Jump to content

Mimicry: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
OAbot (talk | contribs)
m Open access bot: hdl updated in citation with #oabot.
 
(478 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|Evolutionary strategy}}
{{Redirect|Mimic}}
{{Redirect|Mimic}}
{{good article}}
{{good article}}
{{use British English|date=November 2013}}
{{use British English|date=November 2013}}
{{use dmy dates|date=February 2023}}
[[File:Batesplate ArM.jpg|right|thumb|300px|[[Photographic plate|Plate]] from [[Henry Walter Bates]] (1862) illustrating [[Batesian mimicry]] between ''[[Dismorphia]]'' species (top row, third row) and various ''[[Ithomiini]]'' ([[Nymphalidae]], second row, bottom row)|alt=Photo of page from book showing pairs of butterflies of different species whose appearance closely resembles each other]]


[[File:Wasp mimicry.jpg|thumb|upright=1.2|Many insects including [[hoverfly|hoverflies]] (C, D, E) and the [[wasp beetle]] (F) are [[Batesian mimicry|Batesian mimics]] of stinging [[wasp]]s (A, B), which are [[Müllerian mimicry|Müllerian mimics]] of each other.]]
In [[evolutionary biology]], '''mimicry''' is a similarity of one species to another that protects one or both.<ref>{{cite book | last=King | first=R. C. | last2=Stansfield | first2=W. D. | last3=Mulligan | first3=P. K. | year=2006 | title=A dictionary of genetics | edition=7th | location=Oxford | publisher=Oxford University Press | page=278 | isbn=0-19-530762-3 }}</ref> In the case of prey species, it is a class of [[antipredator adaptation]]. This similarity can be in [[Visual appearance|appearance]], [[behaviour]], [[sound]] or [[scent]]. Mimics occur in the same areas as their models.<ref name="Wickler"/>


In [[evolutionary biology]], '''mimicry''' is an evolved resemblance between an [[organism]] and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. In the simplest case, as in [[Batesian mimicry]], a mimic resembles a model, so as to deceive a dupe, all three being of different species. A Batesian mimic, such as a [[hoverfly]], is harmless, while its model, such as a [[wasp]], is harmful, and is avoided by the dupe, such as an insect-eating bird. Birds hunt by sight, so the mimicry in that case is visual, but in other cases mimicry may make use of any of the senses. Most types of mimicry, including Batesian, are deceptive, as the mimics are not harmful, but [[Müllerian mimicry]], where different harmful species resemble each other, is [[Honest signal|honest]], as when species of wasps and of bees all have genuinely [[Aposematism|aposematic]] warning coloration. More complex types may be bipolar, involving only two species, such as when the model and the dupe are the same; this occurs for example in [[aggressive mimicry]], where a [[Predation|predator]] in [[Wolf in sheep's clothing|wolf-in-sheep's-clothing]] style resembles its prey, allowing it to hunt undetected. Mimicry is not limited to animals; in [[Pouyannian mimicry]], an orchid flower is the mimic, resembling a female bee, its model; the dupe is the male bee of the same species, which tries to copulate with the flower, enabling it to transfer pollen, so the mimicry is again bipolar. In [[automimicry]], another bipolar system, model and mimic are the same, as when blue [[Lycaenidae|lycaenid]] butterflies have 'tails' or [[Eyespot (mimicry)|eyespot]]s on their wings that mimic their own heads, misdirecting predator dupes to strike harmlessly. Many other types of mimicry exist.<!--this is not a list! please don't add 'new' material here, it will be removed. The lead is only a summary of the cited text in the article body. Many thanks.-->
Mimicry occurs when a group of organisms,{{efn|This "group" is often a species, but can also be a subgroup such as one particular sex or morph.}} the mimics, [[evolution|evolve]] to share [[perception|perceived]] characteristics with another group, the '''models'''.{{efn|In its broadest definition mimicry can include non-living models.}} The evolution is driven by the [[Selection (biology)|selective]] action of a signal-receiver or dupe.<ref name="Wickler 1965">{{cite journal | author=Wickler, W. | authorlink=Wolfgang Wickler | year=1965 | title=Mimicry and the evolution of animal communication | journal=[[Nature (journal)|Nature]]| volume=208 | issue=5010| pages=519–21 | doi=10.1038/208519a0 |bibcode=1965Natur.208..519W }}</ref> Birds, for example, use sight to identify palatable insects (the mimics), whilst avoiding the noxious models.


== Etymology ==
The model is usually another species, except in cases of [[#Automimicry|automimicry]]. The [[Deception in animals|deceived]] signal-receiver is typically another organism, such as the common [[predator]] of two species.{{efn|In a few cases prey mimic their predator, enabling them to hide in plain sight, as when the metalmark moth ''[[Brenthia hexaselena]]'' mimics one of its predators, the jumping spider ''[[Phiale formosa]]''.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Rota|first1=Jadranka|last2=Wagner|first2=David L.|title=Predator Mimicry: Metalmark Moths Mimic Their Jumping Spider Predators|journal=PLOS ONE|date=2006|volume=e45|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0000045|url=http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0000045|pmid=17183674|pmc=1762363|pages=e45}}</ref>}} As an [[biological interaction|interaction]], mimicry is in most cases advantageous to the mimic and harmful to the receiver, but may increase, reduce or have no effect on the [[fitness (biology)|fitness]] of the model depending on the situation. The model may be hard to identify: for example, [[eyespot (mimicry)|eye spots]] may not resemble any specific organism's eyes, and camouflage often cannot be attributed to a particular model.


Use of the word [[wikt:mimicry|mimicry]] dates to 1637. It derives from the [[Greek language|Greek]] term ''mimetikos'', "imitative", in turn from ''mimetos'', the verbal adjective of ''mimeisthai'', "to imitate".<ref name="EtymDict"/> "Mimicry" was first used in zoology by the English [[entomologist]]s [[William Kirby (entomologist)|William Kirby]] and [[William Spence (entomologist)|William Spence]] in 1823.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref name="Kirby Spence 1823">{{cite book |last1=Kirby |first1=William |author1-link=William Kirby (entomologist) |last2=Spence |first2=William |author2-link=William Spence (entomologist) |year=1823 |title=An Introduction to Entomology |volume=2 |publisher=[[Longman, Hurst, Rees, Orme & Brown]] |edition=3rd |page=405 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ItZjam0xBBYC}}</ref> Originally used to describe people, "mimetic" was used in zoology from 1851.<ref name="EtymDict">{{cite web |title=Online Etymology Dictionary |url=http://www.etymonline.com/index.php?search=mimicry&searchmode=none |last=Harper |first=Douglas |access-date=23 February 2022}}</ref>
[[File:Ctenomorpha chronus02.jpg|thumb|Mimesis in ''[[Ctenomorphodes chronus]]'', [[camouflage]]d as a eucalyptus twig]]


== History ==
Mimicry is related to [[camouflage]], in which a species resembles its surroundings or is otherwise difficult to detect. In particular, '''mimesis''', in which the mimic takes on the properties of a specific object or organism, but one to which the dupe is indifferent, is an area of overlap between camouflage and mimicry.<ref name=Pasteur>{{cite journal | author=Pasteur, G. | date=1982 | title=A classificatory review of mimicry systems | journal=[[Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics]] | volume=13| pages=169–199 | doi=10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125 }}</ref> For example, [[animal]]s such as [[flower mantis]]es, [[planthoppers]] and [[geometer moth]] caterpillars that resemble twigs, bark, leaves or flowers are mimetic.<ref name="Wickler"/><sup>p51</sup> The difficulty is sometimes avoided by choosing a different term; "[[crypsis]]" (in the broad sense) is sometimes used to encompass all forms of avoiding detection, such as mimicry, camouflage and hiding.<ref name="Ruxton"/><ref name="Ruxton">{{cite book | author=Ruxton, G. D.; T. N. Sherratt, and M. P. Speed | date=2004 | title=Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals, and mimicry | publisher=Oxford University Press}}</ref>


=== Ancient ===
Though [[visual]] mimicry through [[animal coloration]] is most obvious to [[human]]s, other senses such as [[olfaction]] (smell) or [[hearing (sense)|hearing]] may be involved, and more than one type of [[signalling theory|signal]] may be employed.<ref name="Wickler">{{cite book | author=Wickler, W. | year=1968 | title=Mimicry in plants and animals | publisher=McGraw-Hill | location=New York }}</ref> Mimicry may involve [[morphology (biology)|morphology]], [[behaviour]], and other properties. In any case, the signal always functions to deceive the receiver by preventing it from correctly identifying the mimic. In evolutionary terms, this phenomenon is a form of [[co-evolution]] usually involving an [[evolutionary arms race]].<ref name="Ruxton"/><sup>p161</sup> Mimicry should not be confused with [[convergent evolution]], which occurs when species come to resemble each other by [[Adaptation|adapting]] to similar lifestyles.


