages = e24124 | date = September 2011 | pmid = 21931652 | pmc = 3172232 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0024124 | bibcode = 2011PLoSO...624124B | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite web |url=https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/234450.php |title=Tool That Measures Pain Objectively Under Way |work=Medical News Today |date=15 September 2011 | vauthors = Paddock C |access-date=25 September 2017 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170925230621/https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/234450.php |archive-date=25 September 2017 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite news |quote=Editorial|url=https://www.reuters.com/article/us-pain-diagnostic-idUSTRE78C81920110913 |title=Feeling pain? The computer can tell |date=13 September 2011 |newspaper=Reuters |access-date=25 September 2017 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150617221847/https://www.reuters.com/article/2011/09/13/us-pain-diagnostic-idUSTRE78C81920110913 |archive-date=17 June 2015 }}</ref> |
|||
==Mechanisms== |
|||
=== Nociceptive === |
|||
{{Main|Nociception}} |
|||
[[File:Nociceptive pain.jpg|thumb|288x288px|Mechanism of nociceptive pain]] |
|||
Nociceptive pain is caused by stimulation of [[Sensory nervous system|sensory nerve fibers]] that respond to stimuli approaching or exceeding harmful intensity ([[nociceptor]]s), and may be classified according to the mode of noxious stimulation. The most common categories are "thermal" (e.g. heat or cold), "mechanical" (e.g. crushing, tearing, shearing, etc.) and "chemical" (e.g. [[iodine]] in a cut or chemicals released during [[inflammation]]). Some nociceptors respond to more than one of these modalities and are consequently designated polymodal. |
|||
Nociceptive pain may also be classed according to the site of origin and divided into "visceral", "deep somatic" and "superficial somatic" pain. [[Viscus|Visceral structures]] (e.g., the heart, liver and intestines) are highly sensitive to stretch, [[ischemia]] and [[inflammation]], but relatively insensitive to other stimuli that normally evoke pain in other structures, such as burning and cutting. [[Visceral pain]] is diffuse, difficult to locate and often [[referred pain|referred]] to a distant, usually superficial, structure. It may be accompanied by nausea and vomiting and may be described as sickening, deep, squeezing, and dull.<ref name = Urch>{{cite book | vauthors = Urch CE, Suzuki R |chapter=Pathophysiology of somatic, visceral, and neuropathic cancer pain |editor=Sykes N, Bennett MI & Yuan C-S |title=Clinical pain management: Cancer pain |edition=2d |isbn=978-0340940075 |publisher=Hodder Arnold |location=London |pages=3–12|date=26 September 2008 }}</ref> ''Deep somatic'' pain is initiated by stimulation of nociceptors in ligaments, tendons, bones, blood vessels, [[fascia]]e and muscles, and is dull, aching, poorly-localized pain. Examples include [[sprain]]s and broken bones. ''Superficial somatic'' pain is initiated by activation of nociceptors in the skin or other superficial tissue, and is sharp, well-defined and clearly located. Examples of injuries that produce superficial somatic pain include minor wounds and minor (first degree) burns.<ref name="isbn0-443-05683-8">{{cite book |vauthors=Coda BA, Bonica JJ |veditors=Panswick CC, Main CJ |title=Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach |publisher=Churchill Livingstone |location=Edinburgh |year=2000 |isbn=978-0443056833 |chapter=General considerations of acute pain |chapter-url-access=registration |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/painmanagementin0000main }}</ref> |
|||
=== Neuropathic === |
|||
{{Main|Neuropathic pain}} |
|||
Neuropathic pain is caused by damage or disease affecting any part of the [[nervous system]] involved in bodily feelings (the [[somatosensory system]]).<ref name="Treede">{{cite journal | vauthors = Treede RD, Jensen TS, Campbell JN, Cruccu G, Dostrovsky JO, Griffin JW, Hansson P, Hughes R, Nurmikko T, Serra J | title = Neuropathic pain: redefinition and a grading system for clinical and research purposes | journal = Neurology | volume = 70 | issue = 18 | pages = 1630–1635 | date = April 2008 | pmid = 18003941 | doi = 10.1212/01.wnl.0000282763.29778.59 | hdl = 11573/97043 | s2cid = 30172528 }}</ref> Neuropathic pain may be divided into peripheral, [[central pain syndrome|central]], or mixed (peripheral and central) neuropathic pain. [[Peripheral nervous system|Peripheral]] neuropathic pain is often described as "burning", "tingling", "electrical", "stabbing", or "pins and needles".<ref name=Paice2003>{{cite journal | vauthors = Paice JA | title = Mechanisms and management of neuropathic pain in cancer | journal = The Journal of Supportive Oncology | volume = 1 | issue = 2 | pages = 107–120 | year = 2003 | pmid = 15352654 | url = https://www.supportiveoncology.net/journal/articles/0102107.pdf | df = dmy-all | url-status = dead | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20100107161021/https://www.supportiveoncology.net/journal/articles/0102107.pdf | archive-date = 7 January 2010 | access-date = 8 January 2010 }}</ref> Bumping the "[[funny bone]]" elicits acute peripheral neuropathic pain. |
|||
Some manifestations of neuropathic pain include: traumatic neuropathy, [[tic douloureux]], painful [[diabetic neuropathy]], and [[postherpetic neuralgia]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Campbell JN, Meyer RA | title = Mechanisms of neuropathic pain | journal = Neuron | volume = 52 | issue = 1 | pages = 77–92 | date = October 2006 | pmid = 17015228 | pmc = 1810425 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.021 }}</ref> |
|||
=== Nociplastic === |
|||
{{Main|Nociplastic pain}} |
|||
Nociplastic pain is pain characterized by a changed [[nociception]] (but without evidence of real or threatened tissue damage, or without disease or damage in the [[somatosensory system]]).<ref name="Mind-Body Therapies for Opioid-Trea"/> |
|||
=== Psychogenic === |
|||
{{Main|Psychogenic pain}} |
|||
Psychogenic pain, also called ''psychalgia'' or ''somatoform pain'', is pain caused, increased or prolonged by mental, emotional or behavioral factors.<ref name="Cleveland Clinic"/> Headaches, back pain and stomach pain are sometimes diagnosed as psychogenic.<ref name="Cleveland Clinic">{{cite web |url=https://my.clevelandclinic.org/services/Pain_Management/hic_Psychogenic_Pain.aspx |title=Psychogenic Pain |publisher=[[Cleveland Clinic]] |access-date=25 September 2017 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://archive.wikiwix.com/cache/20110714011822/https://my.clevelandclinic.org/services/Pain_Management/hic_Psychogenic_Pain.aspx |archive-date=14 July 2011 }}</ref> Those affected are often stigmatized, because both medical professionals and the general public tend to think that pain from a psychological source is not "real". However, specialists consider that it is no less actual or hurtful than pain from any other source.<ref name=IASP_definitions>[https://web.archive.org/web/20080512061229/https://www.iasp-pain.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section=General_Resource_Links&Template=%2FCM%2FHTMLDisplay.cfm&ContentID=3058#Pain "International Association for the Study of Pain | Pain Definitions".]. Retrieved 12 October 2010.</ref> |
|||
People with [[chronic pain|long-term pain]] frequently display psychological disturbance, with elevated scores on the [[Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory]] scales of [[hysteria]], depression and [[hypochondriasis]] (the "[[neurotic triad]]"). Some investigators have argued that it is this neuroticism that causes acute pain to turn chronic, but clinical evidence points in the other direction, to chronic pain causing [[neuroticism]]. When long-term pain is relieved by therapeutic intervention, scores on the neurotic triad and [[anxiety]] fall, often to normal levels. [[Self-esteem]], often low in chronic pain patients, also shows improvement once pain has resolved.<ref name = "The challenge of pain"/>{{rp|31–32}} |
|||
===As an aid to diagnosis=== |
|||
Pain is a symptom of many medical conditions. Knowing the time of onset, location, intensity, pattern of occurrence (continuous, intermittent, etc.), exacerbating and relieving factors, and quality (burning, sharp, etc.) of the pain will help the examining physician to accurately diagnose the problem. For example, chest pain described as extreme heaviness may indicate [[myocardial infarction]], while chest pain described as tearing may indicate [[aortic dissection]].<ref name="pmid9786377">{{vcite journal | author = Panju AA, Hemmelgarn BR, Guyatt GH, Simel DL | title = The rational clinical examination. Is this patient having a myocardial infarction? | journal = JAMA | volume = 280 | issue = 14 | pages = 1256–63 | year = 1998 | month = October | pmid = 9786377 | doi = | url = | issn = }}</ref><ref name="pmid1020750">{{vcite journal | author = Slater EE, DeSanctis RW | title = The clinical recognition of dissecting aortic aneurysm | journal = Am. J. Med. | volume = 60 | issue = 5 | pages = 625–33 | year = 1976 | month = May | pmid = 1020750 | doi = 10.1016/0002-9343(76)90496-4 | url = | issn = }}</ref> |
|||
==Management== |
==Management== |
||
{{Main|Pain management}} |
{{Main|Pain management}} |
||
{{Further|Pain management in children|Pain management during childbirth}} |
|||
Pain can be treated through a variety of methods. The most appropriate method depends upon the situation. Management of [[chronic pain]] can be difficult and may require the coordinated efforts of a [[pain management]] team, which typically includes [[medical practitioner]]s, clinical pharmacists, [[clinical psychologist]]s, [[physiotherapist]]s, [[occupational therapist]]s, [[physician assistant]]s, and [[nurse practitioner]]s.<ref>{{cite book|title=Pain management: A practical guide for clinicians|vauthors=Thienhaus O, Cole BE|date=2002|publisher=American Academy of Pain Management|isbn=978-0849322624|veditors=Weiner RS|page=29|chapter=The classification of pain}} |
|||
Inadequate treatment of pain is widespread throughout surgical wards, intensive care units, accident and emergency departments, in general practice, in the management of all forms of chronic pain including cancer pain, and in end of life care.<ref>{{vcite journal | author = Brown AK, Christo PJ, Wu CL | title = Strategies for postoperative pain management | journal = Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol | volume = 18 | issue = 4 | pages = 703–17 | year = 2004 | month = December | pmid = 15460554 | doi = 10.1016/j.bpa.2004.05.004 | url = | issn = }} |
|||
* {{cite book|url=https://archive.org/details/painmanagementin0000main|title=Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach|vauthors=Main CJ, Spanswick CC|date=2000|publisher=Churchill Livingstone|isbn=978-0443056833|quote=Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach.|url-access=registration}}</ref> |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Cullen L, Greiner J, Titler MG | title = Pain management in the culture of critical care | journal = Crit. Care Nurs. Clin. North Am. | volume = 13 | issue = 2 | pages = 151–66 | year = 2001 | month = June | pmid = 11866399 | doi = | url = | issn = }} |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Rupp T, Delaney KA | title = Inadequate analgesia in emergency medicine | journal = Ann. Emerg. Med. | volume = 43 | issue = 4 | pages = 494–503 | year = 2004 | month = April | pmid = 15039693 | doi = 10.1016/j.annemergmed.2003.11.019 | url = | issn = }} |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Smith GF, Toonen TR | title = Primary care of the patient with cancer | journal = Am Fam Physician | volume = 75 | issue = 8 | pages = 1207–14 | year = 2007 | month = April | pmid = 17477104 | doi = | url = | issn = }} |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Jacobson PL, Mann JD | title = Evolving role of the neurologist in the diagnosis and treatment of chronic noncancer pain | journal = Mayo Clin. Proc. | volume = 78 | issue = 1 | pages = 80–4 | year = 2003 | month = January | pmid = 12528880 | doi = 10.4065/78.1.80 | url = | issn = }} |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Deandrea S, Montanari M, Moja L, Apolone G | title = Prevalence of undertreatment in cancer pain. A review of published literature | journal = Ann. Oncol. | volume = 19 | issue = 12 | pages = 1985–91 | year = 2008 | month = December | pmid = 18632721 | pmc = 2733110 | doi = 10.1093/annonc/mdn419 | url = | issn = }} |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Perron V, Schonwetter RS | title = Assessment and management of pain in palliative care patients | journal = Cancer Control | volume = 8 | issue = 1 | pages = 15–24 | year = 2001 | pmid = 11176032 | doi = | url = http://www.moffitt.org/CCJRoot/v8n1/pdf/15.pdf | issn = }}</ref> This neglect is extended to all ages, from neonates to the [[Medically frail|frail]] elderly.<ref>{{vcite book | author = Selbst SM, Fein JA | authorlink = | editor = Henretig FM, Fleisher GR, Ludwig S | others = | title = Textbook of pediatric emergency medicine | edition = | publisher = Lippincott Williams & Wilkins | location = Hagerstwon, MD | year = 2006 | pages = | isbn = 0-7817-5074-1 | chapter = Sedation and analgesia | chapterurl = http://books.google.com.au/books?id=oA7qSOvYZxUC&pg=PA63& | url = | accessdate = }} |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Cleeland CS | title = Undertreatment of cancer pain in elderly patients | journal = JAMA | volume = 279 | issue = 23 | pages = 1914–5 | year = 1998 | month = June | pmid = 9634265 | doi = 10.1001/jama.279.23.1914 | url = | issn = }}</ref> African and Hispanic Americans are more likely than others to suffer needlessly in the hands of a physician;<ref>{{vcite journal | author = Bonham VL | title = Race, ethnicity, and pain treatment: striving to understand the causes and solutions to the disparities in pain treatment | journal = J Law Med Ethics | volume = 29 | issue = 1 | pages = 52–68 | year = 2001 | pmid = 11521272 | doi = | url = http://www.painandthelaw.org/aslme_content/29-1/bonham.pdf | issn = }} |
|||
<p>'''^''' {{vcite journal | author = Green CR, Anderson KO, Baker TA, Campbell LC, Decker S, Fillingim RB, Kalauokalani DA, Kaloukalani DA, Lasch KE, Myers C, Tait RC, Todd KH, Vallerand AH | title = The unequal burden of pain: confronting racial and ethnic disparities in pain | journal = Pain Med | volume = 4 | issue = 3 | pages = 277–94 | year = 2003 | month = September | pmid = 12974827 | doi = 10.1046/j.1526-4637.2003.03034.x | url = | issn = }}</ref> and women's pain is more likely to be undertreated than men's.<ref name="pmid11521267">{{vcite journal | author = Hoffmann DE, Tarzian AJ | title = The girl who cried pain: a bias against women in the treatment of pain | journal = J Law Med Ethics | volume = 29 | issue = 1 | pages = 13–27 | year = 2001 | pmid = 11521267 | doi = | url = | issn = }}</ref> |
|||
