Jean Piaget: Difference between revisions
No edit summary |
GreenC bot (talk | contribs) Reformat 1 archive link. Wayback Medic 2.5 per WP:USURPURL and JUDI batch #20 |
||
(737 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{short description|Swiss psychologist, biologist, logician, philosopher and academic}} |
|||
{{refimprove|date=February 2014}} |
|||
{{Use dmy dates|date=March 2020}} |
|||
{{Infobox scientist |
{{Infobox scientist |
||
| |
| image = Jean Piaget in Ann Arbor (cropped).png |
||
| caption = Piaget at the [[University of Michigan]], {{circa}} 1968 |
|||
<!-- Image and caption --> |
|||
| |
| birth_name = Jean William Fritz Piaget |
||
| birth_date = {{birth date|1896|8|9|df=y}} |
|||
|image_size = 190px |
|||
| birth_place = [[Neuchâtel]], Switzerland |
|||
|caption = Piaget at the [[University of Michigan]], c. 1968 |
|||
| death_date = {{Death date and age|1980|9|16|1896|8|9|df=y}} |
|||
<!-- Information --> |
|||
| death_place = [[Geneva]], Switzerland |
|||
|birth_name = Jean William Fritz Piaget |
|||
| fields = [[Developmental psychology]], [[epistemology]] |
|||
|birth_date = {{birth date|1896|8|9|df=y}} |
|||
| known_for = [[Constructivism (learning theory)|Constructivism]], [[Genevan School]], [[genetic epistemology]], [[theory of cognitive development]], [[object permanence]], [[egocentrism]] |
|||
|birth_place = [[Neuchâtel]], [[Switzerland]] |
|||
| alma_mater = [[University of Neuchâtel]]<br />[[University of Zürich]] |
|||
|death_date = {{Death date and age|1980|9|16|1896|8|9|df=y}} |
|||
|death_place = [[Geneva]], [[Switzerland]] |
|||
|fields = [[Developmental psychology|Developmental Psychology]], [[Epistemology]] |
|||
|known_for = [[Constructivism (learning theory)|Constructivism]], [[Genetic epistemology]], [[Theory of cognitive development]], [[Object permanence]], [[Egocentrism]] |
|||
|opposed = [[Renee Baillargeon]], [[C. Randy Gallistel]], [[Rochel Gelman]]{{Citation needed|date=June 2010}} |
|||
|influences = [[Immanuel Kant]], [[Henri Bergson]], [[Pierre Janet]], [[James Mark Baldwin]]<ref>Piaget, J. (1982). Reflections on Baldwin [interview with J. J. Vonèche]. In J. M. Broughton & D. J. Freeman-Moir (Eds.), ''The cognitive developmental psychology of James Mark Baldwin'' (pp. 80-86). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.</ref> |
|||
|influenced = [[Bärbel Inhelder]],<ref>Inhelder, B. (1989). Bärbel Inhelder [Autobiography] (H. Sinclair & M. Sinclair, Trans.). In G. Lindzey (Ed.), ''A History of Psychology in Autobiography'' (Vol. VIII, pp. 208-243). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Tryphon, A., & Vonèche, J. J. (Eds.). (2001). ''Working with Piaget: Essays in honour of Bärbel Inhelder.'' Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.</ref> [[Jerome Bruner]],<ref>Bruner, J. S. (1983). ''In search of mind: Essays in autobiography.'' New York: Harper & Row.</ref> [[Kenneth Kaye]],{{Citation needed|date=April 2014}} [[Lawrence Kohlberg]],<ref>Kohlberg, L. (1982). Moral development. In J. M. Broughton & D. J. Freeman-Moir (Eds.), The cognitive developmental psychology of James Mark Baldwin: Current theory and research in genetic epistemology (pp. 277-325). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.</ref> [[Robert Kegan]],{{Citation needed|date=April 2014}} [[Howard Gardner]],<ref>Gardner, H. (2008). Wrestling with Jean Piaget, my paragon. What have you changed your mind about? http://www.edge.org/q2008/q08_1.html#gardner</ref> [[Thomas Kuhn]],<ref>Burman, J. T. (2007). Piaget no "remedy" for Kuhn, but the two should be read together: Comment on Tsou’s "Piaget vs. Kuhn on scientific progress". ''Theory & Psychology, 17''(5), 721-732. doi: 10.1177/0959354307079306</ref> [[Seymour Papert]],<ref>Papert, S. (1999, March 29). Child Psychologist: Jean Piaget. ''Time'', 153, 104-107.</ref> [[Umberto Eco]]{{Citation needed|date=June 2010}}, [[Lev Vygotsky]]<ref>Piaget, J. (1979). Comments on Vygotsky's critical remarks. ''Archives de Psychologie, 47''(183), 237-249. Piaget, J. (2000). Commentary on Vygotsky's criticisms of ''Language and Thought of the Child and Judgement'' and ''Reasoning in the Child'' (L. Smith, Trans.). ''New Ideas in Psychology, 18''(2-3), 241-259. (Original work published 1962.)</ref> |
|||
}} |
}} |
||
'''Jean William Fritz Piaget''' ({{IPAc-en|UK|p|i|ˈ|æ|ʒ|eɪ}},<ref name="EPD">{{cite EPD|18}}</ref><ref>{{Cite encyclopedia |url=http://www.lexico.com/definition/Piaget,+Jean |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210507235825/https://www.lexico.com/definition/piaget,_jean |archive-date=2021-05-07 |title=Piaget, Jean |dictionary=[[Lexico]] UK English Dictionary |publisher=[[Oxford University Press]]}}</ref> {{IPAc-en|US|ˌ|p|iː|ə|ˈ|ʒ|eɪ|,_|p|j|ɑː|ˈ|ʒ|eɪ}};<ref>{{Cite American Heritage Dictionary|Piaget|access-date=10 August 2019}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.collinsdictionary.com/dictionary/english/piaget|title=Piaget|work=[[Collins English Dictionary]]|publisher=[[HarperCollins]]|access-date=10 August 2019}}</ref><ref>{{Cite Merriam-Webster|Piaget|access-date=10 August 2019}}</ref> {{IPA|fr|ʒɑ̃ pjaʒɛ|lang}}; 9 August 1896 – 16 September 1980) was a Swiss psychologist known for his work on [[child development]]. [[Piaget's theory of cognitive development]] and [[epistemological]] view are together called [[genetic epistemology]].<ref>[https://www.simplypsychology.org/piaget.html Jean Piaget's Theory and Stages of Cognitive Development], by Saul McLeod, Simply Psychology, updated 2018</ref> |
|||
'''Jean Piaget ''' ({{IPA-fr|ʒɑ̃ pjaʒɛ|lang}}; 9 August 1896 – 16 September 1980) was a [[Swiss people|Swiss]] [[developmental psychologist]] and [[philosopher]] known for his [[epistemological]] studies with children. |
|||
His [[Piaget's theory of cognitive development|theory of cognitive development]] and epistemological view are together called "[[genetic epistemology]]". |
|||
Piaget placed great importance on the education of children. As the Director of the [[International Bureau of Education]], he declared in 1934 that "only education is capable of saving our societies from possible collapse, whether violent, or gradual. |
Piaget placed great importance on the education of children. As the Director of the [[International Bureau of Education]], he declared in 1934 that "only education is capable of saving our societies from possible collapse, whether violent, or gradual".<ref> |
||
{{cite journal |
{{cite journal |
||
| last=Munari | first=Alberto |
| last=Munari | first=Alberto |
||
| year=1994 |
| year=1994 |
||
| journal=Prospects: The Quarterly Review of Comparative Education |
|||
| title=JEAN PIAGET (1896–1880) |
|||
| journal=Prospects: the quarterly review of comparative education |
|||
| volume=XXIV | issue=1/2 |
| volume=XXIV | issue=1/2 |
||
| pages=311–327 |
| pages=311–327 |
||
| url=http://www.ibe.unesco.org/fileadmin/user_upload/archive/publications/ThinkersPdf/piagetf.pdf |
| url=http://www.ibe.unesco.org/fileadmin/user_upload/archive/publications/ThinkersPdf/piagetf.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20081031045550/http://www.ibe.unesco.org/fileadmin/user_upload/archive/publications/ThinkersPdf/piagetf.pdf |archive-date=2008-10-31 |url-status=live |
||
| doi=10.1007/bf02199023 |
| doi=10.1007/bf02199023 |
||
| title=Jean Piaget |
|||
}} |
|||
| s2cid=144657103 |
|||
</ref> |
|||
}} |
|||
</ref> His theory of child development has been studied in pre-service education programs. Nowadays, educators and theorists working in the area of early childhood education persist in incorporating constructivist-based strategies. |
|||
Piaget created the International Center for Genetic |
Piaget created the International Center for Genetic Epistemology in [[Geneva]] in 1955 while on the faculty of the [[University of Geneva]], and directed the center until his death in 1980.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.piaget.org/aboutPiaget.html|title=About Piaget|publisher=Jean Piaget Society|access-date=17 October 2016|archive-date=24 August 2019|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190824192513/http://www.piaget.org/aboutPiaget.html}}</ref> The number of collaborations that its founding made possible, and their impact, ultimately led to the Center being referred to in the scholarly literature as "Piaget's factory".<ref name="Burman-HoP2012">{{cite journal|last=Burman|first=J. T.|title=Jean Piaget: Images of a life and his factory|journal=History of Psychology|year=2012|volume=15|issue=3|pages=283–288|doi=10.1037/a0025930|pmid=23397918|issn=1093-4510}}</ref> |
||
According to [[Ernst von Glasersfeld]], |
According to [[Ernst von Glasersfeld]], Piaget was "the great pioneer of the [[Constructivism (learning theory)|constructivist theory of knowing]]".<ref name="von Glasersfeld 1990">{{cite journal|last=von Glasersfeld|first=E.|title=An exposition of constructivism: Why some like it radical|journal=Journal for Research in Mathematics Education: Monograph|year=1990|volume=4|pages=19–29 & 195–210 [22]|jstor=749910|issn=0883-9530|doi=10.2307/749910}} (p. 22).</ref> His ideas were widely popularized in the 1960s.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hsueh | first1 = Y | year = 2009 | title = Piaget in the United States, 1925–1971. In U. Müller, J. I. M. Carpendale & L. Smith (Eds.), ''The Cambridge Companion to Piaget'' (pp. 344–370). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Müller, U., Burman, J. T., & Hutchinson, S. (2013). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget: A quinquagenary retrospective | journal = Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology | volume = 34 | issue = 1| pages = 52–55 | doi = 10.1016/j.appdev.2012.10.001 }}</ref> This then led to the emergence of the study of development as a major sub-discipline in psychology.<ref>Pickren, W. E. (2012). Joseph McVicker Hunt: Golden age psychologist. In W. E. Pickren, D. A. Dewsbury, & M. Wertheimer (Eds.), ''Portraits of Pioneers in Developmental Psychology'' (pp. 185–203). New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.</ref> By the end of the 20th century, he was second only to [[B. F. Skinner]] as the most-cited psychologist.<ref>{{cite journal|title=The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century|journal=[[Review of General Psychology]]|volume=6|issue=2|year=2002|pages=139–152|url=http://creativity.ipras.ru/texts/top100.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20141225174251/http://creativity.ipras.ru/texts/top100.pdf |archive-date=2014-12-25 |url-status=live|doi=10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139|last1=Haggbloom|first1=Steven J. |last2=Warnick|first2=Renee|last3=Warnick|first3=Jason E.|last4=Jones|first4=Vinessa K.|last5=Yarbrough|first5=Gary L.|last6=Russell|first6=Tenea M.|last7=Borecky|first7=Chris M.|last8=McGahhey|first8=Reagan|last9=Powell|first9=John L. III |last10=Beavers|first10=Jamie|last11=Monte|first11=Emmanuelle|s2cid=145668721}}</ref> |
||
==Personal life== |
==Personal life== |
||
Piaget was born in 1896 in [[Neuchâtel]], in the [[Francophone region of Switzerland]]. He was the oldest son of [[Arthur Piaget]] (Swiss), a professor of [[medieval literature]] at the [[University of Neuchâtel]], and Rebecca Jackson (French). Rebecca Jackson came from a prominent family of French steel foundry owners<ref>{{Cite web|title= Jean Piaget: biography of the father of evolutionary psychology 【NUOVO】|url=https://virtualpsychcentre.com/jean-piaget-biography-of-the-father-of-evolutionary-psychology/|access-date=2022-02-20|website= All about Psychology - VirtualPsychCentre|language=en-US}}</ref> of English descent through her [[Lancashire]]-born great-grandfather, steelmaker [[James Jackson (steelmaker)|James Jackson]].<ref>{{Cite web|title=James Jackson|url=https://www.geni.com/people/James-Jackson/6000000038577199031|access-date=2022-02-20|website=Geni |language=en-US}}</ref> Piaget was a precocious child who developed an interest in [[biology]] and the natural world. His early interest in [[zoology]] earned him a reputation among those in the field after he had published several articles on [[mollusk]]s by the age of 15.<ref>[http://www.biography.com/people/jean-piaget-9439915 "Jean Piaget"], Biography. Accessed 28 February 2012</ref> |
|||
Piaget was born in 1896 in [[Neuchâtel]], in the [[Francophone region of Switzerland]]. He was the oldest son of Arthur Piaget (Swiss), a professor of [[medieval literature]] at the [[University of Neuchâtel]], and Rebecca Jackson (French). Piaget was a precocious child who developed an interest in biology and the natural world. His early interest in zoology earned him a reputation among those in the field after he had published several articles on mollusks by the age of 15.<ref>[http://www.biography.com/people/jean-piaget-9439915 "Jean Piaget"], Biography. Accessed 28 February 2012</ref> He was educated at the University of Neuchâtel, and studied briefly at the [[University of Zürich]]. During this time, he published two philosophical papers that showed the direction of his thinking at the time, but which he later dismissed as adolescent thought.<ref>[http://www.piaget.org/aboutPiaget.html A Brief Biography of Jean Piaget], Jean Piaget Society (Society for the study of knowledge and development)</ref> His interest in [[psychoanalysis]], at the time a burgeoning strain of psychology, can also be dated to this period. Piaget moved from Switzerland to [[Paris, France]] after his graduation and he taught at the Grange-Aux-Belles Street School for Boys. The school was run by [[Alfred Binet]], the developer of the Binet [[intelligence test]], and Piaget assisted in the marking of Binet's intelligence tests. It was while he was helping to mark some of these tests that Piaget noticed that young children consistently gave wrong answers to certain questions. Piaget did not focus so much on the fact of the children's answers being wrong, but that young children consistently made types of mistakes that older children and adults did not. This led him to the theory that young children's cognitive processes are inherently different from those of adults. Ultimately, he was to propose a global theory of cognitive developmental stages in which individuals exhibit certain common patterns of cognition in each period of development. In 1921, Piaget returned to Switzerland as director of the [[Rousseau Institute]] in [[Geneva]]. At this time, the institute was directed by Claparede. Piaget was familiar with many of Claparede's ideas including that of the psychological concept 'groping' which was closely associated with 'trials and errors' observed in our mental patterns.<ref>Voyat, G. (1981). "Jean Piaget: 1896-1980".''The American Journal of Psychology, 94''(4), pp. 645-648.</ref> |
|||
When he was 15, his former nanny wrote to his parents to apologize for having once lied to them about fighting off a would-be kidnapper from baby Jean's pram. There never was a kidnapper. Piaget became fascinated that he had somehow formed a memory of this kidnapping incident, a memory that endured even after he understood it to be false.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Restak |first1=Richard |title=The Naked Brain: How the Emerging Neurosociety is Changing How We Live, Work, and Love |url=https://archive.org/details/nakedbrainhoweme0000rest |url-access=registration |date=2006 |publisher=Harmony |location=New York |page=[https://archive.org/details/nakedbrainhoweme0000rest/page/156 156]|isbn=978-1-4000-9808-8 }}</ref> |
|||
In 1923, he married Valentine Châtenay; the couple had three children, whom Piaget studied from infancy. From 1925 to 1929 Piaget was professor of psychology, sociology, and the philosophy of science at |
|||
the [[University of Neuchatel]].<ref>American Psychologist volume 25. (Jan 1970) pg.66</ref> In 1929, Jean Piaget accepted the post of Director of the International Bureau of Education and remained the head of this international organization until 1968. Every year, he drafted his "Director's Speeches" for the IBE Council and for the International Conference on Public Education in which he explicitly addressed his educational credo. |
|||
He developed an interest in [[epistemology]] due to his godfather's urgings to study the fields of philosophy and logic.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Biehler|first1=Robert F.|title=Psychology Applied to Teaching|date=1978|publisher=Houghton Mifflin|page=113|isbn=978-0-395-11921-1}}</ref> He was educated at the University of Neuchâtel, and studied briefly at the [[University of Zürich]]. During this time, he published two philosophical papers that showed the direction of his thinking at the time, but which he later dismissed as adolescent thought.<ref>[http://www.piaget.org/aboutPiaget.html A Brief Biography of Jean Piaget] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190824192513/http://www.piaget.org/aboutPiaget.html |date=24 August 2019 }}, Jean Piaget Society (Society for the study of knowledge and development)</ref> His interest in [[psychoanalysis]], at the time a burgeoning strain of psychology, can also be dated to this period. |
|||
In 1964, Piaget was invited to serve as chief consultant at two conferences at [[Cornell University]] (March 11–13) and [[University of California, Berkeley]] (March 16–18). The conferences addressed the relationship of cognitive studies and curriculum development and strived to conceive implications of recent investigations of children's cognitive development for curricula.<ref>Verne N. Rockcastle (1964, p. xi), the conference director, wrote in the conference report of the Jean Piaget conferences about Piaget: "Although few of us had any personal contact with Piaget prior to the conference, those who attended came to have the deepest and warmest regard for him both as a scientist and as a person. His sense of humor throughout the conference was a sort of international glue that flavored his lectures and punctuated his informal conversation. To sit at the table with him during a meal was not only an intellectual pleasure but a pure social delight. Piaget was completely unsophisticated in spite of his international stature. We could hardly believe it when he came prepared for two weeks' stay with only his 'serviette' and a small Swissair bag. An American would have hat at least two large suitcases. When Piaget left Berkeley, he had his serviette, the small Swissair bag, and a third, larger bag crammed with botanical specimens. 'Where did you get that bag?' we asked. 'I had it in one of the others,' he replied."</ref> |
|||
Piaget moved from Switzerland to Paris after his graduation and he taught at the Grange-Aux-Belles Street School for Boys. The school was run by [[Alfred Binet]], the developer of the Binet-Simon test (later revised by Lewis Terman to become the [[Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales]]). Piaget assisted in the marking of Binet's intelligence tests. It was while he was helping to mark some of these tests that Piaget noticed that young children consistently gave wrong answers to certain questions.<ref>{{Citation |last=Piaget |first=Jean |title=Jean Piaget. |date=1952 |url=http://content.apa.org/books/11154-011 |work=A History of Psychology in Autobiography, Vol IV. |pages=237–256 |editor-last=Boring |editor-first=Edwin G. |place=Worcester |publisher=Clark University Press |language=en |doi=10.1037/11154-011 |access-date=2022-07-07 |editor2-last=Werner |editor2-first=Heinz |editor3-last=Langfeld |editor3-first=Herbert S. |editor4-last=Yerkes |editor4-first=Robert M.}}</ref> Piaget did not focus so much on the fact of the children's answers being wrong, but that young children consistently made types of mistakes that older children and adults managed to avoid. This led him to the theory that young children's cognitive processes are inherently different from those of adults. Ultimately, he was to propose a global theory of cognitive developmental stages in which individuals exhibit certain common patterns of cognition in each period of development. |
|||
In 1979 he was awarded the [[Balzan Prize]] for Social and Political Sciences. |
|||
In 1921, Piaget returned to Switzerland as director of the [[Rousseau Institute]] in [[Geneva]]. At this time, the institute was directed by [[Édouard Claparède]].<ref>{{cite web|url=http://gseacademic.harvard.edu/~hgsebio/presentations/A%20Brief%20Biography%20of%20Jean%20Piaget.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20060901201210/http://gseacademic.harvard.edu/~hgsebio/presentations/A%20Brief%20Biography%20of%20Jean%20Piaget.pdf |archive-date=2006-09-01 |url-status=live|title=A Brief Biography of Jean Piaget|author=Mayer, Susan |date=21 October 2005|website=gseacademic.harvard.edu}}</ref> Piaget was familiar with many of Claparède's ideas, including that of the psychological concept of ''groping'' which was closely associated with "trials and errors" observed in human mental patterns.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Voyat, G. |year=1981|title=Jean Piaget: 1896–1980|journal=The American Journal of Psychology|volume=94|issue=4|pages= 645–648|pmid=7044156}}</ref> |
|||
He was buried with his family in an unmarked grave in the [[Cimetière des Rois]] (Cemetery of Kings) in Geneva. This was, however, as per his request.<ref>Burman, J. T. (2013). Profiles of international archives: Les Archives Jean Piaget, University of Geneva, Switzerland. History of Psychology, 16(2), 158-161. doi: 10.1037/a0031405. A full-color photo of his grave is available online, [[open access]], courtesy of the [[American Psychological Association]].[http://supp.apa.org/psycarticles/supplemental/a0031405/Burman-PiagetStones%28ColorInset%29-lrgweb.jpg]</ref> |
|||
In 1923, he married Valentine Châtenay (7 January 1899 – 3 July 1983);<ref>[http://www.fondationjeanpiaget.ch/fjp/site/biographie/index_biographie.php Fondation Jean Piaget – Biographie]. Fondationjeanpiaget.ch. Retrieved on 26 February 2018.</ref> the couple had three children, whom Piaget studied from infancy. From 1925 to 1929, Piaget worked as a professor of psychology, sociology, and the philosophy of science at |
|||
the [[University of Neuchatel]].<ref>{{cite journal|doi=10.1037/h0020564|pmid=4910176|title=Distinguished Scientific Contribution Awards: 1969: Citation for Jean Piaget|journal=American Psychologist|volume=25|issue=1|pages=65–79|year=1970|author=Anon}}</ref> In 1929, Jean Piaget accepted the post of Director of the International Bureau of Education and remained the head of this international organization until 1968. Every year, he drafted his "Director's Speeches" for the IBE Council and for the International Conference on Public Education in which he explicitly addressed his educational credo. |
|||
Having taught at the [[University of Geneva]], and at the [[University of Paris]] in 1964, Piaget was invited to serve as chief consultant at two conferences at [[Cornell University]] (11–13 March) and the [[University of California, Berkeley]] (16–18 March). The conferences addressed the relationship of cognitive studies and curriculum development, and strived to conceive implications of recent investigations of children's cognitive development for curricula.<ref>Rockcastle, Verne N. (1964, p. xi), the conference director, wrote in the conference report of the Jean Piaget conferences about Piaget: |
|||
:Although few of us had any personal contact with Piaget prior to the conference, those who attended came to have the deepest and warmest regard for him both as a scientist and as a person. His sense of humor throughout the conference was a sort of international glue that flavored his lectures and punctuated his informal conversation. To sit at the table with him during a meal was not only an intellectual pleasure but a pure social delight. Piaget was completely unsophisticated in spite of his international stature. We could hardly believe it when he came prepared for two weeks' stay with only his 'serviette' and a small Swissair bag. An American would have had at least two large suitcases. When Piaget left Berkeley, he had his serviette, the small Swissair bag, and a third, larger bag crammed with botanical specimens. 'Where did you get that bag?' we asked. 'I had it in one of the others,' he replied.</ref> |
|||
In 1972 Piaget was awarded the [[Erasmus Prize]] and in 1979 the [[Balzan Prize]] for Social and Political Sciences. Piaget died on 16 September 1980, and, as he had requested, was buried with his family in an unmarked grave in the [[Cimetière des Rois]] (Cemetery of Kings) in Geneva.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Burman|first1=Jeremy Trevelyan|year=2013|title=Profiles of international archives: Les archives Jean Piaget, University of Geneva, Switzerland|journal=History of Psychology|volume=16|issue=2|pages=158–61|doi=10.1037/a0031405|pmid=23544355}} A photograph of his grave is available [http://supp.apa.org/psycarticles/supplemental/a0031405/Burman-PiagetStones%28ColorInset%29-lrgweb.jpg here].</ref> |
|||
==Career history== |
==Career history== |
||
[[File:Jean Piaget.jpg|thumb|Bust of Jean Piaget in the [[Parc des Bastions]], [[Geneva]]]] |
[[File:Jean Piaget.jpg|thumb|Bust of Jean Piaget in the [[Parc des Bastions]], [[Geneva]]]] |
||
[[File:Professor Piaget en koningin Juliana tijdens de bijeenkomst, Bestanddeelnr 925-6521.jpg|thumb|Jean Piaget at Award ceremony of the Erasmus Prize, 1972, Amsterdam]] |
|||
Harry Beilin described Jean Piaget's theoretical [[research program]]<ref>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191 | last1 = Beilin | first1 = H. | year = 1992 | title = Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology | url = | journal = Developmental Psychology | volume = 28 | issue = 2| pages = 191–204 }}</ref> as consisting of four phases: |
|||
Harry Beilin described Jean Piaget's theoretical [[research program]]<ref>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191 | last1 = Beilin | first1 = H. | year = 1992 | title = Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology | journal = Developmental Psychology | volume = 28 | issue = 2| pages = 191–204 | s2cid = 14459165 }}</ref> as consisting of four phases: |
|||
# the sociological model of development, |
# the sociological model of development, |
||
# the biological model of intellectual development, |
# the biological model of intellectual development, |
||
# the elaboration of the logical model of intellectual development, |
# the elaboration of the logical model of intellectual development, |
||
# the study of figurative thought. |
# the study of figurative thought. |
||
The resulting theoretical frameworks are sufficiently different from each other that they have been characterized as representing different "Piagets |
The resulting theoretical frameworks are sufficiently different from each other that they have been characterized as representing different "Piagets". More recently, Jeremy Burman responded to Beilin and called for the addition of a phase before his turn to psychology: "the zeroth Piaget".<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Burman | first1 = J. T. | year = 2011 | title = The zeroeth Piaget| journal = [[Theory & Psychology]] | volume = 21 | issue = 1| pages = 130–135 | doi = 10.1177/0959354310361407 | s2cid = 220119333 }}</ref> |
||
=== |
===Before becoming a psychologist=== |
||
Before Piaget became a psychologist, he trained in [[natural history]] and [[philosophy]]. He received a doctorate in 1918 from the [[University of |
Before Piaget became a psychologist, he trained in [[natural history]] and [[philosophy]]. He received a doctorate in 1918 from the [[University of Neuchâtel]]. He then undertook post-doctoral training in Zürich (1918–1919), and Paris (1919–1921). He was hired by [[Théodore Simon]] to standardize [[psychometrics|psychometric measures]] for use with French children in 1919.<ref>{{Cite journal |title = The Early Evolution of Jean Piaget's Clinical Method|last = Mayer|first = Susan|date = 2005|journal = History of Psychology|volume = 8|issue = 4|pages = 362–82|doi = 10.1037/1093-4510.8.4.362|url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/6646116|pmid = 17152748}}</ref> The theorist we recognize today only emerged when he moved to Geneva, to work for [[Édouard Claparède]] as director of research at the [[Rousseau Institute]], in 1922. |
||
=== |
===Sociological model of development=== |
||
Piaget first developed as a psychologist in the 1920s. He investigated the hidden side of |
Piaget first developed as a psychologist in the 1920s. He investigated the hidden side of children's minds. Piaget proposed that children moved from a position of [[egocentrism]] to [[sociocentrism]]. For this explanation he combined the use of psychological and [[clinical method]]s to create what he called a semiclinical [[interview]]. He began the interview by asking children standardized questions and depending on how they answered, he would ask them a series of standard questions. Piaget was looking for what he called "spontaneous conviction" so he often asked questions the children neither expected nor anticipated. In his studies, he noticed there was a gradual progression from intuitive to scientific and socially acceptable responses. Piaget theorized children did this because of the social interaction and the challenge to younger children's ideas by the ideas of those children who were more advanced. |
||
This work was used by [[Elton Mayo]] as the basis for the famous [[Hawthorne effect|Hawthorne Experiments]].<ref>Hsueh, Y. |
This work was used by [[Elton Mayo]] as the basis for the famous [[Hawthorne effect|Hawthorne Experiments]].<ref>{{cite journal|author=Hsueh, Y. |year=2001|title=Basing much of the reasoning upon the work of Jean Piaget, 1927–1936|journal= Archives de Psychologie|volume= 69|issue=268–269|pages=39–62|url=http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2002-15215-002}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|author=Hsueh, Y. |year=2002|title=The Hawthorne Experiments and the introduction of Jean Piaget in American Industrial Psychology, 1929–1932|journal= History of Psychology|volume= 5|issue=2|pages=163–189|doi=10.1037/1093-4510.5.2.163|pmid=12096759}}</ref> For Piaget, it also led to an honorary doctorate from Harvard in 1936.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hsueh | first1 = Y | year = 2004 | title = He sees the development of children's concepts upon a background of sociology": Jean Piaget's honorary degree at Harvard University in 1936 |journal = History of Psychology | volume = 7 | issue = 1| pages = 20–44 | doi = 10.1037/1093-4510.7.1.20 | pmid = 15022668 }}</ref> |
||
=== |
===Biological model of intellectual development=== |
||
In this stage, Piaget believed that the process of thinking and |
In this stage, Piaget believed that the process of thinking and intellectual development could be regarded as an extension of the biological process of the (adaptation) of the species, which has also two ongoing processes: assimilation and accommodation. There is ''assimilation'' when a child responds to a new event in a way that is consistent with an existing [[schema (psychology)|schema]]. There is ''accommodation'' when a child either modifies an existing schema or forms an entirely new schema to deal with a new object or event.<ref name="Ormrod">Ormrod, J.E. (2012). ''Essentials of Educational Psychology: Big Ideas to Guide Effective Teaching''. Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc.</ref> |
||
He argued infants were engaging in |
He argued infants were engaging in the act of assimilation when they sucked on everything in their reach. He claimed infants transform all objects into an object to be sucked. The children were assimilating the objects to conform to their own mental structures. Piaget then made the assumption that whenever one transforms the world to meet individual needs or conceptions, one is, in a way, assimilating it. Piaget also observed his children not only assimilating objects to fit their needs, but also modifying some of their mental structures to meet the demands of the environment. This is the second division of adaptation known as accommodation. To start, the infants only engaged in primarily reflex actions such as sucking, but not long after, they would pick up objects and put them in their mouths. When they do this, they modify their reflex response to accommodate the external objects into reflex actions. Because the two are often in conflict, they provide the impetus for intellectual development{{mdash}}the constant need to balance the two triggers intellectual growth. |
||
To test his theory, Piaget observed the [[ |
To test his theory, Piaget observed the [[habit]]s in his own children. |
||
=== |
===Elaboration of the logical model of intellectual development=== |
||
In the model Piaget developed in stage three, he argued that intelligence develops in a series of stages that are related to age and are progressive because one stage must be accomplished before the next can occur. For each stage of development the child forms a view of reality for that age period. At the next stage, the child must keep up with earlier level of mental abilities to reconstruct concepts. Piaget conceived intellectual development as an upward expanding spiral in which children must constantly reconstruct the ideas formed at earlier levels with new, higher order concepts acquired at the next level. |
In the model Piaget developed in stage three, he argued that intelligence develops in a series of stages that are related to age and are progressive because one stage must be accomplished before the next can occur. For each stage of development the child forms a view of reality for that age period. At the next stage, the child must keep up with earlier level of mental abilities to reconstruct concepts. Piaget conceived intellectual development as an upward expanding spiral in which children must constantly reconstruct the ideas formed at earlier levels with new, higher order concepts acquired at the next level. |
||
It is primarily the "Third Piaget" (the logical model of intellectual development) that was debated by American psychologists when Piaget's ideas were "rediscovered" in the 1960s.<ref>Hsueh, Y. (2005). The lost and found experience: Piaget rediscovered. The Constructivist, 16(1). |
It is primarily the "Third Piaget" (the logical model of intellectual development) that was debated by American psychologists when Piaget's ideas were "rediscovered" in the 1960s.<ref>Hsueh, Y. (2005). The lost and found experience: Piaget rediscovered. The Constructivist, 16(1).</ref> |
||
=== |
===Study of figurative thought=== |
||
Piaget studied areas of intelligence like [[perception]] and [[memory]] that are not entirely logical. Logical concepts are described as being completely reversible because they can always get back to the starting point, meaning that if one starts with a given premise and follows logical steps to reach a conclusion, the same steps may be done in the opposite order, starting from the conclusion to arrive at the premise. The perceptual concepts Piaget studied could not be manipulated. To describe the figurative process, Piaget uses pictures as examples. Pictures cannot be separated because contours cannot be separated from the forms they outline. Memory is the same way: it is never completely reversible; people cannot necessarily recall all the intervening events between two points. During this last period of work, Piaget and his colleague Inhelder also published books on perception, memory, and other figurative processes such as learning.<ref>Guthrie, James W. (2003) "Piaget, Jean (1896–1980)" in ''Encyclopedia of Education''. 2nd ed. Vol. 5. New York, NY: Macmillan Reference USA. pp. 1894–898.</ref><ref>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1007/BF02687400 | last1 = Valsiner | first1 = J. | year = 2005 | title = Participating in Piaget | journal = Society | volume = 42 | issue = 2| pages = 57–61 | s2cid = 145294976 }}</ref> |
|||
{{clarify section|date=December 2012}} |
|||