[[Aristotle]] wrote in his ''[[History of Animals]]'' that [[partridge]]s use a deceptive [[distraction display]] to lure predators away from their flightless young:<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref name="Aristotle on distraction display">[[Aristotle]], ''[[History of Animals]]'', [https://archive.org/details/historiaanimaliu00aris_0/page/n417/mode/2up book 9, chapter 8.]</ref>
Mimics may have different models for different [[Biological life cycle|life cycle]] stages, or they may be [[Polymorphism (biology)|polymorphic]], with different individuals imitating different models. Models themselves may have more than one mimic, though [[frequency dependent selection]] favors mimicry where models outnumber mimics. Models tend to be relatively closely [[Common descent|related]] organisms,<ref name=Campbell /> but mimicry of vastly different species is also known. Most known mimics are [[insect]]s,<ref name="Wickler"/> though many other animal mimics are known. [[Plant]]s and [[fungi]] may also be mimics, though less research has been carried out in this area.<ref>{{cite journal | author=Boyden, T. C. | year=1980 | title=Floral mimicry by ''Epidendrum ibaguense'' (Orchidaceae) in Panama | journal=Evolution | volume=34 | issue=1 | pages=135–136 | doi=10.2307/2408322 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Roy | first1=B. A. | year=1994 | title=The effects of pathogen-induced pseudoflowers and buttercups on each other's insect visitation | journal=Ecology | volume=75 | issue=2 | pages=352–358 | doi=10.2307/1939539 }}</ref><ref name=EB>Wickler, Wolfgang, 1998. "Mimicry". ''[[Encyclopædia Britannica]]'', 15th edition. Macropædia 24, 144–151. http://www.britannica.com/eb/article-11910</ref>


{{blockquote|When a man comes by chance upon a young brood [of partridges], and tries to catch them, the hen-bird rolls in front of the hunter, pretending to be lame: the man every moment thinks he is on the point of catching her, and so she draws him on and on, until every one of her brood has had time to escape; hereupon she returns to the nest and calls the young back.|Aristotle, translated by [[D'Arcy Wentworth Thompson]]<ref name="Aristotle on distraction display"/>}}
==Etymology==
Use of the word [[wikt:mimicry|mimicry]] dates to 1637. It [[etymology|derives]] from the [[Greek language|Greek]] term ''mimetikos'', "imitative", in turn from ''mimetos'', the verbal adjective of ''mimeisthai'', "to imitate". Originally used to describe people, "mimetic" was used in zoology from 1851, "mimicry" from 1861.<ref>{{cite web | title=Online Etymology Dictionary | url=http://www.etymonline.com/index.php?search=mimicry&searchmode=none| author=Douglas Harper | date=2007-10-06}}</ref>


The behaviour is recognised as a form of mimicry by biologists.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/>
==Classification==


=== 19th century ===
Many types of mimicry have been described. An overview of each follows, highlighting the similarities and differences between the various forms. Classification is often based on [[function (biology)|function]] with respect to the mimic (e.g., avoiding harm). Some cases may belong to more than one class, e.g., automimicry and aggressive mimicry are not mutually exclusive, as one describes the species relationship between model and mimic, while the other describes the function for the mimic (obtaining food).


[[File:Fritz Müller 1891 (cropped).jpg|thumb|upright|In 1879, [[Fritz Müller]] created the [[Müllerian mimicry#Model|first mathematical model of mimicry]] to explain why distasteful species should evolve similar appearances.<ref name="Mallet on Müller"/>]]
===Defensive===
[[File:Macroxiphus sp cricket.jpg|thumb|''[[Macroxiphus]]'' sp [[katydid]] mimics an [[ant]]]]Defensive or protective mimicry takes place when organisms are able to avoid harmful encounters by deceiving enemies into treating them as something else.


In 1823, Kirby and Spence, in their book ''An Introduction to Entomology'', used the term "mimicry" informally to depict the way that the structure and coloration of some insects resembled objects in their environments:<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref name="Kirby Spence 1823"/>
The first three such cases discussed here entail mimicry of organisms protected by [[warning coloration]]<!--both UK and US spelling-->:
* [[Batesian mimicry]], where a harmless mimic poses as harmful;
* [[Müllerian mimicry]], where two or more harmful species mutually advertise themselves as harmful;
* [[Mertensian mimicry]], where a deadly mimic resembles a less harmful but lesson-teaching model.
The fourth case, Vavilovian mimicry, where weeds resemble crops, is important for several reasons, and in this case humans are the agent of selection.


{{blockquote|A jumping bug, very similar to the one figured by [[Johann Rudolph Schellenberg|Schellenberg]], also much resembles the [[lichen]]s of the oak on which I took it. The spectre tribe (''[[Phasma]]'') go still further in this mimicry, representing a small branch with its spray.<ref name="Kirby Spence 1823"/>}}
====Batesian====
{{Main|Batesian mimicry}}


The English naturalist [[Henry Walter Bates]] worked for several years on butterflies in the [[Amazon rainforest]]. Returning home, he described multiple forms of mimicry in an 1862 paper at the [[Linnean Society of London|Linnean Society]] in London,<ref name="Bates 1862">{{cite journal |last=Bates |first=Henry W. |author-link=Henry Walter Bates |year=1862 |title=Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera: Heliconidae |journal=Transactions of the Linnean Society |volume=23 |issue=3 |pages=495–566 |doi=10.1111/j.1096-3642.1860.tb00146.x |url=https://zenodo.org/record/1447544}}</ref> and then in his 1863 book ''[[The Naturalist on the River Amazons]]''.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref name="Bates 1863">{{cite book |last=Bates |first=Henry W. |author-link=Henry Walter Bates |year=1863 |title=The naturalist on the river Amazons |title-link=The Naturalist on the River Amazons |publisher=Murray}}</ref> The term "Batesian mimicry" has since been used in his honour, its usage becoming restricted to the situation in which a harmless mimic gains protection from its predators by resembling a distasteful model.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/> Among the observations in Bates's 1862 paper is the statement:
[[File:FinnBirdMimic.jpg|thumb|upright|left|[[Hierococcyx varius|Common hawk-cuckoo]] resembles a predator, the [[shikra]].<ref>{{cite journal | journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B | title=Cuckoo–hawk mimicry? An experimental test | volume=275 | issue=1644 | pages=1817–1822 |year=2008 | author=Davies, NB and JA Welbergen | doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.0331 | pmid=18467298 | pmc=2587796}}</ref>]]


{{blockquote|I was never able to distinguish the [[Dismorphia|Leptalides]] from the species they imitated, although they belong to a family totally different in structure and metamorphosis from the [[Heliconiinae|Heliconidae]], without examining them closely after capture.<ref name="Bates 1862"/>}}
In Batesian mimicry the mimic shares signals similar to the model, but does not have the attribute that makes it unprofitable to predators (e.g., unpalatability). In other words, a Batesian mimic is a [[The Wolf in Sheep's Clothing|sheep in wolf's clothing]]. It is named after [[Henry Walter Bates]], an English naturalist whose work on [[butterflies]] in the [[Amazon rainforest]] (described in ''[[The Naturalist on the River Amazons]]'') was pioneering in this field of study.<ref>{{cite book | author=Bates H. W. | year=1863 | title=The naturalist on the river Amazons | titlelink=The Naturalist on the River Amazons | publisher=Murray}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Bates | first1=H. W. | year=1961 | title=Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera: Heliconidae | journal=Transactions of the Linnean Society | volume=23 | issue=3| pages=495–566 | doi=10.1111/j.1096-3642.1860.tb00146.x}}</ref> Mimics are less likely to be found out (for example by predators) when in low proportion to their model. This phenomenon is called [[negative frequency dependent selection]], and it applies in most forms of mimicry. Batesian mimicry can only be maintained if the harm caused to the predator by eating a model outweighs the benefit of eating a mimic. The nature of learning is weighted in favor of the mimics, for a predator that has a bad first experience with a model tends to avoid anything that looks like it for a long time, and does not re-sample soon to see whether the initial experience was a false negative. However, if mimics become more abundant than models, then the probability of a young predator having a first experience with a mimic increases. Such systems are therefore most likely to be stable where both the model and the mimic occur, and where the model is more abundant than the mimic.<ref>{{cite book | author=Sterns & Hoekstra | title=Evolution: An Introduction | edition=5th | page=464}}</ref> This is not the case in Müllerian mimicry, which is described next.