Inadequate treatment of pain is widespread throughout surgical wards, [[intensive care units]], and accident and [[emergency departments]], in [[General practitioner|general practice]], in the management of all forms of chronic pain including cancer pain, and in [[End-of-life care|end of life care]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Brown AK, Christo PJ, Wu CL | title = Strategies for postoperative pain management | journal = Best Practice & Research. Clinical Anaesthesiology | volume = 18 | issue = 4 | pages = 703–717 | date = December 2004 | pmid = 15460554 | doi = 10.1016/j.bpa.2004.05.004 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Cullen L, Greiner J, Titler MG | title = Pain management in the culture of critical care | journal = Critical Care Nursing Clinics of North America | volume = 13 | issue = 2 | pages = 151–166 | date = June 2001 | pmid = 11866399 | doi = 10.1016/S0899-5885(18)30046-7 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Rupp T, Delaney KA | title = Inadequate analgesia in emergency medicine | journal = Annals of Emergency Medicine | volume = 43 | issue = 4 | pages = 494–503 | date = April 2004 | pmid = 15039693 | doi = 10.1016/j.annemergmed.2003.11.019 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Smith GF, Toonen TR | title = Primary care of the patient with cancer | journal = American Family Physician | volume = 75 | issue = 8 | pages = 1207–1214 | date = April 2007 | pmid = 17477104 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Jacobson PL, Mann JD | title = Evolving role of the neurologist in the diagnosis and treatment of chronic noncancer pain | journal = Mayo Clinic Proceedings | volume = 78 | issue = 1 | pages = 80–84 | date = January 2003 | pmid = 12528880 | doi = 10.4065/78.1.80 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Deandrea S, Montanari M, Moja L, Apolone G | title = Prevalence of undertreatment in cancer pain. A review of published literature | journal = Annals of Oncology | volume = 19 | issue = 12 | pages = 1985–1991 | date = December 2008 | pmid = 18632721 | pmc = 2733110 | doi = 10.1093/annonc/mdn419 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Perron V, Schonwetter RS | title = Assessment and management of pain in palliative care patients | journal = Cancer Control | volume = 8 | issue = 1 | pages = 15–24 | year = 2001 | pmid = 11176032 | doi = 10.1177/107327480100800103 | doi-access = free }}</ref> This neglect extends to all ages, from newborns to [[medically frail]] elderly.<ref>{{cite book |vauthors=Selbst SM, Fein JA |veditors=Henretig FM, Fleisher GR, Ludwig S |title=Textbook of pediatric emergency medicine |publisher=Lippincott Williams & Wilkins |location=Hagerstwon, MD |year=2006 |isbn=978-1605471594 |chapter=Sedation and analgesia |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=oA7qSOvYZxUC&pg=PA63 |access-date=3 February 2016 |archive-date=11 June 2016 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160611170847/https://books.google.com/books?id=oA7qSOvYZxUC&pg=PA63 |url-status=live }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Cleeland CS | title = Undertreatment of cancer pain in elderly patients | journal = JAMA | volume = 279 | issue = 23 | pages = 1914–1915 | date = June 1998 | pmid = 9634265 | doi = 10.1001/jama.279.23.1914 }}</ref> In the US, [[African Americans|African]] and [[Hispanic and Latino Americans|Hispanic Americans]] are more likely than others to suffer unnecessarily while in the care of a physician;<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bonham VL | title = Race, ethnicity, and pain treatment: striving to understand the causes and solutions to the disparities in pain treatment | journal = The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics | volume = 29 | issue = 1 | pages = 52–68 | year = 2001 | pmid = 11521272 | doi = 10.1111/j.1748-720X.2001.tb00039.x | s2cid = 18257031 | url = https://www.painandthelaw.org/aslme_content/29-1/bonham.pdf | df = dmy-all | url-status = dead | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20110719132135/https://www.painandthelaw.org/aslme_content/29-1/bonham.pdf | archive-date = 19 July 2011 | accessdate = 7 March 2022 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Green CR, Anderson KO, Baker TA, Campbell LC, Decker S, Fillingim RB, Kalauokalani DA, Kaloukalani DA, Lasch KE, Myers C, Tait RC, Todd KH, Vallerand AH | title = The unequal burden of pain: confronting racial and ethnic disparities in pain | journal = Pain Medicine | volume = 4 | issue = 3 | pages = 277–294 | date = September 2003 | pmid = 12974827 | doi = 10.1046/j.1526-4637.2003.03034.x | hdl = 2027.42/73822 | url = https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/73822/1/j.1526-4637.2003.03034.x.pdf | access-date = 2 September 2019 | archive-date = 30 March 2021 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20210330014611/https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/handle/2027.42/73822/j.1526-4637.2003.03034.x.pdf;jsessionid=9D282F272A80BFE500C544F4D9C57617?sequence=1 | url-status = live | doi-access = free }}</ref> and women's pain is more likely to be undertreated than men's.<ref name="pmid11521267">{{cite journal | vauthors = Hoffmann DE, Tarzian AJ | title = The girl who cried pain: a bias against women in the treatment of pain | journal = The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics | volume = 29 | issue = 1 | pages = 13–27 | year = 2001 | pmid = 11521267 | doi = 10.1111/j.1748-720X.2001.tb00037.x | s2cid = 219952180 | url = https://digitalcommons.law.umaryland.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1144&context=fac_pubs | access-date = 11 July 2019 | archive-date = 1 November 2019 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20191101132951/https://digitalcommons.law.umaryland.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1144&context=fac_pubs | url-status = live }}</ref> |
|||
The [[International Association for the Study of Pain]] advocates that the relief of pain should be recognized as a [[human right]], that chronic pain should be considered a disease in its own right, and that [[pain medicine]] should have the full status of a specialty.<ref>Delegates to the International Pain Summit of the International Association for the Study of Pain (2010) [http://www.iasp-pain.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section=Declaration_of_MontrandNum233_al "Declaration of Montreal"] Retrieved 4 Jan 2010.</ref> It is a specialty only in China and Australia at this time.<ref name="isbn0-7817-7388-1">{{vcite book | author = Horlocker TT, Cousins MJ, Bridenbaugh PO, Carr DL | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = Cousins and Bridenbaugh's Neural Blockade in Clinical Anesthesia and Pain Medicine | edition = | publisher = Lippincott Williams & Wilkins | location = Hagerstwon, MD | year = 2008 | pages = | isbn = 0-7817-7388-1 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> Elsewhere, pain medicine is a subspecialty under disciplines such as [[anesthesiology]], [[physiatry]], [[neurology]], [[palliative medicine]] and [[psychiatry]].<ref>[http://www.abms.org/Who_We_Help/Consumers/About_Physician_Specialties/physical.aspx "Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation"]. Retrieved 13 October 2010.</ref> |
|||
The [[International Association for the Study of Pain]] advocates that the relief of pain should be recognized as a [[human right]], that chronic pain should be considered a disease in its own right, and that [[pain medicine]] should have the full status of a [[Specialty (medicine)|medical specialty]].<ref>{{cite web | author = Delegates to the International Pain Summit of the International Association for the Study of Pain | date = 2010 | url = https://www.iasp-pain.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section=Declaration_of_MontrandNum233_al | title = Declaration of Montreal | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20110513203221/https://www.iasp-pain.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section=Declaration_of_MontrandNum233_al | archive-date = 13 May 2011 | access-date = 7 March 2022 | url-status = dead }}</ref> It is a specialty only in China and Australia at this time.<ref name="isbn0-7817-7388-1">{{cite book | vauthors = Horlocker TT, Cousins MJ, Bridenbaugh PO, Carr DL |title=Cousins and Bridenbaugh's Neural Blockade in Clinical Anesthesia and Pain Medicine |publisher=Lippincott Williams & Wilkins |location=Hagerstwon, MD |year=2008 |isbn=978-0781773881}}</ref> Elsewhere, pain medicine is a subspecialty under disciplines such as [[anesthesiology]], [[physiatry]], [[neurology]], [[palliative medicine]] and [[psychiatry]].<ref>{{cite web | url = https://www.abms.org/Who_We_Help/Consumers/About_Physician_Specialties/physical.aspx | title = Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20080516091310/https://www.abms.org/Who_We_Help/Consumers/About_Physician_Specialties/physical.aspx | archive-date = 16 May 2008 | access-date = 7 March 2022 | url-status = dead }}</ref> In 2011, [[Human Rights Watch]] alerted that tens of millions of people worldwide are still denied access to inexpensive medications for severe pain.<ref>{{cite web |author=Human Rights Watch |author-link=Human Rights Watch |url=https://www.hrw.org/news/2011/06/02/global-tens-millions-face-death-agony |title=Tens of Millions Face Death in Agony |year=2011 |access-date=26 August 2013 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130901232156/https://www.hrw.org/news/2011/06/02/global-tens-millions-face-death-agony |archive-date=1 September 2013 }}</ref> |
|||
===Medication=== |
===Medication=== |
||
Acute pain is usually managed with medications such as [[analgesic]]s and [[anesthetic]]s.<ref name="researchgate.net">{{cite journal | vauthors = Mallinson TE |title=A review of ketorolac as a prehospital analgesic |journal=Journal of Paramedic Practice |date=2017 |volume=9 |issue=12 |pages=522–526 |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/321640488 |access-date=2 June 2018 |language=en |doi=10.12968/jpar.2017.9.12.522 |doi-access=free |archive-date=5 June 2018 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180605033254/https://www.researchgate.net/publication/321640488_A_review_of_ketorolac_as_a_prehospital_analgesic |url-status=live }}</ref> [[Caffeine]] when added to pain medications such as [[ibuprofen]], may provide some additional benefit.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Derry CJ, Derry S, Moore RA | title = Caffeine as an analgesic adjuvant for acute pain in adults | journal = The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews | volume = 12 | issue = 12 | pages = CD009281 | date = December 2014 | pmid = 25502052 | pmc = 6485702 | doi = 10.1002/14651858.CD009281.pub3 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Derry S, Wiffen PJ, Moore RA | title = Single dose oral ibuprofen plus caffeine for acute postoperative pain in adults | journal = The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews | issue = 7 | pages = CD011509 | date = July 2015 | volume = 2019 | pmid = 26171993 | pmc = 6481458 | doi = 10.1002/14651858.CD011509.pub2 }}</ref> [[Ketamine]] can be used instead of opioids for short-term pain.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Karlow N, Schlaepfer CH, Stoll CR, Doering M, Carpenter CR, Colditz GA, Motov S, Miller J, Schwarz ES | display-authors = 6 | title = A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Ketamine as an Alternative to Opioids for Acute Pain in the Emergency Department | journal = Academic Emergency Medicine | volume = 25 | issue = 10 | pages = 1086–1097 | date = October 2018 | pmid = 30019434 | doi = 10.1111/acem.13502 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Pain medications can cause paradoxical side effects, such as [[opioid-induced hyperalgesia]] (severe generalized pain caused by long-term opioid use).<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Higgins C, Smith BH, Matthews K | title = Evidence of opioid-induced hyperalgesia in clinical populations after chronic opioid exposure: a systematic review and meta-analysis | journal = British Journal of Anaesthesia | volume = 122 | issue = 6 | pages = e114–e126 | date = June 2019 | pmid = 30915985 | doi = 10.1016/j.bja.2018.09.019 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Fishbain DA, Pulikal A | title = Does Opioid Tapering in Chronic Pain Patients Result in Improved Pain or Same Pain vs Increased Pain at Taper Completion? A Structured Evidence-Based Systematic Review | journal = Pain Medicine | volume = 20 | issue = 11 | pages = 2179–2197 | date = November 2019 | pmid = 30597076 | doi = 10.1093/pm/pny231 | url = https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30597076 | access-date = 19 February 2021 | url-status = live | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20210118010043/https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30597076/ | archive-date = 18 January 2021 }}</ref> |
|||
Acute pain is usually managed with medications such as [[analgesics]] and [[anesthetic]]s. Management of chronic pain, however, is much more difficult and may require the coordinated efforts of a [[pain management]] team, which typically includes [[medical practitioner]]s, [[clinical psychologist]]s, [[physiotherapist]]s, [[occupational therapist]]s, [[physician assistant]]s, and [[nurse practitioner]]s.<ref>Thienhaus, O; Cole, BE (2002). "The classification of pain". In Weiner, RS. ''Pain management: A practical guide for clinicians''. American Academy of Pain Management. p. 29. ISBN 0-8493-0926-3.<p>'''^''' Main, Chris J.; Spanswick, Chris C. (2000). [http://books.google.com/?id=wcEQPzTOEAoC&printsec=frontcover&dq=Pain+management:+an+interdisciplinary+approach&cd=1#v=onepage&q= ''Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach''] Churchill Livingstone. ISBN 0-443-05683-8.</ref> |
|||
{{anchor|Oral sugar}} |
{{anchor|Oral sugar}} |
||
Sugar taken orally reduces the total crying time but not the duration of the first cry in newborns undergoing a painful procedure (a single lancing of the heel). It does not moderate the effect of pain on heart rate<ref>Stevens B, Yamada J, Ohlsson A (2010). "Sucrose for analgesia in newborn infants undergoing painful procedures". ''Cochrane Database Syst Rev'' (1): CD001069. {{DOI|10.1002/14651858.CD001069.pub3}} PMID 20091512.</ref> and a recent single study found that sugar did not significantly affect pain-related [[EEG|electrical activity]] in the brains of newborns one second after the heel lance procedure.<ref>Slater R, Cornelissen L, Fabrizi L, et al. (September 2010). "Oral sucrose as an analgesic drug for procedural pain in newborn infants: a randomised controlled trial". ''Lancet''. {{DOI|10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61303-7}} PMID 20817247.</ref><ref>Lasky RE, van Drongelen W (September 2010). "Is sucrose an effective analgesic for newborn babies?" ''Lancet''. {{DOI|10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61358-X}} PMID 20817245.</ref> Sweet oral liquid moderately reduces the incidence and duration of crying caused by immunization injection in children between one and twelve months of age.<ref>{{vcite journal | author = Harrison D, Stevens B, Bueno M ''et al.'' | year = | title = (June 2010). "Efficacy of sweet solutions for analgesia in infants between 1 and 12 months of age: a systematic review" | url = | journal = Arch. Dis. Child | volume = 95 | issue = 6| pages = 406–13 | doi = 10.1136/adc.2009.174227 | pmid = 20463370 }}</ref> |
|||
Sugar ([[sucrose]]) when taken by mouth reduces [[Pain in babies|pain in newborn babies]] undergoing some medical procedures (a [[Incision and drainage|lancing]] of the heel, [[venipuncture]], and [[intramuscular injections]]). Sugar does not remove pain from [[circumcision]], and it is unknown if sugar reduces pain for other procedures.<ref name="StevensYamada2016">{{cite journal | vauthors = Stevens B, Yamada J, Ohlsson A, Haliburton S, Shorkey A | title = Sucrose for analgesia in newborn infants undergoing painful procedures | journal = The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews | volume = 7 | pages = CD001069 | date = July 2016 | issue = 2 | pmid = 27420164 | pmc = 6457867 | doi = 10.1002/14651858.CD001069.pub5 }}</ref> Sugar did not affect pain-related [[EEG|electrical activity]] in the brains of newborns one second after the heel lance procedure.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Lasky RE, van Drongelen W | title = Is sucrose an effective analgesic for newborn babies? | journal = Lancet | volume = 376 | issue = 9748 | pages = 1201–1203 | date = October 2010 | pmid = 20817245 | doi = 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61358-X | s2cid = 18724497 }}</ref> Sweet liquid by mouth moderately reduces the rate and duration of crying caused by immunization injection in children between one and twelve months of age.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Harrison D, Stevens B, Bueno M, Yamada J, Adams-Webber T, Beyene J, Ohlsson A | title = Efficacy of sweet solutions for analgesia in infants between 1 and 12 months of age: a systematic review | journal = Archives of Disease in Childhood | volume = 95 | issue = 6 | pages = 406–413 | date = June 2010 | pmid = 20463370 | doi = 10.1136/adc.2009.174227 | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
===Psychological=== |
|||
Individuals with more [[social support]] experience less cancer pain, take less pain medication, report less labor pain and are less likely to use epidural anesthesia during childbirth or suffer from chest pain after coronary artery bypass surgery.<ref>Eisenberger, NI; Lieberman (2005). [http://webscript.princeton.edu/%7Epsych/psychology/related/socneuconf/pdf/eisenberger-lieberman2.pdf "Why it hurts to be left out: The neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain"] In Williams, KD; Forgas, JP; von Hippel, W. The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 109–127. ISBN 1-84169-424-X.</ref> |
|||
===Psychological=== |
|||
Suggestion can significantly affect pain intensity. About 35% of people report marked relief after receiving a [[Saline (medicine)|saline]] injection they believe to have been [[morphine]]. This "[[placebo]]" effect is more pronounced in people who are prone to anxiety, so anxiety reduction may account for some of the effect, but it does not account for all of the effect. Placebos are more effective in intense pain than mild pain; and they produce progressively weaker effects with repeated administration.<ref name=Melzack_Wall_1996_26-28>Melzack, R; Wall, PD (1996). ''The challenge of pain'' (2 ed.). London: Penguin. pp. 26–28. ISBN 978-0-14-025670-3</ref> |