Piaget studied areas of intelligence like [[perception]] and [[memory]] that aren’t entirely logical. Logical concepts are described as being completely reversible because they can always get back to the starting point. The perceptual concepts Piaget studied could not be manipulated. To describe the figurative process, Piaget uses pictures as examples. Pictures can’t be separated because contours cannot be separated from the forms they outline. Memory is the same way. It is never completely reversible. During this last period of work, Piaget and his colleague Inhelder also published books on perception, memory, and other figurative processes such as learning.<ref>Guthrie, James W. "Piaget, Jean (1896-1980)." Encyclopedia of Education. 2nd ed. Vol. 5. New York, NY: Macmillan Reference USA, 2003. 1894-898.</ref><ref>"Piaget, Jean." Encyclopædia Britannica. 2008. Encyclopædia Britannica Online. 3 November 2008 [http://search.eb.com/eb/article-9059885 search.eb.com]</ref><ref>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1007/BF02687400 | last1 = Valsiner | first1 = J. | year = 2005 | title = Participating in Piaget | url = | journal = Society | volume = 42 | issue = 2| pages = 57–61 }}</ref> |
|||
Because Piaget's theory is based upon biological maturation and stages, the notion of readiness is important. Readiness concerns when certain information or concepts should be taught. According to Piaget's theory children should not be taught certain concepts until they reached the appropriate stage cognitive development. |
Because Piaget's theory is based upon biological maturation and stages, the notion of readiness is important. Readiness concerns when certain information or concepts should be taught. According to Piaget's theory, children should not be taught certain concepts until they reached the appropriate stage of cognitive development.<ref>{{Britannica|459096|Jean Piaget}}</ref> For example, young children in the [[preoperational stage]] engage in "irreversible" thought and cannot comprehend that an item that has been transformed in some way may be returned to its original state.<ref>{{Cite web |url=https://www.simplypsychology.org/preoperational.html |title=Preoperational Stage – Egocentrism |last=McLeod |first=Saul |website=Simply Psychology |access-date=2018-08-07}}</ref> |
||
==Theory== |
==Theory== |
||
{{Main|Piaget's theory of cognitive development}} |
|||
Piaget defined himself as a 'genetic' [[Epistemology|epistemologist]], interested in the process of the qualitative development of knowledge. He considered cognitive structures development as a differentiation of biological regulations. When his entire theory first became known - the theory in itself being based on a structuralist and a cognitivitist approach - it was an outstanding and exciting development in regards to the psychological community at that time.<ref>Howard Gardner, ''The Quest for Mind: Piaget, Levi-Strauss and the Structuralist Movement'', University of Chicago Press, 1981.</ref> |
|||
Piaget defined himself as a 'genetic' [[Epistemology|epistemologist]], interested in the process of the qualitative development of knowledge. He considered cognitive structures' development as a differentiation of biological regulations. When his entire theory first became known – the theory in itself being based on a structuralist and a cognitivitist approach – it was an outstanding and exciting development in regards to the psychological community at that time.<ref>Gardner, Howard (1981) ''The Quest for Mind: Piaget, Levi-Strauss and the Structuralist Movement'', University of Chicago Press.</ref> |
|||
There are a total of four phases in Piaget's research program that included books on certain topics of developmental psychology. In particular, during one period of research, he described himself studying his own three children, and carefully observing and interpreting their cognitive development.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Beilin Harry |title=Piaget's Enduring Contribution to Developmental Psychology|year=1992 |
There are a total of four phases in Piaget's research program that included books on certain topics of developmental psychology. In particular, during one period of research, he described himself studying his own three children, and carefully observing and interpreting their cognitive development.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Beilin Harry |title=Piaget's Enduring Contribution to Developmental Psychology|year=1992|journal=Developmental Psychology|volume=28|issue=2|pages=191–204|doi=10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191}}</ref> In one of his last books, ''Equilibration of Cognitive Structures: The Central Problem of Intellectual Development'', he intends to explain knowledge development as a process of equilibration using two main concepts in his theory, assimilation and accommodation, as belonging not only to biological interactions but also to cognitive ones.<ref>{{Cite web |last=Kurt |first=Dr Serhat |date=2022-11-17 |title=Jean Piaget: Biography, Theory and Cognitive Development |url=https://educationlibrary.org/jean-piaget-biography-theory-and-cognitive-development/ |access-date=2022-11-30 |website=Education Library |language=en-US}}</ref> |
||
He stated that children are born with limited capabilities and his cognition ability develops over age.<ref>{{Cite web |last=Cherry |first=Kendra |date=1 May 2024 |title=Piaget's 4 Stages of Cognitive Development Explained. |url=http://www.verywellmind.com |access-date=3 July 2024 |website=www.verywellmind.com}}</ref> |
|||
Piaget believed answers for the epistemological questions at his time could be answered, or better proposed, if one looked to the genetic aspect of it, hence his experimentations with children and adolescents. As he says in the introduction of his book ''Genetic Epistemology'': "What the genetic epistemology proposes is discovering the roots of the different varieties of knowledge, since its elementary forms, following to the next levels, including also the scientific knowledge." |
Piaget believed answers for the epistemological questions at his time could be answered, or better proposed, if one looked to the genetic aspect of it, hence his experimentations with children and adolescents. As he says in the introduction of his book ''Genetic Epistemology'': "What the genetic epistemology proposes is discovering the roots of the different varieties of knowledge, since its elementary forms, following to the next levels, including also the scientific knowledge." |
||
Line 95: | Line 101: | ||
The four development stages are described in Piaget's theory as: |
The four development stages are described in Piaget's theory as: |
||
{{Ordered list |
|||
'''1.''' ''[[Theory of cognitive development#Sensorimotor stage|Sensorimotor stage]]'': from birth to age two. The children experience the world through movement and their five senses. During the sensorimotor stage children are extremely egocentric, meaning they cannot perceive the world from others' viewpoints. The sensorimotor stage is divided into six substages:<ref name="Santrock, John W. 1998">Santrock, John W. Children. 9. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, 1998.</ref> |
|||
|<p>''[[Theory of cognitive development#Sensorimotor stage|Sensorimotor stage]]'': from birth to age two. The children experience the world through movement and their senses. During the sensorimotor stage children are extremely egocentric, meaning they cannot perceive the world from others' viewpoints. The sensorimotor stage is divided into six substages:<ref name="Santrock, John W. 1998">Santrock, John W. (1998) Children. 9. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.</ref></p> |
|||
{{Ordered list |list_style_type=upper-roman |
|||
:I. Simple reflexes; |
|||
: |
|Simple reflexes: From birth to one month old. At this time infants use reflexes such as rooting and sucking. |
||
|First habits and primary circular reactions: From one month to four months old. During this time infants learn to coordinate sensation and two types of schema (habit and circular reactions). A primary circular reaction is when the infant tries to reproduce an event that happened by accident (ex.: sucking thumb). |
|||
:II. First habits and primary circular reactions; |
|||
|Secondary circular reactions: From four to eight months old. At this time they become aware of things beyond their own body; they are more object-oriented. At this time they might accidentally shake a rattle and continue to do it for sake of satisfaction. |
|||
::From one month to four months old. During this time infants learn to coordinate sensation and two types of schema (habit and circular reactions). A primary circular reaction is when the infant tries to reproduce an event that happened by accident (ex.: sucking thumb). |
|||
|Coordination of secondary circular reactions: From eight months to twelve months old. During this stage they can do things intentionally. They can now combine and recombine schemata and try to reach a goal (ex.: use a stick to reach something). They also begin to understand [[object permanence]] in the later months and early into the next stage. That is, they understand that objects continue to exist even when they can't see them. |
|||
:III. Secondary circular reactions; |
|||
|Tertiary circular reactions, novelty, and curiosity: From twelve months old to eighteen months old. During this stage infants explore new possibilities of objects; they try different things to get different results. |
|||
::From four to eight months old. At this time they become aware of things beyond their own body; they are more object-oriented. At this time they might accidentally shake a rattle and continue to do it for sake of satisfaction. |
|||
|Internalization of schemata. |
|||
:IV. Coordination of secondary circular reactions; |
|||
}} |
|||
::From eight months to twelve months old. During this stage they can do things intentionally. They can now combine and recombine schemata and try to reach a goal (ex.: use a stick to reach something). They also understand [[object permanence]] during this stage. That is, they understand that objects continue to exist even when they can't see them. |
|||
:V. Tertiary circular reactions, novelty, and curiosity; |
|||
::From twelve months old to eighteen months old. During this stage infants explore new possibilities of objects; they try different things to get different results. |
|||
:VI. Internalization of schemata. |
|||
Some followers of Piaget's studies of infancy, such as [[Kenneth Kaye]]<ref name="K. Kaye, 1982">K. |
<p>Some followers of Piaget's studies of infancy, such as [[Kenneth Kaye]]<ref name="K. Kaye, 1982">Kaye, K. (1982) ''The Mental and Social Life of Babies''. U. Chicago Press.</ref> argue that his contribution was as an observer of countless phenomena not previously described, but that he didn't offer explanation of the processes in real time that cause those developments, beyond analogizing them to broad concepts about biological adaptation generally. Kaye's "apprenticeship theory" of cognitive and social development refuted Piaget's assumption that mind developed endogenously in infants until the capacity for symbolic reasoning allowed them to learn language.</p> |
||
|<p>''[[Theory of cognitive development#Preoperational stage|Preoperational stage]]'': Piaget's second stage, the preoperational stage, starts when the child begins to learn to speak at age two and lasts up until the age of seven. During the preoperational stage of cognitive development, Piaget noted that children do not yet understand concrete logic and cannot mentally manipulate information. Children's increase in playing and pretending takes place in this stage. The child still has trouble seeing things from different points of view. The children's play is mainly categorized by symbolic play and manipulating symbols. Such play is demonstrated by the idea of checkers being snacks, pieces of paper being plates, and a box being a table. Their observations of symbols exemplifies the idea of play with the absence of the actual objects involved. By observing sequences of play, Piaget was able to demonstrate that, toward the end of the second year, a qualitatively new kind of psychological functioning occurs, known as the preoperational stage.<ref name="Santrock, John W. 2004">Santrock, John W. (2004). Life-Span Development (9th Ed.). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill College – Chapter 8</ref></p> |
|||
<p>The preoperational stage is sparse and logically inadequate in regard to mental operations. The child is able to form stable concepts as well as magical beliefs, but not perform operations, which are mental tasks, rather than physical. Thinking in this stage is still egocentric, meaning the child has difficulty seeing the viewpoint of others. The preoperational stage is split into two substages: the symbolic function substage, and the intuitive thought substage. The symbolic function substage is when children are able to understand, represent, remember, and picture objects in their mind without having the object in front of them. The intuitive thought substage is when children tend to propose the questions of "why?" and "how come?" This stage is when children want the knowledge of knowing everything.<ref name="Santrock, John W. 2004"/></p> |
|||
<p>The Preoperational Stage is divided into two substages:</p> |
|||
{{Ordered list |list_style_type=upper-roman |
|||
|[[Piaget's theory of cognitive development#Symbolic function substage|Symbolic Function Substage]]. From two to four years of age children find themselves using symbols to represent physical models of the world around them. This is demonstrated through a child's drawing of their family in which people are not drawn to scale or accurate physical traits are given. The child knows they are not accurate but it does not seem to be an issue to them. |
|||
|[[Piaget's theory of cognitive development#Intuitive thought substage|Intuitive Thought Substage]]. At between about the ages of four and seven, children tend to become very curious and ask many questions, beginning the use of primitive reasoning. There is an emergence in the interest of reasoning and wanting to know why things are the way they are. Piaget called it the "intuitive substage" because children realize they have a vast amount of knowledge, but they are unaware of how they acquired it. Centration, conservation, irreversibility, class inclusion, and transitive inference are all characteristics of preoperative thought.<ref name="Santrock, John W. 2004"/> |
|||
}} |
|||
|<p>''[[Theory of cognitive development#Concrete operational stage|Concrete operational stage]]'': from ages seven to eleven. Children can now converse and think logically (they understand reversibility) but are limited to what they can physically manipulate. They are no longer egocentric. During this stage, children become more aware of logic and conservation, topics previously foreign to them. Children also improve drastically with their classification skills.</p> |
|||
|<p>''[[Theory of cognitive development#Formal operational stage|Formal operational stage]]'': from age eleven and onward (development of abstract reasoning). Children develop abstract thought and can easily conserve and think logically in their mind. Abstract thought is newly present during this stage of development. Children are now able to think abstractly and use [[metacognition]]. Along with this, the children in the formal operational stage display more skills oriented toward problem solving, often in multiple steps.</p> |
|||
}} |
|||
===Psychology of functions and correspondences=== |
|||
The pre-operational stage is sparse and logically inadequate in regard to mental operations. The child is able to form stable concepts as well as magical beliefs. The child, however, is still not able to perform operations, which are tasks that the child can do mentally, rather than physically. Thinking in this stage is still egocentric, meaning the child has difficulty seeing the viewpoint of others. The Pre-operational Stage is split into two substages: the symbolic function substage, and the intuitive thought substage. The symbolic function substage is when children are able to understand, represent, remember, and picture objects in their mind without having the object in front of them. The intuitive thought substage is when children tend to propose the questions of "why?" and "how come?" This stage is when children want the knowledge of knowing everything.<ref name="Santrock, John W. 2004"/> |
|||
Piaget had sometimes been criticized for characterizing preoperational children in terms of the cognitive capacities they lacked, rather than their cognitive accomplishments. A ''late turn'' in the development of Piaget's theory saw the emergence of work on the accomplishments of those children within the framework of his psychology of functions and correspondences.<ref name = "correspondences">Piaget, J. (1976). On correspondences and morphisms. ''Jean Piaget Society Newsletter, 5''. (unpaged)</ref><ref name = "Piaget, Grize">Piaget, J. Grize, J.-B., Szeminska, A., & Vinh Bang. (1977). ''Epistemology and psychology of functions. Studies in genetic epistemology. Vol. 23''. Dordrecht, Holland: D. Reldel, 1977.</ref><ref name = "Groupings, 1977, recent">Piaget, J. (1977). Some recent research and its link with a new theory of groupings and conservation based on commutability. In R. W. Rieber and K. Salzinger (Eds.), ''Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 291'', 350-358.</ref> This new phase in Piaget's work was less stage-dependent and reflected greater continuity in human development than would be expected in a stage-bound theory.<ref name = "Schonfeld, 1986">Schonfeld, I. S. (1986). The Genevan and Cattell-Horn conceptions of intelligence compared: The early implementation of numerical solution aids. ''Developmental Psychology, 22'', 204-212. doi.org/10.10'37/0012-1649.22.2.204</ref> This advance in his work took place toward the end of his very productive life and is sometimes absent from developmental psychology textbooks. |
|||
An example of a function can involve sets X and Y and ordered pairs of elements (x,y), in which x is an element of X and y, Y. In a function, an element of X is mapped onto exactly one element of Y (the reverse need not be true). A function therefore involves a unique mapping in one direction, or, as Piaget and his colleagues have written, functions are "univocal to the right" (Piaget et al., 1977, p. 14).<ref name = "Piaget, Grize"/> When each element of X maps onto exactly one element of Y ''and'' each element of Y maps onto exactly one element of X, Piaget and colleagues indicated that the uniqueness condition holds in either direction and called the relationship between the elements of X and Y "biunivocal" or "one-to-one".<ref name = "Piaget, Grize"/> They advanced the idea that the preoperational child manifests some understanding of one-way order functions. |
|||
The Preoperational Stage is divided into two substages: |
|||
:I. [[Piaget's theory of cognitive development#Symbolic function substage|Symbolic Function Substage]] |
|||
::From two to four years of age children find themselves using symbols to represent physical models of the world around them. This is demonstrated through a child's drawing of their family in which people are not drawn to scale or accurate physical traits are given. The child knows they are not accurate but it does not seem to be an issue to them. |
|||
:II. [[Piaget's theory of cognitive development#Intuitive thought substage|Intuitive Thought Substage]] |
|||
::At between about the ages of 4 and 7, children tend to become very curious and ask many questions, beginning the use of primitive reasoning. There is an emergence in the interest of reasoning and wanting to know why things are the way they are. Piaget called it the "intuitive substage" because children realize they have a vast amount of knowledge, but they are unaware of how they acquired it. Centration, conservation, irreversibility, class inclusion, and transitive inference are all characteristics of preoperative thought.<ref name="Santrock, John W. 2004"/> |
|||
According to Piaget's Genevan colleagues,<ref name = "Inhelder 1974">Inhelder, B., Sinclair, H., & Bovet, M. ''Learning and the development of cognition''. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1974</ref> the "semilogic" of these order functions sustains the preoperational child's ability to use of spatial extent to index and compare quantities. The child, for example, could use the length of an array to index the number of objects in the array. Thus, the child would judge the longer of two arrays as having the greater number of objects. Although imperfect, such comparisons are often fair ("semilogical") substitutes for exact quantification. Furthermore, these order functions underlie the child's rudimentary knowledge of environmental regularities.<ref name = "Inhelder 1974"/> Young children are capable of constructing—this reflects the constructivist bent of Piaget's work—sequences of objects of alternating color. They also have an understanding of the pairwise exchanges of cards having pictures of different flowers. |
|||
'''3.''' ''[[Theory of cognitive development#Concrete operational stage|Concrete operational stage]]'': from ages seven to eleven. Children can now conserve and think logically (they understand reversibility) but are limited to what they can physically manipulate. They are no longer egocentric. |
|||
Piaget and colleagues have examined morphisms, which to them differ from the operative transformations observed on concrete operational children.<ref name = "correspondences"/> Piaget (1977) wrote that "correspondences and morphisms are essentially comparisons that do not transform objects to be compared but that extract common forms from them or analogies between them" (p. 351).<ref name = "Groupings, 1977, recent"/> He advanced the idea that this type of knowledge emerges from "primitive applications" of action schemes to objects in the environment.<ref name = "Piaget, Grize"/> In one study of morphisms, Piaget and colleagues asked children to identify items in a series of movable red cutouts that could cover a pre-specified section of each of four base cards—each card had a red area and a white area.<ref name = "Piaget, Grize"/> The task, in effect, asked the child to superimpose the cut-outs on a base card to make the entire card appear to be red. Although there were 12 cutouts in all, only three, which differed slightly from each other, could make an entire base card look red. The youngest children studied—they were age 5—could match, using trial and error, one cut-out to one base card. Piaget et al. called this type of morphism bijection, a term-by-term correspondence. Older children were able to do more by figuring out how to make entire card appear to be red by using three cutouts. In other words, they could perform three to one matching. Piaget et al. (1977) called a many-to-one match surjection. |
|||
'''4.''' ''[[Theory of cognitive development#Formal operational stage|Formal operational stage]]'': from age eleven to sixteen and onwards (development of abstract reasoning). Children develop abstract thought and can easily conserve and think logically in their mind. |
|||
=== |
=== Developmental process === |
||
Piaget provided no concise description of the development process as a whole. Broadly speaking it consisted of a cycle: |
Piaget provided no concise description of the development process as a whole. Broadly speaking it consisted of a cycle: |
||
*The child performs an action which has an effect on or organizes objects, and the child is able to note the characteristics of the action and its effects. |
*The child performs an action which has an effect on or organizes objects, and the child is able to note the characteristics of the action and its effects. |
||
*Through repeated actions, perhaps with variations or in different contexts or on different kinds of objects, the child is able to differentiate and integrate its elements and effects. This is the process of "reflecting abstraction" (described in detail in Piaget 2001). |
*Through repeated actions, perhaps with variations or in different contexts or on different kinds of objects, the child is able to differentiate and integrate its elements and effects. This is the process of "reflecting abstraction" (described in detail in Piaget 2001). |
||
*At the same time, the child is able to identify the properties of objects by the way different kinds of |
*At the same time, the child is able to identify the properties of objects by the way different kinds of actions affect them. This is the process of "empirical abstraction". |
||
*By repeating this process across a wide range of objects and actions, the child establishes a new level of knowledge and insight. This is the process of forming a new "[[cognitive]] stage". This dual process allows the child to construct new ways of dealing with objects and new knowledge about objects themselves. |
*By repeating this process across a wide range of objects and actions, the child establishes a new level of knowledge and insight. This is the process of forming a new "[[cognitive]] stage". This dual process allows the child to construct new ways of dealing with objects and new knowledge about objects themselves. |
||
* |
*Once the child has constructed these new kinds of knowledge, he or she starts to use them to create still more complex objects and to carry out still more complex actions. As a result, the child starts to recognize still more complex patterns and to construct still more complex objects. Thus a new stage begins, which will only be completed when all the child's activity and experience have been re-organized on this still higher level. |
||
This process may not be wholly gradual, but new evidence shows that the passage into new stages is more gradual than once thought. Once a new level of organization, knowledge and insight proves to be effective, it will quickly be generalized to other areas ''if they exist''. As a result, transitions between stages can seem to be rapid and radical, but oftentimes the child has grasped one aspect of the new stage of cognitive functioning but not addressed others. The bulk of the time spent in a new stage consists of refining this new cognitive level |
This process may not be wholly gradual, but new evidence shows that the passage into new stages is more gradual than once thought. Once a new level of organization, knowledge and insight proves to be effective, it will quickly be generalized to other areas ''if they exist''. As a result, transitions between stages can seem to be rapid and radical, but oftentimes the child has grasped one aspect of the new stage of cognitive functioning but not addressed others. The bulk of the time spent in a new stage consists of refining this new cognitive level; it does not always happen quickly. For example, a child may see that two different colors of Play-Doh have been fused together to make one ball, based on the color. If sugar is mixed into water or iced tea, then the sugar "disappeared" and therefore does not exist to the child at that stage. These levels of one concept of cognitive development are not realized all at once, giving us a gradual realization of the world around us.<ref>Miller, Patrica H. (2009) ''Theories of Developmental Psychology'' 5th Edition, Worth Publishers.</ref> |
||
It is because this process takes this [[dialectic]]al form, in which each new stage is created through the further differentiation, integration, and synthesis of new structures out of the old, that the sequence of cognitive stages are logically necessary rather than simply empirically correct. Each new stage emerges only because the child can take for granted the achievements of its predecessors, and yet there are still more sophisticated forms of knowledge and action that are capable of being developed. |
It is because this process takes this [[dialectic]]al form, in which each new stage is created through the further differentiation, integration, and synthesis of new structures out of the old, that the sequence of cognitive stages are logically necessary rather than simply empirically correct. Each new stage emerges only because the child can take for granted the achievements of its predecessors, and yet there are still more sophisticated forms of knowledge and action that are capable of being developed. |
||
Line 140: | Line 154: | ||
Because it covers both how we gain knowledge about objects and our reflections on our own actions, Piaget's model of development explains a number of features of human knowledge that had never previously been accounted for. For example, by showing how children progressively enrich their understanding of things by acting on and reflecting on the effects of their own previous knowledge, they are able to organize their knowledge in increasingly complex structures. Thus, once a young child can consistently and accurately recognize different kinds of animals, he or she then acquires the ability to organize the different kinds into higher groupings such as "birds", "fish", and so on. This is significant because they are now able to know things about a new animal simply on the basis of the fact that it is a bird – for example, that it will lay eggs. |
Because it covers both how we gain knowledge about objects and our reflections on our own actions, Piaget's model of development explains a number of features of human knowledge that had never previously been accounted for. For example, by showing how children progressively enrich their understanding of things by acting on and reflecting on the effects of their own previous knowledge, they are able to organize their knowledge in increasingly complex structures. Thus, once a young child can consistently and accurately recognize different kinds of animals, he or she then acquires the ability to organize the different kinds into higher groupings such as "birds", "fish", and so on. This is significant because they are now able to know things about a new animal simply on the basis of the fact that it is a bird – for example, that it will lay eggs. |
||
At the same time, by reflecting on their own actions, |
At the same time, by reflecting on their own actions, children develop an increasingly sophisticated awareness of the "rules" that govern them in various ways. For example, it is by this route that Piaget explains this child's growing awareness of notions such as "right", "valid", "necessary", "proper", and so on. In other words, it is through the process of [[objectification]], [[Introspection|reflection]] and [[abstraction]] that the child constructs the principles on which action is not only effective or correct but also ''justified''. |
||
One of Piaget's most famous studies focused purely on the discriminative abilities of children between the ages of two and a half years old, and four and a half years old. He began the study by taking children of different ages and placing two lines of sweets, one with the sweets in a line spread further apart, and one with the same number of sweets in a line placed more closely together. He found that, "Children between 2 years, 6 months old and 3 years, 2 months old correctly discriminate the relative number of objects in two rows; between 3 years, 2 months and 4 years, 6 months they indicate a longer row with fewer objects to have "more"; after 4 years, 6 months they again discriminate correctly" (''Cognitive Capacity of Very Young Children'', p. 141). Initially younger children were not studied, because if at four years old a child could not [[Conservation of quantity|conserve quantity]], then a younger child presumably could not either. The results show |
One of Piaget's most famous studies focused purely on the discriminative abilities of children between the ages of two and a half years old, and four and a half years old. He began the study by taking children of different ages and placing two lines of sweets, one with the sweets in a line spread further apart, and one with the same number of sweets in a line placed more closely together. He found that, "Children between 2 years, 6 months old and 3 years, 2 months old correctly discriminate the relative number of objects in two rows; between 3 years, 2 months and 4 years, 6 months they indicate a longer row with fewer objects to have "more"; after 4 years, 6 months they again discriminate correctly" (''Cognitive Capacity of Very Young Children'', p. 141). Initially younger children were not studied, because if at four years old a child could not [[Conservation of quantity|conserve quantity]], then a younger child presumably could not either. The results show that children that are younger than three years and two months have quantity conservation, but as they get older they lose this quality, and do not recover it until four and a half years old. This attribute may be lost due to a temporary inability to solve because of an overdependence on perceptual strategies, which correlates more candy with a longer line of candy, or due to the inability for a four-year-old to reverse situations. |
||
By the end of this experiment several results were found. First, younger children have a discriminative ability that shows the logical capacity for cognitive operations exists earlier than acknowledged. This study also reveals that young children can be equipped with certain qualities for cognitive operations, depending on how logical the structure of the task is. Research also shows that children develop explicit understanding at age 5 and as a result, the child will count the sweets to decide which has more. Finally the study found that overall quantity conservation is not a basic characteristic of humans' native inheritance. |
By the end of this experiment several results were found. First, younger children have a discriminative ability that shows the logical capacity for cognitive operations exists earlier than acknowledged. This study also reveals that young children can be equipped with certain qualities for cognitive operations, depending on how logical the structure of the task is. Research also shows that children develop explicit understanding at age 5 and as a result, the child will count the sweets to decide which has more. Finally the study found that overall quantity conservation is not a basic characteristic of humans' native inheritance. |
||
=== Genetic epistemology === |
=== Genetic epistemology === |
||
According to Jean Piaget, [[genetic epistemology]] |
According to Jean Piaget, [[genetic epistemology]] attempts to "explain knowledge, and in particular [[scientific]] knowledge, on the basis of its history, its sociogenesis, and especially the psychological origins of the notions and operations upon which it is based". Piaget believed he could test [[epistemology|epistemological]] questions by studying the development of thought and action in children. As a result, Piaget created a field known as genetic epistemology with its own methods and problems. He defined this field as the study of [[child development]] as a means of answering epistemological questions. |
||
=== |
===Schema=== |
||
A |
A schema (plural form: ''schemata'') is a structured cluster of concepts, it can be used to represent objects, scenarios or sequences of events or relations. The philosopher [[Immanuel Kant]] first proposed the concept of schemata as innate structures used to help us perceive the world.<ref>Eysenck, Michael W. and Keane, Mark. T. (2010). ''Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook'', (6th.). East Sussex: Psychology Press.. |
||
</ref> |
</ref> |
||
A schema |
A schema is a mental framework that is created as children interact with their physical and social environments.<ref>Naested, I., Potvin, B., & Waldron, P. (2004). Understanding the landscape of teaching. Toronto, Ontario: Pearson Education Canada.</ref> For example, many 3-year-olds insist that the sun is alive because it comes up in the morning and goes down at night. According to Piaget, these children are operating based on a simple cognitive schema that things that move are alive. At any age, children rely on their current cognitive structures to understand the world around them. Moreover, younger and older children may often interpret and respond to the same objects and events in very different ways because cognitive structures take different forms at different ages.<ref>Shaffer, D. R., Wood, E., & Willoughby, T. (2005). Developmental psychology: Childhood and adolescence. Toronto, Ontario: Nelson Education Canada.</ref> |
||
Piaget (1953) described three kinds of intellectual structures: behavioural (or sensorimotor) schemata, symbolic schemata, and operational schemata. |
Piaget (1953) described three kinds of intellectual structures: behavioural (or sensorimotor) schemata, symbolic schemata, and operational schemata. |
||