The German naturalist [[Fritz Müller]] also spent many years studying butterflies in the Amazon rainforest. He first published a journal article on mimicry in German in 1878,<ref name="Müller 1878">{{cite journal |last=Müller |first=Fritz |author-link=Fritz Müller |year=1878 |title=Ueber die Vortheile der Mimicry bei Schmetterlingen |language=de |trans-title=On the Advantages of Mimicry in Butterflies |journal=Zoologischer Anzeiger |volume=1 |pages=54–55}}</ref> followed in 1879 by a paper to the [[Entomological Society of London]] (translated and presented by Ralph Meldola).<ref name="Müller 1879">{{cite journal |last=Müller |first=Fritz |author-link=Fritz Müller |translator=R. Meldola |year=1879 |title=''Ituna'' and ''Thyridia''; a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies |journal=Proclamations of the Entomological Society of London |volume=1879 |pages=20–29 |url=https://www.ucl.ac.uk/taxome/lit/muller_1879.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20240302102124/https://www.ucl.ac.uk/taxome/lit/muller_1879.pdf |archive-date=2 March 2024 |url-status=live}}</ref> He described a situation where different species were each unpalatable to predators, and shared similar, genuine, warning signals. Bates found it hard to explain why this should be so, asking why they should need to mimic each other if both were harmful and could warn off predators on their own. Müller put forward [[Müllerian mimicry#Model|the first mathematical model of mimicry]] for this phenomenon: if a common predator confuses the two species, individuals in both those species are more likely to survive, as fewer individuals of either species are killed by the predator. The term [[Müllerian mimicry]], named in his honour, has since been used for this mutualistic form of mimicry.<ref name="Mallet on Müller">{{cite web |last=Mallet |first=James |author-link=James Mallet |title=Fritz Müller in 1891 |url=http://www.ucl.ac.uk/taxome/jim/Mim/Muller.html |access-date=18 November 2017 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20240630163011/http://www.ucl.ac.uk/taxome/jim/Mim/Muller.html |archive-date=30 June 2024 |url-status=live}}</ref><ref name="Sherratt 2008">{{cite journal |last=Sherratt |first=Thomas |author-link=Thomas N. Sherratt |title=The Evolution of Müllerian Mimicry |journal=Die Naturwissenschaften |date=2008 |pages=681–695 |doi=10.1007/s00114-008-0403-y |pmid=18542902 |volume=95 |issue=8 |bibcode=2008NW.....95..681S |pmc=2443389 }}</ref>
[[File:Wasp mimicry.jpg|thumb|upright=<!--size for multiple image-->1.2|Many insects including [[hoverfly|hoverflies]] and the [[wasp beetle]] are [[Batesian mimicry|Batesian mimics]] of stinging [[wasp]]s.]]
There are many Batesian mimics in the order [[Lepidoptera]]. ''[[Consul fabius]]'' and ''[[Eresia eunice]]'' imitate unpalatable ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterflies such as ''[[Heliconius ismenius|H. ismenius]]''.<ref name="pinheiro" /> Several palatable moths produce ultrasonic click calls to mimic unpalatable tiger moths.<ref name=tigermoth>{{cite journal | last1=Barber | first1=J. R. | last2=Conner | first2=W. E. | year=2007 | title=Acoustic mimicry in a predator–prey interaction | url=http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/figsonly/104/22/9331 | journal=Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. | volume=104 | issue=22| pages=9331–9334 | doi=10.1073/pnas.0703627104 | pmid=17517637 | pmc=1890494 }}</ref> [[Octopuses]] of the genus ''Thaumoctopus'' (the [[mimic octopus]]) are able to intentionally alter their body shape and coloration to resemble dangerous [[sea snakes]] or [[Pterois|lionfish]].<ref>[http://marinebio.org/species.asp?id=260 Mimic Octopus, Thaumoctopus mimicus at MarineBio.org]</ref> In the Amazon, the [[helmeted woodpecker]] (''[[Helmeted woodpecker|Dryocopus galeatus]]''), a rare species which lives in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil, Paraguay, and Argentina, has a similar red crest, black back, and barred underside to two larger woodpeckers: [[Lineated woodpecker|''Dryocopus lineatus'']] and ''[[Robust woodpecker|Campephilus robustus]]''. This mimicry reduces attacks on ''Dryocopus galeatus'' from other animals. Scientists had falsely believed that ''D. galeatus'' was a close cousin of the other two species, because of the visual similarity, and because the three species live in the same habitat and eat similar food.<ref>{{Cite web |title=Deceptive Woodpecker Uses Mimicry to Avoid Competition |url=http://www.amnh.org/explore/news-blogs/research-posts/deceptive-woodpecker-uses-mimicry-to-avoid-competition |website=AMNH |accessdate=12 August 2015}}</ref>


Müller wrote that
====Müllerian====


{{blockquote|The resemblance of the genera named [''[[Lycorea ilione|Ituna]]'' and ''[[Thyridia]]''] is the more worthy of notice since it occurs between insects both belonging to the group of butterflies which are protected by distastefulness. The explanation which applies in ordinary cases of [Batesian] mimicry—and no other has, so far as I know, been offered—cannot obtain for this imitation among protected species.<ref name="Müller 1879"/>}}
{{Main|Müllerian kona}}


<gallery class=center mode=nolines widths=220 heights=220>
[[File:Heliconius mimicry.png|upright=<!--size for multiple image-->1.2|thumb|left|The ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterflies from the tropics of the Western Hemisphere are the classical model for [[Müllerian mimicry]].<ref>{{cite journal | author=Meyer, A. | year=2006 | title=Repeating Patterns of Mimicry | url=| journal=PLoS Biol | volume=4 | issue=10| page=e341 | doi=10.1371/journal.pbio.0040341 | pmid=17048984 | pmc=1617347}}</ref>]]
File:Batesplate ArM.jpg|Plate from [[Henry Walter Bates]]'s 1862 paper illustrating [[Batesian mimicry]] between harmless ''[[Dismorphia]]'' species (top and third row) and distasteful [[Ithomiini]] ([[Nymphalidae]], second and bottom row).<ref name="Bates 1862"/>
File:Müllerian mimicry.jpg|Mutual [[Müllerian mimicry]] in distasteful ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterflies. Plate LXII from Müller's collected writings, 1881
File:Darwinism 1889 page 259 image of mimicry.jpg|Page from [[Alfred Russel Wallace]]'s 1889 book ''[[Darwinism (book)|Darwinism]]'', showing a beetle (below) mimicking a wasp
</gallery>


== Overview ==
Müllerian mimicry, named for the German naturalist [[Fritz Müller]], describes a situation where two or more species have similar warning or aposematic signals and both share genuine [[anti-predation]] attributes (e.g., being unpalatable). At first, Bates could not explain why this should be so—if both were harmful why did one need to mimic another? Müller put forward the first explanation for this phenomenon: if a common predator confuses two species, individuals in both those speces are more likely to survive.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Müller | first1=Fritz | year=1878 | title=Ueber die Vortheile der Mimicry bei Schmetterlingen | journal=Zoologischer Anzeiger | volume=1 | pages=54–55 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Müller | first1=F. | year=1879 | title=''Ituna'' and ''Thyridia''; a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies (translated by Meldola, R.) | journal=Proclamations of the Entomological Society of London | volume=1879 | pages=20–29}}</ref> This type of mimicry is unique in several respects. Firstly, both the mimic and the model benefit from the interaction, which could thus be classified as [[Mutualism (biology)|mutualism]] in this respect. The signal receiver is also advantaged by this system, despite being deceived about species identity, as it avoids potentially harmful encounters. The usually clear distinction between mimic and model is also blurred. Where one species is scarce and another abundant, the rare species can be said to be the mimic. When both are present in similar numbers, however, it is more realistic to speak of each as a ''co-mimic'' than of distinct 'mimic' and 'model' species, as their warning signals tend to converge.<ref>{{cite web| author=Flannery, T. F. |date=2007 |title=Community ecology: Mimicry complexes |website=Encyclopædia Britannica Online |url=http://www.britannica.com/eb/article-9117280/community-ecology}}</ref> Also, the two species may exist on a continuum from harmless to highly noxious, so Batesian mimicry grades smoothly into Müllerian convergence.<ref>{{cite journal | author=Huheey, James E. | year=1976 | title=Studies in warning coloration and mimicry VII — Evolutionary consequences of a Batesian–Müllerian spectrum: A model for Müllerian mimicry | journal=Evolution | volume=30 | issue=1| pages=86–93 | doi=10.2307/2407675 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | author=Benson, W. W. | year=1977 | title=On the Supposed Spectrum Between Batesian and Mullerian Mimicry | journal=Evolution | volume=31 | issue=2 | pages=454–455 | doi=10.2307/2407770 }}</ref>