|||
Individuals with more [[social support]] experience less cancer pain, take less pain medication, report less labor pain and are less likely to use [[Epidural administration|epidural anesthesia]] during childbirth, or suffer from chest pain after [[coronary artery bypass surgery]].<ref name="Eisenberger_2005" /> |
|||
It is possible for many chronic pain |
[[Suggestion]] can significantly affect pain intensity. About 35% of people report marked relief after receiving a [[Saline (medicine)|saline]] injection they believed to be [[morphine]]. This [[placebo]] effect is more pronounced in people who are prone to anxiety, and so anxiety reduction may account for some of the effect, but it does not account for all of it. Placebos are more effective for intense pain than mild pain; and they produce progressively weaker effects with repeated administration.<ref name = "The challenge of pain"/>{{rp|26–28}} It is possible for many with chronic pain to become so absorbed in an activity or entertainment that the pain is no longer felt, or is greatly diminished.<ref name = "The challenge of pain"/>{{rp|22–23}} |
||
A number of meta-analyses have found [[clinical hypnosis]] to be effective in controlling pain associated with diagnostic and surgical procedures in both adults and children, as well as pain associated with cancer and childbirth.<ref name="pmid18714889">{{cite journal | vauthors = Wark DM | title = What we can do with hypnosis: a brief note | journal = The American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis | volume = 51 | issue = 1 | pages = 29–36 | date = July 2008 | pmid = 18714889 | doi = 10.1080/00029157.2008.10401640 | s2cid = 12240662 }}</ref> A 2007 review of 13 studies found evidence for the efficacy of hypnosis in the reduction of chronic pain under some conditions, though the number of patients enrolled in the studies was low, raising issues related to the statistical power to detect group differences, and most lacked credible controls for placebo or expectation. The authors concluded that "although the findings provide support for the general applicability of hypnosis in the treatment of chronic pain, considerably more research will be needed to fully determine the effects of hypnosis for different chronic-pain conditions."<ref name= Elkins2007>{{cite journal | vauthors = Elkins G, Jensen MP, Patterson DR | title = Hypnotherapy for the management of chronic pain | journal = The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis | volume = 55 | issue = 3 | pages = 275–287 | date = July 2007 | pmid = 17558718 | pmc = 2752362 | doi = 10.1080/00207140701338621 }}</ref> |
|||
[[Cognitive behavioral therapy]] (CBT) is effective in reducing the suffering associated with chronic pain in some patients but the reduction in suffering is quite modest, and the CBT method employed seems to have no effect on outcome.<ref>{{vcite journal | pmid = 15599126 | volume=21 | issue=1 | title=Cognitive-behavioral treatments for chronic pain: what works for whom? | year=2005 | author=Vlaeyen JW, Morley S | pages=1–8 | doi=10.1097/00002508-200501000-00001}}</ref> |
|||
===Alternative medicine=== |
===Alternative medicine=== |
||
An analysis of the 13 highest quality studies of pain treatment with [[acupuncture]], published in January 2009, concluded there was little difference in the effect of real, fake and no acupuncture.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Madsen MV, Gøtzsche PC, Hróbjartsson A | title = Acupuncture treatment for pain: systematic review of randomised clinical trials with acupuncture, placebo acupuncture, and no acupuncture groups | journal = BMJ | volume = 338 | pages = a3115 | date = January 2009 | pmid = 19174438 | pmc = 2769056 | doi = 10.1136/bmj.a3115 }}</ref> However, more recent reviews have found some benefit.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Chiu HY, Hsieh YJ, Tsai PS | title = Systematic review and meta-analysis of acupuncture to reduce cancer-related pain | journal = European Journal of Cancer Care | volume = 26 | issue = 2 | pages = e12457 | date = March 2017 | pmid = 26853524 | doi = 10.1111/ecc.12457 | s2cid = 20096639 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Chang SC, Hsu CH, Hsu CK, Yang SS, Chang SJ | title = The efficacy of acupuncture in managing patients with chronic prostatitis/chronic pelvic pain syndrome: A systemic review and meta-analysis | journal = Neurourology and Urodynamics | volume = 36 | issue = 2 | pages = 474–481 | date = February 2017 | pmid = 26741647 | doi = 10.1002/nau.22958 | s2cid = 46827576 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Ji M, Wang X, Chen M, Shen Y, Zhang X, Yang J | title = The Efficacy of Acupuncture for the Treatment of Sciatica: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis | journal = Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine | volume = 2015 | pages = 192808 | date = 2015 | pmid = 26425130 | pmc = 4575738 | doi = 10.1155/2015/192808 | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
{{DOI|10.1016/j.pain.2008.11.010}} {{ISSN|0304-3959}} PMID 19084336.</ref> |
|||
Additionally, there is tentative evidence for a few herbal medicines.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Gagnier JJ, Oltean H, van Tulder MW, Berman BM, Bombardier C, Robbins CB | title = Herbal Medicine for Low Back Pain: A Cochrane Review | journal = Spine | volume = 41 | issue = 2 | pages = 116–133 | date = January 2016 | pmid = 26630428 | doi = 10.1097/BRS.0000000000001310 }}</ref> |
|||
A 2007 review of 13 studies found evidence for the efficacy of [[hypnosis]] in the reduction of pain in some conditions, though the number of patients enrolled in the studies was low, bringing up issues of power to detect group differences, and most lacked credible controls for placebo and/or expectation. The authors concluded that "although the findings provide support for the general applicability of hypnosis in the treatment of chronic pain, considerably more research will be needed to fully determine the effects of hypnosis for different chronic-pain conditions."<ref name= Elkins2007>{{vcite journal | author = Elkins G, Jensen MP, Jensen DR, Patterson | year = 2007 | title = Hypnotherapy for the management of chronic pain | url = | journal = International journal of clinical and experimental hypnosis | volume = 55 | issue = 3| page = 283 | doi = 10.1080/00207140701338621 | pmid = 17558718 | pmc=2752362}}</ref> |
|||
For chronic (long-term) [[Low back pain|lower back pain]], [[spinal manipulation]] produces tiny, [[Clinical significance|clinically insignificant]], short-term improvements in pain and function, compared with [[sham therapy]] and other interventions.<ref name="Rubinstein CD008880">{{cite journal | vauthors = Rubinstein SM, Terwee CB, Assendelft WJ, de Boer MR, van Tulder MW | title = Spinal manipulative therapy for acute low-back pain | journal = The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews | issue = 9 | pages = CD008880 | date = September 2012 | volume = 2012 | pmid = 22972127 | pmc = 6885055 | doi = 10.1002/14651858.CD008880.pub2 }}</ref> Spinal manipulation produces the same outcome as other treatments, such as general practitioner care, pain-relief drugs, physical therapy, and exercise, for acute (short-term) lower back pain.<ref name="Rubinstein CD008880"/> |
|||
A 2003 meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials found that [[spinal manipulation]] was "more effective than sham therapy but was no more or less effective than general practitioner care, analgesics, physical therapy, exercise, or back school" in the treatment of [[low back pain]].<ref>Assendelft WJJ, Morton SC, Yu EI, Suttorp MJ, Shekelle PG (2004). "Spinal manipulative therapy for low back pain". [[Cochrane Database Syst Rev]] (1): CD000447. {{DOI|10.1002/14651858.CD000447.pub2}} PMID 14973958.</ref> |
|||
There has been some interest in the relationship between [[vitamin D]] and pain, but the evidence so far from [[Clinical trial|controlled trials]] for such a relationship, other than in [[osteomalacia]], is inconclusive.<ref name= Straube>{{cite journal | vauthors = Straube S, Andrew Moore R, Derry S, McQuay HJ | title = Vitamin D and chronic pain | journal = Pain | volume = 141 | issue = 1–2 | pages = 10–13 | date = January 2009 | pmid = 19084336 | doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2008.11.010 | s2cid = 17244398 }}</ref> |
|||
The International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) says that due to a lack of evidence from high quality research, it does not endorse the general use of cannabinoids to treat pain.<ref>{{cite web |access-date=2024-05-10|language=en|publisher=iasp-pain|title=IASP Position Statement on the Use of Cannabinoids to Treat Pain|url=https://www.iasp-pain.org/publications/iasp-news/iasp-position-statement-on-the-use-of-cannabinoids-to-treat-pain/}}</ref> |
|||
==Epidemiology== |
==Epidemiology== |
||
Pain is the main reason for visiting |
Pain is the main reason for visiting an [[emergency department]] in more than 50% of cases,<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Cordell WH, Keene KK, Giles BK, Jones JB, Jones JH, Brizendine EJ | title = The high prevalence of pain in emergency medical care | journal = The American Journal of Emergency Medicine | volume = 20 | issue = 3 | pages = 165–169 | date = May 2002 | pmid = 11992334 | doi = 10.1053/ajem.2002.32643 }}</ref> and is present in 30% of family practice visits.<ref name="pmid12160512">{{cite journal | vauthors = Hasselström J, Liu-Palmgren J, Rasjö-Wrååk G | title = Prevalence of pain in general practice | journal = European Journal of Pain | volume = 6 | issue = 5 | pages = 375–385 | year = 2002 | pmid = 12160512 | doi = 10.1016/S1090-3801(02)00025-3 | s2cid = 798849 }}</ref> Several [[epidemiological]] studies have reported widely varying prevalence rates for chronic pain, ranging from 12 to 80% of the population.<ref name="pmid20358856">{{cite journal | vauthors = Abu-Saad Huijer H | title = Chronic pain: a review | journal = Le Journal Medical Libanais. The Lebanese Medical Journal | volume = 58 | issue = 1 | pages = 21–27 | year = 2010 | pmid = 20358856 }}</ref> It becomes more common as people approach death. A study of 4,703 patients found that 26% had pain in the last two years of life, increasing to 46% in the last month.<ref name=Death2010>{{cite journal | vauthors = Smith AK, Cenzer IS, Knight SJ, Puntillo KA, Widera E, Williams BA, Boscardin WJ, Covinsky KE | title = The epidemiology of pain during the last 2 years of life | journal = Annals of Internal Medicine | volume = 153 | issue = 9 | pages = 563–569 | date = November 2010 | pmid = 21041575 | pmc = 3150170 | doi = 10.7326/0003-4819-153-9-201011020-00005 }}</ref> |
||
A survey of 6,636 children (0–18 years of age) found that, of the 5,424 respondents, 54% had experienced pain in the preceding three months. A quarter reported having experienced recurrent or continuous pain for three months or more, and a third of these reported frequent and intense pain. The intensity of chronic pain was higher for girls, and girls' reports of chronic pain increased markedly between ages 12 and 14.<ref name="pmid10863045">{{ |
A survey of 6,636 children (0–18 years of age) found that, of the 5,424 respondents, 54% had experienced pain in the preceding three months. A quarter reported having experienced recurrent or continuous pain for three months or more, and a third of these reported frequent and intense pain. The intensity of chronic pain was higher for girls, and girls' reports of chronic pain increased markedly between ages 12 and 14.<ref name="pmid10863045">{{cite journal | vauthors = Perquin CW, Hazebroek-Kampschreur AA, Hunfeld JA, Bohnen AM, van Suijlekom-Smit LW, Passchier J, van der Wouden JC | title = Pain in children and adolescents: a common experience | journal = Pain | volume = 87 | issue = 1 | pages = 51–58 | date = July 2000 | pmid = 10863045 | doi = 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00269-4 | s2cid = 9813003 }}</ref> |
||
==Society and culture== |
==Society and culture== |
||
[[File:Lamani v kole.jpg|thumb|The [[breaking wheel]] was a torture method used for [[Capital punishment#Public execution|public execution]] by breaking the bones of a criminal or [[Club (weapon)|bludgeoning]] them to death.]] |
|||
[[File:Catlin Okipa.jpg|thumb|The [[Mandan#Religion|okipa]] ceremony as witnessed by [[George Catlin]], circa 1835.]] |
|||
[[File:TUCANDEIRA RITUAL - panoramio.jpg|thumb|The [[Mawé people]] of Brazil intentionally use [[bullet ant]] stings as a [[rite of passage]] into manhood.]] |
|||
The nature or meaning of physical pain has been diversely understood by religious or secular traditions from antiquity to modern times.<ref name="isbn0-674-39968-4">{{vcite book | author = Rey R | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = The history of pain | edition = | publisher = Harvard University Press | location = Cambridge | year = 1995 | pages = | isbn = 0-674-39968-4 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref><ref name="isbn0-520-08276-1">{{vcite book | author = Morris DR | authorlink = | editor = | others = | title = The culture of pain | edition = | publisher = University of California Press | location = Berkeley | year = 1991 | pages = | isbn = 0-520-08276-1 | oclc = | doi = | url =}}</ref> |
|||
Physical pain is a universal experience, and a strong motivator of human and animal behavior. As such, physical pain is used politically in relation to various issues such as [[pain management]] policy, [[Drug policy|drug control]], [[animal rights]] or [[animal welfare]], [[torture]], and [[pain compliance]]. The deliberate infliction of pain and the medical management of pain are both important aspects of [[biopower]], a concept that encompasses the "set of mechanisms through which the basic biological features of the human species became the object of a political strategy".<ref>{{Cite book| vauthors = Foucault M |title=Security, Territory, Population: Lectures at the College de France, 1977–78|publisher=Palgrave Macmillan|year=2007|page=1}}</ref> |
|||
In various contexts, the deliberate infliction of pain in the form of [[corporal punishment]] is used as retribution for an offence, for the purpose of disciplining or reforming a wrongdoer, or to deter attitudes or behaviour deemed unacceptable. In Western societies, the intentional infliction of severe pain (torture) was principally used to extract confession prior to its abolition in the latter part of the 19th century. Torture as a means to punish the [[Citizenship|citizen]] has been reserved for offences posing a severe threat to the social fabric (for example, [[treason]]).<ref name="fall-rise-of-torture">{{cite journal | vauthors = Einolf C | date=2007 | title=The Fall and Rise of Torture: A Comparative and Historical Analysis | jstor=20453071 | journal=Social Theory | volume=25 | issue=2 | pages=101–121 | doi=10.1111/j.1467-9558.2007.00300.x | s2cid=53345959 | url=https://works.bepress.com/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1008&context=christopher_einolf }}</ref> |
|||
The administration of torture on bodies othered by the cultural narrative, those observed as not 'full members of society' <ref name="fall-rise-of-torture" />{{rp|101–121[AD1] }} met a resurgence in the 20th century, possibly due to the heightened warfare.<ref name="fall-rise-of-torture" />{{rp|101–121 [AD2] }} |
|||
Many cultures use painful ritual practices as a catalyst for psychological transformation.<ref name="ritual-experience">{{cite journal | vauthors = Morinis A | date=1985 | title=The ritual experience: pain and the transformation of consciousness in ordeals of initiation | jstor=639985 | journal=Ethos | volume=13 | issue=2 | pages=150–174 | doi=10.1525/eth.1985.13.2.02a00040 |doi-access=free}}</ref> The use of pain to transition to a 'cleansed and purified' state is seen in religious [[self-flagellation]] practices (particularly those of [[Self-flagellation#Christianity|Christianity]] and [[Self-flagellation#Islam|Islam]]), or personal [[catharsis]] in [[Suspension (body modification)|neo-primitive body suspension]] experiences.<ref name="flesh-journeys">{{cite journal | vauthors = Atkinson M, Young K | date=2001 | title=Flesh journeys: neo primitives and the contemporary rediscovery of radical body modification | journal=Deviant Behavior | volume=22 | issue=2 | pages=117–146 | doi=10.1080/016396201750065018 | s2cid=146525156 }}</ref> |