*''Behavioural schemata'': organized patterns of behaviour that are used to represent and respond to objects and experiences. |
*''Behavioural schemata'': organized patterns of behaviour that are used to represent and respond to objects and experiences. |
||
*''Symbolic schemata'': internal mental symbols (such as images or verbal codes) that one uses to represent aspects of experience. |
*''Symbolic schemata'': internal mental symbols (such as images or verbal codes) that one uses to represent aspects of experience. |
||
*''Operational schemata'': internal mental activity that one performs on objects of thought.<ref>Piaget, J. (1953). The origin of intelligence in the child. New Fetter Lane, New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.</ref> |
*''Operational schemata'': internal mental activity that one performs on objects of thought.<ref>Piaget, J. (1953). The origin of intelligence in the child. New Fetter Lane, New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.</ref> |
||
According to Piaget, children use the process of [[Constructivist theory|assimilation and accommodation]] to create a schema or mental framework for how they perceive and/or interpret what they are experiencing. As a result, the early concepts of young children tend to be more global or general in nature.<ref name="Auger, W. F. 2007">Auger, W. F., & Rich, S. J. (2007). Curriculum theory and methods: Perspectives on learning and teaching. Mississauga, Ontario: John Wiley & Sons Canada.</ref> |
According to Piaget, children use the process of [[Constructivist theory|assimilation and accommodation]] to create a schema or mental framework for how they perceive and/or interpret what they are experiencing. As a result, the early concepts of young children tend to be more global or general in nature.<ref name="Auger, W. F. 2007">Auger, W. F., & Rich, S. J. (2007). Curriculum theory and methods: Perspectives on learning and teaching. Mississauga, Ontario: John Wiley & Sons Canada.</ref> |
||
Similarly, Gallagher and Reid (1981) maintained that adults view |
Similarly, Gallagher and Reid (1981) maintained that adults view children's concepts as highly generalized and even inaccurate. With added experience, interactions, and maturity, these concepts become refined and more detailed. Overall, making sense of the world from a child's perspective is a very complex and time-consuming process.<ref>Gallagher, J. M., & Reid, D. K. (1981). The learning theory of Piaget and Inhelder. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.</ref> |
||
A schema is: |
|||
Schemata are: |
|||
* |
*A critically important building block of conceptual development |
||
*Constantly in the process of being modified or changed |
*Constantly in the process of being modified or changed |
||
*Modified by on-going experiences |
*Modified by on-going experiences |
||
Line 174: | Line 186: | ||
These schemata are constantly being revised and elaborated upon each time the child encounters new experiences. In doing this children create their own unique understanding of the world, interpret their own experiences and knowledge, and subsequently use this knowledge to solve more complex problems. In a neurological sense, the brain/mind is constantly working to build and rebuild itself as it takes in, adapts/modifies new information, and enhances understanding.<ref name="Auger, W. F. 2007"/> |
These schemata are constantly being revised and elaborated upon each time the child encounters new experiences. In doing this children create their own unique understanding of the world, interpret their own experiences and knowledge, and subsequently use this knowledge to solve more complex problems. In a neurological sense, the brain/mind is constantly working to build and rebuild itself as it takes in, adapts/modifies new information, and enhances understanding.<ref name="Auger, W. F. 2007"/> |
||
====The physical microstructure of schemata==== |
|||
In his ''Biology and Knowledge'' (1967+ / French 1965), Piaget tentatively hinted at possible physical embodiments for his abstract schema entities. At the time, there was much talk and research about RNA as such an agent of learning, and Piaget considered some of the evidence. However, he did not offer any firm conclusions, and confessed that this was beyond his area of expertise. Piaget died in 1980, and by then the RNA theory had lost its appeal. |
|||
==Research methods== |
==Research methods== |
||
Piaget wanted to revolutionize the way |
Piaget wanted to revolutionize the way research was conducted. Although he started researching with his colleagues using a traditional method of data collection, he was not fully satisfied with the results and wanted to keep trying to find new ways of researching using a combination of data, which included [[naturalistic observation]], [[psychometrics]], and the psychiatric clinical examination, in order to have a less guided form of research that would produce more empirically valid results. |
||
As Piaget developed new research methods, he wrote a book called ''The Language and Thought of the Child'', which aimed to synthesize the methods he was using in order to study the |
As Piaget developed new research methods, he wrote a book called ''The Language and Thought of the Child'', which aimed to synthesize the methods he was using in order to study the conclusions children drew from situations and how they arrived at such conclusions. The main idea was to observe how children responded and articulated certain situations with their own reasoning, in order to examine their thought processes (Mayer, 2005). |
||
Piaget administered a test in 15 boys with ages ranging from |
Piaget administered a test in 15 boys with ages ranging from 10 to 14 years in which he asked participants to describe the relationship between a mixed bouquet of flowers and a bouquet with flowers of the same color. The purpose of this study was to analyze the thinking process the boys had and to draw conclusions about the logic processes they had used, which was a psychometric technique of research. Piaget also used the psychoanalytic method initially developed by Sigmund Freud. The purpose of using such method was to examine the unconscious mind, as well as to continue parallel studies using different research methods. Psychoanalysis was later rejected by Piaget, as he thought it was insufficiently empirical (Mayer, 2005). |
||
Piaget argued that children and adults used speech for different purposes. In order to confirm his argument, he experimented analyzing a |
Piaget argued that children and adults used speech for different purposes. In order to confirm his argument, he experimented analyzing a child's interpretation of a story. In the experiment, the child listened to a story and then told a friend that same story in his/her/their own words. The purpose of this study was to examine how children verbalize and understand each other without adult intervention. Piaget wanted to examine the limits of naturalistic observation, in order to understand a child's reasoning. He realized the difficulty of studying children's thoughts, as it is hard to know if a child is pretending to believe their thoughts or not. Piaget was the pioneer researcher to examine children's conversations in a social context – starting from examining their speech and actions – where children were comfortable and spontaneous (Kose, 1987). |
||
===Issues and possible solutions=== |
===Issues and possible solutions=== |
||
After conducting many studies, Piaget |
After conducting many studies, Piaget found significant differences in the way adults and children reason. He could not find the path of logic reasoning and the unspoken thoughts children had, which would allow him to study a child's intellectual development over time (Mayer, 2005). In his third book, ''The Child's Conception of the World'', Piaget recognized the difficulties of his prior techniques and the importance of psychiatric clinical examination. The researcher believed that the way clinical examinations were conducted influenced how a child's inner realities surfaced. Children would likely respond according to the way the research is conducted, the questions asked, or the familiarity they have with the environment. The clinical examination conducted for his third book provides a thorough investigation into a child's thinking process. An example of a question used to research such process was: "Can you see a thought?" (Mayer, 2005, p. 372). |
||
In his third book, ''The Child’s Conception of the World'', Piaget recognized the difficulties of his prior techniques and the importance of psychiatric clinical examination. The researcher believed that the way clinical examinations were conducted influenced how a child’s inner realities surfaced. Children would likely respond according to the way the research is conducted, the questions asked, or the familiarity they have with the environment. The clinical examination conducted for his third book provides a thorough investigation into a child’s thinking process. An example of a question used to research such process was: "Can you see a thought?" (Mayer, 2005, p. 372). |
|||
===Development of new methods=== |
|||
Piaget recognized that psychometric tests had its limitations, as children were not able to provide the researcher with their deepest thoughts and inner intellect. It was also difficult to know if the results of child examination reflected what children believed or if it is just a pretend situation. For example, it is very difficult to know with certainty if a child who has a conversation with a toy believes the toy is alive or if the child is just pretending. |
|||
Soon after drawing conclusions about psychometric studies, Piaget started developing the clinical method of examination. The clinical method included questioning a child and carefully examining their responses -in order to observe how the child reasoned according to the questions asked - and then examine the child’s perception of the world through their responses. Piaget recognized the difficulties of interviewing a child and the importance of recognizing the difference between "liberated" versus "spontaneous" responses (Mayer, 2005, p. 372). |
|||
====Criticism of Piaget's research methods==== |
|||
"The developmental theory of Jean Piaget has been criticized on the grounds that it is conceptually limited, empirically false, or philosophically and epistemologically untenable." (Lourenço & Machado, 1996, p. 143) Piaget responded to criticism by acknowledging that the vast majority of critics did not understand the outcomes he wished to obtain from his research (Lourenço & Machado, 1996). |
|||
===Piaget replaces psychometric tests with the clinical method approach=== |
|||
As Piaget believed development was a universal process, his initial sample sizes were inadequate, particularly in the formulation of his theory of infant development.<ref>Siegel, L. S. (1993). Amazing new discovery: Piaget was wrong! Canadian Psychology, 34(3): 234-249.</ref> Piaget’s theories of infant development were based on his observations of his own three children. While this clearly presents problems with the sample size, Piaget also probably introduced confounding variables and social desirability into his observations and his conclusions based on his observations. It is entirely possible Piaget conditioned his children to respond in a desirable manner, so, rather than having an understanding of object permanence, his children might have learned to behave in a manner that indicated they understood object permanence. The sample was also very homogenous, as all three children had a similar genetic heritage and environment. Piaget did, however, have larger sample sizes during his later years. |
|||
Piaget recognized that psychometric tests had its limitations, as children were not able to provide the researcher with their deepest thoughts and inner intellect. It was also difficult to know if the results of child examination reflected what children believed or if it is just a pretend situation. For example, it is very difficult to know with certainty if a child who has a conversation with a toy believes the toy is alive or if the child is just pretending. Soon after drawing conclusions about psychometric studies, Piaget started developing the clinical method of examination. The clinical method included questioning a child and carefully examining their responses – in order to observe how the child reasoned according to the questions asked – and then examining the child's perception of the world through their responses. Piaget recognized the difficulties of interviewing a child and the importance of recognizing the difference between "liberated" versus "spontaneous" responses (Mayer, 2005, p. 372). |
|||
Piaget wanted to research in environments that would allow children to connect with some existing aspects of the world. The idea was to change the approach described in his book ''The Child's Conception of the World'' and move away from the vague questioning interviews. This new approach was described in his book ''The Child's Conception of Physical Causality'', where children were presented with dilemmas and had to think of possible solutions on their own. Later, after carefully analyzing previous methods, Piaget developed a combination of naturalistic observation with clinical interviewing in his book ''Judgment and Reasoning in the Child'', where a child's intellect was tested with questions and close monitoring. Piaget was convinced he had found a way to analyze and access a child's thoughts about the world in a very effective way (Mayer, 2005). Piaget's research provided a combination of theoretical and practical research methods and it has offered a crucial contribution to the field of developmental psychology (Beilin, 1992). "Piaget is often criticized because his method of investigation, though somewhat modified in recent years, is still largely clinical". He observes a child's surroundings and behavior. He then comes up with a hypothesis testing it and focusing on both the surroundings and behavior after changing a little of the surrounding.<ref>Phillips, John L. (1969). ''The Origin of Intellect: Piaget's Theory''. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. {{ISBN|0-7167-0579-6}}</ref> |
|||
===Development of research methods=== |
|||
Piaget wanted to research in environments that would allow children to connect with some existing aspects of the world. The idea was to change the approach described in his book ''The Child’s Conception of the World'' and move away from the vague questioning interviews. This new approach was described in his book ''The Child’s Conception of Physical Causality'', where children were presented with dilemmas and had to think of possible solutions on their own. |
|||
Later, after carefully analyzing previous methods, Piaget developed a combination of naturalistic observation with clinical interviewing in his book ''Judgment and Reasoning in the Child'', where a child's intellect was tested with questions and close monitoring. Piaget was convinced he had found a way to analyze and access a child’s thoughts about the world in a very effective way. (Mayer, 2005) |
|||
Piaget’s research provided a combination of theoretical and practical research methods and it has offered a crucial contribution to the field of developmental psychology (Beilin, 1992). |
|||
"Piaget is often criticized because his method of investigation, though somewhat modified in recent years, is still largely clinical". He observes a child's surroundings and behavior. He then comes up with a hypothesis testing it and focusing on both the surroundings and behavior after changing a little of the surrounding. (Phillips, 1969) |
|||
== Influence == |
== Influence == |
||
[[File:JPiaget-PBovet-1925.jpg|thumb |
[[File:JPiaget-PBovet-1925.jpg|thumb|Photo of the Jean Piaget Foundation with [[Pierre Bovet]] (1878–1965) first row (with large beard) and Jean Piaget (1896–1980) first row (on the right, with glasses) in front of the [[Rousseau Institute]] (Geneva), 1925]] |
||
Despite his ceasing to be a fashionable [[psychologist]], the magnitude of Piaget's continuing influence can be measured by the global scale and activity of the [[Jean Piaget Society]], which holds annual conferences and attracts around 700 participants.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.piaget.org/Symposium/2011/JPS-2011-DRAFT-Program-May14.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150911221631/http://www.piaget.org/Symposium/2011/JPS-2011-DRAFT-Program-May14.pdf |archive-date=2015-09-11 |url-status=live |title=41st Annual Meeting of The Jean Piaget Society |website=Piaget.prg |date=2 June 2011 |access-date=17 October 2016}}</ref> His [[theory of cognitive development]] has proved influential in many different areas: |
|||
Despite his ceasing to be a fashionable [[psychologist]], the magnitude of Piaget's continuing influence can be measured by the global scale and activity of the [[Jean Piaget Society]], which holds annual conferences and attracts around 700 participants.<ref>http://www.piaget.org/Symposium/2011/JPS-2011-DRAFT-Program-May14.pdf</ref> His [[theory of cognitive development]] has proved influential in many different areas: |
|||
*[[Developmental psychology]] |
*[[Developmental psychology]] |
||
*[[Education]] and [[Morality]] |
*[[Education]] and [[Morality]] |
||
Line 221: | Line 217: | ||
=== Developmental psychology === |
=== Developmental psychology === |
||
Piaget is considered the most influential figure in developmental psychology, though many of aspects of his theories are no longer accepted by mainstream psychologists. Developmental psychologists today do not view development as taking place in [[Stage theory|stages]]<ref name="Gopnik 1996">{{cite journal |title=The Post-Piaget Era |journal=Psychological Science |year=1996 |last=Gopnik |first=Alison |volume=7 |issue=4 |pages=221–225 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00363.x |s2cid=143973228 |url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00363.x |access-date=2021-04-01 }}</ref><ref name="Hopkins 2011" /> and many of Piaget's empirical findings have been overturned by subsequent research.<ref name="Gopnik Wellman">{{cite journal |title=Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory |journal=Psychological Bulletin |year=2012 |last1=Gopnik |first1=A. |last2=Wellman |first2=H.M. |volume=138 |issue=6 |pages=1085–1108 |doi=10.1037/a0028044 |pmid=22582739 |pmc=3422420 }}</ref> For example, psychologists no longer view young children as being incapable of understanding abstract concepts,<ref name="IzardSann2009" /> and no longer believe that babies do not understand object permanence.<ref name="Dehaene 2020" /> Despite this, developmental psychologists do acknowledge the importance of Jean Piaget's legacy as the founder of their field. They recognize his innovative empirical work, his attempts to integrate his results into a unified theoretical model and the way he created a path for subsequent researchers to follow.<ref name="Klahr" /> Indeed, many developmental psychology researchers today work in a post-Piagetian or [[Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development|neo-Piagetian]] framework.<ref name="Martí">{{cite encyclopedia |last=Martí |first=E. |encyclopedia=Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education |title=Post-Piagetian Perspectives of Cognitive Development |year=2020 |publisher=Oxford University Press |doi=10.1093/acrefore/9780190264093.013.914 |isbn=978-0-19-026409-3 }}</ref><ref name="Morra">{{cite book | last = Morra | first = Sergio | title = Cognitive development: neo-Piagetian perspectives | publisher = Erlbaum | location = New York | year = 2008 | isbn = 978-0-8058-6350-5 }}</ref> |
|||
Piaget is the most influential developmental psychologist to date (Lourenço, O. and Machado, A., 1996), influencing not only the work of [[Lev Vygotsky]] and of [[Lawrence Kohlberg]] but whole generations of eminent academics.{{Clarify|date=December 2012}} Although subjecting his ideas to massive scrutiny led to innumerable improvements and qualifications of his original model and the emergence of a plethora of neo-Piagetian and post-Piagetian variants, Piaget's original model has proved to be remarkably robust (Lourenço and Machado 1996). |
|||
=== Education |
=== Education === |
||
During the 1970s and 1980s, Piaget's works also inspired the transformation of European and American [[education]], including both theory and practice, leading to a more ‘child-centered’ approach. In ''Conversations with Jean Piaget'', he says: "Education, for most people, means trying to lead the child to resemble the typical adult of his society ... but for me and no one else, education means making creators... You have to make inventors, innovators—not conformists" (Bringuier, 1980, p. 132). |
|||
By using Piaget's theory, educators focus on their students as learners. As a result of this focus, [[education]] is learner-centered and constructivist-based to an extent. It allows teachers to view students as individual learners who add new concepts to prior knowledge to construct, or build, understanding for themselves.<ref name="ReferenceA">{{cite journal|last1=Henson|first1=Kenneth|title=Foundations for Learner-Centered Education: A Knowledge Base|journal=Education|date=2003|volume=1124|issue=1|pages=5–16}}</ref> Teachers who use a learner-centered approach as a basis for their professional practices incorporate the several dispositions.<ref name="ReferenceA"/> They provide experience-based educational opportunities. These teachers also contemplate the learners' individual qualities and attitudes during curriculum planning. Educators allow learners' insights to alter the curriculum. They nourish and support learners' curiosity. They also involve learners' emotions and create a learning environment in which students feel safe.<ref name="ReferenceA"/> |
|||
There are two differences between the preoperational and concrete operational stages that apply to education. These differences are reversibility and decentration. At times, reversibility and decentration occur at the same time.<ref name="Educational Psychology">{{cite book|last1=Seifert|first1=Kelvin|last2=Sutton|first2=Rosemary|title=Educational Psychology|date=2009|publisher=Orange Grove|location=Florida|isbn=978-1-61610-154-1|edition=2nd|url=http://www.saylor.org/site/wp-content/uploads/2012/06/Educational-Psychology.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131025015203/http://www.saylor.org/site/wp-content/uploads/2012/06/Educational-Psychology.pdf |archive-date=2013-10-25 |url-status=live|access-date=22 June 2015}}</ref> When students think about the steps to complete a task without using a particular logical, sequential order, they are using reversibility.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> Decentration allows them to concentrate on multiple components of a problematic task at a time.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> Students use both reversibility and decentration to function throughout the school day, follow directions, and complete assignments. |
|||
An example of a student using reversibility is when learning new vocabulary. The student creates a list of unfamiliar words from a literary text. Then, he researches the definition of those words before asking classmate to test him. His teacher has given a set of particular instructions that he must follow in a particular order: he must write the word before defining it, and complete these two steps repeatedly.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> A child in the preoperational stage gets confused during this process and needs assistance from the teacher to stay on task. The teacher refers him back to his text in order to notate the next word before he can define it.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> A child in the preoperational stage does not understand the organization required to complete this assignment. One in the concrete operational stage understands the organization, and can recall the steps in any order while being able to follow the order given.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> Using decentration, the child has the two activities on his mind: identify words and find them in the dictionary.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> |
|||
A sample of decentration is a preschooler may use a toy banana as a pretend telephone. The child knows the difference between the fruit and a phone. In this form of play, he is operating on two levels at once.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> In an older child at the concrete operational level, decentration allows him to complete subtraction of two-digit numbers and indicate which of the problems also involved borrowing from the other column. The student simultaneously does both.<ref name="Educational Psychology"/> Using reversibility, the student has to move mentally between two subtasks. |
|||
Regarding the giving of praise by teachers, praise is a reinforcer for students. Adolescents undergo social-emotional development such that they seek rapport with peers. Thus, teacher praise is not as powerful for students who see teachers as authority figures. They give no value to praise provided by adults, or they have no respect for the individual who is giving praise.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hawkins|first1=Shannon M.|last2=Heflin|first2=L. Juane|title=Increasing Secondary Teachers' Behavior-Specific Praise Using a Video Self-Modeling and Visual Performance Feedback Intervention|journal=Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions|date=2001|volume=12|issue=2|pages=97–108|doi=10.1177/1098300709358110|s2cid=143631715}}<!--|access-date=July 7, 2015--></ref> |
|||
During the 1970s and 1980s, Piaget's works also inspired the transformation of European and American education, including theory and practice, leading to a more 'child-centered' approach. In ''Conversations with Jean Piaget'', Bringuier says: "Education, for most people, means trying to lead the child to resemble the typical adult of his society ... but for me and no one else, education means making creators... You have to make inventors, innovators—not conformists" (Bringuier, 1980, p. 132). |
|||
His [[Piaget's theory of cognitive development|theory of cognitive development]] can be used as a tool in the [[early childhood education|early childhood]] classroom. According to Piaget, children developed best in a classroom with [[Social interaction|interaction]]. |
His [[Piaget's theory of cognitive development|theory of cognitive development]] can be used as a tool in the [[early childhood education|early childhood]] classroom. According to Piaget, children developed best in a classroom with [[Social interaction|interaction]]. |
||
Piaget defined knowledge as the ability to modify, transform, and "operate on" an object or idea, such that it is understood by the operator through the process of transformation.<ref name="Piaget">Piaget, J. (1964). Development and learning. In R.E. Ripple |
Piaget defined knowledge as the ability to modify, transform, and "operate on" an object or idea, such that it is understood by the operator through the process of transformation.<ref name="Piaget">Piaget, J. (1964). "Development and learning". In R.E. Ripple and V.N. Rockcastle (Eds.), ''Piaget Rediscovered: A Report on the Conference of Cognitive Studies and Curriculum Development'' (pp. 7–20). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.</ref> Learning, then, occurs as a result of experience, both physical and logical, with the objects themselves and how they are acted upon. Thus, knowledge must be assimilated in an active process by a learner with matured mental capacity, so that knowledge can build in complexity by scaffolded understanding. Understanding is scaffolded by the learner through the process of equilibration, whereby the learner balances new knowledge with previous understanding, thereby compensating for "transformation" of knowledge.<ref name="Piaget"/> |
||
Learning, then, can also be supported by instructors in an educational setting. Piaget specified that knowledge cannot truly be formed until the learner has matured the mental structures to which that learning is specific, and thereby development constrains learning. Nevertheless, knowledge can also be "built" by building on simpler operations and structures that have already been formed. Basing operations of an advanced structure on those of simpler structures thus scaffolds learning to build on operational abilities as they develop. Good teaching, then, is built around the operational abilities of the students such that they can excel in their operational stage and build on preexisting structures and abilities and thereby "build" learning.<ref name="Piaget"/> |
Learning, then, can also be supported by instructors in an educational setting. Piaget specified that knowledge cannot truly be formed until the learner has matured the mental structures to which that learning is specific, and thereby development constrains learning. Nevertheless, knowledge can also be "built" by building on simpler operations and structures that have already been formed. Basing operations of an advanced structure on those of simpler structures thus scaffolds learning to build on operational abilities as they develop. Good teaching, then, is built around the operational abilities of the students such that they can excel in their operational stage and build on preexisting structures and abilities and thereby "build" learning.<ref name="Piaget"/> |
||
Evidence of the effectiveness of a contemporary curricular design building on Piaget's theories of developmental progression and the support of maturing mental structures can be seen in Griffin and Case's "Number Worlds" curriculum.<ref name |
Evidence of the effectiveness of a contemporary curricular design building on Piaget's theories of developmental progression and the support of maturing mental structures can be seen in Griffin and Case's "Number Worlds" curriculum.<ref name="Number Worlds">{{cite journal | last1 = Griffin | first1 = S.A. | year = 2004 | title = Building number sense with Number Worlds: a mathematics program for young children | journal = Early Childhood Research Quarterly | volume = 19 | pages = 173–180 | doi=10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.012}}</ref> The curriculum works toward building a "central conceptual structure" of number sense in young children by building on five instructional processes, including aligning curriculum to the developmental sequencing of acquisition of specific skills. By outlining the developmental sequence of number sense, a conceptual structure is built and aligned to individual children as they develop. |
||
The cognitive scientist Karen Fuson has argued that the impact of Piagetian theories in education has not been entirely positive because his work has frequently been misinterpreted. In particular, Piaget's focus on children's interactions with objects in the concrete operational stage has led to an approach to education in which young children are encouraged to learn mathematics by manipulating real objects, but without the necessary direct instruction from teachers that they need to understand what they are doing and to link their activities to symbolic mathematics. This has had a particularly negative impact on low-attaining children who need more support from a more knowledgeable other to make meaning and progress with their learning.<ref name="Fuson2009">{{cite journal|last1=Fuson|first1=Karen C.|title=Avoiding misinterpretations of Piaget and Vygotsky: Mathematical teaching without learning, learning without teaching, or helpful learning-path teaching?|journal=Cognitive Development|volume=24|issue=4|year=2009|pages=343–361|issn=0885-2014|doi=10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.09.009}}</ref> |
|||
=== Education: Morality === |
|||
Piaget believed in two basic principles relating to [[moral education]]: that children develop moral ideas in stages and that children create their conceptions of the world. According to Piaget, "the child is someone who constructs his own moral world view, who forms ideas about right and wrong, and fair and unfair, that are not the direct product of adult teaching and that are often maintained in the face of adult wishes to the contrary" (Gallagher, 1978, p. 26). Piaget believed that children made moral judgments based on their own observations of the world. |
|||
Psychologist [[Mark Seidenberg]] has criticised the field of [[Education Studies]] for placing too much emphasis on the works of Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky and other historical psychologists while failing to keep up with the major advances in cognitive science in the decades since they were active.<ref name=Seidenberg2017>{{cite book | last = Seidenberg | first = Mark | title = Language at the speed of sight: how we read, why so many can't, and what can be done about it | publisher = Basic Books | page=260 | location = New York | year = 2017 | isbn = 978-1-5416-1715-5 }}</ref> Meanwhile, a 2016 [[systematic review]] of education research showed that constructivist approaches to early childhood education inspired by Piaget and Vygotsky are less effective than comprehensive approaches that incorporate direct skills teaching.<ref name="Chambers Cheung Slavin 2016 pp. 88–111">{{cite journal | last1=Chambers | first1=Bette | last2=Cheung | first2=Alan C.K. | last3=Slavin | first3=Robert E. | title=Literacy and language outcomes of comprehensive and developmental-constructivist approaches to early childhood education: A systematic review | journal=Educational Research Review | publisher=Elsevier BV | volume=18 | year=2016 | issn=1747-938X | doi=10.1016/j.edurev.2016.03.003 | pages=88–111}}</ref> |
|||
Piaget's theory of [[morality]] was radical when his book ''The Moral Judgment of the Child'' was published in 1932 for two reasons: his use of philosophical criteria to define morality (as universalizable, generalizable, and obligatory) and his rejection of equating [[Norm (sociology)|cultural norms]] with moral norms. Piaget, drawing on [[Immanuel Kant|Kantian]] theory, proposed that morality developed out of peer interaction and that it was autonomous from authority mandates. Peers, not parents, were a key source of moral concepts such as equality, reciprocity, and justice. |
|||
=== Morality === |
|||
Piaget attributed different types of psychosocial processes to different forms of social [[Interpersonal relationship|relationships]], introducing a fundamental distinction between different types of said relationships. Where there is constraint because one participant holds more power than the other the relationship is [[asymmetrical]], and, importantly, the [[knowledge]] that can be acquired by the dominated participant takes on a fixed and inflexible form. Piaget refers to this process as one of social transmission, illustrating it through reference to the way in which the elders of a [[tribe]] initiate younger members into the patterns of beliefs and practices of the group. Similarly, where adults exercise a dominating influence over the growing child, it is through social transmission that children can acquire knowledge. By contrast, in [[cooperative]] relations, power is more evenly distributed between participants so that a more symmetrical relationship emerges. Under these conditions, authentic forms of intellectual exchange become possible; each partner has the freedom to project his or her own thoughts, consider the positions of others, and defend his or her own point of view. In such circumstances, where children’s thinking is not limited by a dominant influence, Piaget believed "the reconstruction of knowledge", or favorable conditions for the emergence of constructive solutions to problems, exists. Here the knowledge that emerges is open, flexible and regulated by the logic of argument rather than being determined by an external authority. In short, cooperative relations provide the arena for the emergence of operations, which for Piaget requires the absence of any constraining influence, and is most often illustrated by the relations that form between peers (for more on the importance of this distinction see Duveen & Psaltis, 2008; Psaltis & Duveen, 2006, 2007). |
|||