=== Evolved resemblance ===
The [[Monarch (butterfly)|monarch butterfly]] (''Danaus plexippus'') is a member of a Müllerian complex with the [[viceroy butterfly]] (''Limenitis archippus''), sharing coloration patterns and display behavior. The viceroy has [[subspecies]] with somewhat different coloration, each closely matching the local ''[[Danaus (genus)|Danaus]]'' species. For example, in [[Florida]], the pairing is of the viceroy and the [[Queen (butterfly)|queen butterfly]], whereas in [[Mexico]] the viceroy resembles the [[Soldier (butterfly)|soldier butterfly]]. The viceroy is thus involved in three different Müllerian pairs.<ref name=Ritland1>{{cite journal | author=Ritland, D. B. | year=1995 | title=Comparative unpalatability of mimetic viceroy butterflies (''Limenitis archippus'') from four south-eastern United States populations | journal=Oecologia | volume=103 | issue=3| pages=327–336 | doi=10.1007/BF00328621 }}</ref> This example was long believed to be Batesian, with the viceroy mimicking the monarch, but the viceroy is actually the ''more'' unpalatable species.<ref>{{cite journal | author=Ritland, D. |author2=L. P. Brower | year=1991 | title=The viceroy butterfly is not a Batesian mimic | journal=Nature |volume=350 | issue= 6318 | pages=497–498 | doi=10.1038/350497a0 | url=http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v350/n6318/abs/350497a0.html | quote=Viceroys are as unpalatable as monarchs, and significantly more unpalatable than queens from representative Florida populations. | bibcode=1991Natur.350..497R }}</ref> The genus ''[[Morpho]]'' is palatable, but some species (such as ''[[Morpho amathonte|M. amathonte]]'') are strong fliers; birds – even species that specialize in catching butterflies on the wing – find it hard to catch them.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Young | first1=A. M. | year=1971 | title=Wing colouration and reflectance in ''Morpho'' butterflies as related to reproductive behaviour and escape from avian predators | journal=Oecologia | volume=7 | issue=3 | pages=209–222 | doi=10.1007/BF00345212}}</ref> The conspicuous blue coloration shared by most ''Morpho'' species may be Müllerian,<ref name="pinheiro" /> or may be "pursuit aposematism".<ref>M. Edmunds. 1974. ''Defence in Animals: a survey of anti-predator defences''. Harlow, Essex and New York, Longman. ISBN 0-582-44132-3. On p 255–256 there is a discussion of "pursuit aposematism": "Young suggested that the brilliant blue colours and bobbing flight of ''Morpho'' butterflies may induce pursuit... ''Morpho amathonte'' is a very fast flier... It is possible that birds that have chased several unsuccessfully may learn not to pursue butterflies of that [type]... In one area, Young found that 80% of less brilliant species of ''Morpho'' had beak marks on their wings... but none out of 31 ''M. amathonte''... "If brilliant colour was a factor in courtship, then the conflicting selection pressures of sexual selection and predator selection might lead to different results in quite closely related species".</ref>
The "orange complex" of distasteful butterfly species includes the [[heliconiine]]s ''[[Agraulis vanillae]]'', ''[[Dryadula phaetusa]]'', and ''[[Dryas iulia]]''.<ref name="pinheiro">{{cite journal | last1=Pinheiro | first1=Carlos E. G. | year=1996 | title=Palatability and escaping ability in Neotropical butterflies: tests with wild kingbirds (''Tyrannus melancholicus'', Tyrannidae) | url=http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/ap/bj/1996/00000059/00000004/art00069 | journal=Biological Journal of the Linnean Society | volume=59 | issue=4| pages=351–365 | doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1996.tb01471.x }}</ref>
At least seven species of [[millipede]]s in the genera ''[[Apheloria]]'' and ''[[Brachoria]]'' ([[Xystodesmidae]]) form a Müllerian mimicry ring in the eastern United States, in which unrelated polymorphic species converge on similar color patterns where their range overlaps.<ref name=Marek&Bond2009>{{cite journal |last=Marek |first=P. E. |author2=Bond, J. E. |title=A Mullerian mimicry ring in Appalachian millipedes |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |year=2009 |volume=106 |issue=24 |pages=9755–9760 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0810408106 |url=http://www.pnas.org/content/106/24/9755 |pmc=2700981 |pmid=19487663}}</ref>


[[File:Batesian vs Müllerian mimicry.svg|thumb|center|upright=2|Batesian vs Müllerian mimicry: the former is [[Deception in animals|deceptive]], the latter [[Honest signal|honest]].]]
====<span id="Emsleyan">Emsleyan</span>/<span id="Mertensian">Mertensian</span>====


Mimicry is an [[evolved]] resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. Often, mimicry functions [[anti-predator adaptation|to protect from predators]].<ref>{{cite book |last1=King |first1=R. C. |last2=Stansfield |first2=W. D. |last3=Mulligan |first3=P. K. |year=2006 |title=A dictionary of genetics |url=https://archive.org/details/dictionarygeneti02king |url-access=limited |edition=7th |publisher=[[Oxford University Press]] |page=[https://archive.org/details/dictionarygeneti02king/page/n290 278] |isbn=978-0-19-530762-7}}</ref> Mimicry systems have three basic roles: a mimic, a model, and a dupe. When these correspond to three separate species, the system is called disjunct; when the roles are taken by just two species, the system is called bipolar.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref name="KikuchiPfennig2013">{{cite journal |last1=Kikuchi |first1=D. W. |last2=Pfennig |first2=D. W. |date=2013 |title=Imperfect Mimicry and the Limits of Natural Selection |journal=[[The Quarterly Review of Biology]] |volume=88 |issue=4 |pages=297–315 |doi=10.1086/673758|pmid=24552099 |s2cid=11436992 |url=https://cdr.lib.unc.edu/downloads/ms35tj93n }}</ref> Mimicry evolves if a dupe (such as a predator) perceives a mimic (such as a palatable prey) as a model (the organism it resembles), and [[Deception in animals|is deceived to change its behaviour]] to the mimic's selective advantage.<ref name="Dalziell 2016">{{Cite journal |last1=Dalziell |first1=Anastasia H. |last2=Welbergen |first2=Justin A. |date=27 April 2016 |title=Mimicry for all modalities |journal=[[Ecology Letters]] |volume=19 |issue=6 |pages=609–619 |doi=10.1111/ele.12602 |pmid=27117779 |bibcode=2016EcolL..19..609D}}</ref> The resemblances can be via any sensory modality, including any combination of visual, acoustic, chemical, tactile, or electric.<ref name="Dalziell 2016"/><ref name="Wickler">{{cite book |title=Mimicry in plants and animals |url=https://archive.org/details/mimicryinplantsa00wick |url-access=registration |last=Wickler |first=Wolfgang |author-link=Wolfgang Wickler |publisher=McGraw-Hill |year=1968}}</ref> Mimicry may be to the advantage of both organisms that share a resemblance, in which case it is [[Mutualism (biology)|mutualistic]]; or it can be to the detriment of one, making it [[Parasitism|parasitic]] or [[competition|competitive]]. The evolutionary convergence between groups is driven by the [[Natural selection|selective]] action of a dupe.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref name="Wickler 1965">{{cite journal |last=Wickler |first=Wolfgang |author-link=Wolfgang Wickler |year=1965 |title=Mimicry and the Evolution of Animal Communication |journal=[[Nature (journal)|Nature]]|volume=208 |issue=5010 |pages=519–21 |doi=10.1038/208519a0 |bibcode=1965Natur.208..519W |s2cid=37649827}}</ref> Birds, for example, use sight to identify palatable insects,<ref name="Radford and Frazier (2017)">{{cite journal |last1=Radford |first1=Benjamin |author1-link=Benjamin Radford |last2=Frazier |first2=Kendrick |author2-link=Kendrick Frazier |title=Cheats and Deceits: How Animals and Plants Exploit and Mislead |journal=Skeptical Inquirer |date=January 2017 |volume=41 |issue=1 |page=60}}</ref> whilst avoiding noxious ones. Over time, palatable insects may evolve to resemble noxious ones, making them mimics and the noxious ones models. Models do not have to be more abundant than mimics.<ref name="Ruxton"/> In the case of mutualism, each model is also a mimic; all such species can be called "co-mimics".<ref name="Ruxton"/> Many harmless species such as hoverflies are [[Batesian mimicry|Batesian mimics]] of strongly defended species such as wasps, while many such well-defended species form [[Müllerian mimicry]] rings of co-mimics.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/> In the evolution of wasp-like appearance, it has been argued that insects evolve to masquerade wasps since predatory wasps do not attack each other, and that this mimetic resemblance has had the useful side-effect of deterring vertebrate predators.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Boppré |first1=Michael |last2=Vane-Wright |first2=Richard I. |last3=Wickler |first3=Wolfgang |author-link=Wolfgang Wickler |date=2017-01-01 |title=A hypothesis to explain accuracy of wasp resemblances |journal=Ecology and Evolution |volume=7 |issue=1 |pages=73–81 |doi=10.1002/ece3.2586 |pmc=5214283 |pmid=28070276 |bibcode=2017EcoEv...7...73B}}</ref>
[[File:Micrurus tener.jpg|thumb|The deadly Texas coral snake, ''[[Micrurus tener]]'' (the mimic)]]
[[File:Lampropeltis triangulum annulata.jpg|thumb|The harmless Mexican milk snake, ''[[Lampropeltis triangulum annulata]]'' (the model)]]