|||
Physical pain is an important political topic in relation to various issues, including [[pain management]] policy, [[drug control]], [[animal rights]] or [[animal welfare]], [[torture]], and [[pain compliance]]. In various contexts, the deliberate infliction of pain in the form of [[corporal punishment]] is used as retribution for an offence, or for the purpose of disciplining or reforming a wrongdoer, or to deter attitudes or behaviour deemed unacceptable. In some cultures, extreme practices such as [[mortification of the flesh]] or painful [[rites of passage]] are highly regarded. |
|||
Beliefs about pain play an important role in sporting cultures. Pain may be viewed positively, exemplified by the 'no pain, no gain' attitude, with pain seen as an essential part of training. Sporting culture tends to normalise experiences of pain and injury and celebrate athletes who 'play hurt'.<ref>{{Cite book| veditors = Loland S, Skirstad B, Waddington I |title=Pain and injury in sport: Social and ethical analysis |publisher=Routledge |year=2006|isbn=978-0415357043 |location=London and New York |pages=17–20}}</ref> |
|||
[[Pain (philosophy)|Philosophy of pain]] is a branch of [[philosophy of mind]] that deals essentially with physical pain, especially in connection with such views as [[Dualism (philosophy of mind)|dualism]], [[Type physicalism|identity theory]], and [[Functionalism (philosophy of mind)|functionalism]]. |
|||
Pain has psychological, social, and physical dimensions, and is greatly influenced by cultural factors.<ref name="how-culture-pain-experience">{{cite journal | vauthors = Narayan MC | title = Culture's effects on pain assessment and management | journal = The American Journal of Nursing | volume = 110 | issue = 4 | pages = 38–47 | date = April 2010 | pmid = 20335689 | doi = 10.1097/01.NAJ.0000370157.33223.6d | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
More generally, it is often as a part of pain in the broad sense, i.e. [[suffering]], that physical pain is dealt with in culture, religion, philosophy, or society. |
|||
==Non-humans== |
|||
==In other animals== |
|||
{{Main|Pain in animals|Pain in invertebrates}} |
{{Main|Pain in animals|Pain in invertebrates}} |
||
[[File:Jan Baptist Weenix - Portrait of René Descartes.jpg|thumb|right|Portrait of René Descartes by Jan Baptist Weenix 1647-1649]] |
|||
[[René Descartes]] argued that animals lack consciousness and therefore do not experience pain and suffering in the way that humans do.<ref name="nuffield45">Working party of the Nuffield Council on Bioethics (2005). [https://web.archive.org/web/20080625033250/https://www.nuffieldbioethics.org/fileLibrary/pdf/RIA_Report_FINAL-opt.pdf "The ethics of research involving animals. London: Nuffield Council on Bioethics."] {{ISBN|978-1904384106}}. Archived from the original on 25 June 2008. Retrieved 12 January 2010.</ref> [[Bernard Rollin]] of [[Colorado State University]], the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals,{{efn|Rollin drafted the 1985 Health Research Extension Act and an [[animal welfare]] amendment to the 1985 Food Security Act.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Rollin BE | title = Animal research: a moral science. Talking Point on the use of animals in scientific research | journal = EMBO Reports | volume = 8 | issue = 6 | pages = 521–525 | date = June 2007 | pmid = 17545990 | pmc = 2002540 | doi = 10.1038/sj.embor.7400996 }}</ref>}} wrote that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain, and that veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were simply taught to ignore animal pain.<ref name="Rollin117">{{cite book | vauthors = Rollin B | date = 1989 | title = The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science | location = New York | publisher = Oxford University Press | pages = 117–118 }} cited in {{cite book | vauthors = Carbone L | title = What animals want: expertise and advocacy in laboratory animal welfare policy. | publisher = Oxford University Press | location = US| date = 2004 | page = 150 }}</ref><ref name="pmid14658059">{{cite journal | vauthors = Griffin DR, Speck GB | title = New evidence of animal consciousness | journal = Animal Cognition | volume = 7 | issue = 1 | pages = 5–18 | date = January 2004 | pmid = 14658059 | doi = 10.1007/s10071-003-0203-x | s2cid = 8650837 }}</ref> The ability of invertebrate species of animals, such as insects, to feel pain and suffering is unclear.<ref name="Sherwin, 2001">{{cite journal | vauthors = Sherwin CM | title = Can invertebrates suffer? Or, how robust is argument-by-analogy? | journal = Animal Welfare | date = February 2001 | volume = 10 | issue = 1 | pages = 103–118 | doi = 10.1017/S0962728600023551 | s2cid = 54126137 | url = https://www.ingentaconnect.com/contentone/ufaw/aw/2001/00000010/a00101s1/art00010;jsessionid=1dtup2ob3at3b.x-ic-live-02 | access-date = 22 December 2021 | archive-date = 7 March 2022 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20220307174545/https://www.ingentaconnect.com/contentone/ufaw/aw/2001/00000010/a00101s1/art00010;jsessionid=1dtup2ob3at3b.x-ic-live-02 | url-status = live }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Lockwood JA |year=1987 |title=The Moral Standing of Insects and the Ethics of Extinction |jstor=3495093 |journal=The Florida Entomologist |volume=70 |issue=1 |pages=70–89 |doi=10.2307/3495093}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = DeGrazia D, Rowan A | title = Pain, suffering, and anxiety in animals and humans | journal = Theoretical Medicine | volume = 12 | issue = 3 | pages = 193–211 | date = September 1991 | pmid = 1754965 | doi = 10.1007/BF00489606 | s2cid = 34920699 }}</ref> |
|||
Specialists believe that all vertebrates can feel pain, and that certain invertebrates, like the octopus, may also.<ref name="Sherwin, 2001" /><ref>{{cite web | url = https://sencanada.ca/content/sen/committee/372/lega/witn/shelly-e.htm | title = Do Invertebrates Feel Pain? | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20100106084119/https://sencanada.ca/content/sen/committee/372/lega/witn/shelly-e.htm | archive-date=6 January 2010 | work = The Senate Standing Committee on Legal and Constitutional Affairs | publisher = The [[Parliament of Canada]] | access-date = 11 June 2008 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Smith JA |year=1991 |title=A Question of Pain in Invertebrates |url=https://www.abolitionist.com/darwinian-life/invertebrate-pain.html |journal=Institute for Laboratory Animal Research Journal |volume=33 |pages=1–2 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20111008212237/https://www.abolitionist.com/darwinian-life/invertebrate-pain.html |archive-date=8 October 2011 }}</ref> The presence of pain in animals is unknown, but can be inferred through physical and behavioral reactions,<ref name="pmid7715946">{{cite journal | vauthors = Abbott FV, Franklin KB, Westbrook FR | title = The formalin test: scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats | journal = Pain | volume = 60 | issue = 1 | pages = 91–102 | date = January 1995 | pmid = 7715946 | doi = 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V | s2cid = 35448280 }}</ref> such as paw withdrawal from various noxious mechanical stimuli in rodents.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Jones JM, Foster W, Twomey CR, Burdge J, Ahmed OM, Pereira TD, Wojick JA, Corder G, Plotkin JB, Abdus-Saboor I | display-authors = 6 | title = A machine-vision approach for automated pain measurement at millisecond timescales | journal = eLife | volume = 9 | pages = e57258 | date = August 2020 | pmid = 32758355 | pmc = 7434442 | doi = 10.7554/eLife.57258 | doi-access = free }}</ref> |
|||
While [[plants]], as living beings, can perceive and communicate physical stimuli and damage, they do not feel pain simply because of the lack of any pain receptors, nerves, or a brain,<ref name="EncyBrit"/> and, by extension, a lack of consciousness.<ref>{{cite journal| last1 = Draguhn| first1 = Andreas | last2 = Mallatt| first2 = Jon M. | last3 = Robinson| first3 = David G. | author-link = | title = Anesthetics and plants: no pain, no brain, and therefore no consciousness| journal = Protoplasma| volume = 258| issue = 2| pages = 239–248| publisher = Springer | date = 2021| language = | jstor = | issn = | doi = 10.1007/s00709-020-01550-9| pmid = 32880005 | pmc = 7907021 | id = 32880005| mr = | zbl = | jfm = }}</ref> Many plants are known to perceive and respond to mechanical stimuli at a cellular level, and some plants such as the [[venus flytrap]] or [[Mimosa pudica|touch-me-not]], are known for their "obvious sensory abilities".<ref name="EncyBrit"/> Nevertheless, no member of the plant kingdom does feel pain notwithstanding their abilities to respond to sunlight, gravity, wind, and any external stimuli such as insect bites since they lack any nervous system. The primary reason for this is that, unlike the members of the [[Animal|animal kingdom]] whose evolutionary successes and failures are shaped by suffering, the evolution of plants are simply shaped by life and death.<ref name="EncyBrit">{{cite web |
|||
In vertebrates, endogenous [[opioid]]s are neurochemicals that moderate pain by interacting with opiate receptors. Opioids and opiate receptors occur naturally in crustaceans and, although at present no certain conclusion can be drawn,<ref name="Sømme">L. Sømme (2005). "Sentience and pain in invertebrates: Report to Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety". Norwegian University of Life Sciences, Oslo.</ref> their presence indicates that lobsters may be able to experience pain.<ref name="Sømme"/><ref name=afa>{{vcite book|title=Cephalopods and decapod crustaceans: their capacity to experience pain and suffering |publisher=Advocates for Animals |year=2005 |url=http://www.advocatesforanimals.org.uk/pdf/crustreport.pdf}}</ref> Opioids may mediate their pain in the same way as in vertebrates.<ref name=afa/> [[Veterinary medicine]] uses, for actual or potential animal pain, the same analgesics and anesthetics as used in humans.<ref name="pmid17553712">{{vcite journal | author = Viñuela-Fernández I, Jones E, Welsh EM, Fleetwood-Walker SM | title = Pain mechanisms and their implication for the management of pain in farm and companion animals | journal = Vet. J. | volume = 174 | issue = 2 | pages = 227–39 | year = 2007 | month = September | pmid = 17553712 | doi = 10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.02.002 | url = | issn = }}</ref> |
|||
| url = https://www.britannica.com/story/do-plants-feel-pain |
|||
| title = Do Plants Feel Pain? |
|||
| last = Petruzzello |
|||
| first = Melissa |
|||
| date = 2016 |
|||
| website = Encyclopedia Britannica |
|||
| access-date = 8 January 2023 |
|||
| quote = Given that plants do not have pain receptors, nerves, or a brain, they do not feel pain as we members of the animal kingdom understand it. Uprooting a carrot or trimming a hedge is not a form of botanical torture, and you can bite into that apple without worry.}}</ref> |
|||
== |
== See also == |
||
* [[Feeling]], a perceptual state of conscious experience. |
|||
First attested in English in 1297, the word ''pain'' comes from the [[Old French]] ''peine'', in turn from [[Latin]] ''poena'', "punishment, penalty"<ref>[http://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0059%3Aentry%3Dpoena poena], Charlton T. Lewis, Charles Short, ''A Latin Dictionary'', on Perseus Digital Library</ref> (in L.L. also "torment, hardship, suffering") and that from [[Greek language|Greek]] "ποινή" (''poine''), generally "price paid", "penalty", "punishment".<ref>[http://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0057%3Aentry%3Dpoinh%2F ποινή], Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, ''A Greek-English Lexicon'', on Perseus Digital Library</ref><ref>[http://www.etymonline.com/index.php?term=pain pain], Online Etymology Dictionary</ref> It also exists in Frisian as "pine" which in turn is related to the English verb "to pine" which means to long for. |
|||
* [[Hedonic adaptation]], the tendency to quickly return to a relatively stable level of happiness despite major positive or negative events |
|||
* [[Pain (philosophy)]], the branch of philosophy concerned with suffering and physical pain |
|||
* [[Pain and suffering]], the legal term for the physical and emotional stress caused from an injury |
|||
== Explanatory notes == |
|||
==References== |
|||
{{notelist}} |
|||
{{Reflist|colwidth=30em}} |
|||
== |
== References == |
||
{{Reflist}} |
|||
{{Wikisource1911Enc}} |
|||
* {{cite book | vauthors = Casey K |date=2019 |title=Chasing Pain: The Search for a Neurobiological Mechanism |location=New York |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0190880231 }} |
|||
* {{DMOZ|/Health/Senses/Touch_and_Sensation/Pain}} |
|||
*[http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pain/ Pain] Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy |
|||
== External links == |
|||
{{Commons category}} |
|||
{{Wikiquote}} |
|||
{{wikibooks|Sensory Systems|Physiology of Pain}} |
|||
{{wikibooks|Internal Medicine|Pain}} |
|||
* [https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pain/ "Pain"], ''[[Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy]]'' |
|||
{{Medical resources |
|||
| ICD11 = {{ICD11|MG30}} (chronic), {{ICD11|MG31}} (acute), {{ICD11|8E43.00}} (phantom pain) |
|||
| ICD10 = {{ICD10|R52}} |
|||
| ICD9 = {{ICD9|338}} |
|||
| MedlinePlus = 002164 |
|||
| DiseasesDB = 9503 |
|||
| MeshID = D010146 |
|||
}} |
|||
{{Emotion-footer}} |
{{Emotion-footer}} |
||
{{Neuroscience}} |
{{Neuroscience}} |
||
{{Pain}} |
{{Pain}} |
||
{{Somatosensory system}} |
{{Somatosensory system}} |
||
{{Sensation and perception}} |
|||
{{Sensory system}} |
|||
{{Authority control}} |
|||
{{anchor|Editbox}} |
|||
[[Category:Pain| ]] |
[[Category:Pain| ]] |
||
[[Category:Acute pain]] |
|||
[[Category:Nociception]] |
[[Category:Nociception]] |
||
[[Category:Sensory |
[[Category:Sensory systems]] |
||
[[Category:Greek loanwords]] |
|||
[[Category:Suffering]] |
[[Category:Suffering]] |
||
[[Category:Symptoms and signs]] |
|||
[[ar:ألم]] |
|||
[[be:Боль]] |
|||
[[be-x-old:Боль]] |
|||
[[bg:Болка]] |
|||
[[bs:Bol]] |
|||
[[ca:Dolor]] |
|||
[[cs:Bolest]] |
|||
[[sn:Kurwadziwa]] |
|||
[[da:Smerte]] |
|||
[[de:Schmerz]] |
|||
[[et:Valu]] |
|||
[[el:Πόνος]] |
|||
[[es:Dolor]] |
|||
[[eo:Doloro]] |
|||
[[eu:Min]] |
|||
[[fa:درد]] |
|||
[[fr:Douleur]] |
|||
[[gl:Dor]] |
|||
[[ko:고통]] |
|||
[[hi:दर्द]] |
|||
[[hr:Bol (osjećaj)]] |
|||
[[io:Doloro]] |
|||
[[it:Dolore]] |
|||
[[he:כאב]] |
|||
[[kn:ನೋವು]] |
|||
[[ht:Doulè]] |
|||
[[lv:Sāpes]] |
|||
[[lt:Skausmas]] |
|||
[[hu:Fájdalom]] |
|||
[[ml:വേദന]] |
|||
[[mr:वेदना]] |
|||
[[nl:Pijn]] |
|||
[[ne:दुखाई]] |
|||
[[ja:疼痛]] |
|||
[[no:Smerte]] |
|||
[[nn:Smerte]] |
|||
[[pl:Ból]] |
|||
[[pt:Dor]] |
|||
[[ro:Durere]] |
|||
[[qu:Nanay]] |
|||
[[ru:Боль]] |
|||
[[sq:Dhimbja]] |
|||
[[scn:Dogghia]] |
|||
[[simple:Pain]] |
|||
[[sk:Bolesť]] |
|||
[[sl:Bolečina]] |
|||
[[ckb:ئازار (ھەست)]] |
|||
[[sr:Бол]] |
|||
[[fi:Kipuaisti]] |
|||
[[sv:Smärta]] |
|||
[[tl:Sakit]] |
|||
[[ta:வலி]] |
|||
[[te:నొప్పి]] |
|||
[[th:ความเจ็บปวด]] |
|||
[[uk:Біль]] |
|||
[[vi:Đau]] |
|||
[[yi:ווייטאג]] |
|||
[[zh-yue:痛]] |
|||
[[bat-smg:Skausmos]] |
|||
[[zh:疼痛]] |
Latest revision as of 22:53, 27 November 2024
Pain | |
---|---|
An illustration of wrist pain | |
Specialty | Neurology Pain medicine |
Symptoms | Unpleasant sensory and emotional sensations[1] |
Duration | Typically depends on the cause |
Types | Physical, psychological, psychogenic |
Medication | Analgesic |
Pain is a distressing feeling often caused by intense or damaging stimuli. The International Association for the Study of Pain defines pain as "an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with, or resembling that associated with, actual or potential tissue damage."[1]
Pain motivates organisms to withdraw from damaging situations, to protect a damaged body part while it heals, and to avoid similar experiences in the future.[2] Most pain resolves once the noxious stimulus is removed and the body has healed, but it may persist despite removal of the stimulus and apparent healing of the body. Sometimes pain arises in the absence of any detectable stimulus, damage or disease.[3]
Pain is the most common reason for physician consultation in most developed countries.[4][5] It is a major symptom in many medical conditions, and can interfere with a person's quality of life and general functioning.[6] People in pain experience impaired concentration, working memory, mental flexibility, problem solving and information processing speed, and are more likely to experience irritability, depression, and anxiety.