Piaget believed in two basic principles relating to [[character education]]: that children develop moral ideas in stages and that children create their conceptions of the world. According to Piaget, "the child is someone who constructs his own moral world view, who forms ideas about right and wrong, and fair and unfair, that are not the direct product of adult teaching and that are often maintained in the face of adult wishes to the contrary" (Gallagher, 1978, p. 26). Piaget believed that children made moral judgments based on their own observations of the world. |
|||
Piaget's theory of [[morality]] was radical when his book ''[[The Moral Judgment of the Child]]'' was published in 1932 for two reasons: his use of philosophical criteria to define morality (as universalizable, generalizable, and obligatory) and his rejection of equating [[social norm|cultural norms]] with moral norms. Piaget, drawing on [[Immanuel Kant|Kantian]] theory, proposed that morality developed out of peer interaction and that it was autonomous from authority mandates. Peers, not parents, were a key source of moral concepts such as equality, reciprocity, and justice. |
|||
Piaget attributed different types of psychosocial processes to different forms of social [[Interpersonal relationship|relationships]], introducing a fundamental distinction between different types of said relationships. Where there is constraint because one participant holds more power than the other the relationship is [[asymmetrical]], and, importantly, the [[knowledge]] that can be acquired by the dominated participant takes on a fixed and inflexible form. Piaget refers to this process as one of social transmission, illustrating it through reference to the way in which the elders of a [[tribe]] initiate younger members into the patterns of beliefs and practices of the group. Similarly, where adults exercise a dominating influence over the growing child, it is through social transmission that children can acquire knowledge. By contrast, in [[cooperative]] relations, power is more evenly distributed between participants so that a more symmetrical relationship emerges. Under these conditions, authentic forms of intellectual exchange become possible; each partner has the freedom to project his or her own thoughts, consider the positions of others, and defend his or her own point of view. In such circumstances, where children's thinking is not limited by a dominant influence, Piaget believed "the reconstruction of knowledge", or favorable conditions for the emergence of constructive solutions to problems, exists. Here the knowledge that emerges is open, flexible and regulated by the logic of argument rather than being determined by an external authority. |
|||
In short, cooperative relations provide the arena for the emergence of operations, which for Piaget requires the absence of any constraining influence, and is most often illustrated by the relations that form between peers (for more on the importance of this distinction see Duveen & Psaltis, 2008; Psaltis & Duveen, 2006, 2007). This is thus how, according to Piaget, children learn ''moral judgement'' as opposed to ''cultural norms'' (or maybe [[ideology|ideological]] norms). |
|||
Piaget's research on morality was highly influential in subsequent work on [[moral development]], particularly in the case of Lawrence Kohlberg's highly influential [[Lawrence Kohlberg's stages of moral development|stage theory of moral development]]<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Levine |first1=Charles |last2=Kohlberg |first2=Lawrence |last3=Hewer |first3=Alexandra |title=The Current Formulation of Kohlberg's Theory and a Response to Critics |journal=Human Development |date=1985 |volume=28 |issue=2 |pages=94–100 |doi=10.1159/000272945}}</ref> which dominated moral psychology research until the end of the twentieth century.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Shweder |first1=Richard A. |last2=Haidt |first2=Jonathan |title=The Future of Moral Psychology: Truth, Intuition, and the Pluralist Way |journal=Psychological Science |date=6 May 2016 |volume=4 |issue=6 |pages=360–365 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00582.x|s2cid=143483576 }}</ref> |
|||
=== Historical studies of thought and cognition === |
=== Historical studies of thought and cognition === |
||
Line 245: | Line 260: | ||
Notable examples include: |
Notable examples include: |
||
*[[Michael Horace Barnes]]' study of the co-evolution of religious and scientific thinking<ref name="isbn0-19-513389-7">{{cite book |author=Barnes, Michael Horace |title=Stages of thought: the co-evolution of religious thought and science |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford [Oxfordshire] |year=2000 |
*[[Michael Horace Barnes]]' study of the co-evolution of religious and scientific thinking<ref name="isbn0-19-513389-7">{{cite book |author=Barnes, Michael Horace |title=Stages of thought: the co-evolution of religious thought and science |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford [Oxfordshire] |year=2000 |isbn=978-0-19-513389-9 |url=https://archive.org/details/stagesofthoughtc00barn }}</ref> |
||
*Peter Damerow's theory of prehistoric and archaic thought<ref name=Damerow1998>{{cite |
*Peter Damerow's theory of prehistoric and archaic thought<ref name=Damerow1998>{{cite book | author = Damerow, P. | year = 1998 | title = Prehistory And Cognitive Development | journal = Piaget, Evolution, and Development | url = https://books.google.com/books?id=haCAIME9vnEC&q=Prehistory+and+cognitive+development&pg=PA247 | access-date = 24 March 2008 | publisher = Routledge | isbn = 978-0-8058-2210-6}}</ref> |
||
*[[Kieran Egan (educationist)|Kieran Egan]]'s [[The Educated Mind|stages of understanding]]<ref>{{cite book|author= |
*[[Kieran Egan (educationist)|Kieran Egan]]'s [[The Educated Mind|stages of understanding]]<ref>{{cite book|author=Kieran Egan |title=The educated mind: How Cognitive Tools Shape Our Understanding|location=Chicago | publisher=[[University of Chicago Press]] |year=1997 |isbn=978-0-226-19036-5|author-link=Kieran Egan (educationist)}}</ref> |
||
*[[James W. Fowler]]'s [[stages of faith development]] |
*[[James W. Fowler]]'s [[stages of faith development]] |
||
* |
*Suzi Gablik's stages of art history<ref name="isbn0847800822.">{{cite book |author=Gablik, Suzi |title=Progress in art |publisher=Rizzoli |location=New York |year=1977 |isbn=978-0-8478-0082-7 }}</ref> |
||
*Christopher Hallpike's studies of changes in cognition and moral judgment in pre-historical, archaic and classical periods ... (Hallpike 1979, 2004) |
*Christopher Hallpike's studies of changes in cognition and moral judgment in pre-historical, archaic and classical periods ... (Hallpike 1979, 2004) |
||
*[[Lawrence Kohlberg]]'s [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development|stages of moral development]] |
*[[Lawrence Kohlberg]]'s [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development|stages of moral development]] |
||
*Don Lepan's theory of the origins of modern thought and drama<ref name="isbn0-333-45796-X">{{cite book |author=LePan, Don |title=The cognitive revolution in Western culture |publisher=Macmillan |location=New York |year=1989 |
*Don Lepan's theory of the origins of modern thought and drama<ref name="isbn0-333-45796-X">{{cite book |author=LePan, Don |title=The cognitive revolution in Western culture |publisher=Macmillan |location=New York |year=1989 |isbn=978-0-333-45796-2 |url=https://archive.org/details/cognitiverevolut0000lepa }}</ref> |
||
*Charles Radding's theory of the medieval intellectual development<ref name="isbn0-8078-1664-7">{{cite book |author=Radding, Charles |title=A world made by men: cognition and society, |
*Charles Radding's theory of the medieval intellectual development<ref name="isbn0-8078-1664-7">{{cite book |author=Radding, Charles |title=A world made by men: cognition and society, 400–1200 |publisher=University of North Carolina Press |location=Chapel Hill |year=1985 |isbn=978-0-8078-1664-6 }}</ref> |
||
*[[Jürgen Habermas]]'s reworking of [[historical materialism]]. |
*[[Jürgen Habermas]]'s reworking of [[historical materialism]]. |
||
=== Non |
=== Non-human development === |
||
Neo-Piagetian stages have been applied to the maximum stage attained by various animals. For example [[spider]]s attain the circular sensory motor stage, coordinating actions and perceptions. [[Pigeon]]s attain the sensory motor stage, forming concepts.{{ |
Neo-Piagetian stages have been applied to the maximum stage attained by various animals. For example, [[spider]]s attain the circular sensory motor stage, coordinating actions and perceptions. [[Pigeon]]s attain the sensory motor stage, forming concepts.<ref>{{cite web |last1=Dore |first1=F.Y |title=Psychology of animal cognition: Piagetian studies. |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1987-33562-001 |website=psychnet.apa |publisher=Psychological Bulletin |access-date=8 October 2020}}</ref> |
||
===Origins=== |
===Origins=== |
||
Line 263: | Line 278: | ||
=== Primatology === |
=== Primatology === |
||
Piaget's models of cognition have also been applied outside the human sphere, and some primatologists assess the development and abilities of primates in terms of Piaget's model.<ref name="isbn0-8018-6012-1">{{cite book | |
Piaget's models of cognition have also been applied outside the human sphere, and some primatologists assess the development and abilities of primates in terms of Piaget's model.<ref name="isbn0-8018-6012-1">{{cite book |author1=McKinney, Michael L. |author2=Parker, Sue Taylor |title=Origins of intelligence: the evolution of cognitive development in monkeys, apes, and humans |publisher=Johns Hopkins University Press |location=Baltimore |year=1999 |isbn=978-0-8018-6012-6}}</ref> |
||
=== Philosophy === |
=== Philosophy === |
||
Philosophers have used Piaget's work. For example, the [[philosopher]] and [[social theorist]] [[Jürgen Habermas]] has incorporated Piaget into his work, most notably in ''[[The Theory of Communicative Action]].'' The philosopher [[Thomas Kuhn]] credited Piaget's work with helping him to understand the transition between modes of thought which characterized his theory of [[paradigm shift]]s.<ref name="Burman, J. T. 2007">{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1177/0959354307079306 | last1 = Burman | first1 = J. T. | year = 2007 | title = Piaget No 'Remedy' for Kuhn, But the Two Should be Read Together: Comment on Tsou's 'Piaget vs. Kuhn on Scientific Progress' | journal = Theory & Psychology | volume = 17 | issue = 5| pages = 721–732 | s2cid = 145497321 }}</ref> Yet, that said, it is also noted that the implications of his later work do indeed remain largely unexamined.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Burman | first1 = J. T. | year = 2008 | title = Experimenting in relation to Piaget: Education is a chaperoned process of adaptation | journal = Perspectives on Science | volume = 16 | issue = 2| pages = 160–195 | doi = 10.1162/posc.2008.16.2.160 | s2cid = 57572564 }}</ref> Shortly before his death (September 1980), Piaget was involved in a debate about the relationships between innate and acquired features of language, at the Centre Royaumont pour une Science de l'Homme, where he discussed his point of view with the linguist [[Noam Chomsky]] as well as [[Hilary Putnam]] and [[Stephen Toulmin]]. |
|||
=== Artificial intelligence === |
=== Artificial intelligence === |
||
Piaget also had a considerable effect in the field of [[computer science]] and [[artificial intelligence]]. |
Piaget also had a considerable effect in the field of [[computer science]] and [[artificial intelligence]]. [[Seymour Papert]] used Piaget's work while developing the [[Logo programming language]]. [[Alan Kay]] used Piaget's theories as the basis for the [[Dynabook]] programming system concept, which was first discussed within the confines of the Xerox Palo Alto Research Center ([[Xerox PARC]]). These discussions led to the development of the [[Alto (computer)|Alto]] prototype, which explored for the first time all the elements of the [[graphical user interface]] (GUI), and influenced the creation of user interfaces in the 1980s and beyond.<ref name="isbn978-0262041201">{{cite book |author=Drescher, Gary |title=Made-Up Minds: A Constructivist Approach to Artificial Intelligence |publisher=MIT Press |location=Boston |year=1991 |page=236 |isbn=978-0-262-04120-1}}</ref> |
||
== |
==Criticisms== |
||
Piaget's theory, however vital in understanding child psychology, did not go without scrutiny. A main figure whose ideas contradicted Piaget's ideas was the Russian psychologist [[Lev Vygotsky]]. Vygotsky stressed the importance of a child's cultural background as an effect to the stages of development. Because different cultures stress different social interactions, this challenged Piaget's theory that the hierarchy of learning development had to develop in succession. Vygotsky introduced the term [[Zone of proximal development]] as an overall task a child would have to develop that would be too difficult to develop alone. |
|||
===Criticisms of Piaget's methods=== |
|||
Also, the so-called [[neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development]] maintained that Piaget's theory does not do justice either to the underlying mechanisms of [[information processing]] that explain transition from stage to stage or [[individual differences]] in cognitive development. According to these theories, changes in information processing mechanisms, such as [[speed of processing]] and [[working memory]], are responsible for ascension from stage to stage. Moreover, differences between individuals in these processes explain why some individuals develop faster than other individuals ([[Demetriou]], 1998). |
|||
Judged by today's standards of psychological research, Piaget's research methods can be considered problematic. One modern reviewer said many of his "pioneering investigations would probably be rejected from most modern journals on methodological grounds of sample size, non-standard measurement, and lack of inter-rater reliability".<ref name="Klahr">{{cite book |last1=Klahr |first1=D. |title=Developmental psychology: revisiting the classic studies |chapter=Revisiting Piaget: A perspective from studies of children's problem-solving abilities |editor-last=Slater |editor-first=A.M. |editor2-last=Quinn |editor2-first=P.C. |location=London |publisher=SAGE |year=2012 |isbn=978-0-85702-757-3}}</ref> |
|||
Over time, alternative theories of Child Development have been put forward, and empirical findings have done a lot to undermine Piaget's theories. For example Esther Thelen and colleagues<ref>{{Cite doi|10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00955.x}}</ref> found that babies would not make the [[A-not-B error]] if they had small weights added to their arms during the first phase of the experiment that were then removed before the second phase of the experiment. This minor change should not impact babies' understanding of object permanence, so the difference that this makes to babies' performance on the A-not-B task cannot be explained by Piagetian theory. Thelen and colleagues also found that various other factors also influenced performance on the A-not-B task (including strength of memory trace, salience of targets, waiting time and stance), and proposed that this could be better explained using a [[dynamic systems theory]] approach than using Piagetian theory. [[Alison Gopnik]] and Betty Repacholi<ref>{{cite journal | title = Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds | journal = Developmental Psychology | year = 1997 | first = Betty | last = Repacholi |author2=Alison Gopnik | volume = 3 | pages = 12–21| id = | url = http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/dev/33/1/12/ | accessdate = 29 October 2011 | doi=10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12}}</ref> found that babies as young as 18 months old can understand that other people have desires, and that these desires could be very different from their own desires. This strongly contradicts Piaget's view that children are very egocentric at this age. |
|||
Piaget's research relied on very small [[Sample (statistics)|samples]] that were not [[selection bias|randomly selected]]. His book ''The Origins of Intelligence in Children'' was based on the study of just his own three children.<ref name="Hopkins 2011">{{cite journal |title=The Enduring Influence of Jean Piaget |journal=APS Observer |year=2011 |last=Hopkins |first=J.R. |volume=24 |issue=10}}</ref> This means that it is difficult to generalize his findings to the broader population. He interacted closely with his research subjects and did not follow a set script, meaning that experimental conditions were not the same from participant to participant. |
|||
See also [[Brian Rotman]]'s ''Jean Piaget: Psychologist of the Real'', an exposition and critique of Piaget's ideas, and Jonathan Tudge and Barbara Rogoff's "Peer influences on cognitive development: Piagetian and Vygotskian perpectives".<ref>{{cite book |chapterurl=http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=RnWym5-bm8kC&oi=fnd&pg=PA32 |title=Lev Vygotsky: Critical Assessments, Volume 3 |first1=Jonathan |last1=Tudge |author2=Barbara Rogoff |editor1=Peter Lloyd |editor2=Charles Fernyhough |publisher=Routledge |year=1998 |chapter=Peer influences on cognitive development: Piagetian and Vygotskian perpectives |isbn=978-0-415-11154-6 }}</ref> |
|||
[https://www.webmd.com/children/piaget-stages-of-development Other shortcomings of Piaget’s theory include overestimating an adolescent's cognitive abilities, underestimating an infant’s, and overlooking how much cultural and social factors affect children’s thinking..] |
|||
As Piaget worked in the era before widespread use of voice recording equipment, his data collection method was simply to make handwritten notes in the field, which he would analyse himself.<ref name="Klahr" /> This differs from the modern practice of using multiple [[Coding (social sciences)|coders]] to ensure [[test validity]]. Critics such as [[Linda Siegel]] have argued that his experiments did not adequately control for social context and the child's understanding (or lack of understanding) of the language used in the test task, leading to mistaken conclusions about children's lack of reasoning skills.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1037/h0078835|title=Amazing new discovery: Piaget was wrong!|journal=Canadian Psychology|volume=34|issue=3|pages=239–245|year=1993|last1=Siegel|first1=Linda S}}</ref> |
|||
These methodological issues mean scientists trying to replicate Piaget's experiments have found that small changes to his procedures lead to different results. For example, in his tests of object-permanence and conservation of number, the ages at which children pass the tests varies greatly based on small variations in the test procedure, challenging his theoretical interpretations of his test results.<ref name="Klahr" /><ref name="Dehaene 2020">{{cite book | last = Dehaene | first = Stanislas | title = How we learn: why brains learn better than any machine ... for now | pages=162–163 | publisher = Viking | location = New York, New York | year = 2020 | isbn = 978-0-525-55989-4 }}</ref> |
|||
===Criticisms of Piaget's theoretical ideas=== |
|||
Piaget's theories have not gone without scrutiny. A figure whose ideas contradicted Piaget's ideas was the Russian psychologist [[Lev Vygotsky]]. Vygotsky stressed the importance of a child's cultural background as an effect on the stages of development. Because different cultures stress different social interactions, this challenged Piaget's theory that the hierarchy of learning development had to develop in succession. Vygotsky introduced the term [[Zone of proximal development]] as an overall task a child would have to develop that would be too difficult to develop alone. |
|||
Also, the so-called [[neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development]] maintained that Piaget's theory does not do justice either to the underlying mechanisms of [[Information processing (psychology)|information processing]] that explain transition from stage to stage or [[individual differences]] in cognitive development. According to these theories, changes in information processing mechanisms, such as [[cognitive processing speed|speed of processing]] and [[working memory]], are responsible for ascension from stage to stage. Moreover, differences between individuals in these processes explain why some individuals develop faster than other individuals ([[Demetriou]], 1998). |
|||
Over time, alternative theories of child development have been put forward, and empirical findings have done a lot to undermine Piaget's theories. For example, Esther Thelen and colleagues<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Spencer | first1 = J. P. | last2 = Clearfield | first2 = M. | last3 = Corbetta | first3 = D. | last4 = Ulrich | first4 = B. | last5 = Buchanan | first5 = P. | last6 = Schöner | first6 = G. | doi = 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00955.x | title = Moving Toward a Grand Theory of Development: In Memory of Esther Thelen | journal = Child Development | volume = 77 | issue = 6 | pages = 1521–1538 | year = 2006 | pmid = 17107442| citeseerx = 10.1.1.531.5232 }}</ref> found that babies would not make the [[A-not-B error]] if they had small weights added to their arms during the first phase of the experiment that were then removed before the second phase of the experiment. This minor change should not impact babies' understanding of object permanence, so the difference that this makes to babies' performance on the A-not-B task cannot be explained by Piagetian theory. Thelen and colleagues also found that various other factors also influenced performance on the A-not-B task (including strength of memory trace, salience of targets, waiting time and stance), and proposed that this could be better explained using a [[dynamic systems theory]] approach than using Piagetian theory. [[Alison Gopnik]] and Betty Repacholi<ref>{{cite journal | title = Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds | journal = Developmental Psychology | year = 1997 | first = Betty | last = Repacholi |author2=Alison Gopnik | volume = 3 | issue = 1 | pages = 12–21| doi=10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12 | pmid = 9050386}}</ref> found that babies as young as 18 months old can understand that other people have desires, and that these desires could be very different from their own desires. This contradicts Piaget's view that children are very egocentric at this age. |
|||
Modern cognitive science had undermined Piaget's view that young children are unable to comprehend numbers as they are not able to work with abstract concepts in the sensorimotor stage. This Piagetian view has led many educators to believe that it is not appropriate to teach simple arithmetic to young children as it will not lead to real understanding.<ref name="Dehaene 2011">{{cite book |last1=Dehaene |first1=Stanislas |title=The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics |edition=2 |location=New York |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-19-975387-1 }}</ref> Experiments by Starkey et al. have shown that children have an understanding of abstract numbers from as young as 6 months old while more recent studies by Izard et al. have shown that even newborns can perceive abstract numbers.<ref name="StarkeySpelke1990">{{cite journal|last1=Starkey|first1=Prentice|last2=Spelke|first2=Elizabeth S.|last3=Gelman|first3=Rochel|title=Numerical abstraction by human infants|journal=Cognition|volume=36|issue=2|year=1990|pages=97–127|issn=0010-0277|doi=10.1016/0010-0277(90)90001-Z|pmid=2225757|s2cid=706365}}</ref><ref name="IzardSann2009">{{cite journal|last1=Izard|first1=V.|last2=Sann|first2=C.|last3=Spelke|first3=E.S.|last4=Streri|first4=A.|title=Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|volume=106|issue=25|year=2009|pages=10382–10385|issn=0027-8424|doi=10.1073/pnas.0812142106|pmid=19520833|pmc=2700913|bibcode=2009PNAS..10610382I|doi-access=free}}</ref> For a full discussion of this, see [[Stanislas Dehaene|Stanislas Dehaene's]] ''The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics''.<ref name="Dehaene 2011" /> |
|||
Some supporters of Piaget counter that his critics' arguments depend on misreadings of Piaget's theory.<ref name="lm">{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.143 | last1 = Lourenço | first1 = O. | last2 = Machado | first2 = A. | year = 1996 | title = In defense of Piaget's theory: A reply to ten common criticisms | journal = Psychological Review | volume = 103 | issue = 1| pages = 143–164 | s2cid = 32390745 | url = http://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/2dec/e70e2b917d7e3f846db9285878811d1ddbe3.pdf | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20200410214048/http://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/2dec/e70e2b917d7e3f846db9285878811d1ddbe3.pdf | archive-date = 2020-04-10 }}</ref> See also [[Brian Rotman]]'s ''Jean Piaget: Psychologist of the Real'', an exposition and critique of Piaget's ideas, and Jonathan Tudge and Barbara Rogoff's "Peer influences on cognitive development: Piagetian and Vygotskian perspectives".<ref>{{cite book |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=RnWym5-bm8kC&pg=PA32 |title=Lev Vygotsky: Critical Assessments, Volume 3 |first1=Jonathan |last1=Tudge |author2=Barbara Rogoff |editor1=Peter Lloyd |editor2=Charles Fernyhough |publisher=Routledge |year=1998 |chapter=Peer influences on cognitive development: Piagetian and Vygotskian perspectives |isbn=978-0-415-11154-6 }}</ref> |
|||
== List of major achievements == |
|||
=== Appointments === |
|||
*1921–25 Research Director (''Chef des travaux''), [[Rousseau Institute|Institut Jean-Jacques Rousseau]], Geneva |
|||
*1925–29 Professor of Psychology, Sociology and the Philosophy of Science, [[University of Neuchatel]] |
|||
*1929–39 ''Professeur extraordinaire'' of the History of Scientific Thought, [[University of Geneva]] |
|||
*1929–67 Director, [[International Bureau of Education]], Geneva |
|||
*1932–71 Director, [[Institute of Educational Sciences]], University of Geneva |
|||
*1938–51 Professor of Experimental Psychology and Sociology, [[University of Lausanne]] |
|||
*1939–51 Professor of Sociology, University of Geneva |
|||
*1940–71 ''Professeur ordinaire'' of Experimental Psychology, University of Geneva |
|||
*1952–64 Professor of Genetic Psychology, [[University of Paris|Sorbonne]], Paris |
|||
*1954–57 President, [[International Union of Psychological Science|International Union of Scientific Psychology]] <!-- Election reported in the American Journal of Psychology, 67(3), 553 --> |
|||
*1955–80 Director, [[International Centre for Genetic Epistemology]], Geneva |
|||
*1971–80 Emeritus Professor, University of Geneva |
|||
=== Honorary doctorates === |
|||
*1936 Harvard |
|||
*1946 Sorbonne |
|||
*1949 University of Brazil |
|||
*1949 Bruxelles |
|||
*1953 Chicago |
|||
*1954 McGill |
|||
*1958 Warsaw |
|||
*1959 Manchester |
|||
*1960 Oslo |
|||
*1960 Cambridge |
|||
*1962 Brandeis |
|||
*1964 Montreal |
|||
*1964 Aix-Marseille |
|||
*1966 Pennsylvania<ref>The list is certain only to 1966. The source is p. xviii of F. Bresson & M. de Montmollin, 1966, ''Psychologie et épistémologie génétique: thèmes Piagétiens (Hommage à Jean Piaget avec une bibliographie complète de ses oeuvres)''. Paris: Dunod. (Note: This list provides "Varsovie" instead of Warsaw, as this is the French name for the capital of Poland.)</ref> |
|||
*1966? Barcelona<ref>Reported in 1971, in Anuario de psicología, as part of the proceedings of a celebration of Piaget's 70th birthday, [http://www.raco.cat/index.php/AnuarioPsicologia/issue/view/4930/showToc raco.cat]</ref> |
|||
*1970 Yale<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Kessen | first1 = W | year = 1996 | title = American Psychology just before Piaget | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 7 | issue = 4| pages = 196–199 | doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00358.x | jstor=40062944| s2cid = 145615307 }}</ref> |
|||
*1971 Temple<ref>[https://www.temple.edu/about/history-traditions/honorary-degrees/past-honorary-degree-recipients Past Honorary Degree Recipients], Temple University</ref> |
|||
<!-- |
|||
== Piagetian and post-Piagetian stage theories == |
|||
*Cheryl Armon's stages of reasoning about the good (Armon, 1984) |
|||
*[[Michael Horace Barnes]]' historical stages of religious and scientific thinking<ref name="isbn0-19-513389-7"/> |
|||
*[[Michael Commons]]' [[Model of hierarchical complexity]]<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Commons | first1 = M. L. | last2 = Goodheart | first2 = E. A. | last3 = Pekker | first3 = A. | last4 = Dawson | first4 = T.L. | last5 = Draney | first5 = K. | last6 = Adams | first6 = K. M. | year = 2008 | title = Using Rasch Scaled Stage Scores To Validate Orders of Hierarchical Complexity of Balance Beam Task Sequences | journal = Journal of Applied Measurement | volume = 9 | issue = 2| pages = 182–99 | pmid = 18480514 }}</ref> |
|||
*[[Peter Damerow]]'s theory of prehistoric and archaic thought [https://books.google.com/books?id=Zo4FGwAACAAJ&cad=2] (Damerow 1995) |
|||
*[[Andreas Demetriou]]'s [[Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development]] (Demetriou, et al. 2010) |
|||
*[[Kieran Egan (educationist)|Kieran Egan]]'s [[The Educated Mind|stages of understanding]] |
|||
*Kurt W. Fischer's dynamic skill theory (Fischer, 1980) |
|||
*[[James W. Fowler]]'s [[stages of faith development]] |
|||
*[[Christopher Hallpike]]'s historical stages of cognitive moral understanding (Hallpike 1979, 2004) |
|||
*[[Allen Ivey]]'s developmental counseling and therapy (DCT) (Ivey 1986) |
|||
*[[Robert Kegan]]'s constructive-developmental theory (Kegan 1982) |
|||
*[[Lawrence Kohlberg]]'s [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development|stages of moral development]] |
|||
*[[Don Lepan]]'s theory of the origins of modern thought and drama (LePan 1989) |
|||
*[[Keith S. Lockwood]]'s constructivist practice with children who are deaf or hard of hearing (Lockwood 2006) |
|||
*[[Gablik]]'s stages of art history<ref name="isbn0847800822."/> |
|||
*[[Charles Radding]]'s theory of the medieval intellectual development (Radding 1985) |
|||
*[[R.J. Robinson]]'s stages of history (Robinson 2004) |
|||
*[[Zendra Marie Moore]]'s Theory of color. (Moore 2006) |
|||
*[[Constance Kamii]]'s research and practice on teaching math to young children (Kamii 1985) |
|||
--> |
|||
== List of major works == |
== List of major works == |
||
The following groupings are based on the number of citations in [[Google Scholar]]. |
The following groupings are based on the number of citations in [[Google Scholar]]. |
||
=== |
=== The most-cited publications in English === <!--1,500 to more than 2,500 citations in Google Scholar--> |
||
*''The Language and Thought of the Child'' (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, |
*''The Language and Thought of the Child'' (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1926) [''Le Langage et la pensée chez l'enfant'' (1923)] |
||
*''The Child's Conception of the World'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1928) [''La Représentation du monde chez l'enfant'' (1926, orig. pub. as an article, 1925)] |
*''The Child's Conception of the World'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1928) [''La Représentation du monde chez l'enfant'' (1926, orig. pub. as an article, 1925)] |
||
*[[:File:Piaget first edition copyright page.jpg#filehistory|''Judgment and Reasoning in the Child'' (Harcourt, Brace and Company 1928).]] |
|||
*''[https://archive.org/details/moraljudgmentoft005613mbp The Moral Judgment of the Child]'' (London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co., 1932) [{{lang|fr|Le jugement moral chez l'enfant}} (1932)] |
|||
*''[[iarchive:moraljudgmentoft005613mbp|The Moral Judgment of the Child]]'' (London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co., 1932) [''Le jugement moral chez l'enfant'' (1932)] |
|||
*''The Origins of Intelligence in Children'' (New York: International University Press, 1952) [La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant (1936), also translated as ''The Origin of Intelligence in the Child'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1953)]. |
|||
*''The Origins of Intelligence in Children'' (New York: International University Press, 1952) [''La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant'' (1936), also translated as ''The Origin of Intelligence in the Child'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1953)]. |
|||
*''Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood'' (New York: Norton, 1962) [''La formation du symbole chez l'enfant; imitation, jeu et reve, image et représentation'' (1945)]. |
*''Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood'' (New York: Norton, 1962) [''La formation du symbole chez l'enfant; imitation, jeu et reve, image et représentation'' (1945)]. |
||
*''The Psychology of Intelligence'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, |
*''The Psychology of Intelligence'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1950) [''La psychologie de l'intelligence'' (1947)]. |
||
*''The construction of reality in the child'' (New York: Basic Books, 1954) [''La construction du réel chez l'enfant'' (1950), also translated as ''The Child's Construction of Reality'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1955)]. |
*''The construction of reality in the child'' (New York: Basic Books, 1954) [''La construction du réel chez l'enfant'' (1950), also translated as ''The Child's Construction of Reality'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1955)]. |
||
*With Inhelder, B., ''The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence'' (New York: Basic Books, 1958) [''De la logique de l'enfant à la logique de l'adolescent'' (1955)]. |
*With Inhelder, B., ''The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence'' (New York: Basic Books, 1958) [''De la logique de l'enfant à la logique de l'adolescent'' (1955)]. |
||
*With Inhelder, B., ''The Psychology of the Child'' (New York: Basic Books, 1962) [''La psychologie de l'enfant'' (1966, orig. pub. as an article, 1950)]. |
*With Inhelder, B., ''The Psychology of the Child'' (New York: Basic Books, 1962) [''La psychologie de l'enfant'' (1966, orig. pub. as an article, 1950)]. |
||
*''The early growth of logic in the child'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1964) [''La genèse des structures logiques elementaires'' (1959)]. |
|||
=== Major works === <!--More than 1,000 citations--> |
|||
*''The early growth of logic in the child'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1964) [''La genese des structures logiques elementaires'' (1959)]. |
|||
*With Inhelder, B., ''The Child's Conception of Space'' (New York: W.W. Norton, 1967). |
*With Inhelder, B., ''The Child's Conception of Space'' (New York: W.W. Norton, 1967). |
||
*"Piaget's theory" in P. Mussen (ed.), ''Handbook of Child Psychology'', Vol. 1. (4th ed., New York: Wiley, 1983). |
*"Piaget's theory" in P. Mussen (ed.), ''Handbook of Child Psychology'', Vol. 1. (4th ed., New York: Wiley, 1983). |
||
*''The Child's Conception of Number'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1952) [''La |
*''The Child's Conception of Number'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1952) [''La genèse du nombre chez l'enfant'' (1941)]. |
||
*''Structuralism'' (New York: Harper & Row, 1970) [''Le Structuralisme'' (1968)]. |
*''Structuralism'' (New York: Harper & Row, 1970) [''Le Structuralisme'' (1968)]. |
||
*''Genetic epistemology'' (New York: W.W. Norton, 1971, ISBN |
*''Genetic epistemology'' (New York: W.W. Norton, 1971, {{ISBN|978-0-393-00596-7}}). |
||
=== |
=== Less-cited === <!--250 to more than 500 citations--> |
||
*''The child's conception of physical causality'' (London: Kegan Paul, 1930) [''La causalite physique chez l'enfant'' (1927)] |
*''The child's conception of physical causality'' (London: Kegan Paul, 1930) [''La causalite physique chez l'enfant'' (1927)] |
||
*''Child's Conception of Geometry'' (New York, Basic Books, 1960) [''La Géométrie spontanée de l'enfant'' (1948)]. |
*''Child's Conception of Geometry'' (New York, Basic Books, 1960) [''La Géométrie spontanée de l'enfant'' (1948)]. |
||
*''The Principles of Genetic Epistemology'' (New York: Basic Books, 1972, ISBN |