<!--this paragraph is a bit of a ragbag on different themes, needs breaking up/rewriting-->
Emsleyan<ref name=Pasteur /> or Mertensian mimicry describes unusual cases where deadly prey mimic a less dangerous species. It was first proposed by Emsley<ref name=Emsley>{{cite journal | last1=Emsley | first1=M. G. | year=1966 | title=The mimetic significance of ''Erythrolamprus aesculapii ocellatus'' Peters from Tobago | url=| journal=Evolution | volume=20 | issue=4| pages=663–64 | doi=10.2307/2406599 }}</ref> as a possible answer for the theoretical difficulties a predator species faces when associating an aposematic phenotype of potentially dangerous animals, such as the [[coral snake]], with unprofitability when the predator has an increased risk of death, negating any learned behaviour. The theory was developed by the German biologist [[Wolfgang Wickler]] in a chapter of ''Mimicry in Plants and Animals'',<ref name="Wickler" /> who named it after the [[Germany|German]] [[herpetologist]] [[Robert Mertens]].<ref name=Mertens>{{cite journal | last1=Mertens | first1=R. | year=1956 | title=Das Problem der Mimikry bei Korallenschlangen |language=de| url=| journal=Zool. Jahrb. Syst | volume=84 | issue=| pages=541–76 }}</ref> Sheppard points out that Hecht and Marien put forward a similar hypothesis ten years earlier.<ref name=Hecht>{{cite journal | last1=Hecht | first1=M. K. | last2=Marien | first2=D. | year=1956 | title=The coral snake mimic problem: a reinterpretation | journal=Journal of Morphology | volume=98 | issue=2 | pages=335–365 | doi=10.1002/jmor.1050980207 }}</ref><ref name=Sheppard>{{cite journal | last1=Sheppard | first1=P. M. | year=1969 | title=Review of ''Mimicry in plants and animals'' by Wolfgang Wickler | journal=Journal of Animal Ecology | volume=38 | page=243 | doi=10.2307/2762 | last2=Wickler | first2=Wolfgang }}</ref> This scenario is a little more difficult to understand, since in other types of mimicry it is usually the most harmful species that is the model. But if a predator dies, it cannot [[learning|learn]] to recognize a warning signal, e.g., bright colors in a certain pattern. In other words, there is no advantage in being aposematic for an organism that is likely to kill any predator it succeeds in poisoning; such an [[animal]] is better off being camouflaged, to avoid attacks altogether. If, however, there were some other species that were harmful but not deadly as well as aposematic, the predator could learn to recognize its particular warning colors and avoid such animals. A deadly species could then profit by mimicking the less dangerous aposematic organism if this reduces the number of attacks.<ref name=Hecht/><ref name=Sheppard/> The exception here, ignoring any chance of animals [[observational learning|learning by watching]] a conspecific die (see Jouventin ''et al.'' for a discussion of observational learning and mimicry),<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Jouventin | first1=P. | last2=Pasteur | first2=G. | last3=Cambefort | first3=J. P. | year=1977 | title=Observational Learning of Baboons and Avoidance of Mimics: Exploratory Tests | journal=Evolution | volume=31 | issue=1| pages=214–218 | doi=10.2307/2407558 }}</ref> is the possibility of not having to learn that it is harmful in the first place: [[instinct]]ive genetic programming to be wary of certain signals. In this case, other organisms could benefit from this programming, and Batesian or Müllerian mimics of it could potentially evolve. In fact, it has been shown that some species do have an innate recognition of certain aposematic warnings. Hand-reared [[turquoise-browed motmot]]s (''Eumomota superciliosa''), avian predators, instinctively avoid snakes with red and yellow rings.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Smith | first1=S. M. | year=1975 | title=Innate Recognition of Coral Snake Pattern by a Possible Avian Predator | journal=[[Science (journal)|Science]] | volume=187 | issue=4178| pages=759–760 | doi=10.1126/science.187.4178.759 |bibcode=1975Sci...187..759S | pmid=17795249}}</ref> Other colors with the same pattern, and even red and yellow ''stripes'' with the same width as rings, were tolerated. However, models with red and yellow rings were feared, with the birds flying away and giving [[alarm call]]s in some cases. This provides an alternative explanation to Mertensian mimicry. See Greene and McDiarmid for a review of the subject.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Greene | first1=H. W. | last2=McDiarmid | first2=R. W. | year=1981 | title=Coral snake mimicry: Does it occur? | url=| journal=Science | volume=213 | issue=4513| pages=1207–12 | doi=10.1126/science.213.4513.1207 | pmid=17744739 |bibcode=1981Sci...213.1207G }}</ref>
Mimicry can result in an [[evolutionary arms race]] if mimicry negatively affects the model, in which case the model can evolve a different appearance from the mimic.<ref name="Ruxton"/><sup>p161</sup> Mimics may have different models for different [[Biological life cycle|life cycle]] stages, or they may be [[Polymorphism (biology)|polymorphic]], with different individuals imitating different models, as occurs in [[Heliconius]] butterflies. Models tend to be relatively closely related to their mimics,<ref name=Campbell/> but mimicry can be of vastly different species, for example when spiders mimic ants. Most known mimics are insects,<ref name="Wickler"/> though many other examples including [[mimicry in vertebrates|vertebrates]], plants, and fungi exist.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Boyden |first=T. C. |year=1980 |title=Floral mimicry by ''Epidendrum ibaguense'' (Orchidaceae) in Panama |journal=Evolution |volume=34 |issue=1 |pages=135–136 |doi=10.2307/2408322 |pmid=28563205 |jstor=2408322}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Roy |first1=B. A. |year=1994 |title=The effects of pathogen-induced pseudoflowers and buttercups on each other's insect visitation |journal=Ecology |volume=75 |issue=2 |pages=352–358 |doi=10.2307/1939539 |jstor=1939539 |bibcode=1994Ecol...75..352R}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=gztuDQAAQBAJ&q=floral+mimicry+2016&pg=PP1 |title=Floral Mimicry |last1=Johnson |first1=Steven D. |last2=Schiestl |first2=Florian P. |date=2016 |publisher=[[Oxford University Press]] |isbn=978-0-19-104723-7}}</ref>


=== Evolutionary explanations ===
Some harmless [[milk snake]] (''Lampropeltis triangulum'') subspecies, the moderately toxic [[false coral snakes]] (genus ''Erythrolamprus''), and the deadly [[coral snakes]] (genus ''[[Micrurus]]'') all have a red background color with black and white / yellow rings. In this system, both the milk snakes and the deadly coral snakes are mimics, whereas the false coral snakes are the model.<ref name=Emsley/> It has also been suggested that this system could be an instance of pseudomimicry, the similar colour patterns having evolved independently in similar habitats.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Grobman |first1=Arnold B. |title=An Alternative Solution to the Coral Snake Mimic Problem (Reptilia, Serpentes, Elapidae) |journal=Journal of Herpetology |date=1978 |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=1–11 |doi=10.2307/1563495 |jstor=1563495}}</ref>