Simple pain medications are useful in 20% to 70% of cases.[7] Psychological factors such as social support, cognitive behavioral therapy, excitement, or distraction can affect pain's intensity or unpleasantness.[8][9]
Etymology
[edit]First attested in English in 1297, the word peyn comes from the Old French peine, in turn from Latin poena meaning "punishment, penalty"[10][11] (also meaning "torment, hardship, suffering" in Late Latin) and that from Greek ποινή (poine), generally meaning "price paid, penalty, punishment".[12][13]
Classification
[edit]The International Association for the Study of Pain recommends using specific features to describe a patient's pain:
- Region of the body involved (e.g. abdomen, lower limbs)
- System whose dysfunction may be causing the pain (e.g., nervous, gastrointestinal)
- Duration and pattern of occurrence
- Intensity
- Cause[14]
Chronic versus acute
[edit]Pain is usually transitory, lasting only until the noxious stimulus is removed or the underlying damage or pathology has healed. But some painful conditions, such as rheumatoid arthritis, peripheral neuropathy, cancer, and idiopathic pain, may persist for years. Pain that lasts a long time is called "chronic" or "persistent", and pain that resolves quickly is called "acute". Traditionally, the distinction between acute and chronic pain has relied upon an arbitrary interval of time between onset and resolution; the two most commonly used markers being 3 months and 6 months since the onset of pain,[15] though some theorists and researchers have placed the transition from acute to chronic pain at 12 months.[16]: 93 Others apply "acute" to pain that lasts less than 30 days, "chronic" to pain of more than six months' duration, and "subacute" to pain that lasts from one to six months.[17] A popular alternative definition of "chronic pain", involving no arbitrarily fixed duration, is "pain that extends beyond the expected period of healing".[15] Chronic pain may be classified as "cancer-related" or "benign."[17]
Allodynia
[edit]Allodynia is pain experienced in response to a normally painless stimulus.[18] It has no biological function and is classified by characteristics of the stimuli as cold, heat, touch, pressure or a pinprick.[18][19]
Phantom
[edit]Phantom pain is pain felt in a part of the body that has been amputated, or from which the brain no longer receives signals. It is a type of neuropathic pain.[20]
The prevalence of phantom pain in upper limb amputees is nearly 82%, and in lower limb amputees is 54%.[20] One study found that eight days after amputation, 72% of patients had phantom limb pain, and six months later, 67% reported it.[21][22] Some amputees experience continuous pain that varies in intensity or quality; others experience several bouts of pain per day, or it may reoccur less often. It is often described as shooting, crushing, burning or cramping. If the pain is continuous for a long period, parts of the intact body may become sensitized, so that touching them evokes pain in the phantom limb. Phantom limb pain may accompany urination or defecation.[23]: 61–69
Local anesthetic injections into the nerves or sensitive areas of the stump may relieve pain for days, weeks, or sometimes permanently, despite the drug wearing off in a matter of hours; and small injections of hypertonic saline into the soft tissue between vertebrae produces local pain that radiates into the phantom limb for ten minutes or so and may be followed by hours, weeks, or even longer of partial or total relief from phantom pain. Vigorous vibration or electrical stimulation of the stump, or current from electrodes surgically implanted onto the spinal cord, all produce relief in some patients.[23]: 61–69
Mirror box therapy produces the illusion of movement and touch in a phantom limb which in turn may cause a reduction in pain.[24]
Paraplegia, the loss of sensation and voluntary motor control after serious spinal cord damage, may be accompanied by girdle pain at the level of the spinal cord damage, visceral pain evoked by a filling bladder or bowel, or, in five to ten percent of paraplegics, phantom body pain in areas of complete sensory loss. This phantom body pain is initially described as burning or tingling but may evolve into severe crushing or pinching pain, or the sensation of fire running down the legs or of a knife twisting in the flesh. Onset may be immediate or may not occur until years after the disabling injury. Surgical treatment rarely provides lasting relief.[23]: 61–69
Breakthrough
[edit]Breakthrough pain is transitory pain that comes on suddenly and is not alleviated by the patient's regular pain management. It is common in cancer patients who often have background pain that is generally well-controlled by medications, but who also sometimes experience bouts of severe pain that from time to time "breaks through" the medication. The characteristics of breakthrough cancer pain vary from person to person and according to the cause. Management of breakthrough pain can entail intensive use of opioids, including fentanyl.[25][26]
Asymbolia and insensitivity
[edit]The ability to experience pain is essential for protection from injury, and recognition of the presence of injury. Episodic analgesia may occur under special circumstances, such as in the excitement of sport or war: a soldier on the battlefield may feel no pain for many hours from a traumatic amputation or other severe injury.[27]
Although unpleasantness is an essential part of the IASP definition of pain,[28] it is possible in some patients to induce a state known as pain asymbolia, described as intense pain devoid of unpleasantness, with morphine injection or psychosurgery.[29] Such patients report that they have pain but are not bothered by it; they recognize the sensation of pain but suffer little, or not at all.[30] Indifference to pain can also rarely be present from birth; these people have normal nerves on medical investigations, and find pain unpleasant, but do not avoid repetition of the pain stimulus.[31]
Insensitivity to pain may also result from abnormalities in the nervous system. This is usually the result of acquired damage to the nerves, such as spinal cord injury, diabetes mellitus (diabetic neuropathy), or leprosy in countries where that disease is prevalent.[32] These individuals are at risk of tissue damage and infection due to undiscovered injuries. People with diabetes-related nerve damage, for instance, sustain poorly-healing foot ulcers as a result of decreased sensation.[33]
A much smaller number of people are insensitive to pain due to an inborn abnormality of the nervous system, known as "congenital insensitivity to pain".[31] Children with this condition incur carelessly-repeated damage to their tongues, eyes, joints, skin, and muscles. Some die before adulthood, and others have a reduced life expectancy.[34] Most people with congenital insensitivity to pain have one of five hereditary sensory and autonomic neuropathies (which includes familial dysautonomia and congenital insensitivity to pain with anhidrosis).[35] These conditions feature decreased sensitivity to pain together with other neurological abnormalities, particularly of the autonomic nervous system.[31][35] A very rare syndrome with isolated congenital insensitivity to pain has been linked with mutations in the SCN9A gene, which codes for a sodium channel (Nav1.7) necessary in conducting pain nerve stimuli.[36]
Functional effects
[edit]Experimental subjects challenged by acute pain and patients in chronic pain experience impairments in attention control, working memory capacity, mental flexibility, problem solving, and information processing speed.[37] Pain is also associated with increased depression, anxiety, fear, and anger.[38]
If I have matters right, the consequences of pain will include direct physical distress, unemployment, financial difficulties, marital disharmony, and difficulties in concentration and attention…
— Harold Merskey 2000[39]
On subsequent negative emotion
[edit]Although pain is considered to be aversive and unpleasant and is therefore usually avoided, a meta-analysis which summarized and evaluated numerous studies from various psychological disciplines, found a reduction in negative affect. Across studies, participants that were subjected to acute physical pain in the laboratory subsequently reported feeling better than those in non-painful control conditions, a finding which was also reflected in physiological parameters.[40] A potential mechanism to explain this effect is provided by the opponent-process theory.
Theory
[edit]Historical
[edit]Before the relatively recent discovery of neurons and their role in pain, various body functions were proposed to account for pain. There were several competing early theories of pain among the ancient Greeks: Hippocrates believed that it was due to an imbalance in vital fluids.[41] In the 11th century, Avicenna theorized that there were a number of feeling senses, including touch, pain, and titillation.[42]
In 1644, René Descartes theorized that pain was a disturbance that passed along nerve fibers until the disturbance reached the brain.[41][43] The work of Descartes and Avicenna prefigured the 19th-century development of specificity theory. Specificity theory saw pain as "a specific sensation, with its own sensory apparatus independent of touch and other senses".[44] Another theory that came to prominence in the 18th and 19th centuries was intensive theory, which conceived of pain not as a unique sensory modality, but an emotional state produced by stronger than normal stimuli such as intense light, pressure or temperature.[45] By the mid-1890s, specificity was backed primarily by physiologists and physicians, and psychologists mostly backed the intensive theory. However, after a series of clinical observations by Henry Head and experiments by Max von Frey, the psychologists migrated to specificity almost en masse. By the century's end, most physiology and psychology textbooks presented pain specificity as fact.[42][44]
Modern
[edit]Some sensory fibers do not differentiate between noxious and non-noxious stimuli, while others (i.e., nociceptors) respond only to noxious, high-intensity stimuli. At the peripheral end of the nociceptor, noxious stimuli generate currents that, above a given threshold, send signals along the nerve fiber to the spinal cord. The "specificity" (whether it responds to thermal, chemical, or mechanical features of its environment) of a nociceptor is determined by which ion channels it expresses at its peripheral end. So far, dozens of types of nociceptor ion channels have been identified, and their exact functions are still being determined.[46]
The pain signal travels from the periphery to the spinal cord along A-delta and C fibers. Because the A-delta fiber is thicker than the C fiber, and is thinly sheathed in an electrically insulating material (myelin), it carries its signal faster (5–30 m/s) than the unmyelinated C fiber (0.5–2 m/s).[47] Pain evoked by the A-delta fibers is described as sharp and is felt first. This is followed by a duller pain—often described as burning—carried by the C fibers.[48] These A-delta and C fibers enter the spinal cord via Lissauer's tract and connect with spinal cord nerve fibers in the central gelatinous substance of the spinal cord. These spinal cord fibers then cross the cord via the anterior white commissure and ascend in the spinothalamic tract. Before reaching the brain, the spinothalamic tract splits into the lateral, neospinothalamic tract and the medial, paleospinothalamic tract. The neospinothalamic tract carries the fast, sharp A-delta signal to the ventral posterolateral nucleus of the thalamus. The paleospinothalamic tract carries the slow, dull C fiber pain signal. Some of the paleospinothalamic fibers peel off in the brain stem—connecting with the reticular formation or midbrain periaqueductal gray—and the remainder terminate in the intralaminar nuclei of the thalamus.[49]
Pain-related activity in the thalamus spreads to the insular cortex (thought to embody, among other things, the feeling that distinguishes pain from other homeostatic emotions such as itch and nausea) and anterior cingulate cortex (thought to embody, among other things, the affective/motivational element, the unpleasantness of pain),[50] and pain that is distinctly located also activates the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex.[51]
Spinal cord fibers dedicated to carrying A-delta fiber pain signals and others that carry both A-delta and C fiber pain signals to the thalamus have been identified. Other spinal cord fibers, known as wide dynamic range neurons, respond to A-delta and C fibers and the much larger, more heavily myelinated A-beta fibers that carry touch, pressure, and vibration signals.[47]
Ronald Melzack and Patrick Wall introduced their gate control theory in the 1965 Science article "Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory".[52] The authors proposed that the thin C and A-delta (pain) and large diameter A-beta (touch, pressure, vibration) nerve fibers carry information from the site of injury to two destinations in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord, and that A-beta fiber signals acting on inhibitory cells in the dorsal horn can reduce the intensity of pain signals sent to the brain.[43]
Three dimensions of pain
[edit]In 1968, Ronald Melzack and Kenneth Casey described chronic pain in terms of its three dimensions:
- "sensory-discriminative" (sense of the intensity, location, quality, and duration of the pain),
- "affective-motivational" (unpleasantness and urge to escape the unpleasantness) and
- "cognitive-evaluative" (cognitions such as appraisal, cultural values, distraction, and hypnotic suggestion).
They theorized that pain intensity (the sensory discriminative dimension) and unpleasantness (the affective-motivational dimension) are not simply determined by the magnitude of the painful stimulus, but "higher" cognitive activities can influence perceived intensity and unpleasantness. Cognitive activities may affect both sensory and affective experience, or they may modify primarily the affective-motivational dimension. Thus, excitement in games or war appears to block both the sensory-discriminative and affective-motivational dimensions of pain, while suggestion and placebos may modulate only the affective-motivational dimension and leave the sensory-discriminative dimension relatively undisturbed.[53] (p. 432)
The paper ends with a call to action: "Pain can be treated not only by trying to cut down the sensory input by anesthetic block, surgical intervention and the like, but also by influencing the motivational-affective and cognitive factors as well."[53] (p. 435)
Evolutionary and behavioral role
[edit]Pain is part of the body's defense system, producing a reflexive retraction from the painful stimulus, and tendencies to protect the affected body part while it heals, and avoid that harmful situation in the future.[54][55] It is an important part of animal life, vital to healthy survival. People with congenital insensitivity to pain have reduced life expectancy.[31]
In The Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution, biologist Richard Dawkins addresses the question of why pain should have the quality of being painful. He describes the alternative as a mental raising of a "red flag". To argue why that red flag might be insufficient, Dawkins argues that drives must compete with one another within living beings. The most "fit" creature would be the one whose pains are well balanced. Those pains which mean certain death when ignored will become the most powerfully felt. The relative intensities of pain, then, may resemble the relative importance of that risk to our ancestors.[a] This resemblance will not be perfect, however, because natural selection can be a poor designer. This may have maladaptive results such as supernormal stimuli.[56]
Pain, however, does not only wave a "red flag" within living beings but may also act as a warning sign and a call for help to other living beings. Especially in humans who readily helped each other in case of sickness or injury throughout their evolutionary history, pain might be shaped by natural selection to be a credible and convincing signal of the need for relief, help, and care.[57]
Idiopathic pain (pain that persists after the trauma or pathology has healed, or that arises without any apparent cause) may be an exception to the idea that pain is helpful to survival, although some psychodynamic psychologists argue that such pain is psychogenic, enlisted as a protective distraction to keep dangerous emotions unconscious.[58]
Thresholds
[edit]In pain science, thresholds are measured by gradually increasing the intensity of a stimulus in a procedure called quantitative sensory testing which involves such stimuli as electric current, thermal (heat or cold), mechanical (pressure, touch, vibration), ischemic, or chemical stimuli applied to the subject to evoke a response.[59] The "pain perception threshold" is the point at which the subject begins to feel pain, and the "pain threshold intensity" is the stimulus intensity at which the stimulus begins to hurt. The "pain tolerance threshold" is reached when the subject acts to stop the pain.[59]
Assessment
[edit]A person's self-report is the most reliable measure of pain.[60][61][62] Some health care professionals may underestimate pain severity.[63] A definition of pain widely employed in nursing, emphasizing its subjective nature and the importance of believing patient reports, was introduced by Margo McCaffery in 1968: "Pain is whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever he says it does".[64] To assess intensity, the patient may be asked to locate their pain on a scale of 0 to 10, with 0 being no pain at all, and 10 the worst pain they have ever felt. Quality can be established by having the patient complete the McGill Pain Questionnaire indicating which words best describe their pain.[6]
Visual analogue scale
[edit]The visual analogue scale is a common, reproducible tool in the assessment of pain and pain relief.[65] The scale is a continuous line anchored by verbal descriptors, one for each extreme of pain where a higher score indicates greater pain intensity. It is usually 10 cm in length with no intermediate descriptors as to avoid marking of scores around a preferred numeric value. When applied as a pain descriptor, these anchors are often 'no pain' and 'worst imaginable pain". Cut-offs for pain classification have been recommended as no pain (0–4mm), mild pain (5–44mm), moderate pain (45–74mm) and severe pain (75–100mm).[66][check quotation syntax]
Multidimensional pain inventory
[edit]The Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI) is a questionnaire designed to assess the psychosocial state of a person with chronic pain. Combining the MPI characterization of the person with their IASP five-category pain profile is recommended for deriving the most useful case description.[15]
Assessment in non-verbal people
[edit]Non-verbal people cannot use words to tell others that they are experiencing pain. However, they may be able to communicate through other means, such as blinking, pointing, or nodding.[67]
With a non-communicative person, observation becomes critical, and specific behaviors can be monitored as pain indicators. Behaviors such as facial grimacing and guarding (trying to protect part of the body from being bumped or touched) indicate pain, as well as an increase or decrease in vocalizations, changes in routine behavior patterns and mental status changes. Patients experiencing pain may exhibit withdrawn social behavior and possibly experience a decreased appetite and decreased nutritional intake. A change in condition that deviates from baseline, such as moaning with movement or when manipulating a body part, and limited range of motion are also potential pain indicators. In patients who possess language but are incapable of expressing themselves effectively, such as those with dementia, an increase in confusion or display of aggressive behaviors or agitation may signal that discomfort exists, and further assessment is necessary. Changes in behavior may be noticed by caregivers who are familiar with the person's normal behavior.[67]
Infants do feel pain, but lack the language needed to report it, and so communicate distress by crying. A non-verbal pain assessment should be conducted involving the parents, who will notice changes in the infant which may not be obvious to the health care provider. Pre-term babies are more sensitive to painful stimuli than those carried to full term.[68]
Another approach, when pain is suspected, is to give the person treatment for pain, and then watch to see whether the suspected indicators of pain subside.[67]
Other reporting barriers
[edit]The way in which one experiences and responds to pain is related to sociocultural characteristics, such as gender, ethnicity, and age.[69][70] An aging adult may not respond to pain in the same way that a younger person might. Their ability to recognize pain may be blunted by illness or the use of medication. Depression may also keep older adult from reporting they are in pain. Decline in self-care may also indicate the older adult is experiencing pain. They may be reluctant to report pain because they do not want to be perceived as weak, or may feel it is impolite or shameful to complain, or they may feel the pain is a form of deserved punishment.[71][72]
Cultural barriers may also affect the likelihood of reporting pain. Patients may feel that certain treatments go against their religious beliefs. They may not report pain because they feel it is a sign that death is near. Many people fear the stigma of addiction, and avoid pain treatment so as not to be prescribed potentially addicting drugs. Many Asians do not want to lose respect in society by admitting they are in pain and need help, believing the pain should be borne in silence, while other cultures feel they should report pain immediately to receive immediate relief.[68]
Gender can also be a perceived factor in reporting pain. Gender differences can be the result of social and cultural expectations, with, in some cultures, women expected to be more emotional and show pain, and men to be more stoic.[68] As a result, female pain may be at a higher risk of being stigmatized, leading to less urgent treatment of women based on social expectations of their ability to accurately report it.[73] This has been postulated to lead to extended emergency room wait times for women and frequent dismissal of their ability to accurately report pain.[74][75]
Diagnostic aid
[edit]Pain is a symptom of many medical conditions. Knowing the time of onset, location, intensity, pattern of occurrence (continuous, intermittent, etc.), exacerbating and relieving factors, and quality (burning, sharp, etc.) of the pain will help the examining physician to accurately diagnose the problem. For example, chest pain described as extreme heaviness may indicate myocardial infarction, while chest pain described as tearing may indicate aortic dissection.[76][77]
Physiological measurement
[edit]Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain scanning has been used to measure pain, and correlates well with self-reported pain.[78][79][80]
Mechanisms
[edit]Nociceptive
[edit]Nociceptive pain is caused by stimulation of sensory nerve fibers that respond to stimuli approaching or exceeding harmful intensity (nociceptors), and may be classified according to the mode of noxious stimulation. The most common categories are "thermal" (e.g. heat or cold), "mechanical" (e.g. crushing, tearing, shearing, etc.) and "chemical" (e.g. iodine in a cut or chemicals released during inflammation). Some nociceptors respond to more than one of these modalities and are consequently designated polymodal.