*''The Principles of Genetic Epistemology'' (New York: Basic Books, 1972, {{ISBN|978-0-393-00596-7}}) [''L'épistémologie génétique'' (1950)]. |
||
*''To understand is to invent: The future of education'' (New York: Grossman Publishers, 1973) [tr. of ''Ou va l'education'' (1971) and ''Le droit a l'education dans le monde actuel'' (1948)]. |
*''To understand is to invent: The future of education'' (New York: Grossman Publishers, 1973) [tr. of ''Ou va l'education'' (1971) and ''Le droit a l'education dans le monde actuel'' (1948)]. |
||
*''Six psychological studies'' (New York: Random House, 1967) [''Six études de psychologie'' (1964)]. |
*''Six psychological studies'' (New York: Random House, 1967) [''Six études de psychologie'' (1964)]. |
||
Line 314: | Line 402: | ||
*Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini (ed.), ''Language and learning: the debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky'' (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1980) [''Theories du language, theories de l'apprentissage'' (1979)]. |
*Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini (ed.), ''Language and learning: the debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky'' (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1980) [''Theories du language, theories de l'apprentissage'' (1979)]. |
||
*''Development and learning''. |
*''Development and learning''. |
||
=== Other works === <!--More than 250 citations--> |
|||
*''The Grasp of Consciousness: Action and concept in the young child'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1977) [''La prise de conscience'' (1974)]. |
*''The Grasp of Consciousness: Action and concept in the young child'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1977) [''La prise de conscience'' (1974)]. |
||
*''The Mechanisms of Perception'' (New York: Basic Books, 1969) [''Les mécanismes perceptifs: modèles probabilistes, analyse génétique, relations avec l'intelligence'' (1961)]. |
*''The Mechanisms of Perception'' (New York: Basic Books, 1969) [''Les mécanismes perceptifs: modèles probabilistes, analyse génétique, relations avec l'intelligence'' (1961)]. |
||
Line 325: | Line 411: | ||
*''Mental imagery in the child: a study of the development of imaginal representation'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1971) [L'image mentale chez l'enfant : études sur le développement des représentations imaginées (1966)]. |
*''Mental imagery in the child: a study of the development of imaginal representation'' (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1971) [L'image mentale chez l'enfant : études sur le développement des représentations imaginées (1966)]. |
||
*''Intelligence and Affectivity. Their Relationship during Child Development'' (Palo Alto: Annual Reviews, 1981) [''Les relations entre l'intelligence et l'affectivité dans le développement de l'enfant'' (1954)]. |
*''Intelligence and Affectivity. Their Relationship during Child Development'' (Palo Alto: Annual Reviews, 1981) [''Les relations entre l'intelligence et l'affectivité dans le développement de l'enfant'' (1954)]. |
||
*With Garcia, R. ''Psychogenesis and the History of Science'' (New York: Columbia University Press, 1989) [''Psychogenèse et histoire des sciences'' (1983). |
|||
*With [[E.W. Beth|Beth, E. W.]],''Mathematical Epistemology and Psychology'' (Dordrecht: D. Reidel, 1966) [''Épistémologie mathématique et psychologie: Essai sur les relations entre la logique formelle et la pensée réelle''] (1961). |
*With [[E.W. Beth|Beth, E. W.]],''Mathematical Epistemology and Psychology'' (Dordrecht: D. Reidel, 1966) [''Épistémologie mathématique et psychologie: Essai sur les relations entre la logique formelle et la pensée réelle''] (1961). |
||
* With Brown, Terrance. Eames, Stewart (Translator). ''Adaptation and Intelligence: Organic Selection and Phenocopy'' (University of Chicago Press, 1980). |
|||
*With Garcia, R. ''Psychogenesis and the History of Science'' (New York: Columbia University Press, 1989) ''Psychogenèse et histoire des sciences'' (1983). |
|||
=== New translations === |
=== New translations === |
||
*Piaget, J. (1995). ''Sociological Studies''. London: Routledge. |
*Piaget, J. (1995). ''Sociological Studies''. London: Routledge. |
||
*{{Cite journal | last1 = Piaget | first1 = J. | year = 2000 | title = Commentary on Vygotsky |
*{{Cite journal | last1 = Piaget | first1 = J. | year = 2000 | title = Commentary on Vygotsky | journal = New Ideas in Psychology | volume = 18 | issue = 2–3 | pages = 241–59 | doi = 10.1016/S0732-118X(00)00012-X }} |
||
*Piaget, J. (2001). ''Studies in Reflecting Abstraction''. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. |
*Piaget, J. (2001). ''Studies in Reflecting Abstraction''. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. |
||
<!-- |
<!-- |
||
Line 337: | Line 424: | ||
*Recherche (1918) |
*Recherche (1918) |
||
*Classes, relations et nombres: essai sur les groupements de la logistique et sur la réversibilité de la pensée (1942) |
*Classes, relations et nombres: essai sur les groupements de la logistique et sur la réversibilité de la pensée (1942) |
||
*Piaget, J. (1950). ''Introduction à |
*Piaget, J. (1950). ''Introduction à l'Épistémologie Génétique.'' Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. |
||
*Essai sur les transformations des opérations logiques: les 256 opérations ternaires de la logique bivalente des propositions (1952) |
*Essai sur les transformations des opérations logiques: les 256 opérations ternaires de la logique bivalente des propositions (1952) |
||
*Les liaisons analytiques et synthétiques dans les comportements du sujet (1957) |
*Les liaisons analytiques et synthétiques dans les comportements du sujet (1957) |
||
Line 343: | Line 430: | ||
*Histoire et méthode (1963) |
*Histoire et méthode (1963) |
||
*Piaget, J. (1967). ''Logique et Connaissance scientifique'', Encyclopédie de la Pléiade. |
*Piaget, J. (1967). ''Logique et Connaissance scientifique'', Encyclopédie de la Pléiade. |
||
. |
|||
*Recherches sur la généralisation (1978) |
*Recherches sur la généralisation (1978) |
||
*Piaget, J. (1980). ''Les Formes Élémentaires de la Dialectique''. Paris, Editions Gallimard. |
*Piaget, J. (1980). ''Les Formes Élémentaires de la Dialectique''. Paris, Editions Gallimard. |
||
Line 355: | Line 441: | ||
=== Other works === |
=== Other works === |
||
*Piaget, J. (1942). Les trois structures fondamentales de la vie psychique: rythme, régulation et groupement. ''Rev. Suisse de Psychologie Appliquée'', 1/2 9–21. |
*Piaget, J. (1942). Les trois structures fondamentales de la vie psychique: rythme, régulation et groupement. ''Rev. Suisse de Psychologie Appliquée'', 1/2 9–21. |
||
*Piaget, J., Beth, E.W., Dieudonné, J., Lichnerowicz, A., Choquet, G., Gattegno, C. (1955). ''L'enseignement des mathématiques'', Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé. |
*Piaget, J., Beth, E.W., Dieudonné, J., Lichnerowicz, A., Choquet, G., Gattegno, C. (1955). ''L'enseignement des mathématiques'', Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé. |
||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1080/00207596608247041 | last1 = Piaget | first1 = J. | year = 1966 | title = Nécessité et signification des recherches comparatives en psychologie génétique |
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1080/00207596608247041 | last1 = Piaget | first1 = J. | year = 1966 | title = Nécessité et signification des recherches comparatives en psychologie génétique | journal = Journal International de Psychologie | volume = 1 | issue = 1| pages = 3–13 }} |
||
*Piaget, J. (1972). ''Insights and Illusions of Philosophy.'' London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. |
*Piaget, J. (1972). ''Insights and Illusions of Philosophy.'' London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. |
||
*Piaget, J. (1974). ''Experiments in Contradiction.'' Chicago: University of Chicago Press. |
*Piaget, J. (1974). ''Experiments in Contradiction.'' Chicago: University of Chicago Press. |
||
Line 367: | Line 452: | ||
*Piaget, J., and Garcia, R. (1991). ''Towards a Logic of Meanings''. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
*Piaget, J., and Garcia, R. (1991). ''Towards a Logic of Meanings''. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
||
--> |
--> |
||
== Major commentaries and critiques == |
|||
Piaget inspired innumerable studies and even new areas of inquiry. The following is a list of critiques and commentaries, organized using the same citation-based method as the list of his own major works (above). These represent the significant and influential post-Piagetian writings in their respective sub-disciplines. |
|||
=== Exemplars === <!--More than 5,000 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*[[Lev Vygotsky|Vygotsky, L.]] (1963). ''Thought and language''. [12630 citations] |
|||
=== Classics === <!--More than 1,000 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*[[Seymour Papert|Papert, S.]] (1980). ''[[Mindstorms: Children, Computers, and Powerful Ideas]]''. [4089] |
|||
*[[Marvin Minsky|Minsky, M.]] (1988). ''[[Society of Mind|The society of mind]]''. [3950] |
|||
*[[Lawrence Kohlberg|Kohlberg, L.]] (1969). ''Stage And Sequence: The Cognitive-Developmental Approach To Socialization''. [3118] |
|||
*[[John H. Flavell|Flavell, J.]] (1963). ''The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget''. [2333]<ref name="Muller-JADP2013">The development of the project that became this book, and its impact, is discussed in detail by {{cite journal|last1=Müller|first1=U.|last2=Burman|first2=J. T.|last3=Hutchison|first3=S. M.|title=The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget: A quinquagenary retrospective|journal=Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology|year=2013|volume=34|issue=1|pages=52–55|doi=10.1016/j.appdev.2012.10.001|issn=0193-3973}}</ref> |
|||
*[[Eleanor J. Gibson|Gibson, E. J.]] (1973). ''Principles of perceptual learning and development''. [1903] |
|||
*[[J. McVicker Hunt|Hunt, J. McV.]] (1961). ''Intelligence and Experience''. [617+395+384+111+167+32=1706] |
|||
*[[Andrew N. Meltzoff|Meltzoff, A. N.]] & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. [1497] |
|||
*Case, R. (1985). ''Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood''. [1456] |
|||
*Fischer, K. W. (1980). ''A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills''. [1001] |
|||
=== Major works === <!--More than 500 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*[[Elizabeth Bates|Bates, E.]] (1976). ''Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics''. [959] |
|||
*[[Herbert Ginsburg|Ginsburg, H. P.]] & Opper, S. (1969). ''Piaget's theory of intellectual development''. [931] |
|||
*Singley, M. K. & [[John Robert Anderson (psychologist)|Anderson, J. R.]] (1989). ''The transfer of cognitive skill''. [836] |
|||
*[[Eleanor Duckworth|Duckworth, E.]] (1973). The having of wonderful ideas. [775] |
|||
*Youniss, J. (1982). Parents and peers in social development: A Sullivan-Piaget perspective. [763] |
|||
*Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. [563] |
|||
*Schaffer, H. R. & Emerson, P. E. (1964). The development of social attachments in infancy. [535] |
|||
=== Works of significance === <!--More than 250 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*Shatz, M. & [[Rochel Gelman|Gelman, R.]] (1973). The Development of Communication Skills: Modifications in the Speech of Young Children as a Function of Listener. ''Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38''(5), pp. 1–37.[470] |
|||
*Broke, H. (1971). Interpersonal perception of young children: Egocentrism or Empathy? ''Developmental Psychology, 5''(2), pp. 263–269.[469] |
|||
*Wadsworth, B. J. (1989). Piaget's theory of cognitive and affective development [421] |
|||
*[[Annette Karmiloff-Smith|Karmiloff-Smith, A.]] (1992). ''Beyond Modularity''. [419] |
|||
*Bodner, G. M. (1986). ''Constructivism: A theory of knowledge''. [403] |
|||
*Shantz, C. U. (1975). ''The Development of Social Cognition''. [387] |
|||
*[[Adele Diamond|Diamond, A.]] & [[Patricia Goldman-Rakic|Goldman-Rakic, P. S.]] (1989). Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. ''Experimental Brain Research, 74''(1), pp. 24–40. [370] |
|||
*[[Howard Gruber|Gruber, H.]] & Voneche, H. (1982). ''The Essential Piaget''. [348] |
|||
*Walkerdine, V. (1984). Developmental psychology and the child-centred pedagogy: The insertion of Piaget into early education. [338] |
|||
*[[Constance Kamii|Kamii, C.]] & DeClark, G. (1985). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget's theory [335] |
|||
*Riegel, K. F. (1973). Dialectic operations: The final period of cognitive development [316] |
|||
*[[Albert Bandura|Bandura, A.]] & McDonald, F. J. (1963). Influence of social reinforcement and the behavior of models in shaping children's moral judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(3), pp. 274–281. [314] |
|||
*[[Robert Karplus|Karplus, R.]] (1980). Teaching for the development of reasoning. [312] |
|||
*Brainerd, C. (1978). The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory. [311] |
|||
*Brainerd, C. (1978). Piaget's theory of intelligence. [292] |
|||
*[[Carol Gilligan|Gilligan, C.]] (1997). Moral orientation and moral development [285] |
|||
*[[Adele Diamond|Diamond, A.]] (1991). Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development [284] |
|||
*Braine, M. D. S., & Rumain, B. (1983). Logical reasoning. [276] |
|||
*John-Steiner, V. (2000). Creative collaboration. [266] |
|||
*Pascual-Leone, J. (1987). Organismic processes for neo-Piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development. [261] |
|||
*Hallpike, C. R. (1979). ''The foundations of primitive thought'' [261] |
|||
*[[Hans G. Furth|Furth, H.]] (1969). ''Piaget and Knowledge'' [261] |
|||
*[[Rochel Gelman|Gelman, R.]] & [[Renee Baillargeon|Baillargeon, R.]] (1983). A review of some Piagetian concepts. [260] |
|||
*O'Loughlin, M. (1992). Rethinking science education: Beyond piagetian constructivism. Toward a sociocultural model of teaching and learning. [252] |
|||
== List of major achievements == |
|||
=== Appointments === |
|||
*1921-25 Research Director (''Chef des travaux''), Institut Jean-Jacques Rousseau, Geneva |
|||
*1925-29 Professor of Psychology, Sociology and the Philosophy of Science, University of Neuchatel |
|||
*1929-39 ''Professeur extraordinaire'' of the History of Scientific Thought, University of Geneva |
|||
*1929-67 Director, International Bureau of Education, Geneva |
|||
*1932-71 Director, Institute of Educational Sciences, University of Geneva |
|||
*1938-51 Professor of Experimental Psychology and Sociology, University of Lausanne |
|||
*1939-51 Professor of Sociology, University of Geneva |
|||
*1940-71 ''Professeur ordinaire'' of Experimental Psychology, University of Geneva |
|||
*1952-64 Professor of Genetic Psychology, Sorbonne, Paris |
|||
*1954-57 President, [[International Union of Scientific Psychology]] <!-- Election reported in the American Journal of Psychology, 67(3), 553 --> |
|||
*1955-80 Director, International Centre for Genetic Epistemology, Geneva |
|||
*1971-80 Emeritus Professor, University of Geneva |
|||
=== Honorary doctorates === |
|||
*1936 Harvard |
|||
*1946 Sorbonne |
|||
*1949 University of Brazil |
|||
*1949 Bruxelles |
|||
*1953 Chicago |
|||
*1954 McGill |
|||
*1958 Warsaw |
|||
*1959 Manchester |
|||
*1960 Oslo |
|||
*1960 Cambridge |
|||
*1962 Brandeis |
|||
*1964 Montreal |
|||
*1964 Aix-Marseille |
|||
*1966 Pennsylvania<ref>The list is certain only to 1966. The source is p. xviii of F. Bresson & M. de Montmollin, 1966, ''Psychologie et épistémologie génétique: thèmes Piagétiens (Hommage à Jean Piaget avec une bibliographie complète de ses oeuvres)''. Paris: Dunod. (Note: This list provides "Varsovie" instead of Warsaw, as this is the French name for the capital of Poland.)</ref> |
|||
*1966? Barcelona<ref>Reported in 1971, in Anuario de psicología, as part of the proceedings of a celebration of Piaget's 70th birthday, [http://www.raco.cat/index.php/AnuarioPsicologia/issue/view/4930/showToc raco.cat]</ref> |
|||
*1970 Yale<ref>Noted on p. 196 of Kessen, W. (1996). American Psychology just before Piaget. ''Psychological Science, 7''(4), 196-199. [http://www.jstor.org/pss/40062944 jstor.org]</ref> |
|||
<!-- |
|||
== Piagetian and post-Piagetian stage theories == |
|||
*Cheryl Armon's stages of reasoning about the good (Armon, 1984) |
|||
*[[Michael Horace Barnes]]' historical stages of religious and scientific thinking (Barnes 2000) |
|||
*[[Michael Commons]]' [[Model of hierarchical complexity]] (Commons, et al. 2008) |
|||
*[[Peter Damerow]]'s theory of prehistoric and archaic thought [http://books.google.com/books?id=Zo4FGwAACAAJ&dq=Prehistory+and+cognitive+development&cad=2] (Damerow 1995) |
|||
*[[Andreas Demetriou]]'s [[Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development]] (Demetriou, et al. 2010) |
|||
*[[Kieran Egan (educationist)|Kieran Egan]]'s [[The Educated Mind|stages of understanding]] |
|||
*Kurt W. Fischer's dynamic skill theory (Fischer, 1980) |
|||
*[[James W. Fowler]]'s [[stages of faith development]] |
|||
*[[Christopher Hallpike]]'s historical stages of cognitive moral understanding (Hallpike 1979, 2004) |
|||
*[[Allen Ivey]]'s developmental counseling and therapy (DCT) (Ivey 1986) |
|||
*[[Robert Kegan]]'s constructive-developmental theory (Kegan 1982) |
|||
*[[Lawrence Kohlberg]]'s [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development|stages of moral development]] |
|||
*[[Don Lepan]]'s theory of the origins of modern thought and drama (LePan 1989) |
|||
*[[Keith S. Lockwood]]'s constructivist practice with children who are deaf or hard of hearing (Lockwood 2006) |
|||
*[[Gablik]]'s stages of art history (Gablik 1977) |
|||
*[[Charles Radding]]'s theory of the medieval intellectual development (Radding 1985) |
|||
*[[R.J. Robinson]]'s stages of history (Robinson 2004) |
|||
*[[Zendra Marie Moore]]'s Theory of color. (Moore 2006) |
|||
*[[Constance Kamii]]'s research and practice on teaching math to young children (Kamii 1985) |
|||
--> |
|||
== Quotations == |
|||
* "Intelligence is what you use when you don't know what to do."{{Citation needed|date=September 2011}} |
|||
* "Intelligence organizes the world by organizing itself."<ref>''La Construction du Réel Chez l'Enfant'' by Jean Piaget (1937)</ref> |
|||
* "The principal goal of education in the schools should be creating men and women who are capable of doing new things, not simply repeating what other generations have done."<ref>Piaget, J. (1953) ''The Origins of Intelligence in Children''. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.</ref> |
|||
==See also== |
==See also== |
||
{{Portal|Psychology}} |
|||
* [[Active learning]] |
* [[Active learning]] |
||
* [[Cognitive acceleration]] |
* [[Cognitive acceleration]] |
||
Line 489: | Line 460: | ||
* [[Developmental psychology]] |
* [[Developmental psychology]] |
||
* [[Fluid and crystallized intelligence]] |
* [[Fluid and crystallized intelligence]] |
||
* [[Guðmundur Finnbogason]] |
|||
* [[Horizontal and vertical décalage]] |
|||
* [[Inquiry-based learning]] |
* [[Inquiry-based learning]] |
||
* [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development]] |
* [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development]] |
||
* [[Psychosocial development]] |
* [[Psychosocial development]] |
||
* [[Religious development]] |
|||
* [[Water-level task]] |
* [[Water-level task]] |
||
===Collaborators=== |
===Collaborators=== |
||
{{Div col|colwidth=20em}} |
|||
*[[Edith Ackermann]] |
|||
*[[Leo Apostel]] |
*[[Leo Apostel]] |
||
*[[Edgar Ascher]] |
*[[Edgar Ascher]] |
||
Line 501: | Line 477: | ||
*[[Guy Cellérier]] |
*[[Guy Cellérier]] |
||
*[[Paul Fraisse]] |
*[[Paul Fraisse]] |
||
*[[Rolando García ( |
*[[Rolando García (Argentine scientist)|Rolando García]] |
||
*[[Pierre Gréco]] |
*[[Pierre Gréco]] |
||
*[[Jean-Blaise Grize]] |
*[[Jean-Blaise Grize]] |
||
Line 513: | Line 489: | ||
*[[Hermina Sinclair de-Zwart]] |
*[[Hermina Sinclair de-Zwart]] |
||
*[[Alina Szeminska]] |
*[[Alina Szeminska]] |
||
*[[Huê Vinh-Bang]] |
*[[Huê Vinh-Bang]]{{citation needed|date=January 2022}} |
||
{{div col end}} |
|||
===Translators=== |
===Translators=== |
||
*[[Eleanor |
*[[Eleanor Duckworth]] |
||
*[[Wolfe Mays]] |
*[[Wolfe Mays]] |
||
==Notes== |
==Notes== |
||
{{Reflist|colwidth=30em}} |
{{Reflist|colwidth=30em}} |
||
62. http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=RnWym5-bm8kC&oi=fnd&pg=PA32&dq=challenging+piagetian+stages+of+cognitive+development&ots=6weMo4KEk_&sig=_zYhvLeClpOXBp3wEH2MpGUeYkM#v=onepage&q&f=false |
|||
== References == |
== References == |
||
{{Refbegin|colwidth=60em}} |
{{Refbegin|colwidth=60em}} |
||
* Aqueci, F. (2003). ''Ordine e trasformazione: morale, mente, discorso in Piaget''. Acireale-Roma: Bonanno. ISBN |
* Aqueci, F. (2003). ''Ordine e trasformazione: morale, mente, discorso in Piaget''. Acireale-Roma: Bonanno. {{ISBN|88-7796-148-1}}. |
||
* {{Cite journal | last1 = Amann-Gainotti | first1 = M. | last2 = Ducret | first2 = J.-J. | year = 1992 | title = Jean Piaget, disciple of |
* {{Cite journal | last1 = Amann-Gainotti | first1 = M. | last2 = Ducret | first2 = J.-J. | year = 1992 | title = Jean Piaget, disciple of Pierre Janet: Influence of behavior psychology and relations with psychoanalysis | journal = Information Psychiatrique | volume = 68 | pages = 598–606 | title-link = Pierre Janet }} |
||
* {{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191 | last1 = Beilin | first1 = H. | year = 1992 | title = Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology |
* {{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191 | last1 = Beilin | first1 = H. | year = 1992 | title = Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology | journal = Developmental Psychology | volume = 28 | issue = 2| pages = 191–204 | s2cid = 14459165 }} |
||
*Beilin, H. (1994). Jean Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology. A century of developmental psychology (pp. 257–290). Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association. |
*Beilin, H. (1994). Jean Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology. A century of developmental psychology (pp. 257–290). Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association. |
||
* Bringuier, J.-C. (1980). ''Conversations with Jean Piaget'' (B.M. Gulati, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1977) ISBN |
* Bringuier, J.-C. (1980). ''Conversations with Jean Piaget'' (B.M. Gulati, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1977) {{ISBN|0-226-07503-6}}. |
||
* Chapman, M. (1988). ''Constructive evolution: Origins and development of Piaget's thought''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN |
* Chapman, M. (1988). ''Constructive evolution: Origins and development of Piaget's thought''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. {{ISBN|0-521-36712-3}}. |
||
* {{Cite journal | last1 = Commons | first1 = M. L. | last2 = Goodheart | first2 = E. A. | last3 = Pekker | first3 = A. | last4 = Dawson | first4 = T.L. | last5 = Draney | first5 = K. | last6 = Adams | first6 = K. M. | year = 2008 | title = Using Rasch Scaled Stage Scores To Validate Orders of Hierarchical Complexity of Balance Beam Task Sequences | url = | journal = Journal of Applied Measurement | volume = 9 | issue = 2| pages = 182–99 | pmid = 18480514 }} |
|||
* Demetriou, A. (1998). Cognitive development. In A. Demetriou, W. Doise, K. F. M. van Lieshout (Eds.), ''Life-span developmental psychology'' (pp. 179–269). London: Wiley. |
* Demetriou, A. (1998). Cognitive development. In A. Demetriou, W. Doise, K. F. M. van Lieshout (Eds.), ''Life-span developmental psychology'' (pp. 179–269). London: Wiley. |
||
*Demetriou, A., Mouyi, A., & Spanoudis, G. (2010). The development of mental processing. Nesselroade, J. R. (2010). Methods in the study of life-span human development: Issues and answers. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), ''Biology, cognition and methods across the life-span. Volume 1 of the Handbook of life-span development'' (pp. 36–55), Editor-in-chief: R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. |
*Demetriou, A., Mouyi, A., & Spanoudis, G. (2010). The development of mental processing. Nesselroade, J. R. (2010). Methods in the study of life-span human development: Issues and answers. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), ''Biology, cognition and methods across the life-span. Volume 1 of the Handbook of life-span development'' (pp. 36–55), Editor-in-chief: R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. |
||
* Duveen, G. & Psaltis, C. (2008). The constructive role of asymmetries in social interaction. In U. Mueller, J. I. M. Carpendale, N. Budwig & B. Sokol (Eds.), ''Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development''. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. |
* Duveen, G. & Psaltis, C. (2008). The constructive role of asymmetries in social interaction. In U. Mueller, J. I. M. Carpendale, N. Budwig & B. Sokol (Eds.), ''Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development''. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. |
||
* |
*Eckblad, G. (1981). ''Scheme theory: A conceptual framework for cognitive-motivational processes''. London and New York: Academic Press. {{ISBN|978-0-12-229550-8}}. |
||
* |
*Flavell, J. (1967). ''The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget''. New York: D. Van Nostrand Company. {{ISBN|0-442-02413-4}}. |
||
*Fowler, J. W. (1981). ''Stages of faith: The psychology of human development and the quest for meaning''. San Francisco: Harper & Row. {{ISBN|0-06-062866-9}}. |
|||
*Gattico, E. (2001). ''Jean Piaget''. Milano: Bruno Mondadori. ISBN 88-424-9741-X. |
|||
* |
*Gattico, E. (2001). ''Jean Piaget''. Milano: Bruno Mondadori. {{ISBN|88-424-9741-X}}. |
||
* |
*Hallpike, C.R. (1979). ''The foundations of primitive thought''. Oxford: Oxford University Press. {{ISBN|0-19-823196-2}}. |
||
*Ivey, A. (1986). ''Developmental therapy''. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. {{ISBN|1-55542-022-2}}. |
|||
*Kamii, C. (1985). ''Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget's theory.'' New York: Teachers College Press. |
*Kamii, C. (1985). ''Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget's theory.'' New York: Teachers College Press. |
||
*Kesselring, T. (1999). ''Jean Piaget''. München: Beck. ISBN |
*Kesselring, T. (1999). ''Jean Piaget''. München: Beck. {{ISBN|3-406-44512-8}}. |
||
*Kassotakis, M. & Flouris, G. (2006) ''Μάθηση & Διδασκαλία'', |
*Kassotakis, M. & Flouris, G. (2006) ''Μάθηση & Διδασκαλία'', Athens. |
||
*Kitchener, R. (1986). ''Piaget's theory of knowledge: Genetic epistemology & scientific reason''. New Haven: Yale University Press. ISBN |
*Kitchener, R. (1986). ''Piaget's theory of knowledge: Genetic epistemology & scientific reason''. New Haven: Yale University Press. {{ISBN|0-300-03579-9}}. |
||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/h0091442 | last1 = Kose | first1 = G. | year = 1987 | title = A philosopher's conception of Piaget: Piagetian theory reconsidered |
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/h0091442 | last1 = Kose | first1 = G. | year = 1987 | title = A philosopher's conception of Piaget: Piagetian theory reconsidered | journal = Theoretical & Philosophical Psychology | volume = 7 | issue = 1| pages = 52–57 }} |
||
[http://cohort09devpsyc.commons.gc.cuny.edu/files/2010/01/Lourenco_1999.pdf CUNY pdf] |
|||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.143 | last1 = Lourenço | first1 = O. | last2 = Machado | first2 = A. | year = 1996 | title = In defense of Piaget's theory: A reply to ten common criticisms | url = | journal = Psychological Review | volume = 103 | issue = 1| pages = 143–164 }} |
|||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/1093-4510.8.4.362 | last1 = Mayer | first1 = S. | year = 2005 | title = The early evolution of Jean Piaget's clinical method |
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1037/1093-4510.8.4.362 | last1 = Mayer | first1 = S. | year = 2005 | title = The early evolution of Jean Piaget's clinical method | journal = History of Psychology | volume = 8 | issue = 4| pages = 362–382 | pmid = 17152748 }} |
||
*Messerly, J.G. (1992). ''Piaget's conception of evolution: Beyond Darwin and Lamarck''. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. ISBN |
*Messerly, J.G. (1992). ''Piaget's conception of evolution: Beyond Darwin and Lamarck''. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. {{ISBN|0-8476-8243-9}}. |
||
*Perret-Clermont, A.-N. & Barrelet, J.-M. (Eds) (2008/2014). ''Jean Piaget and Neuchâtel. The Learner and the Scholar''. Hove and New York: Psychology Press |
|||
*Phillips, John L. (1969). ''The Origin of Intellect: Piaget's Theory''. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. ISBN 0-7167-0579-6. |
|||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1002/ejsp.308 | last1 = Psaltis | first1 = C. | last2 = Duveen | first2 = G. | year = 2006 | title = Social relations and cognitive development: The influence of conversation type and representations of gender |
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1002/ejsp.308 | last1 = Psaltis | first1 = C. | last2 = Duveen | first2 = G. | year = 2006 | title = Social relations and cognitive development: The influence of conversation type and representations of gender | journal = European Journal of Social Psychology | volume = 36 | issue = 3| pages = 407–430 }} |
||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1348/026151005X91415 | last1 = Psaltis | first1 = C. | last2 = Duveen | first2 = G. | year = 2007 | title = Conversation types and conservation: Forms of recognition and cognitive development |
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1348/026151005X91415 | last1 = Psaltis | first1 = C. | last2 = Duveen | first2 = G. | year = 2007 | title = Conversation types and conservation: Forms of recognition and cognitive development | journal = British Journal of Developmental Psychology | volume = 25 | issue = 1| pages = 79–102 }} |
||
*Ripple, R.E., & Rockcastle, V.N. (Eds.) (1964). ''Piaget rediscovered. A report of the conference on cognitive studies and curriculum development.'' Cornell University: School of Education. |
|||
*Robinson, R.J. (2005). ''The birth of reason''. Prometheus Research Group. (Available online at [http://www.prometheus.org.uk prometheus.org.uk]) |
*Robinson, R.J. (2005). ''The birth of reason''. Prometheus Research Group. (Available online at [http://www.prometheus.org.uk prometheus.org.uk]) |
||
*Smith, L. (Ed.) (1992). ''Jean Piaget: Critical assessments'' (4 Vols.). London: Routledge. ISBN |
*Smith, L. (Ed.) (1992). ''Jean Piaget: Critical assessments'' (4 Vols.). London: Routledge. {{ISBN|0-415-04408-1}}. |
||
*Smith, L. (1993). ''Necessary knowledge: Piagetian perspectives on constructivism''. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum. ISBN |
*Smith, L. (1993). ''Necessary knowledge: Piagetian perspectives on constructivism''. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum. {{ISBN|0-86377-270-6}}. |
||
*Smith, L. (Ed.) (1996). ''Critical readings on Piaget''. London: Routledge. ISBN |
*Smith, L. (Ed.) (1996). ''Critical readings on Piaget''. London: Routledge. {{ISBN|0-415-13317-3}}. |
||
*Smith, L. (2001). Jean Piaget. In J. A. Palmer (Ed.), ''50 modern thinkers on education: From Piaget to the present''. London: Routledge. |
*Smith, L. (2001). Jean Piaget. In J. A. Palmer (Ed.), ''50 modern thinkers on education: From Piaget to the present''. London: Routledge. |
||
* |
*Vidal, F. (1994). ''Piaget before Piaget''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. {{ISBN|0-674-66716-6}}. |
||
*Traill, R.R. (2005a) ''........ ''. Melbourne: Ondwelle. [http://www.ondwelle.com/OSM01.pdf ondwelle.com] |
|||
*Traill, R.R. (2005b / 2008) ''Thinking by Molecule, Synapse, or both? — From Piaget's Schema, to the Selecting/Editing of ncRNA''. Melbourne: Ondwelle. [http://www.ondwelle.com/OSM02.pdf ondwelle.com] [Also in French: [http://www.ondwelle.com/FrSM02.pdf ondwelle.com] |
|||
*Vidal, F. (1994). ''Piaget before Piaget''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-66716-6. |
|||
*Vonèche, J.J. (1985). Genetic epistemology: Piaget's theory. In T. Husén & T.N. Postlethwaite (Eds.-in-chief), ''International encyclopedia of education'' (Vol. 4). Oxford: Pergamon. |
*Vonèche, J.J. (1985). Genetic epistemology: Piaget's theory. In T. Husén & T.N. Postlethwaite (Eds.-in-chief), ''International encyclopedia of education'' (Vol. 4). Oxford: Pergamon. |
||
*{{Cite journal | last1 = Wynn | first1 = T. | year = 1979 | title = The intelligence of later Acheulean hominids |
*{{Cite journal | last1 = Wynn | first1 = T. | year = 1979 | title = The intelligence of later Acheulean hominids | journal = Man |series=New Series | volume = 14 | issue = 3 | pages = 371–391 | doi=10.2307/2801865| jstor = 2801865 }} |
||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1016/S0047-2484(81)80046-2 | last1 = Wynn | first1 = T. | year = 1981 | title = The intelligence of Oldowan hominids |
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1016/S0047-2484(81)80046-2 | last1 = Wynn | first1 = T. | year = 1981 | title = The intelligence of Oldowan hominids | journal = Journal of Human Evolution | volume = 10 | issue = 7| pages = 529–541 | bibcode = 1981JHumE..10..529W }}— |
||
{{Refend}} |
{{Refend}} |
||
==Further reading== |
|||
Piaget inspired innumerable studies and even new areas of inquiry. The following is a list of critiques and commentaries, organized using the same citation-based method as the list of his own major works (above). These represent the significant and influential post-Piagetian writings in their respective sub-disciplines. |
|||
=== Exemplars === <!--More than 5,000 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*[[Lev Vygotsky|Vygotsky, L.]] (1963). ''Thought and language''. [12630 citations] |
|||
=== Classics === <!--More than 1,000 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*[[Seymour Papert|Papert, S.]] (1980). ''[[Mindstorms: Children, Computers, and Powerful Ideas]]''. [4089] |
|||
*[[Marvin Minsky|Minsky, M.]] (1988). ''[[Society of Mind|The society of mind]]''. [3950] |
|||
*[[Lawrence Kohlberg|Kohlberg, L.]] (1969). ''Stage And Sequence: The Cognitive-Developmental Approach To Socialization''. [3118] |
|||
*[[John H. Flavell|Flavell, J.]] (1963). ''The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget''. [2333] [The development of the project that became this book, and its impact, is discussed in detail by {{cite journal|last1=Müller|first1=U.|last2=Burman|first2=J. T.|last3=Hutchison|first3=S. M.|title=The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget: A quinquagenary retrospective|journal=Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology|year=2013|volume=34|issue=1|pages=52–55|doi=10.1016/j.appdev.2012.10.001|issn=0193-3973}}] |
|||
*[[Eleanor J. Gibson|Gibson, E. J.]] (1973). ''Principles of perceptual learning and development''. [1903] |
|||
*[[J. McVicker Hunt|Hunt, J. McV.]] (1961). ''Intelligence and Experience''. [617+395+384+111+167+32=1706] |
|||
*[[Andrew N. Meltzoff|Meltzoff, A. N.]] & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. [1497] |
|||
*Case, R. (1985). ''Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood''. [1456] |
|||
*Fischer, K. W. (1980). ''A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills''. [1001] |
|||
=== Major works === <!--More than 500 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*[[Elizabeth Bates|Bates, E.]] (1976). ''Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics''. [959] |
|||
*[[Herbert Ginsburg|Ginsburg, H. P.]] & Opper, S. (1969). ''Piaget's theory of intellectual development''. [931] |
|||
*Singley, M. K. & [[John Robert Anderson (psychologist)|Anderson, J. R.]] (1989). ''The transfer of cognitive skill''. [836] |
|||
*[[Eleanor Duckworth|Duckworth, E.]] (1973). The having of wonderful ideas. [775] |
|||
*Youniss, J. (1982). Parents and peers in social development: A Sullivan-Piaget perspective. [763] |
|||
*Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. [563] |
|||
*Schaffer, H. R. & Emerson, P. E. (1964). The development of social attachments in infancy. [535] |
|||
*Hopper, A. (2016). ''Introduction to the Qualitative Development of Intelligence''. |
|||
=== Works of significance === <!--More than 250 citations in Google Scholar--> |
|||