It is widely accepted that mimicry evolves as a positive adaptation. The [[Lepidopterology|lepidopterist]] and novelist [[Vladimir Nabokov]] however argued that although natural selection might stabilize a "mimic" form, it would not be necessary to create it.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Alexander |first1=Victoria N. |year=2002 |title=Nabokov, Teleology and Insect Mimicry |journal=[[Nabokov Studies]] |volume=7 |pages=177–213 |doi=10.1353/nab.2010.0004|s2cid=42675699}}</ref> The most widely accepted model used to explain the evolution of mimicry in butterflies is the two-step hypothesis. The first step involves [[mutation]] in [[modifier gene]]s that regulate a complex cluster of linked genes that cause large changes in morphology. The second step consists of selections on genes with smaller [[phenotype|phenotypic]] effects, creating an increasingly close resemblance. This model is supported by empirical evidence that suggests that a few single point mutations cause large phenotypic effects, while numerous others produce smaller effects. Some regulatory elements collaborate to form a [[supergene]] for the development of butterfly color patterns. The model is supported by computational simulations of [[population genetics]].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Holmgren |first1=N. M. A. |last2=Enquist |first2=M. |year=1999 |title=Dynamics of mimicry evolution |url=http://www.comp.leeds.ac.uk/biosystems/reading/paper/dynamics.pdf |journal=Biological Journal of the Linnean Society |volume=66 |issue=2 |pages=145–158 |doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01880.x |doi-access=free}}</ref> The Batesian mimicry in ''Papilio polytes'' is controlled by the ''[[doublesex]]'' gene.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kunte |first1=K. |last2=Zhang |first2=W. |last3=Tenger-Trolander |first3=A. |last4=Palmer |first4=D. H. |last5=Martin |first5=A. |last6=Reed |first6=R. D. |last7=Mullen |first7=S. P. |last8=Kronforst |first8=M. R. |title=doublesex is a mimicry supergene |journal=Nature |volume=507 |issue=7491 |date=2014 |pages=229–232 |doi=10.1038/nature13112 |pmid=24598547 |bibcode=2014Natur.507..229K |s2cid=4448793}}</ref>
====Wasmannian====
{{further|ant mimicry}}


Some mimicry is imperfect. Natural selection drives mimicry only far enough to deceive predators. For example, when predators avoid a mimic that imperfectly resembles a coral snake, the mimic is sufficiently protected.<ref name=Wilson>{{cite journal |last1=Wilson |first1=J. |last2=Jahner |first2=J. |last3=Williams |first3=K. |last4=Forister |first4=M. |date=2013 |title=Ecological and Evolutionary Processes Drive the Origin and Maintenance of Imperfect Mimicry |journal=PLOS ONE |volume=8 |issue=4 |pages=e61610 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0061610 |pmid=23593490 |pmc=3625143 |bibcode=2013PLoSO...861610W |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kikuchi |first1=D. |last2=Pfenning |first2=D. |date=2010 |title=Predator Cognition Permits Imperfect Coral Snake Mimicry |journal=The American Naturalist |volume=176 |issue=6 |pages=830–834 |doi=10.1086/657041 |pmid=20950143|s2cid=35411437}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Howse |first1=P. E. |last2=Allen |first2=J. A. |date=1994 |title=Satyric Mimicry: The Evolution of Apparent Imperfection |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B |volume=257 |issue=1349 |pages=111–114 |doi=10.1098/rspb.1994.0102 |bibcode=1994RSPSB.257..111H |s2cid=84458742}}</ref>
[[Erich Wasmann|Wasmannian]] mimicry refers to cases where the mimic resembles a model along with which it lives ([[inquiline]]) in a nest or colony. Most of the models here are [[eusociality|social]] insects such as ants, termites, bees and wasps.<ref>Wasmann, E. 1894. Kritisches Verzeichniss der myrmecophilin und termitophilen Arthropoden. Felix Dames, Berlin xi + 231 pp.</ref>


[[Convergent evolution]] is an alternative explanation for why coral reef fish have come to resemble each other;<ref>{{cite journal |title=Who resembles whom? Mimetic and coincidental look-alikes among tropical reef fishes |last=Robertson |first=D. Ross |date=2013 |journal=PLOS ONE |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0054939 |pages=e54939 |volume=8 |issue=1 |pmid=23372795 |pmc=3556028|bibcode=2013PLoSO...854939R |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |title=Coincidental resemblances among coral reef fishes from different oceans |last=Robertson |first=D. Ross |journal=Coral Reefs |date=2015 |page=977 |volume=34 |issue=3 |doi=10.1007/s00338-015-1309-8|bibcode=2015CorRe..34..977R |doi-access=free}}</ref> the same applies to benthic marine invertebrates such as [[sponge]]s and [[nudibranch]]s.<ref>{{cite book |title=Antipredatory defensive roles of natural products from marine invertebrates |last=Pawlik |first=J.R. |publisher=Springer |year=2012 |isbn=978-90-481-3833-3 |pages=677–710 |editor-first=E. |editor-last=Fattorusso |editor-last2=Gerwick |editor-first2=W.H. |editor-first3=O. |editor-last3=Taglialatela-Scafati |chapter=12}}</ref>
====Mimetic weeds====


{{anchor|Masquerade}}
{{Main|Vavilovian mimicry}}
=== Living and non-living models ===
<!--
! Pasteur classifies this as "aggressive / mutualistic / reproductive", and definitely not defensive -->
[[File:Secale cereale.jpg|right|thumb|[[Rye]] is a secondary crop, originally being a mimetic weed of [[wheat]].]]


In its broadest definition, mimicry can include non-living models. The specific terms '''masquerade''' and [[mimesis (biology)|mimesis]] are sometimes used when the models are inanimate, and the mimicry's purpose is [[crypsis]].<ref>{{cite journal |title=The Evolution and Ecology of Masquerade |last1=Skelhorn |first1=John |last2=Rowland |first2=Hannah M. |last3=Ruxton |first3=Graeme D. |author3-link=Graeme D. Ruxton |journal=Biological Journal of the Linnean Society|year=2010 |volume=99 |pages=1–8 |doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.2009.01347.x |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Wickler"/><ref name="Pasteur 1982">{{cite journal |last=Pasteur |first=G. |date=1982 |title=A Classificatory Review of Mimicry Systems |journal=[[Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics]] |volume=13 |pages=169–199 |doi=10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125 |jstor=2097066}}</ref> For example, animals such as [[flower mantis]]es, [[planthoppers]], [[Polygonia c-album|comma]] and [[geometer moth]] caterpillars resemble twigs, bark, leaves, bird droppings or flowers.<ref name="Wickler"/><ref name="Ruxton">{{cite book |last1=Ruxton |first1=Graeme D. |author1-link=Graeme Ruxton |last2=Sherratt |first2=Thomas N. |author2-link=Thomas N. Sherratt |last3=Speed |first3=M. P. |date=2004 |title=Avoiding Attack: the Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals, and Mimicry |publisher=[[Oxford University Press]]}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Wiklund |first1=Christer |last2=Tullberg |first2=Birgitta S. |date=September 2004 |title=Seasonal polyphenism and leaf mimicry in the comma butterfly |journal=[[Animal Behaviour (journal)|Animal Behaviour]] |volume=68 |issue=3 |pages=621–627 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.12.008 |s2cid=54270418}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Endler |first1=John A. |title=An Overview of the Relationships Between Mimicry and Crypsis |journal=Biological Journal of the Linnean Society |date=August 1981 |volume=16 |issue=1 |pages=25–31 |doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x}}</ref> In addition, predators may make use of resemblance to harmless objects in aggressive masquerade, to enable them to approach prey.<ref name="Pembury Smith Ruxton 2020"/> This [[wolf in sheep's clothing]] strategy differs from the more specific resemblance to the prey in [[aggressive mimicry]], where the prey is both model and dupe.<ref name="Pembury Smith Ruxton 2020">{{cite journal |last=Pembury Smith |first=Matilda Q. R. |last2=Ruxton |first2=Graeme D. |title=Camouflage in predators |journal=Biological Reviews |volume=95 |issue=5 |date=2020 |doi=10.1111/brv.12612 |doi-access=free |pages=1325–1340|hdl=10023/19948 |hdl-access=free }}</ref>
Vavilovian mimicry describes [[weed]]s that come to share characteristics with a [[Crop|domesticated plant]] through [[artificial selection]].<ref name=Pasteur /> It is named after Russian [[botanist]] and [[geneticist]] [[Nikolai Vavilov]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Vavilov | first1=N. I. | year=1951 | title=The origin, variation, immunity and breeding of cultivated plants (translation by K. S. Chester) | url=| journal=Chronica Botanica | volume=13 | issue=| pages=1–366 }}</ref> Selection against the weed may occur either by manually killing the weed, or by separating its seeds from those of the crop. The latter process, known as [[winnowing]], can be done manually or by a machine.