Nociceptive pain may also be classed according to the site of origin and divided into "visceral", "deep somatic" and "superficial somatic" pain. Visceral structures (e.g., the heart, liver and intestines) are highly sensitive to stretch, ischemia and inflammation, but relatively insensitive to other stimuli that normally evoke pain in other structures, such as burning and cutting. Visceral pain is diffuse, difficult to locate and often referred to a distant, usually superficial, structure. It may be accompanied by nausea and vomiting and may be described as sickening, deep, squeezing, and dull.[81] Deep somatic pain is initiated by stimulation of nociceptors in ligaments, tendons, bones, blood vessels, fasciae and muscles, and is dull, aching, poorly-localized pain. Examples include sprains and broken bones. Superficial somatic pain is initiated by activation of nociceptors in the skin or other superficial tissue, and is sharp, well-defined and clearly located. Examples of injuries that produce superficial somatic pain include minor wounds and minor (first degree) burns.[16]
Neuropathic
[edit]Neuropathic pain is caused by damage or disease affecting any part of the nervous system involved in bodily feelings (the somatosensory system).[82] Neuropathic pain may be divided into peripheral, central, or mixed (peripheral and central) neuropathic pain. Peripheral neuropathic pain is often described as "burning", "tingling", "electrical", "stabbing", or "pins and needles".[83] Bumping the "funny bone" elicits acute peripheral neuropathic pain.
Some manifestations of neuropathic pain include: traumatic neuropathy, tic douloureux, painful diabetic neuropathy, and postherpetic neuralgia.[84]
Nociplastic
[edit]Nociplastic pain is pain characterized by a changed nociception (but without evidence of real or threatened tissue damage, or without disease or damage in the somatosensory system).[9]
Psychogenic
[edit]Psychogenic pain, also called psychalgia or somatoform pain, is pain caused, increased or prolonged by mental, emotional or behavioral factors.[85] Headaches, back pain and stomach pain are sometimes diagnosed as psychogenic.[85] Those affected are often stigmatized, because both medical professionals and the general public tend to think that pain from a psychological source is not "real". However, specialists consider that it is no less actual or hurtful than pain from any other source.[29]
People with long-term pain frequently display psychological disturbance, with elevated scores on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory scales of hysteria, depression and hypochondriasis (the "neurotic triad"). Some investigators have argued that it is this neuroticism that causes acute pain to turn chronic, but clinical evidence points in the other direction, to chronic pain causing neuroticism. When long-term pain is relieved by therapeutic intervention, scores on the neurotic triad and anxiety fall, often to normal levels. Self-esteem, often low in chronic pain patients, also shows improvement once pain has resolved.[23]: 31–32
Management
[edit]Pain can be treated through a variety of methods. The most appropriate method depends upon the situation. Management of chronic pain can be difficult and may require the coordinated efforts of a pain management team, which typically includes medical practitioners, clinical pharmacists, clinical psychologists, physiotherapists, occupational therapists, physician assistants, and nurse practitioners.[86]
Inadequate treatment of pain is widespread throughout surgical wards, intensive care units, and accident and emergency departments, in general practice, in the management of all forms of chronic pain including cancer pain, and in end of life care.[87][88][89][90][91][92][93] This neglect extends to all ages, from newborns to medically frail elderly.[94][95] In the US, African and Hispanic Americans are more likely than others to suffer unnecessarily while in the care of a physician;[96][97] and women's pain is more likely to be undertreated than men's.[98]
The International Association for the Study of Pain advocates that the relief of pain should be recognized as a human right, that chronic pain should be considered a disease in its own right, and that pain medicine should have the full status of a medical specialty.[99] It is a specialty only in China and Australia at this time.[100] Elsewhere, pain medicine is a subspecialty under disciplines such as anesthesiology, physiatry, neurology, palliative medicine and psychiatry.[101] In 2011, Human Rights Watch alerted that tens of millions of people worldwide are still denied access to inexpensive medications for severe pain.[102]
Medication
[edit]Acute pain is usually managed with medications such as analgesics and anesthetics.[103] Caffeine when added to pain medications such as ibuprofen, may provide some additional benefit.[104][105] Ketamine can be used instead of opioids for short-term pain.[106] Pain medications can cause paradoxical side effects, such as opioid-induced hyperalgesia (severe generalized pain caused by long-term opioid use).[107][108]
Sugar (sucrose) when taken by mouth reduces pain in newborn babies undergoing some medical procedures (a lancing of the heel, venipuncture, and intramuscular injections). Sugar does not remove pain from circumcision, and it is unknown if sugar reduces pain for other procedures.[109] Sugar did not affect pain-related electrical activity in the brains of newborns one second after the heel lance procedure.[110] Sweet liquid by mouth moderately reduces the rate and duration of crying caused by immunization injection in children between one and twelve months of age.[111]
Psychological
[edit]Individuals with more social support experience less cancer pain, take less pain medication, report less labor pain and are less likely to use epidural anesthesia during childbirth, or suffer from chest pain after coronary artery bypass surgery.[8]
Suggestion can significantly affect pain intensity. About 35% of people report marked relief after receiving a saline injection they believed to be morphine. This placebo effect is more pronounced in people who are prone to anxiety, and so anxiety reduction may account for some of the effect, but it does not account for all of it. Placebos are more effective for intense pain than mild pain; and they produce progressively weaker effects with repeated administration.[23]: 26–28 It is possible for many with chronic pain to become so absorbed in an activity or entertainment that the pain is no longer felt, or is greatly diminished.[23]: 22–23
A number of meta-analyses have found clinical hypnosis to be effective in controlling pain associated with diagnostic and surgical procedures in both adults and children, as well as pain associated with cancer and childbirth.[112] A 2007 review of 13 studies found evidence for the efficacy of hypnosis in the reduction of chronic pain under some conditions, though the number of patients enrolled in the studies was low, raising issues related to the statistical power to detect group differences, and most lacked credible controls for placebo or expectation. The authors concluded that "although the findings provide support for the general applicability of hypnosis in the treatment of chronic pain, considerably more research will be needed to fully determine the effects of hypnosis for different chronic-pain conditions."[113]
Alternative medicine
[edit]An analysis of the 13 highest quality studies of pain treatment with acupuncture, published in January 2009, concluded there was little difference in the effect of real, fake and no acupuncture.[114] However, more recent reviews have found some benefit.[115][116][117]
Additionally, there is tentative evidence for a few herbal medicines.[118]
For chronic (long-term) lower back pain, spinal manipulation produces tiny, clinically insignificant, short-term improvements in pain and function, compared with sham therapy and other interventions.[119] Spinal manipulation produces the same outcome as other treatments, such as general practitioner care, pain-relief drugs, physical therapy, and exercise, for acute (short-term) lower back pain.[119]
There has been some interest in the relationship between vitamin D and pain, but the evidence so far from controlled trials for such a relationship, other than in osteomalacia, is inconclusive.[120]
The International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) says that due to a lack of evidence from high quality research, it does not endorse the general use of cannabinoids to treat pain.[121]
Epidemiology
[edit]Pain is the main reason for visiting an emergency department in more than 50% of cases,[122] and is present in 30% of family practice visits.[123] Several epidemiological studies have reported widely varying prevalence rates for chronic pain, ranging from 12 to 80% of the population.[124] It becomes more common as people approach death. A study of 4,703 patients found that 26% had pain in the last two years of life, increasing to 46% in the last month.[125]
A survey of 6,636 children (0–18 years of age) found that, of the 5,424 respondents, 54% had experienced pain in the preceding three months. A quarter reported having experienced recurrent or continuous pain for three months or more, and a third of these reported frequent and intense pain. The intensity of chronic pain was higher for girls, and girls' reports of chronic pain increased markedly between ages 12 and 14.[126]
Society and culture
[edit]Physical pain is a universal experience, and a strong motivator of human and animal behavior. As such, physical pain is used politically in relation to various issues such as pain management policy, drug control, animal rights or animal welfare, torture, and pain compliance. The deliberate infliction of pain and the medical management of pain are both important aspects of biopower, a concept that encompasses the "set of mechanisms through which the basic biological features of the human species became the object of a political strategy".[127]
In various contexts, the deliberate infliction of pain in the form of corporal punishment is used as retribution for an offence, for the purpose of disciplining or reforming a wrongdoer, or to deter attitudes or behaviour deemed unacceptable. In Western societies, the intentional infliction of severe pain (torture) was principally used to extract confession prior to its abolition in the latter part of the 19th century. Torture as a means to punish the citizen has been reserved for offences posing a severe threat to the social fabric (for example, treason).[128]
The administration of torture on bodies othered by the cultural narrative, those observed as not 'full members of society' [128]: 101–121[AD1] met a resurgence in the 20th century, possibly due to the heightened warfare.[128]: 101–121 [AD2]
Many cultures use painful ritual practices as a catalyst for psychological transformation.[129] The use of pain to transition to a 'cleansed and purified' state is seen in religious self-flagellation practices (particularly those of Christianity and Islam), or personal catharsis in neo-primitive body suspension experiences.[130]
Beliefs about pain play an important role in sporting cultures. Pain may be viewed positively, exemplified by the 'no pain, no gain' attitude, with pain seen as an essential part of training. Sporting culture tends to normalise experiences of pain and injury and celebrate athletes who 'play hurt'.[131]
Pain has psychological, social, and physical dimensions, and is greatly influenced by cultural factors.[132]
Non-humans
[edit]René Descartes argued that animals lack consciousness and therefore do not experience pain and suffering in the way that humans do.[133] Bernard Rollin of Colorado State University, the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals,[b] wrote that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain, and that veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were simply taught to ignore animal pain.[135][136] The ability of invertebrate species of animals, such as insects, to feel pain and suffering is unclear.[137][138][139]
Specialists believe that all vertebrates can feel pain, and that certain invertebrates, like the octopus, may also.[137][140][141] The presence of pain in animals is unknown, but can be inferred through physical and behavioral reactions,[142] such as paw withdrawal from various noxious mechanical stimuli in rodents.[143]
While plants, as living beings, can perceive and communicate physical stimuli and damage, they do not feel pain simply because of the lack of any pain receptors, nerves, or a brain,[144] and, by extension, a lack of consciousness.[145] Many plants are known to perceive and respond to mechanical stimuli at a cellular level, and some plants such as the venus flytrap or touch-me-not, are known for their "obvious sensory abilities".[144] Nevertheless, no member of the plant kingdom does feel pain notwithstanding their abilities to respond to sunlight, gravity, wind, and any external stimuli such as insect bites since they lack any nervous system. The primary reason for this is that, unlike the members of the animal kingdom whose evolutionary successes and failures are shaped by suffering, the evolution of plants are simply shaped by life and death.[144]
See also
[edit]- Feeling, a perceptual state of conscious experience.
- Hedonic adaptation, the tendency to quickly return to a relatively stable level of happiness despite major positive or negative events
- Pain (philosophy), the branch of philosophy concerned with suffering and physical pain
- Pain and suffering, the legal term for the physical and emotional stress caused from an injury
Explanatory notes
[edit]- ^ For example, lack of food, extreme cold, or serious injuries are felt as exceptionally painful, whereas minor damage is felt as mere discomfort.
- ^ Rollin drafted the 1985 Health Research Extension Act and an animal welfare amendment to the 1985 Food Security Act.[134]
References
[edit]- ^ a b Raja SN, Carr DB, Cohen M, Finnerup NB, Flor H, Gibson S, et al. (September 2020). "The revised International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises". Pain. 161 (9): 1976–1982. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001939. PMC 7680716. PMID 32694387.
- ^ Cervero F (2012). Understanding Pain : Exploring the Perception of Pain. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. Chapter 1. ISBN 9780262305433. OCLC 809043366.
- ^ Raj PP (2007). "Taxonomy and classification of pain". In: The Handbook of Chronic Pain. Nova Biomedical Books. ISBN 9781600210440. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 3 February 2016.
- ^ Debono DJ, Hoeksema LJ, Hobbs RD (August 2013). "Caring for patients with chronic pain: pearls and pitfalls". The Journal of the American Osteopathic Association. 113 (8): 620–7. doi:10.7556/jaoa.2013.023. PMID 23918913.
- ^ Turk DC, Dworkin RH (2004). "What should be the core outcomes in chronic pain clinical trials?". Arthritis Research & Therapy. 6 (4): 151–4. doi:10.1186/ar1196. PMC 464897. PMID 15225358.
- ^ a b Breivik H, Borchgrevink PC, Allen SM, Rosseland LA, Romundstad L, Hals EK, Kvarstein G, Stubhaug A (July 2008). "Assessment of pain". British Journal of Anaesthesia. 101 (1): 17–24. doi:10.1093/bja/aen103. PMID 18487245.
- ^ Moore RA, Wiffen PJ, Derry S, Maguire T, Roy YM, Tyrrell L (November 2015). "Non-prescription (OTC) oral analgesics for acute pain – an overview of Cochrane reviews". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 11 (11): CD010794. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD010794.pub2. PMC 6485506. PMID 26544675.
- ^ a b Eisenberger NI, Lieberman M (2005). "Why it hurts to be left out: The neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain". In Williams KD (ed.). The Social Outcast: Ostracism, Social Exclusion, Rejection, & Bullying (Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology). East Sussex: Psychology Press. p. 210. ISBN 9781841694245.