*{{cite journal | last1 = Shatz | first1 = M. | author-link2 = Rochel Gelman | last2 = Gelman | first2 = R. | year = 1973 | title = The Development of Communication Skills: Modifications in the Speech of Young Children as a Function of Listener | journal = Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development | volume = 38 | issue = 5| pages = 1–37 | doi=10.2307/1165783| jstor = 1165783 | pmid = 4764473 }} [470] |
|||
*{{cite journal | last1 = Broke | first1 = H | year = 1971 | title = Interpersonal perception of young children: Egocentrism or Empathy? | journal = Developmental Psychology | volume = 5 | issue = 2| pages = 263–269 | doi=10.1037/h0031267}} [469] |
|||
*Wadsworth, B. J. (1989). Piaget's theory of cognitive and affective development [421] |
|||
*[[Annette Karmiloff-Smith|Karmiloff-Smith, A.]] (1992). ''Beyond Modularity''. [419] |
|||
*Bodner, G. M. (1986). ''Constructivism: A theory of knowledge''. [403] |
|||
*Shantz, C. U. (1975). ''The Development of Social Cognition''. [387] |
|||
*{{cite journal | last1 = Diamond | first1 = A. | author-link = Adele Diamond | author-link2 = Patricia Goldman-Rakic | last2 = Goldman-Rakic | first2 = P. S. | year = 1989 | title = Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex | journal = Experimental Brain Research | volume = 74 | issue = 1| pages = 24–40 | doi=10.1007/bf00248277| pmid = 2924839 | s2cid = 2310409 }} [370] |
|||
*[[Howard Gruber|Gruber, H.]] & Voneche, H. (1982). ''The Essential Piaget''. [348] |
|||
*Walkerdine, V. (1984). Developmental psychology and the child-centred pedagogy: The insertion of Piaget into early education. [338] |
|||
*[[Constance Kamii|Kamii, C.]] & DeClark, G. (1985). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget's theory [335] |
|||
*Riegel, K. F. (1973). Dialectic operations: The final period of cognitive development [316] |
|||
*{{cite journal | last1 = Bandura | first1 = A. | author-link = Albert Bandura | last2 = McDonald | first2 = F. J. | year = 1963 | title = Influence of social reinforcement and the behavior of models in shaping children's moral judgment | journal = Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology | volume = 67 | issue = 3| pages = 274–281 | doi=10.1037/h0044714| pmid = 14054361 | s2cid = 24159020 }} [314] |
|||
*[[Robert Karplus|Karplus, R.]] (1980). Teaching for the development of reasoning. [312] |
|||
*Brainerd, C. (1978). The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory. [311] |
|||
*Brainerd, C. (1978). Piaget's theory of intelligence. [292] |
|||
*[[Carol Gilligan|Gilligan, C.]] (1997). Moral orientation and moral development [285] |
|||
*[[Adele Diamond|Diamond, A.]] (1991). Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development [284] |
|||
*Braine, M. D. S., & Rumain, B. (1983). Logical reasoning. [276] |
|||
*John-Steiner, V. (2000). Creative collaboration. [266] |
|||
*Pascual-Leone, J. (1987). Organismic processes for neo-Piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development. [261] |
|||
*Hallpike, C. R. (1979). ''The foundations of primitive thought'' [261] |
|||
*[[Hans G. Furth|Furth, H.]] (1969). ''Piaget and Knowledge'' [261] |
|||
*[[Rochel Gelman|Gelman, R.]] & [[Renee Baillargeon|Baillargeon, R.]] (1983). A review of some Piagetian concepts. [260] |
|||
*O'Loughlin, M. (1992). Rethinking science education: Beyond piagetian constructivism. Toward a sociocultural model of teaching and learning. [252] |
|||
*Messerly, John G. (1996). "Psychogenesis and the History of Science: Piaget and the Problem of Scientific Change", ''The Modern Schoolman'' LXXIII, 295–307. |
|||
==External links== |
==External links== |
||
*{{Helveticat}} |
|||
{{Sister project links | wikt=no | commons=Category:Jean Piaget | v=no | q=Jean Piaget | s=no | b=no}} |
{{Sister project links | wikt=no | commons=Category:Jean Piaget | v=no | q=Jean Piaget | s=no | b=no}} |
||
*[http://www.piaget.org/ Jean Piaget Society], society for the study of knowledge and development. |
*[http://www.piaget.org/ Jean Piaget Society], society for the study of knowledge and development. |
||
*[http://www.unige.ch/piaget/Presentations/presentg.html The Jean Piaget Archives], with full bibliography. |
*[http://www.unige.ch/piaget/Presentations/presentg.html The Jean Piaget Archives] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20031211121717/http://www.unige.ch/piaget/Presentations/presentg.html |date=11 December 2003 }}, with full bibliography. |
||
*[http://boletimef.org/biblioteca/2996 Interview with Jean Piaget and Bärbel Inhelder] by Elizabeth Hall (1970) |
*{{usurped|1=[https://web.archive.org/web/20150215220621/http://boletimef.org/biblioteca/2996 Interview with Jean Piaget and Bärbel Inhelder]}} by Elizabeth Hall (1970) (archived 15 February 2015) |
||
*[http://www.DevPsy.org/topics/piaget.html Jean Piaget @ Teaching & Learning Developmental Psychology], Piaget as a scientist with resources for classes. |
*[http://www.DevPsy.org/topics/piaget.html Jean Piaget @ Teaching & Learning Developmental Psychology], Piaget as a scientist with resources for classes. |
||
*[http:// |
*[http://campber.people.clemson.edu/piaget.html Jean Piaget's Genetic Epistemology: Appreciation and Critique] by Robert Campbell (2002), extensive summary of work and biography. |
||
*[http://www.butler-bowdon.com/piaget-language-and-thought-of-the-child Piaget's The Language and Thought of the Child (1926)] |
*[http://www.butler-bowdon.com/piaget-language-and-thought-of-the-child Piaget's The Language and Thought of the Child (1926)] – a brief introduction |
||
*[https://archive.org/details/moraljudgmentoft005613mbp The Moral Judgment of the Child] by Jean Piaget (1932) |
*[https://archive.org/details/moraljudgmentoft005613mbp The Moral Judgment of the Child] by Jean Piaget (1932), at [[Internet Archive]] |
||
*[http://www.marxists.org/reference/subject/philosophy/works/fr/piaget2.htm The Construction of Reality in the Child] by Jean Piaget (1955) |
*[http://www.marxists.org/reference/subject/philosophy/works/fr/piaget2.htm The Construction of Reality in the Child] by Jean Piaget (1955) |
||
*Piaget's role in the [http://www.ibe.unesco.org/organization/director/Piaget/Dir_Piaget.htm International Bureau of Education] and the [http://www.ibe.unesco.org/policy/ice.htm International Conference on Education] |
*Piaget's role in the [http://www.ibe.unesco.org/organization/director/Piaget/Dir_Piaget.htm International Bureau of Education] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080603220458/http://www.ibe.unesco.org/organization/director/Piaget/Dir_Piaget.htm |date=3 June 2008 }} and the [http://www.ibe.unesco.org/policy/ice.htm International Conference on Education] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080611071547/http://www.ibe.unesco.org/policy/ice.htm |date=11 June 2008 }} |
||
*[http://www.marxists.org/reference/subject/philosophy/works/fr/piaget.htm Genetic Epistemology] by Jean Piaget (1968) |
*[http://www.marxists.org/reference/subject/philosophy/works/fr/piaget.htm Genetic Epistemology] by Jean Piaget (1968) |
||
*[http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/comment/piaget.htm Comments on Vygotsky] by Jean Piaget (1962) |
*[http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/comment/piaget.htm Comments on Vygotsky] by Jean Piaget (1962) |
||
*{{YouTube|o_EkfWS2Wks|Piaget's Developmental Theory: An Overview – Part 1}}, a 27-minute documentary film used primarily in higher education (on [[YouTube]]) |
|||
*[http://www.learningandteaching.info/learning/piaget.htm Piaget's Development Theory] |
|||
*{{YouTube| |
*{{YouTube|SzZWY5f8RQQ|Piaget's Developmental Theory: An Overview – Part 2}}, a 27-minute documentary film used primarily in higher education (on [[YouTube]]) |
||
*[http://www.fondationjeanpiaget.ch/ Foundation Jean Piaget for research in psychology and epistemology] – French version only – diffuse to the world community writings and talks of the Swiss scientist. |
|||
*{{YouTube|SzZWY5f8RQQ|Piaget's Developmental Theory: An Overview - Part 2}}, a 27-minute documentary film used primarily in higher education. |
|||
*[https://archive.today/20121210175634/http://www.regispetit.com/snha.htm Human Nervous System model in accordance with Piaget's Learning Theory] – French version only (archived 10 December 2012) |
|||
*[http://www.fondationjeanpiaget.ch/ Foundation Jean Piaget for research in psychology and epistemology] - French version only - diffuse to the world community writings and talks of the Swiss scientist. |
|||
*[http://www.regispetit.com/snha.htm Human Nervous System model in accordance with Piaget's Learning Theory] - French version only |
|||
*[http://www.jeanpiaget.ch/index-en.html Jean Piaget and Neuchâtel] The site is maintained by the Institute of Psychology and Education, Neuchâtel University |
*[http://www.jeanpiaget.ch/index-en.html Jean Piaget and Neuchâtel] The site is maintained by the Institute of Psychology and Education, Neuchâtel University |
||
*[ |
*[https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/659425 Jean Piaget's 1931 essay "The Spirit of Solidarity in Children and International Cooperation"] (re-published in the Spring 2011 issue of ''Schools: Studies in Education'') |
||
*[ |
*[https://books.google.com/books?id=j3bTqBYYtBYC Jean Piaget: A Most Outrageous Deception] by Webster R. Callaway |
||
*[https://www.purepsychology.uk/ Pure Psychology: the Qualitative Development of Intelligence.] |
|||
{{Navboxes|list= |
|||
{{Humandevelopment}} |
|||
{{Human psychological development}} |
|||
{{Attachment theory}} |
|||
{{Psychology}} |
{{Psychology}} |
||
{{Philosophy of education}} |
|||
{{Epistemology}} |
|||
{{E. L. Thorndike Award |state=autocollapse}} |
{{E. L. Thorndike Award |state=autocollapse}} |
||
{{Time 100: The Most Important People of the Century}} |
|||
{{Use dmy dates|date=October 2010}} |
|||
}} |
|||
{{Portal bar|Psychology|Switzerland}} |
|||
{{Authority control}} |
|||
{{Authority control|VIAF=39382513}} |
|||
{{Persondata |
|||
|NAME = Piaget, Jean |
|||
|ALTERNATIVE NAMES = |
|||
|SHORT DESCRIPTION = Swiss philosopher; developmental psychologist |
|||
|DATE OF BIRTH = 9 August 1896 |
|||
|PLACE OF BIRTH = [[Neuchâtel]], Switzerland |
|||
|DATE OF DEATH = 16 September 1980 |
|||
|PLACE OF DEATH = Switzerland |
|||
}} |
|||
{{DEFAULTSORT:Piaget, Jean}} |
{{DEFAULTSORT:Piaget, Jean}} |
||
[[Category:1896 births]] |
[[Category:1896 births]] |
||
[[Category:1980 deaths]] |
[[Category:1980 deaths]] |
||
[[Category: |
[[Category:20th-century psychologists]] |
||
[[Category:20th-century Swiss philosophers]] |
|||
[[Category:20th-century Swiss writers]] |
[[Category:20th-century Swiss writers]] |
||
[[Category: |
[[Category:Burials at Cimetière des Rois]] |
||
[[Category:Child development]] |
|||
[[Category:Child psychologists]] |
[[Category:Child psychologists]] |
||
[[Category:Cognitive scientists]] |
|||
[[Category:Developmental psychologists]] |
[[Category:Developmental psychologists]] |
||
[[Category:Epistemologists]] |
[[Category:Epistemologists]] |
||
[[Category: |
[[Category:Foreign associates of the National Academy of Sciences]] |
||
[[Category:Mathematical cognition researchers]] |
[[Category:Mathematical cognition researchers]] |
||
[[Category: |
[[Category:Moral psychologists]] |
||
[[Category:People associated with the University of Zurich]] |
|||
[[Category:People from Neuchâtel]] |
|||
[[Category:Structuralists]] |
|||
[[Category:Swiss educational theorists]] |
|||
[[Category:Swiss-French people]] |
[[Category:Swiss-French people]] |
||
[[Category:Swiss philosophers]] |
[[Category:Swiss philosophers]] |
||
[[Category:Swiss Protestants]] |
[[Category:Swiss Protestants]] |
||
[[Category:Swiss psychologists]] |
[[Category:Swiss psychologists]] |
||
[[Category:University of Geneva |
[[Category:Academic staff of the University of Geneva]] |
||
[[Category: |
[[Category:Academic staff of the University of Lausanne]] |
||
[[Category:People associated with the University of Zurich]] |
|||
[[Category:Burials at Cimetière des Rois]] |
|||
[[Category:Consciousness researchers and theorists]] |
|||
[[Category:University of Neuchâtel alumni]] |
[[Category:University of Neuchâtel alumni]] |
||
[[Category:Academic staff of the University of Paris]] |
|||
[[Category:APA Distinguished Scientific Award for an Early Career Contribution to Psychology recipients]] |
Latest revision as of 18:48, 21 December 2024
Jean Piaget | |
---|---|
Born | Jean William Fritz Piaget 9 August 1896 Neuchâtel, Switzerland |
Died | 16 September 1980 Geneva, Switzerland | (aged 84)
Alma mater | University of Neuchâtel University of Zürich |
Known for | Constructivism, Genevan School, genetic epistemology, theory of cognitive development, object permanence, egocentrism |
Scientific career | |
Fields | Developmental psychology, epistemology |
Jean William Fritz Piaget (UK: /piˈæʒeɪ/,[1][2] US: /ˌpiːəˈʒeɪ, pjɑːˈʒeɪ/;[3][4][5] French: [ʒɑ̃ pjaʒɛ]; 9 August 1896 – 16 September 1980) was a Swiss psychologist known for his work on child development. Piaget's theory of cognitive development and epistemological view are together called genetic epistemology.[6]
Piaget placed great importance on the education of children. As the Director of the International Bureau of Education, he declared in 1934 that "only education is capable of saving our societies from possible collapse, whether violent, or gradual".[7] His theory of child development has been studied in pre-service education programs. Nowadays, educators and theorists working in the area of early childhood education persist in incorporating constructivist-based strategies.
Piaget created the International Center for Genetic Epistemology in Geneva in 1955 while on the faculty of the University of Geneva, and directed the center until his death in 1980.[8] The number of collaborations that its founding made possible, and their impact, ultimately led to the Center being referred to in the scholarly literature as "Piaget's factory".[9]
According to Ernst von Glasersfeld, Piaget was "the great pioneer of the constructivist theory of knowing".[10] His ideas were widely popularized in the 1960s.[11] This then led to the emergence of the study of development as a major sub-discipline in psychology.[12] By the end of the 20th century, he was second only to B. F. Skinner as the most-cited psychologist.[13]
Personal life
[edit]Piaget was born in 1896 in Neuchâtel, in the Francophone region of Switzerland. He was the oldest son of Arthur Piaget (Swiss), a professor of medieval literature at the University of Neuchâtel, and Rebecca Jackson (French). Rebecca Jackson came from a prominent family of French steel foundry owners[14] of English descent through her Lancashire-born great-grandfather, steelmaker James Jackson.[15] Piaget was a precocious child who developed an interest in biology and the natural world. His early interest in zoology earned him a reputation among those in the field after he had published several articles on mollusks by the age of 15.[16]
When he was 15, his former nanny wrote to his parents to apologize for having once lied to them about fighting off a would-be kidnapper from baby Jean's pram. There never was a kidnapper. Piaget became fascinated that he had somehow formed a memory of this kidnapping incident, a memory that endured even after he understood it to be false.[17]
He developed an interest in epistemology due to his godfather's urgings to study the fields of philosophy and logic.[18] He was educated at the University of Neuchâtel, and studied briefly at the University of Zürich. During this time, he published two philosophical papers that showed the direction of his thinking at the time, but which he later dismissed as adolescent thought.[19] His interest in psychoanalysis, at the time a burgeoning strain of psychology, can also be dated to this period.
Piaget moved from Switzerland to Paris after his graduation and he taught at the Grange-Aux-Belles Street School for Boys. The school was run by Alfred Binet, the developer of the Binet-Simon test (later revised by Lewis Terman to become the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales). Piaget assisted in the marking of Binet's intelligence tests. It was while he was helping to mark some of these tests that Piaget noticed that young children consistently gave wrong answers to certain questions.[20] Piaget did not focus so much on the fact of the children's answers being wrong, but that young children consistently made types of mistakes that older children and adults managed to avoid. This led him to the theory that young children's cognitive processes are inherently different from those of adults. Ultimately, he was to propose a global theory of cognitive developmental stages in which individuals exhibit certain common patterns of cognition in each period of development.
In 1921, Piaget returned to Switzerland as director of the Rousseau Institute in Geneva. At this time, the institute was directed by Édouard Claparède.[21] Piaget was familiar with many of Claparède's ideas, including that of the psychological concept of groping which was closely associated with "trials and errors" observed in human mental patterns.[22]
In 1923, he married Valentine Châtenay (7 January 1899 – 3 July 1983);[23] the couple had three children, whom Piaget studied from infancy. From 1925 to 1929, Piaget worked as a professor of psychology, sociology, and the philosophy of science at the University of Neuchatel.[24] In 1929, Jean Piaget accepted the post of Director of the International Bureau of Education and remained the head of this international organization until 1968. Every year, he drafted his "Director's Speeches" for the IBE Council and for the International Conference on Public Education in which he explicitly addressed his educational credo.
Having taught at the University of Geneva, and at the University of Paris in 1964, Piaget was invited to serve as chief consultant at two conferences at Cornell University (11–13 March) and the University of California, Berkeley (16–18 March). The conferences addressed the relationship of cognitive studies and curriculum development, and strived to conceive implications of recent investigations of children's cognitive development for curricula.[25]
In 1972 Piaget was awarded the Erasmus Prize and in 1979 the Balzan Prize for Social and Political Sciences. Piaget died on 16 September 1980, and, as he had requested, was buried with his family in an unmarked grave in the Cimetière des Rois (Cemetery of Kings) in Geneva.[26]
Career history
[edit]Harry Beilin described Jean Piaget's theoretical research program[27] as consisting of four phases:
- the sociological model of development,
- the biological model of intellectual development,
- the elaboration of the logical model of intellectual development,
- the study of figurative thought.
The resulting theoretical frameworks are sufficiently different from each other that they have been characterized as representing different "Piagets". More recently, Jeremy Burman responded to Beilin and called for the addition of a phase before his turn to psychology: "the zeroth Piaget".[28]
Before becoming a psychologist
[edit]Before Piaget became a psychologist, he trained in natural history and philosophy. He received a doctorate in 1918 from the University of Neuchâtel. He then undertook post-doctoral training in Zürich (1918–1919), and Paris (1919–1921). He was hired by Théodore Simon to standardize psychometric measures for use with French children in 1919.[29] The theorist we recognize today only emerged when he moved to Geneva, to work for Édouard Claparède as director of research at the Rousseau Institute, in 1922.
Sociological model of development
[edit]Piaget first developed as a psychologist in the 1920s. He investigated the hidden side of children's minds. Piaget proposed that children moved from a position of egocentrism to sociocentrism. For this explanation he combined the use of psychological and clinical methods to create what he called a semiclinical interview. He began the interview by asking children standardized questions and depending on how they answered, he would ask them a series of standard questions. Piaget was looking for what he called "spontaneous conviction" so he often asked questions the children neither expected nor anticipated. In his studies, he noticed there was a gradual progression from intuitive to scientific and socially acceptable responses. Piaget theorized children did this because of the social interaction and the challenge to younger children's ideas by the ideas of those children who were more advanced.
This work was used by Elton Mayo as the basis for the famous Hawthorne Experiments.[30][31] For Piaget, it also led to an honorary doctorate from Harvard in 1936.[32]
Biological model of intellectual development
[edit]In this stage, Piaget believed that the process of thinking and intellectual development could be regarded as an extension of the biological process of the (adaptation) of the species, which has also two ongoing processes: assimilation and accommodation. There is assimilation when a child responds to a new event in a way that is consistent with an existing schema. There is accommodation when a child either modifies an existing schema or forms an entirely new schema to deal with a new object or event.[33]
He argued infants were engaging in the act of assimilation when they sucked on everything in their reach. He claimed infants transform all objects into an object to be sucked. The children were assimilating the objects to conform to their own mental structures. Piaget then made the assumption that whenever one transforms the world to meet individual needs or conceptions, one is, in a way, assimilating it. Piaget also observed his children not only assimilating objects to fit their needs, but also modifying some of their mental structures to meet the demands of the environment. This is the second division of adaptation known as accommodation. To start, the infants only engaged in primarily reflex actions such as sucking, but not long after, they would pick up objects and put them in their mouths. When they do this, they modify their reflex response to accommodate the external objects into reflex actions. Because the two are often in conflict, they provide the impetus for intellectual development—the constant need to balance the two triggers intellectual growth.
To test his theory, Piaget observed the habits in his own children.
Elaboration of the logical model of intellectual development
[edit]In the model Piaget developed in stage three, he argued that intelligence develops in a series of stages that are related to age and are progressive because one stage must be accomplished before the next can occur. For each stage of development the child forms a view of reality for that age period. At the next stage, the child must keep up with earlier level of mental abilities to reconstruct concepts. Piaget conceived intellectual development as an upward expanding spiral in which children must constantly reconstruct the ideas formed at earlier levels with new, higher order concepts acquired at the next level.
It is primarily the "Third Piaget" (the logical model of intellectual development) that was debated by American psychologists when Piaget's ideas were "rediscovered" in the 1960s.[34]
Study of figurative thought
[edit]Piaget studied areas of intelligence like perception and memory that are not entirely logical. Logical concepts are described as being completely reversible because they can always get back to the starting point, meaning that if one starts with a given premise and follows logical steps to reach a conclusion, the same steps may be done in the opposite order, starting from the conclusion to arrive at the premise. The perceptual concepts Piaget studied could not be manipulated. To describe the figurative process, Piaget uses pictures as examples. Pictures cannot be separated because contours cannot be separated from the forms they outline. Memory is the same way: it is never completely reversible; people cannot necessarily recall all the intervening events between two points. During this last period of work, Piaget and his colleague Inhelder also published books on perception, memory, and other figurative processes such as learning.[35][36]
Because Piaget's theory is based upon biological maturation and stages, the notion of readiness is important. Readiness concerns when certain information or concepts should be taught. According to Piaget's theory, children should not be taught certain concepts until they reached the appropriate stage of cognitive development.[37] For example, young children in the preoperational stage engage in "irreversible" thought and cannot comprehend that an item that has been transformed in some way may be returned to its original state.[38]
Theory
[edit]Piaget defined himself as a 'genetic' epistemologist, interested in the process of the qualitative development of knowledge. He considered cognitive structures' development as a differentiation of biological regulations. When his entire theory first became known – the theory in itself being based on a structuralist and a cognitivitist approach – it was an outstanding and exciting development in regards to the psychological community at that time.[39]
There are a total of four phases in Piaget's research program that included books on certain topics of developmental psychology. In particular, during one period of research, he described himself studying his own three children, and carefully observing and interpreting their cognitive development.[40] In one of his last books, Equilibration of Cognitive Structures: The Central Problem of Intellectual Development, he intends to explain knowledge development as a process of equilibration using two main concepts in his theory, assimilation and accommodation, as belonging not only to biological interactions but also to cognitive ones.[41]
He stated that children are born with limited capabilities and his cognition ability develops over age.[42]
Piaget believed answers for the epistemological questions at his time could be answered, or better proposed, if one looked to the genetic aspect of it, hence his experimentations with children and adolescents. As he says in the introduction of his book Genetic Epistemology: "What the genetic epistemology proposes is discovering the roots of the different varieties of knowledge, since its elementary forms, following to the next levels, including also the scientific knowledge."
Stages
[edit]The four development stages are described in Piaget's theory as:
Sensorimotor stage: from birth to age two. The children experience the world through movement and their senses. During the sensorimotor stage children are extremely egocentric, meaning they cannot perceive the world from others' viewpoints. The sensorimotor stage is divided into six substages:[43]
- Simple reflexes: From birth to one month old. At this time infants use reflexes such as rooting and sucking.
- First habits and primary circular reactions: From one month to four months old. During this time infants learn to coordinate sensation and two types of schema (habit and circular reactions). A primary circular reaction is when the infant tries to reproduce an event that happened by accident (ex.: sucking thumb).
- Secondary circular reactions: From four to eight months old. At this time they become aware of things beyond their own body; they are more object-oriented. At this time they might accidentally shake a rattle and continue to do it for sake of satisfaction.
- Coordination of secondary circular reactions: From eight months to twelve months old. During this stage they can do things intentionally. They can now combine and recombine schemata and try to reach a goal (ex.: use a stick to reach something). They also begin to understand object permanence in the later months and early into the next stage. That is, they understand that objects continue to exist even when they can't see them.
- Tertiary circular reactions, novelty, and curiosity: From twelve months old to eighteen months old. During this stage infants explore new possibilities of objects; they try different things to get different results.
- Internalization of schemata.
Some followers of Piaget's studies of infancy, such as Kenneth Kaye[44] argue that his contribution was as an observer of countless phenomena not previously described, but that he didn't offer explanation of the processes in real time that cause those developments, beyond analogizing them to broad concepts about biological adaptation generally. Kaye's "apprenticeship theory" of cognitive and social development refuted Piaget's assumption that mind developed endogenously in infants until the capacity for symbolic reasoning allowed them to learn language.
Preoperational stage: Piaget's second stage, the preoperational stage, starts when the child begins to learn to speak at age two and lasts up until the age of seven. During the preoperational stage of cognitive development, Piaget noted that children do not yet understand concrete logic and cannot mentally manipulate information. Children's increase in playing and pretending takes place in this stage. The child still has trouble seeing things from different points of view. The children's play is mainly categorized by symbolic play and manipulating symbols. Such play is demonstrated by the idea of checkers being snacks, pieces of paper being plates, and a box being a table. Their observations of symbols exemplifies the idea of play with the absence of the actual objects involved. By observing sequences of play, Piaget was able to demonstrate that, toward the end of the second year, a qualitatively new kind of psychological functioning occurs, known as the preoperational stage.[45]
The preoperational stage is sparse and logically inadequate in regard to mental operations. The child is able to form stable concepts as well as magical beliefs, but not perform operations, which are mental tasks, rather than physical. Thinking in this stage is still egocentric, meaning the child has difficulty seeing the viewpoint of others. The preoperational stage is split into two substages: the symbolic function substage, and the intuitive thought substage. The symbolic function substage is when children are able to understand, represent, remember, and picture objects in their mind without having the object in front of them. The intuitive thought substage is when children tend to propose the questions of "why?" and "how come?" This stage is when children want the knowledge of knowing everything.[45]
The Preoperational Stage is divided into two substages:
- Symbolic Function Substage. From two to four years of age children find themselves using symbols to represent physical models of the world around them. This is demonstrated through a child's drawing of their family in which people are not drawn to scale or accurate physical traits are given. The child knows they are not accurate but it does not seem to be an issue to them.
- Intuitive Thought Substage. At between about the ages of four and seven, children tend to become very curious and ask many questions, beginning the use of primitive reasoning. There is an emergence in the interest of reasoning and wanting to know why things are the way they are. Piaget called it the "intuitive substage" because children realize they have a vast amount of knowledge, but they are unaware of how they acquired it. Centration, conservation, irreversibility, class inclusion, and transitive inference are all characteristics of preoperative thought.[45]
Concrete operational stage: from ages seven to eleven. Children can now converse and think logically (they understand reversibility) but are limited to what they can physically manipulate. They are no longer egocentric. During this stage, children become more aware of logic and conservation, topics previously foreign to them. Children also improve drastically with their classification skills.
Formal operational stage: from age eleven and onward (development of abstract reasoning). Children develop abstract thought and can easily conserve and think logically in their mind. Abstract thought is newly present during this stage of development. Children are now able to think abstractly and use metacognition. Along with this, the children in the formal operational stage display more skills oriented toward problem solving, often in multiple steps.
Psychology of functions and correspondences
[edit]Piaget had sometimes been criticized for characterizing preoperational children in terms of the cognitive capacities they lacked, rather than their cognitive accomplishments. A late turn in the development of Piaget's theory saw the emergence of work on the accomplishments of those children within the framework of his psychology of functions and correspondences.[46][47][48] This new phase in Piaget's work was less stage-dependent and reflected greater continuity in human development than would be expected in a stage-bound theory.[49] This advance in his work took place toward the end of his very productive life and is sometimes absent from developmental psychology textbooks.
An example of a function can involve sets X and Y and ordered pairs of elements (x,y), in which x is an element of X and y, Y. In a function, an element of X is mapped onto exactly one element of Y (the reverse need not be true). A function therefore involves a unique mapping in one direction, or, as Piaget and his colleagues have written, functions are "univocal to the right" (Piaget et al., 1977, p. 14).[47] When each element of X maps onto exactly one element of Y and each element of Y maps onto exactly one element of X, Piaget and colleagues indicated that the uniqueness condition holds in either direction and called the relationship between the elements of X and Y "biunivocal" or "one-to-one".[47] They advanced the idea that the preoperational child manifests some understanding of one-way order functions.
According to Piaget's Genevan colleagues,[50] the "semilogic" of these order functions sustains the preoperational child's ability to use of spatial extent to index and compare quantities. The child, for example, could use the length of an array to index the number of objects in the array. Thus, the child would judge the longer of two arrays as having the greater number of objects. Although imperfect, such comparisons are often fair ("semilogical") substitutes for exact quantification. Furthermore, these order functions underlie the child's rudimentary knowledge of environmental regularities.[50] Young children are capable of constructing—this reflects the constructivist bent of Piaget's work—sequences of objects of alternating color. They also have an understanding of the pairwise exchanges of cards having pictures of different flowers.
Piaget and colleagues have examined morphisms, which to them differ from the operative transformations observed on concrete operational children.[46] Piaget (1977) wrote that "correspondences and morphisms are essentially comparisons that do not transform objects to be compared but that extract common forms from them or analogies between them" (p. 351).[48] He advanced the idea that this type of knowledge emerges from "primitive applications" of action schemes to objects in the environment.[47] In one study of morphisms, Piaget and colleagues asked children to identify items in a series of movable red cutouts that could cover a pre-specified section of each of four base cards—each card had a red area and a white area.[47] The task, in effect, asked the child to superimpose the cut-outs on a base card to make the entire card appear to be red. Although there were 12 cutouts in all, only three, which differed slightly from each other, could make an entire base card look red. The youngest children studied—they were age 5—could match, using trial and error, one cut-out to one base card. Piaget et al. called this type of morphism bijection, a term-by-term correspondence. Older children were able to do more by figuring out how to make entire card appear to be red by using three cutouts. In other words, they could perform three to one matching. Piaget et al. (1977) called a many-to-one match surjection.
Developmental process
[edit]Piaget provided no concise description of the development process as a whole. Broadly speaking it consisted of a cycle:
- The child performs an action which has an effect on or organizes objects, and the child is able to note the characteristics of the action and its effects.