Many animals bear [[eyespot (mimicry)|eyespots]], which are hypothesized to resemble the eyes of larger animals. They may not resemble any specific organism's eyes, and whether or not animals respond to them as eyes is also unclear.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Stevens |first1=Martin |author1-link=Martin Stevens (biologist) |last2=Hopkins |first2=Elinor |last3=Hinde |first3=William |last4=Adcock |first4=Amabel |last5=Connolly |first5=Yvonne |last6=Troscianko |first6=Tom |last7=Cuthill |first7=Innes C. |author7-link=Innes Cuthill |title=Field Experiments on the effectiveness of 'eyespots' as predator deterrents |journal=[[Animal Behaviour (journal)|Animal Behaviour]] |date=November 2007 |volume=74 |issue=5 |pages=1215–1227 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.031 |s2cid=53186893}}</ref><ref name="Stevens 2007">{{cite journal |last1=Stevens |first1=Martin |author1-link=Martin Stevens (biologist) |title=Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |date=22 June 2007 |volume=274|issue=1617 |pages=1457–1464 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2007.0220 |pmid=17426012 |pmc=1950298}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Stevens |first1=Martin |author1-link=Martin Stevens (biologist) |last2=Stubbins |first2=Claire L. |last3=Hardman |first3=Chloe J. |title=The anti-predator function of 'eyespots' on camouflaged and conspicuous prey |journal=Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology |date=30 May 2008 |volume=62 |issue=11 |pages=1787–1793 |doi=10.1007/s00265-008-0607-3|s2cid=28288920}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hossie |first1=Thomas John |last2=Sherratt |first2=Thomas N. |author2-link=Thomas N. Sherratt |title=Defensive posture and eyespots deter avian predators from attacking caterpillar models |journal=Animal Behaviour |date=August 2013|volume=86 |issue=2 |pages=383–389 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.029 |s2cid=53263767}}</ref> The model is usually another species, except in [[automimicry]], where members of the species mimic other members, or other parts of their own bodies, and in inter-sexual mimicry, where members of one sex mimic members of the other.<ref name="Ruxton"/>
Vavilovian mimicry presents an illustration of unintentional (or rather 'anti-intentional') selection by man. While some cases of artificial selection go in the direction desired, such as [[selective breeding]], this case presents the opposite characteristics. Weeders do not want to select weeds that look increasingly like the cultivated plant, yet there is no other option. One case is ''[[Echinochloa oryzoides]]'', a species of grass found as a weed in [[rice]] (''Oryza sativa'') fields. The plant looks similar to rice; its seeds are often mixed in rice and have become difficult to separate through Vavilovian mimicry.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Barrett | first1=S. | year=1983 | title=Mimicry in Plants | url=| journal=[[Scientific American]] | volume=257 | issue=3| pages=76–83 | doi=10.1038/scientificamerican0987-76 }}</ref> A similar problem in agriculture is [[pesticide resistance]]: farmers do not wish to select for weeds that have increasingly similar resistance to pesticides as the crop itself, yet that is the inevitable effect. Vavilovian mimics may eventually be domesticated themselves, as in the case of rye in wheat; Vavilov called these weed-crops ''secondary crops''.


=== Types ===
Vavilovian mimicry can be classified as [[defensive mimicry]], in that the weed mimics a protected species. This bears strong similarity to Batesian mimicry in that the weed does not share the properties that give the model its protection, and both the model and the dupe (in this case people) are harmed by its presence. There are some key differences, though; in [[Batesian mimicry]], the model and signal receiver are enemies (the predator would eat the protected species if it could), whereas here the crop and its [[human]] growers are in a mutualistic relationship: the crop benefits from being dispersed and protected by people, despite being eaten by them. In fact, the crop's only "protection" relevant here is its usefulness to humans. Secondly, the weed is not eaten, but simply destroyed. The only motivation for killing the weed is its effect on crop yields. Finally, this type of mimicry does not occur in ecosystems unaltered by humans.


Many types of mimicry have been described. An overview of each follows, highlighting the similarities and differences between the various forms. Classification is often based on [[function (biology)|function]] with respect to the mimic (e.g., avoiding harm). Some cases may belong to more than one class, e.g., automimicry and aggressive mimicry are not mutually exclusive, as one describes the species relationship between model and mimic, while the other describes the function for the mimic (obtaining food). The terminology used has been debated, as classifications have differed or overlapped; attempts to clarify definitions have led to the partial replacement of old terms with new ones.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref>{{cite journal |last=Endler |first=John A. |author-link=John Endler |title=An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis |journal=Biological Journal of the Linnean Society |volume=16 |pages=25–31 |year=1981 |doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Allen |first1=J. A. |last2=Cooper |first2=J. M. |title=Crypsis and masquerade |journal=Journal of Biological Education |volume=19 |issue=4 |page=268 |year=2010 |doi=10.1080/00219266.1985.9654747}}</ref>
====Protective egg decoys====


{|class="wikitable sortable" style="margin: 1em auto;"
Unlike the previously mentioned forms of mimicry, Gilbertian mimicry involves only two species. The potential host (or prey) drives away its parasite (or predator) by mimicking it, the reverse of host-parasite aggressive mimicry. It was coined by Pasteur as a phrase for such rare mimicry systems,<ref name=Pasteur /> and is named after the American [[ecologist]] Lawrence E. Gilbert.<ref>L. E. Gilbert (1975) Ecological consequences of a coevolved mutualism between butterflies and plants. In L. E. Gilbert and P. H. Raven (eds.) ''Coevolution of Animal and Plants'' pp. 210–40. Austin and London, University of Texas Press.</ref>
|+ Some kinds of mimicry classified by Pasteur 1982<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/>
|-
! Name !! No. of<br/>spp. !! Function !! Dupe finds<br/>Model !! [[Deception in animals|Deception]] !! Description (mimic, model, dupe)
|-
|[[Distraction display|Aristotelian]] ||2 ||Protective ||Agreeable ||Deceptive ||Brooding bird mimics itself with broken wing, luring predator away from nest<ref>Pasteur cites [[Aristotle]], ''[[History of Animals]]'', book 9, chapter 9.</ref>
|-
|[[Automimicry]] ||1 or 2 ||Protective ||Agreeable ||Deceptive ||Multiple forms, e.g. one sex mimics the other, tail mimics head, etc.
|-
|[[Bakerian mimicry|Bakerian]] ||2 ||Reproductive ||Forbidding ||Deceptive ||Female flower resembles male flower, cheating pollinator
|-
|[[Batesian mimicry|Batesian]] ||3 ||Protective ||Forbidding ||Deceptive ||Palatable mimic resembles distasteful model, deceives dupe
|-
|[[Browerian mimicry|Browerian]] ||2 ||Protective ||Forbidding ||Deceptive ||Palatable butterfly resembles toxic member of same species
|-
|[[Emsleyan mimicry|Emsleyan]] ||3 ||Protective ||Forbidding ||Deceptive ||Deadly [[snake]] resembles less deadly species, predators get chance to learn to avoid them
|-
|[[Gilbertian mimicry|Gilbertian]] ||2 ||Protective ||Forbidding ||Deceptive ||[[Host (biology)|Host]]/prey mimics and so repels [[Parasitism|parasite]]/[[Predation|predator]]
|-
|[[Kirbyan mimicry|Kirbyan]] ||2 ||[[Aggressive mimicry|Aggressive]] ||Agreeable ||Deceptive ||[[Brood parasitism|Brood parasite]] adult or egg mimics host which raises the young as its own
|-
|[[Müllerian mimicry|Müllerian]] ||3 or more ||Protective ||Forbidding ||[[Honest signal|Honest]] ||Distasteful co-mimics resemble each other, [[Aposematism|aposematically]] warning off predators<ref name="Rowland Thalainen Lindström 2007"/>
|-
|[[Pouyannian mimicry|Pouyannian]] ||2 ||Reproductive ||Agreeable ||Deceptive ||Plant mimic resembles female bee, deceives male, gets itself pollinated
|-
|[[Vavilovian mimicry|Vavilovian]] ||3 ||Reproductive ||Agreeable ||Deceptive ||Mimic resembles [[crop]], deceives [[farmer]]
|-
|[[Wasmannian mimicry|Wasmannian]] ||2 ||[[Commensalism|Commensal]]ist ||Agreeable ||Deceptive ||Mimic resembles and deceives [[ant]], lives in ant nest
|-
|[[Aggressive mimicry|Wicklerian]] ||2 ||[[Aggressive mimicry|Aggressive]] ||Agreeable ||Deceptive ||[[Predation|Predator]] or parasite resembles and attacks prey or host; parasite may get itself swallowed
|-
|[[Camouflage]] ||2 ||Protective ||Uninteresting ||Deceptive ||Mimic resembles background (plant parts, or inanimate)
|}


== Defensive ==
This form of protective mimicry occurs in the genus ''[[Passiflora]]''. The leaves of this plant contain toxins that deter herbivorous animals—however some ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterfly larvae have evolved enzymes that break down these toxins, allowing them to [[specialist species|specialize]] on this genus. This has created further selection pressure on the host plants, which have evolved [[stipules]] that mimic mature ''Heliconius'' eggs near the point of hatching. These butterflies tend to avoid laying eggs near each existing ones, which helps avoid exploitative [[intraspecific competition]] between caterpillars — those that lay on vacant leaves provide their offspring with a greater chance of survival. Most ''Heliconius'' larvae are [[cannibalism (zoology)|cannibalistic]], meaning that on leaves older eggs hatch first and eat the new arrivals. Thus, it seems that such plants have evolved egg dummies due to these grazing herbivore enemies. In addition, the decoy eggs are also [[nectaries]], attracting predators of the caterpillars such as ants and wasps.<ref name=Campbell>[[Campbell, N. A.]] (1996) Biology (4th edition), Chapter 50. Benjamin Cummings, New York. ISBN 0-8053-1957-3.</ref> This acts as a further defense of the plant against the caterpillars.