- ^ a b Garland EL, Brintz CE, Hanley AW, Roseen EJ, Atchley RM, Gaylord SA, et al. (January 2020). "Mind-Body Therapies for Opioid-Treated Pain: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis". JAMA Internal Medicine. 180 (1): 91–105. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2019.4917. PMC 6830441. PMID 31682676.
- ^ Lewis CT, Short C. "Poena". A Latin Dictionary. Archived from the original on 13 May 2011 – via Perseus Digital Library.
- ^ Lavoie, Anne; Toledo, Paloma (1 September 2013). "Multimodal Postcesarean Delivery Analgesia". Clinics in Perinatology. Pain Management in the Peripartum Period. 40 (3): 443–455. doi:10.1016/j.clp.2013.05.008. ISSN 0095-5108. PMID 23972750.
- ^ Liddell HG, Scott R. "ποινή". A Greek-English Lexicon. Archived from the original on 13 May 2011 – via Perseus Digital Library.
- ^ "Pain". Archived from the original on 28 July 2011 – via Online Etymology Dictionary.
- ^ Merskey H, Bogduk N (1994). Classification of Chronic Pain (2 nd ed.). Seattle: International Association for the Study of Pain. pp. 3 & 4. ISBN 978-0931092053.
- ^ a b c Turk DC, Okifuji A (2001). "Pain terms and taxonomies of pain". In Bonica JJ, Loeser JD, Chapman CR, Turk DC (eds.). Bonica's management of pain. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-0781768276.
- ^ a b Coda BA, Bonica JJ (2000). "General considerations of acute pain". In Panswick CC, Main CJ (eds.). Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. ISBN 978-0443056833.
- ^ a b Thienhaus O, Cole BE (2002). "Classification of pain". In Weiner R (ed.). Pain management: a practical guide for clinicians. Boca Raton: CRC Press. pp. 28. ISBN 978-0849322624.
- ^ a b Jensen TS, Finnerup NB (September 2014). "Allodynia and hyperalgesia in neuropathic pain: clinical manifestations and mechanisms". The Lancet. Neurology. 13 (9): 924–935. doi:10.1016/s1474-4422(14)70102-4. PMID 25142459. S2CID 25011309.
- ^ Lolignier S, Eijkelkamp N, Wood JN (January 2015). "Mechanical allodynia". Pflügers Archiv. 467 (1): 133–139. doi:10.1007/s00424-014-1532-0. PMC 4281368. PMID 24846747.
- ^ a b Kooijman CM, Dijkstra PU, Geertzen JH, Elzinga A, van der Schans CP (July 2000). "Phantom pain and phantom sensations in upper limb amputees: an epidemiological study". Pain. 87 (1): 33–41. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00264-5. PMID 10863043. S2CID 7565030.[permanent dead link ]
- ^ Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P (November 1983). "Phantom limb, phantom pain and stump pain in amputees during the first 6 months following limb amputation". Pain. 17 (3): 243–256. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(83)90097-0. PMID 6657285. S2CID 10304696.
- ^ Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P (March 1985). "Immediate and long-term phantom limb pain in amputees: incidence, clinical characteristics and relationship to pre-amputation limb pain". Pain. 21 (3): 267–278. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(85)90090-9. PMID 3991231. S2CID 24358789.
- ^ a b c d e f Wall PD, Melzack R (1996). The challenge of pain (2nd ed.). New York: Penguin Books. ISBN 978-0140256703.
- ^ Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D (April 1996). "Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 263 (1369): 377–386. Bibcode:1996RSPSB.263..377R. doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0058. PMID 8637922. S2CID 4819370.
- ^ Mishra S, Bhatnagar S, Chaudhary P, Rana SP (January 2009). "Breakthrough cancer pain: review of prevalence, characteristics and management". Indian Journal of Palliative Care. 15 (1): 14–18. doi:10.4103/0973-1075.53506. PMC 2886208. PMID 20606850.
- ^ Caraceni A, Hanks G, Kaasa S, Bennett MI, Brunelli C, Cherny N, Dale O, De Conno F, Fallon M, Hanna M, Haugen DF, Juhl G, King S, Klepstad P, Laugsand EA, Maltoni M, Mercadante S, Nabal M, Pigni A, Radbruch L, Reid C, Sjogren P, Stone PC, Tassinari D, Zeppetella G (February 2012). "Use of opioid analgesics in the treatment of cancer pain: evidence-based recommendations from the EAPC" (PDF). The Lancet. Oncology. 13 (2): e58–68. doi:10.1016/S1470-2045(12)70040-2. PMID 22300860. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 October 2014. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
- ^ Beecher HK (1959). Measurement of subjective responses. New York: Oxford University Press. cited in Melzack R, Wall PD (1996). The challenge of pain (2nd ed.). London: Penguin. p. 7. ISBN 978-0140256703.
- ^ "International Association for the Study of Pain: Pain Definitions". Archived from the original on 13 January 2015. Retrieved 12 January 2015.
Pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage
Alt URL[permanent dead link ] Derived from Bonica JJ (June 1979). "The need of a taxonomy". Pain. 6 (3): 247–248. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(79)90046-0. PMID 460931. S2CID 53161389. - ^ a b "International Association for the Study of Pain | Pain Definitions".. Retrieved 12 October 2010.
- ^ Grahek N (2007). Feeling pain and being in pain (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0262517324. Archived from the original on 27 September 2008.
- ^ a b c d Nagasako EM, Oaklander AL, Dworkin RH (February 2003). "Congenital insensitivity to pain: an update". Pain. 101 (3): 213–219. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00482-7. PMID 12583863. S2CID 206055264.
- ^ Brand PW, Yancey P (1997). The gift of pain: why we hurt & what we can do about it. Grand Rapids, Mich: Zondervan Publ. ISBN 978-0310221449.
- ^ Lipsky BA, Berendt AR, Deery HG, Embil JM, Joseph WS, Karchmer AW, LeFrock JL, Lew DP, Mader JT, Norden C, Tan JS (October 2004). "Diagnosis and treatment of diabetic foot infections". Clinical Infectious Diseases. 39 (7): 885–910. doi:10.1086/424846. PMID 15472838.
- ^ Raina*, Triveni; Dash, Bhagya Ranjan. "AN INTRODUCTORY APPROACH TO PAIN MANAGEMENT THROUGH AYURVEDA WITH BRIEF HOLISTIC REVIEW".
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ a b Axelrod FB, Hilz MJ (December 2003). "Inherited autonomic neuropathies". Seminars in Neurology. 23 (4): 381–390. doi:10.1055/s-2004-817722. PMID 15088259. S2CID 260317729.
- ^ Raouf R, Quick K, Wood JN (November 2010). "Pain as a channelopathy". The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 120 (11): 3745–3752. doi:10.1172/JCI43158. PMC 2965577. PMID 21041956.
- ^ Hart RP, Wade JB, Martelli MF (April 2003). "Cognitive impairment in patients with chronic pain: the significance of stress". Current Pain and Headache Reports. 7 (2): 116–126. doi:10.1007/s11916-003-0021-5. PMID 12628053. S2CID 14104974.
- ^ Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Chont M (March 2009). "Pain-related effects of trait anger expression: neural substrates and the role of endogenous opioid mechanisms". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 33 (3): 475–491. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.12.003. PMC 2756489. PMID 19146872.
- ^ Merskey H (2000). "The History of Psychoanalytic Ideas Concerning Pain". In Weisberg JN, Gatchel RJ (eds.). Personality Characteristics of Patients With Pain. American Psychological Association (APA). ISBN 978-1557986467.
- ^ Bresin K, Kling L, Verona E (2018). "The effect of acute physical pain on subsequent negative emotional affect: A meta-analysis". Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 9 (3): 273–283. doi:10.1037/per0000248. PMC 5624817. PMID 28368146.
- ^ a b Linton. Models of Pain Perception. Elsevier Health, 2005. Print.
- ^ a b Dallenbach KM (July 1939). "Pain: History and present status". American Journal of Psychology. 52 (3): 331–347. doi:10.2307/1416740. JSTOR 1416740.
- ^ a b Melzack R, Katz J (2004). "The Gate Control Theory: Reaching for the Brain". In Craig KD, Hadjistavropoulos T (eds.). Pain: psychological perspectives. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. ISBN 978-0415650618.
- ^ a b Bonica JJ (1990). "History of pain concepts and therapies". The management of pain. Vol. 1 (2 ed.). London: Lea & Febiger. p. 7. ISBN 978-0812111224.
- ^ Finger S (2001). Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. US: Oxford University Press. p. 149. ISBN 978-0195146943.
- ^ Woolf CJ, Ma Q (August 2007). "Nociceptors – noxious stimulus detectors". Neuron. 55 (3): 353–364. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.016. PMID 17678850. S2CID 13576368.
- ^ a b Marchand S (2010). "Applied pain neurophysiology". In Beaulieu P, Lussier D, Porreca F, Dickenson A (eds.). Pharmacology of pain. Seattle: International Association for the Study of Pain Press. pp. 3–26. ISBN 978-0931092787.
- ^ Skevington S (1995). Psychology of pain. New York: Wiley. p. 9. ISBN 978-0471957737.
- ^ Skevington SM (1995). Psychology of pain. Chichester, UK: Wiley. p. 18. ISBN 978-0471957737.
- ^ Craig AD (2003). "Pain mechanisms: labeled lines versus convergence in central processing". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 26: 1–30. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131022. PMID 12651967. S2CID 12387848.
- ^ Romanelli P, Esposito V (July 2004). "The functional anatomy of neuropathic pain". Neurosurgery Clinics of North America. 15 (3): 257–268. doi:10.1016/j.nec.2004.02.010. PMID 15246335.
- ^ Melzack R, Wall PD (November 1965). "Pain mechanisms: a new theory" (PDF). Science. 150 (3699): 971–979. Bibcode:1965Sci...150..971M. doi:10.1126/science.150.3699.971. PMID 5320816. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 January 2012. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
- ^ a b Melzack, Ronald; Casey, Kenneth (1968). "Sensory, Motivational, and Central Control Determinants of Pain". In Kenshalo, Dan (ed.). The Skin Senses. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas.
- ^ Lynn B (1984). "Cutaneous nociceptors". In Winlow W, Holden AV (eds.). The neurobiology of pain: Symposium of the Northern Neurobiology Group, held at Leeds on 18 April 1983. Manchester: Manchester University Press. p. 106. ISBN 978-0719009969. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 3 February 2016.
- ^ Bernston GG, Cacioppo JT (2007). "The neuroevolution of motivation". In Gardner WL, Shah JY (eds.). Handbook of Motivation Science. New York: The Guilford Press. p. 191. ISBN 978-1593855680. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 18 November 2020.
- ^ Dawkins R (2009). The Greatest Show on Earth. Free Press. pp. 392–395. ISBN 978-1416594789.
- ^ Steinkopf L (June 2016). "An Evolutionary Perspective on Pain Communication". Evolutionary Psychology. 14 (2): 100. doi:10.1177/1474704916653964.
- ^ Sarno JE (2006). The divided mind: the epidemic of mindbody disorders. New York: ReganBooks. ISBN 978-0061174308.
- ^ a b Fillingim RB, Loeser JD, Baron R, Edwards RR (September 2016). "Assessment of Chronic Pain: Domains, Methods, and Mechanisms". The Journal of Pain. 17 (9 Suppl): T10–20. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2015.08.010. PMC 5010652. PMID 27586827.
- ^ Amico D (2016). Health & physical assessment in nursing. Boston: Pearson. p. 173. ISBN 978-0133876406.
- ^ Taylor C (2015). Fundamentals of nursing : the art and science of person-centered nursing care. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer Health. p. 241. ISBN 978-1451185614.
- ^ Venes D (2013). Taber's cyclopedic medical dictionary. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis. p. 1716. ISBN 978-0803629776.
- ^ Prkachin KM, Solomon PE, Ross J (June 2007). "Underestimation of pain by health-care providers: towards a model of the process of inferring pain in others". The Canadian Journal of Nursing Research. 39 (2): 88–106. PMID 17679587.
- ^ McCaffery M. (1968). Nursing practice theories related to cognition, bodily pain, and man-environment interactions. Los Angeles: UCLA Students Store.
More recently, McCaffery defined pain as "whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever the experiencing person says it does." Pasero C, McCaffery M (1999). Pain: clinical manual. St. Louis: Mosby. ISBN 978-0815156093. - ^ Kelly AM (May 2001). "The minimum clinically significant difference in visual analogue scale pain score does not differ with severity of pain". Emergency Medicine Journal. 18 (3): 205–207. doi:10.1136/emj.18.3.205. PMC 1725574. PMID 11354213.
- ^ Hawker GA, Mian S, Kendzerska T, French M (November 2011). "Measures of adult pain: Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS Pain), Numeric Rating Scale for Pain (NRS Pain), McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (SF-MPQ), Chronic Pain Grade Scale (CPGS), Short Form-36 Bodily Pain Scale (SF-36 BPS), and Measure of Intermittent and Constant Osteoarthritis Pain (ICOAP)". Arthritis Care & Research. 63 (S11): S240–252. doi:10.1002/acr.20543. PMID 22588748.
- ^ a b c Lewis SM, Bucher L, Heitkemper MM, Harding M (2017). Medical-surgical nursing: Assessment and management of clinical problems (10th ed.). St. Louis, Missouri: Elsevier. p. 126. ISBN 978-0323328524. OCLC 944472408.
- ^ a b c Jarvis C (2007). Physical examination & health assessment. St. Louis, Mo: Elsevier Saunders. pp. 180–192. ISBN 978-1455728107.
- ^ Encandela JA (March 1993). "Social science and the study of pain since Zborowski: a need for a new agenda". Social Science & Medicine. 36 (6): 783–791. doi:10.1016/0277-9536(93)90039-7. PMID 8480223.
- ^ Zborowski M. People in Pain. 1969, San Francisco, CA:Josey-Bass[ISBN missing][page needed]
- ^ Encandela JA (1997). "Social Construction of pain and aging: Individual artfulness within interpretive structures". Symbolic Interaction. 20 (3): 251–273. doi:10.1525/si.1997.20.3.251.
- ^ Lawhorne L, Passerini J (1999). Chronic Pain Management in the Long Term Care Setting: Clinical Practice Guidelines. Baltimore, Maryland: American Medical Directors Association. pp. 1–27.
- ^ Epstein R (19 March 2018). "When Doctors Don't Listen to Women". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 9 May 2019. Retrieved 20 July 2019.
- ^ Fasslet J (15 October 2015). "How Doctors Take Women's Pain Less Seriously". The Atlantic. Archived from the original on 17 July 2019. Retrieved 20 July 2019.
- ^ "Stories of Misunderstanding Women's Pain". The Atlantic. 15 March 2016. Archived from the original on 15 April 2019. Retrieved 20 July 2019.
- ^ Panju AA, Hemmelgarn BR, Guyatt GH, Simel DL (October 1998). "The rational clinical examination. Is this patient having a myocardial infarction?". JAMA. 280 (14): 1256–1263. doi:10.1001/jama.280.14.1256. PMID 9786377.
- ^ Slater EE, DeSanctis RW (May 1976). "The clinical recognition of dissecting aortic aneurysm". The American Journal of Medicine. 60 (5): 625–633. doi:10.1016/0002-9343(76)90496-4. PMID 1020750.
- ^ Brown JE, Chatterjee N, Younger J, Mackey S (September 2011). "Towards a physiology-based measure of pain: patterns of human brain activity distinguish painful from non-painful thermal stimulation". PLOS ONE. 6 (9): e24124. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...624124B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024124. PMC 3172232. PMID 21931652.
- ^ Paddock C (15 September 2011). "Tool That Measures Pain Objectively Under Way". Medical News Today. Archived from the original on 25 September 2017. Retrieved 25 September 2017.
- ^ "Feeling pain? The computer can tell". Reuters. 13 September 2011. Archived from the original on 17 June 2015. Retrieved 25 September 2017.
Editorial
- ^ Urch CE, Suzuki R (26 September 2008). "Pathophysiology of somatic, visceral, and neuropathic cancer pain". In Sykes N, Bennett MI & Yuan C-S (ed.). Clinical pain management: Cancer pain (2d ed.). London: Hodder Arnold. pp. 3–12. ISBN 978-0340940075.