- Through repeated actions, perhaps with variations or in different contexts or on different kinds of objects, the child is able to differentiate and integrate its elements and effects. This is the process of "reflecting abstraction" (described in detail in Piaget 2001).
- At the same time, the child is able to identify the properties of objects by the way different kinds of actions affect them. This is the process of "empirical abstraction".
- By repeating this process across a wide range of objects and actions, the child establishes a new level of knowledge and insight. This is the process of forming a new "cognitive stage". This dual process allows the child to construct new ways of dealing with objects and new knowledge about objects themselves.
- Once the child has constructed these new kinds of knowledge, he or she starts to use them to create still more complex objects and to carry out still more complex actions. As a result, the child starts to recognize still more complex patterns and to construct still more complex objects. Thus a new stage begins, which will only be completed when all the child's activity and experience have been re-organized on this still higher level.
This process may not be wholly gradual, but new evidence shows that the passage into new stages is more gradual than once thought. Once a new level of organization, knowledge and insight proves to be effective, it will quickly be generalized to other areas if they exist. As a result, transitions between stages can seem to be rapid and radical, but oftentimes the child has grasped one aspect of the new stage of cognitive functioning but not addressed others. The bulk of the time spent in a new stage consists of refining this new cognitive level; it does not always happen quickly. For example, a child may see that two different colors of Play-Doh have been fused together to make one ball, based on the color. If sugar is mixed into water or iced tea, then the sugar "disappeared" and therefore does not exist to the child at that stage. These levels of one concept of cognitive development are not realized all at once, giving us a gradual realization of the world around us.[51]
It is because this process takes this dialectical form, in which each new stage is created through the further differentiation, integration, and synthesis of new structures out of the old, that the sequence of cognitive stages are logically necessary rather than simply empirically correct. Each new stage emerges only because the child can take for granted the achievements of its predecessors, and yet there are still more sophisticated forms of knowledge and action that are capable of being developed.
Because it covers both how we gain knowledge about objects and our reflections on our own actions, Piaget's model of development explains a number of features of human knowledge that had never previously been accounted for. For example, by showing how children progressively enrich their understanding of things by acting on and reflecting on the effects of their own previous knowledge, they are able to organize their knowledge in increasingly complex structures. Thus, once a young child can consistently and accurately recognize different kinds of animals, he or she then acquires the ability to organize the different kinds into higher groupings such as "birds", "fish", and so on. This is significant because they are now able to know things about a new animal simply on the basis of the fact that it is a bird – for example, that it will lay eggs.
At the same time, by reflecting on their own actions, children develop an increasingly sophisticated awareness of the "rules" that govern them in various ways. For example, it is by this route that Piaget explains this child's growing awareness of notions such as "right", "valid", "necessary", "proper", and so on. In other words, it is through the process of objectification, reflection and abstraction that the child constructs the principles on which action is not only effective or correct but also justified.
One of Piaget's most famous studies focused purely on the discriminative abilities of children between the ages of two and a half years old, and four and a half years old. He began the study by taking children of different ages and placing two lines of sweets, one with the sweets in a line spread further apart, and one with the same number of sweets in a line placed more closely together. He found that, "Children between 2 years, 6 months old and 3 years, 2 months old correctly discriminate the relative number of objects in two rows; between 3 years, 2 months and 4 years, 6 months they indicate a longer row with fewer objects to have "more"; after 4 years, 6 months they again discriminate correctly" (Cognitive Capacity of Very Young Children, p. 141). Initially younger children were not studied, because if at four years old a child could not conserve quantity, then a younger child presumably could not either. The results show that children that are younger than three years and two months have quantity conservation, but as they get older they lose this quality, and do not recover it until four and a half years old. This attribute may be lost due to a temporary inability to solve because of an overdependence on perceptual strategies, which correlates more candy with a longer line of candy, or due to the inability for a four-year-old to reverse situations.
By the end of this experiment several results were found. First, younger children have a discriminative ability that shows the logical capacity for cognitive operations exists earlier than acknowledged. This study also reveals that young children can be equipped with certain qualities for cognitive operations, depending on how logical the structure of the task is. Research also shows that children develop explicit understanding at age 5 and as a result, the child will count the sweets to decide which has more. Finally the study found that overall quantity conservation is not a basic characteristic of humans' native inheritance.
Genetic epistemology
[edit]According to Jean Piaget, genetic epistemology attempts to "explain knowledge, and in particular scientific knowledge, on the basis of its history, its sociogenesis, and especially the psychological origins of the notions and operations upon which it is based". Piaget believed he could test epistemological questions by studying the development of thought and action in children. As a result, Piaget created a field known as genetic epistemology with its own methods and problems. He defined this field as the study of child development as a means of answering epistemological questions.
Schema
[edit]A schema (plural form: schemata) is a structured cluster of concepts, it can be used to represent objects, scenarios or sequences of events or relations. The philosopher Immanuel Kant first proposed the concept of schemata as innate structures used to help us perceive the world.[52]
A schema is a mental framework that is created as children interact with their physical and social environments.[53] For example, many 3-year-olds insist that the sun is alive because it comes up in the morning and goes down at night. According to Piaget, these children are operating based on a simple cognitive schema that things that move are alive. At any age, children rely on their current cognitive structures to understand the world around them. Moreover, younger and older children may often interpret and respond to the same objects and events in very different ways because cognitive structures take different forms at different ages.[54]
Piaget (1953) described three kinds of intellectual structures: behavioural (or sensorimotor) schemata, symbolic schemata, and operational schemata.
- Behavioural schemata: organized patterns of behaviour that are used to represent and respond to objects and experiences.
- Symbolic schemata: internal mental symbols (such as images or verbal codes) that one uses to represent aspects of experience.
- Operational schemata: internal mental activity that one performs on objects of thought.[55]
According to Piaget, children use the process of assimilation and accommodation to create a schema or mental framework for how they perceive and/or interpret what they are experiencing. As a result, the early concepts of young children tend to be more global or general in nature.[56]
Similarly, Gallagher and Reid (1981) maintained that adults view children's concepts as highly generalized and even inaccurate. With added experience, interactions, and maturity, these concepts become refined and more detailed. Overall, making sense of the world from a child's perspective is a very complex and time-consuming process.[57]
A schema is:
- A critically important building block of conceptual development
- Constantly in the process of being modified or changed
- Modified by on-going experiences
- A generalized idea, usually based on experience or prior knowledge.[56]
These schemata are constantly being revised and elaborated upon each time the child encounters new experiences. In doing this children create their own unique understanding of the world, interpret their own experiences and knowledge, and subsequently use this knowledge to solve more complex problems. In a neurological sense, the brain/mind is constantly working to build and rebuild itself as it takes in, adapts/modifies new information, and enhances understanding.[56]
Research methods
[edit]Piaget wanted to revolutionize the way research was conducted. Although he started researching with his colleagues using a traditional method of data collection, he was not fully satisfied with the results and wanted to keep trying to find new ways of researching using a combination of data, which included naturalistic observation, psychometrics, and the psychiatric clinical examination, in order to have a less guided form of research that would produce more empirically valid results. As Piaget developed new research methods, he wrote a book called The Language and Thought of the Child, which aimed to synthesize the methods he was using in order to study the conclusions children drew from situations and how they arrived at such conclusions. The main idea was to observe how children responded and articulated certain situations with their own reasoning, in order to examine their thought processes (Mayer, 2005).
Piaget administered a test in 15 boys with ages ranging from 10 to 14 years in which he asked participants to describe the relationship between a mixed bouquet of flowers and a bouquet with flowers of the same color. The purpose of this study was to analyze the thinking process the boys had and to draw conclusions about the logic processes they had used, which was a psychometric technique of research. Piaget also used the psychoanalytic method initially developed by Sigmund Freud. The purpose of using such method was to examine the unconscious mind, as well as to continue parallel studies using different research methods. Psychoanalysis was later rejected by Piaget, as he thought it was insufficiently empirical (Mayer, 2005).
Piaget argued that children and adults used speech for different purposes. In order to confirm his argument, he experimented analyzing a child's interpretation of a story. In the experiment, the child listened to a story and then told a friend that same story in his/her/their own words. The purpose of this study was to examine how children verbalize and understand each other without adult intervention. Piaget wanted to examine the limits of naturalistic observation, in order to understand a child's reasoning. He realized the difficulty of studying children's thoughts, as it is hard to know if a child is pretending to believe their thoughts or not. Piaget was the pioneer researcher to examine children's conversations in a social context – starting from examining their speech and actions – where children were comfortable and spontaneous (Kose, 1987).
Issues and possible solutions
[edit]After conducting many studies, Piaget found significant differences in the way adults and children reason. He could not find the path of logic reasoning and the unspoken thoughts children had, which would allow him to study a child's intellectual development over time (Mayer, 2005). In his third book, The Child's Conception of the World, Piaget recognized the difficulties of his prior techniques and the importance of psychiatric clinical examination. The researcher believed that the way clinical examinations were conducted influenced how a child's inner realities surfaced. Children would likely respond according to the way the research is conducted, the questions asked, or the familiarity they have with the environment. The clinical examination conducted for his third book provides a thorough investigation into a child's thinking process. An example of a question used to research such process was: "Can you see a thought?" (Mayer, 2005, p. 372).
Piaget replaces psychometric tests with the clinical method approach
[edit]Piaget recognized that psychometric tests had its limitations, as children were not able to provide the researcher with their deepest thoughts and inner intellect. It was also difficult to know if the results of child examination reflected what children believed or if it is just a pretend situation. For example, it is very difficult to know with certainty if a child who has a conversation with a toy believes the toy is alive or if the child is just pretending. Soon after drawing conclusions about psychometric studies, Piaget started developing the clinical method of examination. The clinical method included questioning a child and carefully examining their responses – in order to observe how the child reasoned according to the questions asked – and then examining the child's perception of the world through their responses. Piaget recognized the difficulties of interviewing a child and the importance of recognizing the difference between "liberated" versus "spontaneous" responses (Mayer, 2005, p. 372).
Piaget wanted to research in environments that would allow children to connect with some existing aspects of the world. The idea was to change the approach described in his book The Child's Conception of the World and move away from the vague questioning interviews. This new approach was described in his book The Child's Conception of Physical Causality, where children were presented with dilemmas and had to think of possible solutions on their own. Later, after carefully analyzing previous methods, Piaget developed a combination of naturalistic observation with clinical interviewing in his book Judgment and Reasoning in the Child, where a child's intellect was tested with questions and close monitoring. Piaget was convinced he had found a way to analyze and access a child's thoughts about the world in a very effective way (Mayer, 2005). Piaget's research provided a combination of theoretical and practical research methods and it has offered a crucial contribution to the field of developmental psychology (Beilin, 1992). "Piaget is often criticized because his method of investigation, though somewhat modified in recent years, is still largely clinical". He observes a child's surroundings and behavior. He then comes up with a hypothesis testing it and focusing on both the surroundings and behavior after changing a little of the surrounding.[58]
Influence
[edit]Despite his ceasing to be a fashionable psychologist, the magnitude of Piaget's continuing influence can be measured by the global scale and activity of the Jean Piaget Society, which holds annual conferences and attracts around 700 participants.[59] His theory of cognitive development has proved influential in many different areas:
- Developmental psychology
- Education and Morality
- Historical studies of thought and cognition
- Evolution
- Philosophy
- Primatology
- Artificial intelligence (AI)
Developmental psychology
[edit]Piaget is considered the most influential figure in developmental psychology, though many of aspects of his theories are no longer accepted by mainstream psychologists. Developmental psychologists today do not view development as taking place in stages[60][61] and many of Piaget's empirical findings have been overturned by subsequent research.[62] For example, psychologists no longer view young children as being incapable of understanding abstract concepts,[63] and no longer believe that babies do not understand object permanence.[64] Despite this, developmental psychologists do acknowledge the importance of Jean Piaget's legacy as the founder of their field. They recognize his innovative empirical work, his attempts to integrate his results into a unified theoretical model and the way he created a path for subsequent researchers to follow.[65] Indeed, many developmental psychology researchers today work in a post-Piagetian or neo-Piagetian framework.[66][67]
Education
[edit]By using Piaget's theory, educators focus on their students as learners. As a result of this focus, education is learner-centered and constructivist-based to an extent. It allows teachers to view students as individual learners who add new concepts to prior knowledge to construct, or build, understanding for themselves.[68] Teachers who use a learner-centered approach as a basis for their professional practices incorporate the several dispositions.[68] They provide experience-based educational opportunities. These teachers also contemplate the learners' individual qualities and attitudes during curriculum planning. Educators allow learners' insights to alter the curriculum. They nourish and support learners' curiosity. They also involve learners' emotions and create a learning environment in which students feel safe.[68]
There are two differences between the preoperational and concrete operational stages that apply to education. These differences are reversibility and decentration. At times, reversibility and decentration occur at the same time.[69] When students think about the steps to complete a task without using a particular logical, sequential order, they are using reversibility.[69] Decentration allows them to concentrate on multiple components of a problematic task at a time.[69] Students use both reversibility and decentration to function throughout the school day, follow directions, and complete assignments.
An example of a student using reversibility is when learning new vocabulary. The student creates a list of unfamiliar words from a literary text. Then, he researches the definition of those words before asking classmate to test him. His teacher has given a set of particular instructions that he must follow in a particular order: he must write the word before defining it, and complete these two steps repeatedly.[69] A child in the preoperational stage gets confused during this process and needs assistance from the teacher to stay on task. The teacher refers him back to his text in order to notate the next word before he can define it.[69] A child in the preoperational stage does not understand the organization required to complete this assignment. One in the concrete operational stage understands the organization, and can recall the steps in any order while being able to follow the order given.[69] Using decentration, the child has the two activities on his mind: identify words and find them in the dictionary.[69]
A sample of decentration is a preschooler may use a toy banana as a pretend telephone. The child knows the difference between the fruit and a phone. In this form of play, he is operating on two levels at once.[69] In an older child at the concrete operational level, decentration allows him to complete subtraction of two-digit numbers and indicate which of the problems also involved borrowing from the other column. The student simultaneously does both.[69] Using reversibility, the student has to move mentally between two subtasks.
Regarding the giving of praise by teachers, praise is a reinforcer for students. Adolescents undergo social-emotional development such that they seek rapport with peers. Thus, teacher praise is not as powerful for students who see teachers as authority figures. They give no value to praise provided by adults, or they have no respect for the individual who is giving praise.[70]
During the 1970s and 1980s, Piaget's works also inspired the transformation of European and American education, including theory and practice, leading to a more 'child-centered' approach. In Conversations with Jean Piaget, Bringuier says: "Education, for most people, means trying to lead the child to resemble the typical adult of his society ... but for me and no one else, education means making creators... You have to make inventors, innovators—not conformists" (Bringuier, 1980, p. 132).
His theory of cognitive development can be used as a tool in the early childhood classroom. According to Piaget, children developed best in a classroom with interaction.
Piaget defined knowledge as the ability to modify, transform, and "operate on" an object or idea, such that it is understood by the operator through the process of transformation.[71] Learning, then, occurs as a result of experience, both physical and logical, with the objects themselves and how they are acted upon. Thus, knowledge must be assimilated in an active process by a learner with matured mental capacity, so that knowledge can build in complexity by scaffolded understanding. Understanding is scaffolded by the learner through the process of equilibration, whereby the learner balances new knowledge with previous understanding, thereby compensating for "transformation" of knowledge.[71]
Learning, then, can also be supported by instructors in an educational setting. Piaget specified that knowledge cannot truly be formed until the learner has matured the mental structures to which that learning is specific, and thereby development constrains learning. Nevertheless, knowledge can also be "built" by building on simpler operations and structures that have already been formed. Basing operations of an advanced structure on those of simpler structures thus scaffolds learning to build on operational abilities as they develop. Good teaching, then, is built around the operational abilities of the students such that they can excel in their operational stage and build on preexisting structures and abilities and thereby "build" learning.[71]
Evidence of the effectiveness of a contemporary curricular design building on Piaget's theories of developmental progression and the support of maturing mental structures can be seen in Griffin and Case's "Number Worlds" curriculum.[72] The curriculum works toward building a "central conceptual structure" of number sense in young children by building on five instructional processes, including aligning curriculum to the developmental sequencing of acquisition of specific skills. By outlining the developmental sequence of number sense, a conceptual structure is built and aligned to individual children as they develop.
The cognitive scientist Karen Fuson has argued that the impact of Piagetian theories in education has not been entirely positive because his work has frequently been misinterpreted. In particular, Piaget's focus on children's interactions with objects in the concrete operational stage has led to an approach to education in which young children are encouraged to learn mathematics by manipulating real objects, but without the necessary direct instruction from teachers that they need to understand what they are doing and to link their activities to symbolic mathematics. This has had a particularly negative impact on low-attaining children who need more support from a more knowledgeable other to make meaning and progress with their learning.[73]
Psychologist Mark Seidenberg has criticised the field of Education Studies for placing too much emphasis on the works of Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky and other historical psychologists while failing to keep up with the major advances in cognitive science in the decades since they were active.[74] Meanwhile, a 2016 systematic review of education research showed that constructivist approaches to early childhood education inspired by Piaget and Vygotsky are less effective than comprehensive approaches that incorporate direct skills teaching.[75]
Morality
[edit]Piaget believed in two basic principles relating to character education: that children develop moral ideas in stages and that children create their conceptions of the world. According to Piaget, "the child is someone who constructs his own moral world view, who forms ideas about right and wrong, and fair and unfair, that are not the direct product of adult teaching and that are often maintained in the face of adult wishes to the contrary" (Gallagher, 1978, p. 26). Piaget believed that children made moral judgments based on their own observations of the world.
Piaget's theory of morality was radical when his book The Moral Judgment of the Child was published in 1932 for two reasons: his use of philosophical criteria to define morality (as universalizable, generalizable, and obligatory) and his rejection of equating cultural norms with moral norms. Piaget, drawing on Kantian theory, proposed that morality developed out of peer interaction and that it was autonomous from authority mandates. Peers, not parents, were a key source of moral concepts such as equality, reciprocity, and justice.
Piaget attributed different types of psychosocial processes to different forms of social relationships, introducing a fundamental distinction between different types of said relationships. Where there is constraint because one participant holds more power than the other the relationship is asymmetrical, and, importantly, the knowledge that can be acquired by the dominated participant takes on a fixed and inflexible form. Piaget refers to this process as one of social transmission, illustrating it through reference to the way in which the elders of a tribe initiate younger members into the patterns of beliefs and practices of the group. Similarly, where adults exercise a dominating influence over the growing child, it is through social transmission that children can acquire knowledge. By contrast, in cooperative relations, power is more evenly distributed between participants so that a more symmetrical relationship emerges. Under these conditions, authentic forms of intellectual exchange become possible; each partner has the freedom to project his or her own thoughts, consider the positions of others, and defend his or her own point of view. In such circumstances, where children's thinking is not limited by a dominant influence, Piaget believed "the reconstruction of knowledge", or favorable conditions for the emergence of constructive solutions to problems, exists. Here the knowledge that emerges is open, flexible and regulated by the logic of argument rather than being determined by an external authority.
In short, cooperative relations provide the arena for the emergence of operations, which for Piaget requires the absence of any constraining influence, and is most often illustrated by the relations that form between peers (for more on the importance of this distinction see Duveen & Psaltis, 2008; Psaltis & Duveen, 2006, 2007). This is thus how, according to Piaget, children learn moral judgement as opposed to cultural norms (or maybe ideological norms).
Piaget's research on morality was highly influential in subsequent work on moral development, particularly in the case of Lawrence Kohlberg's highly influential stage theory of moral development[76] which dominated moral psychology research until the end of the twentieth century.[77]
Historical studies of thought and cognition
[edit]Historical changes of thought have been modeled in Piagetian terms. Broadly speaking these models have mapped changes in morality, intellectual life and cognitive levels against historical changes (typically in the complexity of social systems).
Notable examples include:
- Michael Horace Barnes' study of the co-evolution of religious and scientific thinking[78]
- Peter Damerow's theory of prehistoric and archaic thought[79]
- Kieran Egan's stages of understanding[80]
- James W. Fowler's stages of faith development
- Suzi Gablik's stages of art history[81]
- Christopher Hallpike's studies of changes in cognition and moral judgment in pre-historical, archaic and classical periods ... (Hallpike 1979, 2004)
- Lawrence Kohlberg's stages of moral development
- Don Lepan's theory of the origins of modern thought and drama[82]
- Charles Radding's theory of the medieval intellectual development[83]
- Jürgen Habermas's reworking of historical materialism.
Non-human development
[edit]Neo-Piagetian stages have been applied to the maximum stage attained by various animals. For example, spiders attain the circular sensory motor stage, coordinating actions and perceptions. Pigeons attain the sensory motor stage, forming concepts.[84]
Origins
[edit]The origins of human intelligence have also been studied in Piagetian terms. Wynn (1979, 1981) analysed Acheulian and Oldowan tools in terms of the insight into spatial relationships required to create each kind. On a more general level, Robinson's Birth of Reason (2005) suggests a large-scale model for the emergence of a Piagetian intelligence.
Primatology
[edit]Piaget's models of cognition have also been applied outside the human sphere, and some primatologists assess the development and abilities of primates in terms of Piaget's model.[85]
Philosophy
[edit]Philosophers have used Piaget's work. For example, the philosopher and social theorist Jürgen Habermas has incorporated Piaget into his work, most notably in The Theory of Communicative Action. The philosopher Thomas Kuhn credited Piaget's work with helping him to understand the transition between modes of thought which characterized his theory of paradigm shifts.[86] Yet, that said, it is also noted that the implications of his later work do indeed remain largely unexamined.[87] Shortly before his death (September 1980), Piaget was involved in a debate about the relationships between innate and acquired features of language, at the Centre Royaumont pour une Science de l'Homme, where he discussed his point of view with the linguist Noam Chomsky as well as Hilary Putnam and Stephen Toulmin.
Artificial intelligence
[edit]Piaget also had a considerable effect in the field of computer science and artificial intelligence. Seymour Papert used Piaget's work while developing the Logo programming language. Alan Kay used Piaget's theories as the basis for the Dynabook programming system concept, which was first discussed within the confines of the Xerox Palo Alto Research Center (Xerox PARC). These discussions led to the development of the Alto prototype, which explored for the first time all the elements of the graphical user interface (GUI), and influenced the creation of user interfaces in the 1980s and beyond.[88]
Criticisms
[edit]Criticisms of Piaget's methods
[edit]Judged by today's standards of psychological research, Piaget's research methods can be considered problematic. One modern reviewer said many of his "pioneering investigations would probably be rejected from most modern journals on methodological grounds of sample size, non-standard measurement, and lack of inter-rater reliability".[65]
Piaget's research relied on very small samples that were not randomly selected. His book The Origins of Intelligence in Children was based on the study of just his own three children.[61] This means that it is difficult to generalize his findings to the broader population. He interacted closely with his research subjects and did not follow a set script, meaning that experimental conditions were not the same from participant to participant.
As Piaget worked in the era before widespread use of voice recording equipment, his data collection method was simply to make handwritten notes in the field, which he would analyse himself.[65] This differs from the modern practice of using multiple coders to ensure test validity. Critics such as Linda Siegel have argued that his experiments did not adequately control for social context and the child's understanding (or lack of understanding) of the language used in the test task, leading to mistaken conclusions about children's lack of reasoning skills.[89]
These methodological issues mean scientists trying to replicate Piaget's experiments have found that small changes to his procedures lead to different results. For example, in his tests of object-permanence and conservation of number, the ages at which children pass the tests varies greatly based on small variations in the test procedure, challenging his theoretical interpretations of his test results.[65][64]
Criticisms of Piaget's theoretical ideas
[edit]Piaget's theories have not gone without scrutiny. A figure whose ideas contradicted Piaget's ideas was the Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky. Vygotsky stressed the importance of a child's cultural background as an effect on the stages of development. Because different cultures stress different social interactions, this challenged Piaget's theory that the hierarchy of learning development had to develop in succession. Vygotsky introduced the term Zone of proximal development as an overall task a child would have to develop that would be too difficult to develop alone.
Also, the so-called neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development maintained that Piaget's theory does not do justice either to the underlying mechanisms of information processing that explain transition from stage to stage or individual differences in cognitive development. According to these theories, changes in information processing mechanisms, such as speed of processing and working memory, are responsible for ascension from stage to stage. Moreover, differences between individuals in these processes explain why some individuals develop faster than other individuals (Demetriou, 1998).
Over time, alternative theories of child development have been put forward, and empirical findings have done a lot to undermine Piaget's theories. For example, Esther Thelen and colleagues[90] found that babies would not make the A-not-B error if they had small weights added to their arms during the first phase of the experiment that were then removed before the second phase of the experiment. This minor change should not impact babies' understanding of object permanence, so the difference that this makes to babies' performance on the A-not-B task cannot be explained by Piagetian theory. Thelen and colleagues also found that various other factors also influenced performance on the A-not-B task (including strength of memory trace, salience of targets, waiting time and stance), and proposed that this could be better explained using a dynamic systems theory approach than using Piagetian theory. Alison Gopnik and Betty Repacholi[91] found that babies as young as 18 months old can understand that other people have desires, and that these desires could be very different from their own desires. This contradicts Piaget's view that children are very egocentric at this age.
Modern cognitive science had undermined Piaget's view that young children are unable to comprehend numbers as they are not able to work with abstract concepts in the sensorimotor stage. This Piagetian view has led many educators to believe that it is not appropriate to teach simple arithmetic to young children as it will not lead to real understanding.[92] Experiments by Starkey et al. have shown that children have an understanding of abstract numbers from as young as 6 months old while more recent studies by Izard et al. have shown that even newborns can perceive abstract numbers.[93][63] For a full discussion of this, see Stanislas Dehaene's The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics.[92]
Some supporters of Piaget counter that his critics' arguments depend on misreadings of Piaget's theory.[94] See also Brian Rotman's Jean Piaget: Psychologist of the Real, an exposition and critique of Piaget's ideas, and Jonathan Tudge and Barbara Rogoff's "Peer influences on cognitive development: Piagetian and Vygotskian perspectives".[95]
List of major achievements
[edit]Appointments
[edit]- 1921–25 Research Director (Chef des travaux), Institut Jean-Jacques Rousseau, Geneva
- 1925–29 Professor of Psychology, Sociology and the Philosophy of Science, University of Neuchatel
- 1929–39 Professeur extraordinaire of the History of Scientific Thought, University of Geneva
- 1929–67 Director, International Bureau of Education, Geneva
- 1932–71 Director, Institute of Educational Sciences, University of Geneva
- 1938–51 Professor of Experimental Psychology and Sociology, University of Lausanne
- 1939–51 Professor of Sociology, University of Geneva
- 1940–71 Professeur ordinaire of Experimental Psychology, University of Geneva
- 1952–64 Professor of Genetic Psychology, Sorbonne, Paris
- 1954–57 President, International Union of Scientific Psychology
- 1955–80 Director, International Centre for Genetic Epistemology, Geneva
- 1971–80 Emeritus Professor, University of Geneva
Honorary doctorates
[edit]- 1936 Harvard
- 1946 Sorbonne
- 1949 University of Brazil
- 1949 Bruxelles
- 1953 Chicago
- 1954 McGill
- 1958 Warsaw
- 1959 Manchester
- 1960 Oslo
- 1960 Cambridge
- 1962 Brandeis
- 1964 Montreal
- 1964 Aix-Marseille
- 1966 Pennsylvania[96]
- 1966? Barcelona[97]
- 1970 Yale[98]
- 1971 Temple[99]
List of major works
[edit]The following groupings are based on the number of citations in Google Scholar.
The most-cited publications in English
[edit]- The Language and Thought of the Child (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1926) [Le Langage et la pensée chez l'enfant (1923)]
- The Child's Conception of the World (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1928) [La Représentation du monde chez l'enfant (1926, orig. pub. as an article, 1925)]
- Judgment and Reasoning in the Child (Harcourt, Brace and Company 1928).
- The Moral Judgment of the Child (London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co., 1932) [Le jugement moral chez l'enfant (1932)]
- The Origins of Intelligence in Children (New York: International University Press, 1952) [La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant (1936), also translated as The Origin of Intelligence in the Child (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1953)].
- Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood (New York: Norton, 1962) [La formation du symbole chez l'enfant; imitation, jeu et reve, image et représentation (1945)].
- The Psychology of Intelligence (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1950) [La psychologie de l'intelligence (1947)].
- The construction of reality in the child (New York: Basic Books, 1954) [La construction du réel chez l'enfant (1950), also translated as The Child's Construction of Reality (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1955)].
- With Inhelder, B., The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence (New York: Basic Books, 1958) [De la logique de l'enfant à la logique de l'adolescent (1955)].
- With Inhelder, B., The Psychology of the Child (New York: Basic Books, 1962) [La psychologie de l'enfant (1966, orig. pub. as an article, 1950)].
- The early growth of logic in the child (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1964) [La genèse des structures logiques elementaires (1959)].
- With Inhelder, B., The Child's Conception of Space (New York: W.W. Norton, 1967).
- "Piaget's theory" in P. Mussen (ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 1. (4th ed., New York: Wiley, 1983).
- The Child's Conception of Number (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1952) [La genèse du nombre chez l'enfant (1941)].
- Structuralism (New York: Harper & Row, 1970) [Le Structuralisme (1968)].
- Genetic epistemology (New York: W.W. Norton, 1971, ISBN 978-0-393-00596-7).
Less-cited
[edit]- The child's conception of physical causality (London: Kegan Paul, 1930) [La causalite physique chez l'enfant (1927)]
- Child's Conception of Geometry (New York, Basic Books, 1960) [La Géométrie spontanée de l'enfant (1948)].
- The Principles of Genetic Epistemology (New York: Basic Books, 1972, ISBN 978-0-393-00596-7) [L'épistémologie génétique (1950)].
- To understand is to invent: The future of education (New York: Grossman Publishers, 1973) [tr. of Ou va l'education (1971) and Le droit a l'education dans le monde actuel (1948)].
- Six psychological studies (New York: Random House, 1967) [Six études de psychologie (1964)].
- Biology and Knowledge (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1971) [Biologie et connaissance; essai sur les relations entre les régulations organiques et les processus cognitifs (1967)]
- Science of education and the psychology of the child (New York: Orion Press, 1970) [Psychologie et pédagogie (1969)].
- Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood (Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1977) [L'evolution intellectuelle entre l'adolescence et l'age adulte (1970)].
- The Equilibration of Cognitive Structures: The Central Problem of Intellectual Development (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1985) [L'equilibration des structures cognitives (1975), previously translated as The development of thought: Equilibration of cognitive structures (1977)].
- Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini (ed.), Language and learning: the debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1980) [Theories du language, theories de l'apprentissage (1979)].
- Development and learning.
- The Grasp of Consciousness: Action and concept in the young child (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1977) [La prise de conscience (1974)].
- The Mechanisms of Perception (New York: Basic Books, 1969) [Les mécanismes perceptifs: modèles probabilistes, analyse génétique, relations avec l'intelligence (1961)].
- Psychology and Epistemology: Towards a Theory of Knowledge (Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1972) [Psychologie et epistémologie (1970).
- The Child's Conception of Time (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1969) [Le développement de la notion de temps chez l'enfant (1946)]
- Logic and Psychology (Manchester: Manchester University Press, 1953).
- Memory and intelligence (New York: Basic Books, 1973) [Memoire et intelligence (1968)]
- The Origin of the Idea of Chance in Children (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1975) [La genèse de l'idée de hasard chez l'enfant (1951)].
- Mental imagery in the child: a study of the development of imaginal representation (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1971) [L'image mentale chez l'enfant : études sur le développement des représentations imaginées (1966)].
- Intelligence and Affectivity. Their Relationship during Child Development (Palo Alto: Annual Reviews, 1981) [Les relations entre l'intelligence et l'affectivité dans le développement de l'enfant (1954)].
- With Beth, E. W.,Mathematical Epistemology and Psychology (Dordrecht: D. Reidel, 1966) [Épistémologie mathématique et psychologie: Essai sur les relations entre la logique formelle et la pensée réelle] (1961).