- ^ Treede RD, Jensen TS, Campbell JN, Cruccu G, Dostrovsky JO, Griffin JW, Hansson P, Hughes R, Nurmikko T, Serra J (April 2008). "Neuropathic pain: redefinition and a grading system for clinical and research purposes". Neurology. 70 (18): 1630–1635. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000282763.29778.59. hdl:11573/97043. PMID 18003941. S2CID 30172528.
- ^ Paice JA (2003). "Mechanisms and management of neuropathic pain in cancer" (PDF). The Journal of Supportive Oncology. 1 (2): 107–120. PMID 15352654. Archived from the original (PDF) on 7 January 2010. Retrieved 8 January 2010.
- ^ Campbell JN, Meyer RA (October 2006). "Mechanisms of neuropathic pain". Neuron. 52 (1): 77–92. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.021. PMC 1810425. PMID 17015228.
- ^ a b "Psychogenic Pain". Cleveland Clinic. Archived from the original on 14 July 2011. Retrieved 25 September 2017.
- ^ Thienhaus O, Cole BE (2002). "The classification of pain". In Weiner RS (ed.). Pain management: A practical guide for clinicians. American Academy of Pain Management. p. 29. ISBN 978-0849322624.
- Main CJ, Spanswick CC (2000). Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach. Churchill Livingstone. ISBN 978-0443056833.
Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach.
- Main CJ, Spanswick CC (2000). Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach. Churchill Livingstone. ISBN 978-0443056833.
- ^ Brown AK, Christo PJ, Wu CL (December 2004). "Strategies for postoperative pain management". Best Practice & Research. Clinical Anaesthesiology. 18 (4): 703–717. doi:10.1016/j.bpa.2004.05.004. PMID 15460554.
- ^ Cullen L, Greiner J, Titler MG (June 2001). "Pain management in the culture of critical care". Critical Care Nursing Clinics of North America. 13 (2): 151–166. doi:10.1016/S0899-5885(18)30046-7. PMID 11866399.
- ^ Rupp T, Delaney KA (April 2004). "Inadequate analgesia in emergency medicine". Annals of Emergency Medicine. 43 (4): 494–503. doi:10.1016/j.annemergmed.2003.11.019. PMID 15039693.
- ^ Smith GF, Toonen TR (April 2007). "Primary care of the patient with cancer". American Family Physician. 75 (8): 1207–1214. PMID 17477104.
- ^ Jacobson PL, Mann JD (January 2003). "Evolving role of the neurologist in the diagnosis and treatment of chronic noncancer pain". Mayo Clinic Proceedings. 78 (1): 80–84. doi:10.4065/78.1.80. PMID 12528880.
- ^ Deandrea S, Montanari M, Moja L, Apolone G (December 2008). "Prevalence of undertreatment in cancer pain. A review of published literature". Annals of Oncology. 19 (12): 1985–1991. doi:10.1093/annonc/mdn419. PMC 2733110. PMID 18632721.
- ^ Perron V, Schonwetter RS (2001). "Assessment and management of pain in palliative care patients". Cancer Control. 8 (1): 15–24. doi:10.1177/107327480100800103. PMID 11176032.
- ^ Selbst SM, Fein JA (2006). "Sedation and analgesia". In Henretig FM, Fleisher GR, Ludwig S (eds.). Textbook of pediatric emergency medicine. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-1605471594. Archived from the original on 11 June 2016. Retrieved 3 February 2016.
- ^ Cleeland CS (June 1998). "Undertreatment of cancer pain in elderly patients". JAMA. 279 (23): 1914–1915. doi:10.1001/jama.279.23.1914. PMID 9634265.
- ^ Bonham VL (2001). "Race, ethnicity, and pain treatment: striving to understand the causes and solutions to the disparities in pain treatment" (PDF). The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics. 29 (1): 52–68. doi:10.1111/j.1748-720X.2001.tb00039.x. PMID 11521272. S2CID 18257031. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 July 2011. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
- ^ Green CR, Anderson KO, Baker TA, Campbell LC, Decker S, Fillingim RB, Kalauokalani DA, Kaloukalani DA, Lasch KE, Myers C, Tait RC, Todd KH, Vallerand AH (September 2003). "The unequal burden of pain: confronting racial and ethnic disparities in pain" (PDF). Pain Medicine. 4 (3): 277–294. doi:10.1046/j.1526-4637.2003.03034.x. hdl:2027.42/73822. PMID 12974827. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 2 September 2019.
- ^ Hoffmann DE, Tarzian AJ (2001). "The girl who cried pain: a bias against women in the treatment of pain". The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics. 29 (1): 13–27. doi:10.1111/j.1748-720X.2001.tb00037.x. PMID 11521267. S2CID 219952180. Archived from the original on 1 November 2019. Retrieved 11 July 2019.
- ^ Delegates to the International Pain Summit of the International Association for the Study of Pain (2010). "Declaration of Montreal". Archived from the original on 13 May 2011. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
- ^ Horlocker TT, Cousins MJ, Bridenbaugh PO, Carr DL (2008). Cousins and Bridenbaugh's Neural Blockade in Clinical Anesthesia and Pain Medicine. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-0781773881.
- ^ "Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation". Archived from the original on 16 May 2008. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
- ^ Human Rights Watch (2011). "Tens of Millions Face Death in Agony". Archived from the original on 1 September 2013. Retrieved 26 August 2013.
- ^ Mallinson TE (2017). "A review of ketorolac as a prehospital analgesic". Journal of Paramedic Practice. 9 (12): 522–526. doi:10.12968/jpar.2017.9.12.522. Archived from the original on 5 June 2018. Retrieved 2 June 2018.
- ^ Derry CJ, Derry S, Moore RA (December 2014). "Caffeine as an analgesic adjuvant for acute pain in adults". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 12 (12): CD009281. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD009281.pub3. PMC 6485702. PMID 25502052.
- ^ Derry S, Wiffen PJ, Moore RA (July 2015). "Single dose oral ibuprofen plus caffeine for acute postoperative pain in adults". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2019 (7): CD011509. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD011509.pub2. PMC 6481458. PMID 26171993.
- ^ Karlow N, Schlaepfer CH, Stoll CR, Doering M, Carpenter CR, Colditz GA, et al. (October 2018). "A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Ketamine as an Alternative to Opioids for Acute Pain in the Emergency Department". Academic Emergency Medicine. 25 (10): 1086–1097. doi:10.1111/acem.13502. PMID 30019434.
- ^ Higgins C, Smith BH, Matthews K (June 2019). "Evidence of opioid-induced hyperalgesia in clinical populations after chronic opioid exposure: a systematic review and meta-analysis". British Journal of Anaesthesia. 122 (6): e114–e126. doi:10.1016/j.bja.2018.09.019. PMID 30915985.
- ^ Fishbain DA, Pulikal A (November 2019). "Does Opioid Tapering in Chronic Pain Patients Result in Improved Pain or Same Pain vs Increased Pain at Taper Completion? A Structured Evidence-Based Systematic Review". Pain Medicine. 20 (11): 2179–2197. doi:10.1093/pm/pny231. PMID 30597076. Archived from the original on 18 January 2021. Retrieved 19 February 2021.
- ^ Stevens B, Yamada J, Ohlsson A, Haliburton S, Shorkey A (July 2016). "Sucrose for analgesia in newborn infants undergoing painful procedures". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 7 (2): CD001069. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD001069.pub5. PMC 6457867. PMID 27420164.
- ^ Lasky RE, van Drongelen W (October 2010). "Is sucrose an effective analgesic for newborn babies?". Lancet. 376 (9748): 1201–1203. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61358-X. PMID 20817245. S2CID 18724497.
- ^ Harrison D, Stevens B, Bueno M, Yamada J, Adams-Webber T, Beyene J, Ohlsson A (June 2010). "Efficacy of sweet solutions for analgesia in infants between 1 and 12 months of age: a systematic review". Archives of Disease in Childhood. 95 (6): 406–413. doi:10.1136/adc.2009.174227. PMID 20463370.
- ^ Wark DM (July 2008). "What we can do with hypnosis: a brief note". The American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. 51 (1): 29–36. doi:10.1080/00029157.2008.10401640. PMID 18714889. S2CID 12240662.
- ^ Elkins G, Jensen MP, Patterson DR (July 2007). "Hypnotherapy for the management of chronic pain". The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis. 55 (3): 275–287. doi:10.1080/00207140701338621. PMC 2752362. PMID 17558718.
- ^ Madsen MV, Gøtzsche PC, Hróbjartsson A (January 2009). "Acupuncture treatment for pain: systematic review of randomised clinical trials with acupuncture, placebo acupuncture, and no acupuncture groups". BMJ. 338: a3115. doi:10.1136/bmj.a3115. PMC 2769056. PMID 19174438.
- ^ Chiu HY, Hsieh YJ, Tsai PS (March 2017). "Systematic review and meta-analysis of acupuncture to reduce cancer-related pain". European Journal of Cancer Care. 26 (2): e12457. doi:10.1111/ecc.12457. PMID 26853524. S2CID 20096639.
- ^ Chang SC, Hsu CH, Hsu CK, Yang SS, Chang SJ (February 2017). "The efficacy of acupuncture in managing patients with chronic prostatitis/chronic pelvic pain syndrome: A systemic review and meta-analysis". Neurourology and Urodynamics. 36 (2): 474–481. doi:10.1002/nau.22958. PMID 26741647. S2CID 46827576.
- ^ Ji M, Wang X, Chen M, Shen Y, Zhang X, Yang J (2015). "The Efficacy of Acupuncture for the Treatment of Sciatica: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis". Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 2015: 192808. doi:10.1155/2015/192808. PMC 4575738. PMID 26425130.
- ^ Gagnier JJ, Oltean H, van Tulder MW, Berman BM, Bombardier C, Robbins CB (January 2016). "Herbal Medicine for Low Back Pain: A Cochrane Review". Spine. 41 (2): 116–133. doi:10.1097/BRS.0000000000001310. PMID 26630428.
- ^ a b Rubinstein SM, Terwee CB, Assendelft WJ, de Boer MR, van Tulder MW (September 2012). "Spinal manipulative therapy for acute low-back pain". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2012 (9): CD008880. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD008880.pub2. PMC 6885055. PMID 22972127.
- ^ Straube S, Andrew Moore R, Derry S, McQuay HJ (January 2009). "Vitamin D and chronic pain". Pain. 141 (1–2): 10–13. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.11.010. PMID 19084336. S2CID 17244398.
- ^ "IASP Position Statement on the Use of Cannabinoids to Treat Pain". iasp-pain. Retrieved 10 May 2024.
- ^ Cordell WH, Keene KK, Giles BK, Jones JB, Jones JH, Brizendine EJ (May 2002). "The high prevalence of pain in emergency medical care". The American Journal of Emergency Medicine. 20 (3): 165–169. doi:10.1053/ajem.2002.32643. PMID 11992334.
- ^ Hasselström J, Liu-Palmgren J, Rasjö-Wrååk G (2002). "Prevalence of pain in general practice". European Journal of Pain. 6 (5): 375–385. doi:10.1016/S1090-3801(02)00025-3. PMID 12160512. S2CID 798849.
- ^ Abu-Saad Huijer H (2010). "Chronic pain: a review". Le Journal Medical Libanais. The Lebanese Medical Journal. 58 (1): 21–27. PMID 20358856.
- ^ Smith AK, Cenzer IS, Knight SJ, Puntillo KA, Widera E, Williams BA, Boscardin WJ, Covinsky KE (November 2010). "The epidemiology of pain during the last 2 years of life". Annals of Internal Medicine. 153 (9): 563–569. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-153-9-201011020-00005. PMC 3150170. PMID 21041575.
- ^ Perquin CW, Hazebroek-Kampschreur AA, Hunfeld JA, Bohnen AM, van Suijlekom-Smit LW, Passchier J, van der Wouden JC (July 2000). "Pain in children and adolescents: a common experience". Pain. 87 (1): 51–58. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00269-4. PMID 10863045. S2CID 9813003.
- ^ Foucault M (2007). Security, Territory, Population: Lectures at the College de France, 1977–78. Palgrave Macmillan. p. 1.
- ^ a b c Einolf C (2007). "The Fall and Rise of Torture: A Comparative and Historical Analysis". Social Theory. 25 (2): 101–121. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9558.2007.00300.x. JSTOR 20453071. S2CID 53345959.
- ^ Morinis A (1985). "The ritual experience: pain and the transformation of consciousness in ordeals of initiation". Ethos. 13 (2): 150–174. doi:10.1525/eth.1985.13.2.02a00040. JSTOR 639985.
- ^ Atkinson M, Young K (2001). "Flesh journeys: neo primitives and the contemporary rediscovery of radical body modification". Deviant Behavior. 22 (2): 117–146. doi:10.1080/016396201750065018. S2CID 146525156.
- ^ Loland S, Skirstad B, Waddington I, eds. (2006). Pain and injury in sport: Social and ethical analysis. London and New York: Routledge. pp. 17–20. ISBN 978-0415357043.
- ^ Narayan MC (April 2010). "Culture's effects on pain assessment and management". The American Journal of Nursing. 110 (4): 38–47. doi:10.1097/01.NAJ.0000370157.33223.6d. PMID 20335689.
- ^ Working party of the Nuffield Council on Bioethics (2005). "The ethics of research involving animals. London: Nuffield Council on Bioethics." ISBN 978-1904384106. Archived from the original on 25 June 2008. Retrieved 12 January 2010.
- ^ Rollin BE (June 2007). "Animal research: a moral science. Talking Point on the use of animals in scientific research". EMBO Reports. 8 (6): 521–525. doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400996. PMC 2002540. PMID 17545990.
- ^ Rollin B (1989). The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 117–118. cited in Carbone L (2004). What animals want: expertise and advocacy in laboratory animal welfare policy. US: Oxford University Press. p. 150.
- ^ Griffin DR, Speck GB (January 2004). "New evidence of animal consciousness". Animal Cognition. 7 (1): 5–18. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0203-x. PMID 14658059. S2CID 8650837.
- ^ a b Sherwin CM (February 2001). "Can invertebrates suffer? Or, how robust is argument-by-analogy?". Animal Welfare. 10 (1): 103–118. doi:10.1017/S0962728600023551. S2CID 54126137. Archived from the original on 7 March 2022. Retrieved 22 December 2021.
- ^ Lockwood JA (1987). "The Moral Standing of Insects and the Ethics of Extinction". The Florida Entomologist. 70 (1): 70–89. doi:10.2307/3495093. JSTOR 3495093.
- ^ DeGrazia D, Rowan A (September 1991). "Pain, suffering, and anxiety in animals and humans". Theoretical Medicine. 12 (3): 193–211. doi:10.1007/BF00489606. PMID 1754965. S2CID 34920699.
- ^ "Do Invertebrates Feel Pain?". The Senate Standing Committee on Legal and Constitutional Affairs. The Parliament of Canada. Archived from the original on 6 January 2010. Retrieved 11 June 2008.
- ^ Smith JA (1991). "A Question of Pain in Invertebrates". Institute for Laboratory Animal Research Journal. 33: 1–2. Archived from the original on 8 October 2011.
- ^ Abbott FV, Franklin KB, Westbrook FR (January 1995). "The formalin test: scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats". Pain. 60 (1): 91–102. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V. PMID 7715946. S2CID 35448280.
- ^ Jones JM, Foster W, Twomey CR, Burdge J, Ahmed OM, Pereira TD, et al. (August 2020). "A machine-vision approach for automated pain measurement at millisecond timescales". eLife. 9: e57258. doi:10.7554/eLife.57258. PMC 7434442. PMID 32758355.
- ^ a b c Petruzzello, Melissa (2016). "Do Plants Feel Pain?". Encyclopedia Britannica. Retrieved 8 January 2023.
Given that plants do not have pain receptors, nerves, or a brain, they do not feel pain as we members of the animal kingdom understand it. Uprooting a carrot or trimming a hedge is not a form of botanical torture, and you can bite into that apple without worry.
- ^ Draguhn, Andreas; Mallatt, Jon M.; Robinson, David G. (2021). "Anesthetics and plants: no pain, no brain, and therefore no consciousness". Protoplasma. 258 (2). Springer: 239–248. doi:10.1007/s00709-020-01550-9. PMC 7907021. PMID 32880005. 32880005.
- Casey K (2019). Chasing Pain: The Search for a Neurobiological Mechanism. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0190880231.