- With Brown, Terrance. Eames, Stewart (Translator). Adaptation and Intelligence: Organic Selection and Phenocopy (University of Chicago Press, 1980).
- With Garcia, R. Psychogenesis and the History of Science (New York: Columbia University Press, 1989) Psychogenèse et histoire des sciences (1983).
New translations
[edit]- Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological Studies. London: Routledge.
- Piaget, J. (2000). "Commentary on Vygotsky". New Ideas in Psychology. 18 (2–3): 241–59. doi:10.1016/S0732-118X(00)00012-X.
- Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in Reflecting Abstraction. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
See also
[edit]- Active learning
- Cognitive acceleration
- Cognitivism (learning theory)
- Constructivist epistemology
- Developmental psychology
- Fluid and crystallized intelligence
- Guðmundur Finnbogason
- Horizontal and vertical décalage
- Inquiry-based learning
- Kohlberg's stages of moral development
- Psychosocial development
- Religious development
- Water-level task
Collaborators
[edit]- Edith Ackermann
- Leo Apostel
- Edgar Ascher
- Evert Beth
- Magali Bovet
- Guy Cellérier
- Paul Fraisse
- Rolando García
- Pierre Gréco
- Jean-Blaise Grize
- Gil Henriques
- Bärbel Inhelder
- Benoit Mandelbrot
- Albert Morf
- Pierre Oléron
- Seymour Papert
- Maurice Reuchlin
- Hermina Sinclair de-Zwart
- Alina Szeminska
- Huê Vinh-Bang[citation needed]
Translators
[edit]Notes
[edit]- ^ Jones, Daniel (2011). Roach, Peter; Setter, Jane; Esling, John (eds.). Cambridge English Pronouncing Dictionary (18th ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-15255-6.
- ^ "Piaget, Jean". Lexico UK English Dictionary. Oxford University Press. Archived from the original on 7 May 2021.
- ^ "Piaget". The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language (5th ed.). HarperCollins. Retrieved 10 August 2019.
- ^ "Piaget". Collins English Dictionary. HarperCollins. Retrieved 10 August 2019.
- ^ "Piaget". Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary. Merriam-Webster. Retrieved 10 August 2019.
- ^ Jean Piaget's Theory and Stages of Cognitive Development, by Saul McLeod, Simply Psychology, updated 2018
- ^ Munari, Alberto (1994). "Jean Piaget" (PDF). Prospects: The Quarterly Review of Comparative Education. XXIV (1/2): 311–327. doi:10.1007/bf02199023. S2CID 144657103. Archived (PDF) from the original on 31 October 2008.
- ^ "About Piaget". Jean Piaget Society. Archived from the original on 24 August 2019. Retrieved 17 October 2016.
- ^ Burman, J. T. (2012). "Jean Piaget: Images of a life and his factory". History of Psychology. 15 (3): 283–288. doi:10.1037/a0025930. ISSN 1093-4510. PMID 23397918.
- ^ von Glasersfeld, E. (1990). "An exposition of constructivism: Why some like it radical". Journal for Research in Mathematics Education: Monograph. 4: 19–29 & 195–210 [22]. doi:10.2307/749910. ISSN 0883-9530. JSTOR 749910. (p. 22).
- ^ Hsueh, Y (2009). "Piaget in the United States, 1925–1971. In U. Müller, J. I. M. Carpendale & L. Smith (Eds.), The Cambridge Companion to Piaget (pp. 344–370). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Müller, U., Burman, J. T., & Hutchinson, S. (2013). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget: A quinquagenary retrospective". Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 34 (1): 52–55. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.10.001.
- ^ Pickren, W. E. (2012). Joseph McVicker Hunt: Golden age psychologist. In W. E. Pickren, D. A. Dewsbury, & M. Wertheimer (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Developmental Psychology (pp. 185–203). New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.
- ^ Haggbloom, Steven J.; Warnick, Renee; Warnick, Jason E.; Jones, Vinessa K.; Yarbrough, Gary L.; Russell, Tenea M.; Borecky, Chris M.; McGahhey, Reagan; Powell, John L. III; Beavers, Jamie; Monte, Emmanuelle (2002). "The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century" (PDF). Review of General Psychology. 6 (2): 139–152. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139. S2CID 145668721. Archived (PDF) from the original on 25 December 2014.
- ^ "Jean Piaget: biography of the father of evolutionary psychology 【NUOVO】". All about Psychology - VirtualPsychCentre. Retrieved 20 February 2022.
- ^ "James Jackson". Geni. Retrieved 20 February 2022.
- ^ "Jean Piaget", Biography. Accessed 28 February 2012
- ^ Restak, Richard (2006). The Naked Brain: How the Emerging Neurosociety is Changing How We Live, Work, and Love. New York: Harmony. p. 156. ISBN 978-1-4000-9808-8.
- ^ Biehler, Robert F. (1978). Psychology Applied to Teaching. Houghton Mifflin. p. 113. ISBN 978-0-395-11921-1.
- ^ A Brief Biography of Jean Piaget Archived 24 August 2019 at the Wayback Machine, Jean Piaget Society (Society for the study of knowledge and development)
- ^ Piaget, Jean (1952), Boring, Edwin G.; Werner, Heinz; Langfeld, Herbert S.; Yerkes, Robert M. (eds.), "Jean Piaget.", A History of Psychology in Autobiography, Vol IV., Worcester: Clark University Press, pp. 237–256, doi:10.1037/11154-011, retrieved 7 July 2022
- ^ Mayer, Susan (21 October 2005). "A Brief Biography of Jean Piaget" (PDF). gseacademic.harvard.edu. Archived (PDF) from the original on 1 September 2006.
- ^ Voyat, G. (1981). "Jean Piaget: 1896–1980". The American Journal of Psychology. 94 (4): 645–648. PMID 7044156.
- ^ Fondation Jean Piaget – Biographie. Fondationjeanpiaget.ch. Retrieved on 26 February 2018.
- ^ Anon (1970). "Distinguished Scientific Contribution Awards: 1969: Citation for Jean Piaget". American Psychologist. 25 (1): 65–79. doi:10.1037/h0020564. PMID 4910176.
- ^ Rockcastle, Verne N. (1964, p. xi), the conference director, wrote in the conference report of the Jean Piaget conferences about Piaget:
- Although few of us had any personal contact with Piaget prior to the conference, those who attended came to have the deepest and warmest regard for him both as a scientist and as a person. His sense of humor throughout the conference was a sort of international glue that flavored his lectures and punctuated his informal conversation. To sit at the table with him during a meal was not only an intellectual pleasure but a pure social delight. Piaget was completely unsophisticated in spite of his international stature. We could hardly believe it when he came prepared for two weeks' stay with only his 'serviette' and a small Swissair bag. An American would have had at least two large suitcases. When Piaget left Berkeley, he had his serviette, the small Swissair bag, and a third, larger bag crammed with botanical specimens. 'Where did you get that bag?' we asked. 'I had it in one of the others,' he replied.
- ^ Burman, Jeremy Trevelyan (2013). "Profiles of international archives: Les archives Jean Piaget, University of Geneva, Switzerland". History of Psychology. 16 (2): 158–61. doi:10.1037/a0031405. PMID 23544355. A photograph of his grave is available here.
- ^ Beilin, H. (1992). "Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology". Developmental Psychology. 28 (2): 191–204. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191. S2CID 14459165.
- ^ Burman, J. T. (2011). "The zeroeth Piaget". Theory & Psychology. 21 (1): 130–135. doi:10.1177/0959354310361407. S2CID 220119333.
- ^ Mayer, Susan (2005). "The Early Evolution of Jean Piaget's Clinical Method". History of Psychology. 8 (4): 362–82. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.8.4.362. PMID 17152748.
- ^ Hsueh, Y. (2001). "Basing much of the reasoning upon the work of Jean Piaget, 1927–1936". Archives de Psychologie. 69 (268–269): 39–62.
- ^ Hsueh, Y. (2002). "The Hawthorne Experiments and the introduction of Jean Piaget in American Industrial Psychology, 1929–1932". History of Psychology. 5 (2): 163–189. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.5.2.163. PMID 12096759.
- ^ Hsueh, Y (2004). "He sees the development of children's concepts upon a background of sociology": Jean Piaget's honorary degree at Harvard University in 1936". History of Psychology. 7 (1): 20–44. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.7.1.20. PMID 15022668.
- ^ Ormrod, J.E. (2012). Essentials of Educational Psychology: Big Ideas to Guide Effective Teaching. Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc.
- ^ Hsueh, Y. (2005). The lost and found experience: Piaget rediscovered. The Constructivist, 16(1).
- ^ Guthrie, James W. (2003) "Piaget, Jean (1896–1980)" in Encyclopedia of Education. 2nd ed. Vol. 5. New York, NY: Macmillan Reference USA. pp. 1894–898.
- ^ Valsiner, J. (2005). "Participating in Piaget". Society. 42 (2): 57–61. doi:10.1007/BF02687400. S2CID 145294976.
- ^ Jean Piaget at the Encyclopædia Britannica
- ^ McLeod, Saul. "Preoperational Stage – Egocentrism". Simply Psychology. Retrieved 7 August 2018.
- ^ Gardner, Howard (1981) The Quest for Mind: Piaget, Levi-Strauss and the Structuralist Movement, University of Chicago Press.
- ^ Beilin Harry (1992). "Piaget's Enduring Contribution to Developmental Psychology". Developmental Psychology. 28 (2): 191–204. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191.
- ^ Kurt, Dr Serhat (17 November 2022). "Jean Piaget: Biography, Theory and Cognitive Development". Education Library. Retrieved 30 November 2022.
- ^ Cherry, Kendra (1 May 2024). "Piaget's 4 Stages of Cognitive Development Explained". www.verywellmind.com. Retrieved 3 July 2024.
- ^ Santrock, John W. (1998) Children. 9. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
- ^ Kaye, K. (1982) The Mental and Social Life of Babies. U. Chicago Press.
- ^ a b c Santrock, John W. (2004). Life-Span Development (9th Ed.). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill College – Chapter 8
- ^ a b Piaget, J. (1976). On correspondences and morphisms. Jean Piaget Society Newsletter, 5. (unpaged)
- ^ a b c d e Piaget, J. Grize, J.-B., Szeminska, A., & Vinh Bang. (1977). Epistemology and psychology of functions. Studies in genetic epistemology. Vol. 23. Dordrecht, Holland: D. Reldel, 1977.
- ^ a b Piaget, J. (1977). Some recent research and its link with a new theory of groupings and conservation based on commutability. In R. W. Rieber and K. Salzinger (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 291, 350-358.
- ^ Schonfeld, I. S. (1986). The Genevan and Cattell-Horn conceptions of intelligence compared: The early implementation of numerical solution aids. Developmental Psychology, 22, 204-212. doi.org/10.10'37/0012-1649.22.2.204
- ^ a b Inhelder, B., Sinclair, H., & Bovet, M. Learning and the development of cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1974
- ^ Miller, Patrica H. (2009) Theories of Developmental Psychology 5th Edition, Worth Publishers.
- ^ Eysenck, Michael W. and Keane, Mark. T. (2010). Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook, (6th.). East Sussex: Psychology Press..
- ^ Naested, I., Potvin, B., & Waldron, P. (2004). Understanding the landscape of teaching. Toronto, Ontario: Pearson Education Canada.
- ^ Shaffer, D. R., Wood, E., & Willoughby, T. (2005). Developmental psychology: Childhood and adolescence. Toronto, Ontario: Nelson Education Canada.
- ^ Piaget, J. (1953). The origin of intelligence in the child. New Fetter Lane, New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
- ^ a b c Auger, W. F., & Rich, S. J. (2007). Curriculum theory and methods: Perspectives on learning and teaching. Mississauga, Ontario: John Wiley & Sons Canada.
- ^ Gallagher, J. M., & Reid, D. K. (1981). The learning theory of Piaget and Inhelder. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.
- ^ Phillips, John L. (1969). The Origin of Intellect: Piaget's Theory. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. ISBN 0-7167-0579-6
- ^ "41st Annual Meeting of The Jean Piaget Society" (PDF). Piaget.prg. 2 June 2011. Archived (PDF) from the original on 11 September 2015. Retrieved 17 October 2016.
- ^ Gopnik, Alison (1996). "The Post-Piaget Era". Psychological Science. 7 (4): 221–225. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00363.x. S2CID 143973228. Retrieved 1 April 2021.
- ^ a b Hopkins, J.R. (2011). "The Enduring Influence of Jean Piaget". APS Observer. 24 (10).
- ^ Gopnik, A.; Wellman, H.M. (2012). "Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (6): 1085–1108. doi:10.1037/a0028044. PMC 3422420. PMID 22582739.
- ^ a b Izard, V.; Sann, C.; Spelke, E.S.; Streri, A. (2009). "Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (25): 10382–10385. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10610382I. doi:10.1073/pnas.0812142106. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 2700913. PMID 19520833.
- ^ a b Dehaene, Stanislas (2020). How we learn: why brains learn better than any machine ... for now. New York, New York: Viking. pp. 162–163. ISBN 978-0-525-55989-4.
- ^ a b c d Klahr, D. (2012). "Revisiting Piaget: A perspective from studies of children's problem-solving abilities". In Slater, A.M.; Quinn, P.C. (eds.). Developmental psychology: revisiting the classic studies. London: SAGE. ISBN 978-0-85702-757-3.
- ^ Martí, E. (2020). "Post-Piagetian Perspectives of Cognitive Development". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190264093.013.914. ISBN 978-0-19-026409-3.
- ^ Morra, Sergio (2008). Cognitive development: neo-Piagetian perspectives. New York: Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0-8058-6350-5.
- ^ a b c Henson, Kenneth (2003). "Foundations for Learner-Centered Education: A Knowledge Base". Education. 1124 (1): 5–16.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i Seifert, Kelvin; Sutton, Rosemary (2009). Educational Psychology (PDF) (2nd ed.). Florida: Orange Grove. ISBN 978-1-61610-154-1. Archived (PDF) from the original on 25 October 2013. Retrieved 22 June 2015.
- ^ Hawkins, Shannon M.; Heflin, L. Juane (2001). "Increasing Secondary Teachers' Behavior-Specific Praise Using a Video Self-Modeling and Visual Performance Feedback Intervention". Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions. 12 (2): 97–108. doi:10.1177/1098300709358110. S2CID 143631715.
- ^ a b c Piaget, J. (1964). "Development and learning". In R.E. Ripple and V.N. Rockcastle (Eds.), Piaget Rediscovered: A Report on the Conference of Cognitive Studies and Curriculum Development (pp. 7–20). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.
- ^ Griffin, S.A. (2004). "Building number sense with Number Worlds: a mathematics program for young children". Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 19: 173–180. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.012.
- ^ Fuson, Karen C. (2009). "Avoiding misinterpretations of Piaget and Vygotsky: Mathematical teaching without learning, learning without teaching, or helpful learning-path teaching?". Cognitive Development. 24 (4): 343–361. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.09.009. ISSN 0885-2014.
- ^ Seidenberg, Mark (2017). Language at the speed of sight: how we read, why so many can't, and what can be done about it. New York: Basic Books. p. 260. ISBN 978-1-5416-1715-5.
- ^ Chambers, Bette; Cheung, Alan C.K.; Slavin, Robert E. (2016). "Literacy and language outcomes of comprehensive and developmental-constructivist approaches to early childhood education: A systematic review". Educational Research Review. 18. Elsevier BV: 88–111. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2016.03.003. ISSN 1747-938X.
- ^ Levine, Charles; Kohlberg, Lawrence; Hewer, Alexandra (1985). "The Current Formulation of Kohlberg's Theory and a Response to Critics". Human Development. 28 (2): 94–100. doi:10.1159/000272945.
- ^ Shweder, Richard A.; Haidt, Jonathan (6 May 2016). "The Future of Moral Psychology: Truth, Intuition, and the Pluralist Way". Psychological Science. 4 (6): 360–365. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00582.x. S2CID 143483576.
- ^ Barnes, Michael Horace (2000). Stages of thought: the co-evolution of religious thought and science. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-513389-9.
- ^ Damerow, P. (1998). Prehistory And Cognitive Development. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-8058-2210-6. Retrieved 24 March 2008.
{{cite book}}
:|journal=
ignored (help) - ^ Kieran Egan (1997). The educated mind: How Cognitive Tools Shape Our Understanding. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-19036-5.
- ^ Gablik, Suzi (1977). Progress in art. New York: Rizzoli. ISBN 978-0-8478-0082-7.
- ^ LePan, Don (1989). The cognitive revolution in Western culture. New York: Macmillan. ISBN 978-0-333-45796-2.
- ^ Radding, Charles (1985). A world made by men: cognition and society, 400–1200. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press. ISBN 978-0-8078-1664-6.
- ^ Dore, F.Y. "Psychology of animal cognition: Piagetian studies". psychnet.apa. Psychological Bulletin. Retrieved 8 October 2020.
- ^ McKinney, Michael L.; Parker, Sue Taylor (1999). Origins of intelligence: the evolution of cognitive development in monkeys, apes, and humans. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. ISBN 978-0-8018-6012-6.
- ^ Burman, J. T. (2007). "Piaget No 'Remedy' for Kuhn, But the Two Should be Read Together: Comment on Tsou's 'Piaget vs. Kuhn on Scientific Progress'". Theory & Psychology. 17 (5): 721–732. doi:10.1177/0959354307079306. S2CID 145497321.
- ^ Burman, J. T. (2008). "Experimenting in relation to Piaget: Education is a chaperoned process of adaptation". Perspectives on Science. 16 (2): 160–195. doi:10.1162/posc.2008.16.2.160. S2CID 57572564.
- ^ Drescher, Gary (1991). Made-Up Minds: A Constructivist Approach to Artificial Intelligence. Boston: MIT Press. p. 236. ISBN 978-0-262-04120-1.
- ^ Siegel, Linda S (1993). "Amazing new discovery: Piaget was wrong!". Canadian Psychology. 34 (3): 239–245. doi:10.1037/h0078835.
- ^ Spencer, J. P.; Clearfield, M.; Corbetta, D.; Ulrich, B.; Buchanan, P.; Schöner, G. (2006). "Moving Toward a Grand Theory of Development: In Memory of Esther Thelen". Child Development. 77 (6): 1521–1538. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.531.5232. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00955.x. PMID 17107442.
- ^ Repacholi, Betty; Alison Gopnik (1997). "Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds". Developmental Psychology. 3 (1): 12–21. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12. PMID 9050386.
- ^ a b Dehaene, Stanislas (2011). The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics (2 ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-975387-1.
- ^ Starkey, Prentice; Spelke, Elizabeth S.; Gelman, Rochel (1990). "Numerical abstraction by human infants". Cognition. 36 (2): 97–127. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90001-Z. ISSN 0010-0277. PMID 2225757. S2CID 706365.
- ^ Lourenço, O.; Machado, A. (1996). "In defense of Piaget's theory: A reply to ten common criticisms" (PDF). Psychological Review. 103 (1): 143–164. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.143. S2CID 32390745. Archived from the original (PDF) on 10 April 2020.
- ^ Tudge, Jonathan; Barbara Rogoff (1998). "Peer influences on cognitive development: Piagetian and Vygotskian perspectives". In Peter Lloyd; Charles Fernyhough (eds.). Lev Vygotsky: Critical Assessments, Volume 3. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-11154-6.
- ^ The list is certain only to 1966. The source is p. xviii of F. Bresson & M. de Montmollin, 1966, Psychologie et épistémologie génétique: thèmes Piagétiens (Hommage à Jean Piaget avec une bibliographie complète de ses oeuvres). Paris: Dunod. (Note: This list provides "Varsovie" instead of Warsaw, as this is the French name for the capital of Poland.)
- ^ Reported in 1971, in Anuario de psicología, as part of the proceedings of a celebration of Piaget's 70th birthday, raco.cat
- ^ Kessen, W (1996). "American Psychology just before Piaget". Psychological Science. 7 (4): 196–199. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00358.x. JSTOR 40062944. S2CID 145615307.
- ^ Past Honorary Degree Recipients, Temple University
References
[edit]- Aqueci, F. (2003). Ordine e trasformazione: morale, mente, discorso in Piaget. Acireale-Roma: Bonanno. ISBN 88-7796-148-1.
- Amann-Gainotti, M.; Ducret, J.-J. (1992). "Jean Piaget, disciple of Pierre Janet: Influence of behavior psychology and relations with psychoanalysis". Information Psychiatrique. 68: 598–606.
- Beilin, H. (1992). "Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology". Developmental Psychology. 28 (2): 191–204. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.191. S2CID 14459165.
- Beilin, H. (1994). Jean Piaget's enduring contribution to developmental psychology. A century of developmental psychology (pp. 257–290). Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association.
- Bringuier, J.-C. (1980). Conversations with Jean Piaget (B.M. Gulati, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1977) ISBN 0-226-07503-6.
- Chapman, M. (1988). Constructive evolution: Origins and development of Piaget's thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-36712-3.
- Demetriou, A. (1998). Cognitive development. In A. Demetriou, W. Doise, K. F. M. van Lieshout (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology (pp. 179–269). London: Wiley.
- Demetriou, A., Mouyi, A., & Spanoudis, G. (2010). The development of mental processing. Nesselroade, J. R. (2010). Methods in the study of life-span human development: Issues and answers. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), Biology, cognition and methods across the life-span. Volume 1 of the Handbook of life-span development (pp. 36–55), Editor-in-chief: R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
- Duveen, G. & Psaltis, C. (2008). The constructive role of asymmetries in social interaction. In U. Mueller, J. I. M. Carpendale, N. Budwig & B. Sokol (Eds.), Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Eckblad, G. (1981). Scheme theory: A conceptual framework for cognitive-motivational processes. London and New York: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-229550-8.
- Flavell, J. (1967). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. New York: D. Van Nostrand Company. ISBN 0-442-02413-4.
- Fowler, J. W. (1981). Stages of faith: The psychology of human development and the quest for meaning. San Francisco: Harper & Row. ISBN 0-06-062866-9.
- Gattico, E. (2001). Jean Piaget. Milano: Bruno Mondadori. ISBN 88-424-9741-X.
- Hallpike, C.R. (1979). The foundations of primitive thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-823196-2.
- Ivey, A. (1986). Developmental therapy. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. ISBN 1-55542-022-2.
- Kamii, C. (1985). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget's theory. New York: Teachers College Press.
- Kesselring, T. (1999). Jean Piaget. München: Beck. ISBN 3-406-44512-8.
- Kassotakis, M. & Flouris, G. (2006) Μάθηση & Διδασκαλία, Athens.
- Kitchener, R. (1986). Piaget's theory of knowledge: Genetic epistemology & scientific reason. New Haven: Yale University Press. ISBN 0-300-03579-9.
- Kose, G. (1987). "A philosopher's conception of Piaget: Piagetian theory reconsidered". Theoretical & Philosophical Psychology. 7 (1): 52–57. doi:10.1037/h0091442.
- Mayer, S. (2005). "The early evolution of Jean Piaget's clinical method". History of Psychology. 8 (4): 362–382. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.8.4.362. PMID 17152748.
- Messerly, J.G. (1992). Piaget's conception of evolution: Beyond Darwin and Lamarck. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. ISBN 0-8476-8243-9.
- Perret-Clermont, A.-N. & Barrelet, J.-M. (Eds) (2008/2014). Jean Piaget and Neuchâtel. The Learner and the Scholar. Hove and New York: Psychology Press
- Psaltis, C.; Duveen, G. (2006). "Social relations and cognitive development: The influence of conversation type and representations of gender". European Journal of Social Psychology. 36 (3): 407–430. doi:10.1002/ejsp.308.
- Psaltis, C.; Duveen, G. (2007). "Conversation types and conservation: Forms of recognition and cognitive development". British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 25 (1): 79–102. doi:10.1348/026151005X91415.
- Robinson, R.J. (2005). The birth of reason. Prometheus Research Group. (Available online at prometheus.org.uk)
- Smith, L. (Ed.) (1992). Jean Piaget: Critical assessments (4 Vols.). London: Routledge. ISBN 0-415-04408-1.
- Smith, L. (1993). Necessary knowledge: Piagetian perspectives on constructivism. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum. ISBN 0-86377-270-6.
- Smith, L. (Ed.) (1996). Critical readings on Piaget. London: Routledge. ISBN 0-415-13317-3.
- Smith, L. (2001). Jean Piaget. In J. A. Palmer (Ed.), 50 modern thinkers on education: From Piaget to the present. London: Routledge.
- Vidal, F. (1994). Piaget before Piaget. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-66716-6.
- Vonèche, J.J. (1985). Genetic epistemology: Piaget's theory. In T. Husén & T.N. Postlethwaite (Eds.-in-chief), International encyclopedia of education (Vol. 4). Oxford: Pergamon.
- Wynn, T. (1979). "The intelligence of later Acheulean hominids". Man. New Series. 14 (3): 371–391. doi:10.2307/2801865. JSTOR 2801865.
- Wynn, T. (1981). "The intelligence of Oldowan hominids". Journal of Human Evolution. 10 (7): 529–541. Bibcode:1981JHumE..10..529W. doi:10.1016/S0047-2484(81)80046-2.—
Further reading
[edit]Piaget inspired innumerable studies and even new areas of inquiry. The following is a list of critiques and commentaries, organized using the same citation-based method as the list of his own major works (above). These represent the significant and influential post-Piagetian writings in their respective sub-disciplines.
Exemplars
[edit]- Vygotsky, L. (1963). Thought and language. [12630 citations]
Classics
[edit]- Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: Children, Computers, and Powerful Ideas. [4089]
- Minsky, M. (1988). The society of mind. [3950]
- Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage And Sequence: The Cognitive-Developmental Approach To Socialization. [3118]
- Flavell, J. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. [2333] [The development of the project that became this book, and its impact, is discussed in detail by Müller, U.; Burman, J. T.; Hutchison, S. M. (2013). "The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget: A quinquagenary retrospective". Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 34 (1): 52–55. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.10.001. ISSN 0193-3973.]
- Gibson, E. J. (1973). Principles of perceptual learning and development. [1903]
- Hunt, J. McV. (1961). Intelligence and Experience. [617+395+384+111+167+32=1706]
- Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. [1497]
- Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. [1456]
- Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. [1001]
Major works
[edit]- Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. [959]
- Ginsburg, H. P. & Opper, S. (1969). Piaget's theory of intellectual development. [931]
- Singley, M. K. & Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill. [836]
- Duckworth, E. (1973). The having of wonderful ideas. [775]
- Youniss, J. (1982). Parents and peers in social development: A Sullivan-Piaget perspective. [763]
- Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. [563]
- Schaffer, H. R. & Emerson, P. E. (1964). The development of social attachments in infancy. [535]
- Hopper, A. (2016). Introduction to the Qualitative Development of Intelligence.
Works of significance
[edit]- Shatz, M.; Gelman, R. (1973). "The Development of Communication Skills: Modifications in the Speech of Young Children as a Function of Listener". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 38 (5): 1–37. doi:10.2307/1165783. JSTOR 1165783. PMID 4764473. [470]
- Broke, H (1971). "Interpersonal perception of young children: Egocentrism or Empathy?". Developmental Psychology. 5 (2): 263–269. doi:10.1037/h0031267. [469]
- Wadsworth, B. J. (1989). Piaget's theory of cognitive and affective development [421]
- Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond Modularity. [419]
- Bodner, G. M. (1986). Constructivism: A theory of knowledge. [403]
- Shantz, C. U. (1975). The Development of Social Cognition. [387]
- Diamond, A.; Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). "Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex". Experimental Brain Research. 74 (1): 24–40. doi:10.1007/bf00248277. PMID 2924839. S2CID 2310409. [370]
- Gruber, H. & Voneche, H. (1982). The Essential Piaget. [348]
- Walkerdine, V. (1984). Developmental psychology and the child-centred pedagogy: The insertion of Piaget into early education. [338]
- Kamii, C. & DeClark, G. (1985). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget's theory [335]
- Riegel, K. F. (1973). Dialectic operations: The final period of cognitive development [316]
- Bandura, A.; McDonald, F. J. (1963). "Influence of social reinforcement and the behavior of models in shaping children's moral judgment". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 67 (3): 274–281. doi:10.1037/h0044714. PMID 14054361. S2CID 24159020. [314]
- Karplus, R. (1980). Teaching for the development of reasoning. [312]
- Brainerd, C. (1978). The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory. [311]
- Brainerd, C. (1978). Piaget's theory of intelligence. [292]
- Gilligan, C. (1997). Moral orientation and moral development [285]
- Diamond, A. (1991). Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development [284]
- Braine, M. D. S., & Rumain, B. (1983). Logical reasoning. [276]
- John-Steiner, V. (2000). Creative collaboration. [266]
- Pascual-Leone, J. (1987). Organismic processes for neo-Piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development. [261]
- Hallpike, C. R. (1979). The foundations of primitive thought [261]
- Furth, H. (1969). Piaget and Knowledge [261]
- Gelman, R. & Baillargeon, R. (1983). A review of some Piagetian concepts. [260]
- O'Loughlin, M. (1992). Rethinking science education: Beyond piagetian constructivism. Toward a sociocultural model of teaching and learning. [252]
- Messerly, John G. (1996). "Psychogenesis and the History of Science: Piaget and the Problem of Scientific Change", The Modern Schoolman LXXIII, 295–307.
External links
[edit]- Publications by and about Jean Piaget in the catalogue Helveticat of the Swiss National Library
- Jean Piaget Society, society for the study of knowledge and development.
- The Jean Piaget Archives Archived 11 December 2003 at the Wayback Machine, with full bibliography.
- Interview with Jean Piaget and Bärbel Inhelder[usurped] by Elizabeth Hall (1970) (archived 15 February 2015)
- Jean Piaget @ Teaching & Learning Developmental Psychology, Piaget as a scientist with resources for classes.
- Jean Piaget's Genetic Epistemology: Appreciation and Critique by Robert Campbell (2002), extensive summary of work and biography.
- Piaget's The Language and Thought of the Child (1926) – a brief introduction
- The Moral Judgment of the Child by Jean Piaget (1932), at Internet Archive
- The Construction of Reality in the Child by Jean Piaget (1955)
- Piaget's role in the International Bureau of Education Archived 3 June 2008 at the Wayback Machine and the International Conference on Education Archived 11 June 2008 at the Wayback Machine
- Genetic Epistemology by Jean Piaget (1968)
- Comments on Vygotsky by Jean Piaget (1962)
- Piaget's Developmental Theory: An Overview – Part 1 on YouTube, a 27-minute documentary film used primarily in higher education (on YouTube)
- Piaget's Developmental Theory: An Overview – Part 2 on YouTube, a 27-minute documentary film used primarily in higher education (on YouTube)
- Foundation Jean Piaget for research in psychology and epistemology – French version only – diffuse to the world community writings and talks of the Swiss scientist.
- Human Nervous System model in accordance with Piaget's Learning Theory – French version only (archived 10 December 2012)
- Jean Piaget and Neuchâtel The site is maintained by the Institute of Psychology and Education, Neuchâtel University
- Jean Piaget's 1931 essay "The Spirit of Solidarity in Children and International Cooperation" (re-published in the Spring 2011 issue of Schools: Studies in Education)
- Jean Piaget: A Most Outrageous Deception by Webster R. Callaway
- Pure Psychology: the Qualitative Development of Intelligence.
- 1896 births
- 1980 deaths
- 20th-century psychologists
- 20th-century Swiss philosophers
- 20th-century Swiss writers
- Burials at Cimetière des Rois
- Child psychologists
- Developmental psychologists
- Epistemologists
- Foreign associates of the National Academy of Sciences
- Mathematical cognition researchers
- Moral psychologists
- People associated with the University of Zurich
- People from Neuchâtel
- Structuralists
- Swiss educational theorists
- Swiss-French people
- Swiss philosophers
- Swiss Protestants
- Swiss psychologists
- Academic staff of the University of Geneva
- Academic staff of the University of Lausanne
- University of Neuchâtel alumni
- Academic staff of the University of Paris
- APA Distinguished Scientific Award for an Early Career Contribution to Psychology recipients