Mimicry is defensive or protective when organisms are able to avoid harmful encounters by deceiving enemies into treating them as something else.
====Protective mimicry within a species====


=== Batesian ===
[[File:Monarch Butterfly Danaus plexippus Caterpillar 2000px.jpg|right|thumb|Monarch caterpillars, shown feeding, vary in toxicity depending on their diet.]]


{{Main|Batesian mimicry}}
Browerian mimicry,<ref name=Pasteur /> named after Lincoln P. Brower and Jane Van Zandt Brower,<ref>{{cite book |last=Brower |first=L. P. |year=1970 |chapter=Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain and implications for mimicry theory |editor-first=K. L. |editor-last=Chambers |title=Biochemical Coevolution |location=Corvallis, Oregon, USA |publisher=Oregon State Univ. |pages=69–82 |isbn= }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Brower | first1=L. P. | last2=Van Brower | first2=J. V. Z. | last3=Corvino | first3=J. M. | year=1967 | title=Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain | url=| journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America| volume=57 | issue=4| pages=893–98 | doi=10.1073/pnas.57.4.893 | pmid=5231352 | pmc=224631 | bibcode=1967PNAS...57..893B}}</ref> is a form of ''automimicry''; where the model belongs to the same species as the mimic. This is the analogue of Batesian mimicry within a single species, and occurs when there is a palatability spectrum within a population. Examples include the [[Monarch butterfly|monarch]] and the [[Queen (butterfly)|queen]] from the [[Milkweed butterfly|Danainae]] subfamily, which feed on [[milkweed]] species of varying toxicity. These species store toxins from its host plant, which are maintained even in the adult ([[imago]]) form. As levels of toxin vary depending on diet during the larval stage, some individuals are more toxic than others. Less palatable organisms, therefore, mimic more dangerous individuals, with their likeness already perfected.


[[File:Macroxiphus sp cricket.jpg|thumb|''[[Macroxiphus]]'', a harmless bush cricket, mimics a well-defended [[ant]]. ]]
This is not always the case, however. In sexually dimorphic species, one sex may be more of a threat than the other, which could mimic the protected sex. Evidence for this possibility is provided by the behavior of a monkey from [[Gabon]], which regularly ate male moths of the genus ''Anaphe'', but promptly stopped after it tasted a noxious female.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Bigot | first1=L. | last2=Jouventin | first2=P. | year=1974 | title=Quelques expériences de comestibilité de Lépidoptères gabonais faites avec le mandrill, le cercocèbe à joues grises et le garde-bœufs |language=fr| url=| journal=Terre Vie | volume=28 | issue=| pages=521–43 }}</ref>


In Batesian mimicry, the mimic resembles the model, but does not have the attribute that makes it unprofitable to predators (e.g., unpalatability, or the ability to sting). In other words, a Batesian mimic is a [[The Wolf in Sheep's Clothing|sheep in wolf's clothing]]. Mimics are less likely to be found out (for example by predators) when in low proportion to their model. Such [[negative frequency-dependent selection]] applies in most forms of mimicry. Specifically, Batesian mimicry can only be maintained if the harm caused to the predator by eating a model outweighs the benefit of eating a mimic. The nature of learning is weighted in favor of the mimics, for a predator that has a bad first experience with a model tends to avoid anything that looks like it for a long time, and does not re-sample soon to see whether the initial experience was a false negative. However, if mimics become more abundant than models, then the probability of a young predator having a first experience with a mimic increases. Batesian systems are therefore most likely to be stable where the model is more abundant than the mimic.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Stearns |first1=S. C. |last2=Hoekstra |first2=Rolf F. |title=Evolution: An Introduction |year=2000 |edition=5th |publisher=Oxford University Press |page=464 |isbn=978-0-19-854968-0}}</ref>
===Aggressive===
{{Main|Aggressive mimicry}}


There are many Batesian mimics among [[Lepidoptera|butterflies and moths]]. ''[[Consul fabius]]'' and ''[[Eresia (butterfly)|Eresia eunice]]'' imitate unpalatable ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterflies such as ''[[Heliconius ismenius|H. ismenius]]''.<ref name="pinheiro">{{cite journal |last1=Pinheiro |first1=Carlos E. G. |year=1996 |title=Palatability and escaping ability in Neotropical butterflies: tests with wild kingbirds (''Tyrannus melancholicus'', Tyrannidae) |journal=Biological Journal of the Linnean Society |volume=59 |issue=4 |pages=351–365 |doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1996.tb01471.x |doi-access=free}}</ref> ''[[Limenitis arthemis]]'' imitate the poisonous pipevine swallowtail (''[[Battus philenor]]''). Several palatable moths produce ultrasonic click calls to mimic unpalatable tiger moths.<ref name=tigermoth>{{cite journal |last1=Barber |first1=J. R. |last2=Conner |first2=W. E. |year=2007 |title=Acoustic mimicry in a predator–prey interaction |journal=PNAS |volume=104 |issue=22 |pages=9331–9334 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0703627104 |pmid=17517637 |pmc=1890494 |bibcode=2007PNAS..104.9331B |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Barber |first1=Jesse R. |last2=Chadwell |first2=Brad A. |last3=Garrett |first3=Nick |last4=Schmidt-French |first4=Barbara |last5=Conner |first5=William E. |date=July 2009 |title=Naïve bats discriminate arctiid moth warning sounds but generalize their aposematic meaning |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19561203/ |journal=The Journal of Experimental Biology |volume=212 |issue=Pt 14 |pages=2141–2148 |doi=10.1242/jeb.029991 |issn=0022-0949 |pmid=19561203 |s2cid=1303252}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Barber |first1=Jesse R. |last2=Plotkin |first2=David |last3=Rubin |first3=Juliette J. |last4=Homziak |first4=Nicholas T. |last5=Leavell |first5=Brian C. |last6=Houlihan |first6=Peter R. |last7=Miner |first7=Krystie A. |last8=Breinholt |first8=Jesse W. |last9=Quirk-Royal |first9=Brandt |last10=Padrón |first10=Pablo Sebastián |last11=Nunez |first11=Matias |last12=Kawahara |first12=Akito Y. |display-authors=5 |date=2022-06-21 |title=Anti-bat ultrasound production in moths is globally and phylogenetically widespread |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |volume=119 |issue=25 |pages=e2117485119 |doi=10.1073/pnas.2117485119 |doi-access=free |pmc=9231501 |pmid=35704762|bibcode=2022PNAS..11917485B}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Kawahara |first1=Akito Y. |last2=Barber |first2=Jesse R. |date=2015-05-19 |title=Tempo and mode of antibat ultrasound production and sonar jamming in the diverse hawkmoth radiation |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |volume=112 |issue=20 |pages=6407–6412 |doi=10.1073/pnas.1416679112 |pmc=4443353 |pmid=25941377 |bibcode=2015PNAS..112.6407K |doi-access=free}}</ref> [[Octopuses]] of the genus ''Thaumoctopus'' (the [[mimic octopus]]) are able to intentionally alter their body shape and coloration to resemble dangerous [[sea snakes]] or [[Pterois|lionfish]].<ref>{{Cite web |url=http://marinebio.org/species.asp?id=260 |title=Mimic Octopus, Thaumoctopus mimicus at MarineBio.org |access-date=2007-06-09 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170718195726/http://marinebio.org/species.asp?id=260 |archive-date=2017-07-18}}</ref> In the Amazon, the [[helmeted woodpecker]] (''[[Helmeted woodpecker|Dryocopus galeatus]]''), a rare species which lives in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil, Paraguay, and Argentina, has a similar red crest, black back, and barred underside to two larger woodpeckers: [[Lineated woodpecker|''Dryocopus lineatus'']] and ''[[Robust woodpecker|Campephilus robustus]]''. This mimicry reduces attacks on ''D. galeatus''.<ref>{{Cite web |title=Deceptive Woodpecker Uses Mimicry to Avoid Competition |url=http://www.amnh.org/explore/news-blogs/research-posts/deceptive-woodpecker-uses-mimicry-to-avoid-competition |website=AMNH |access-date=12 August 2015}}</ref> Batesian mimicry occurs in the plant kingdom, where the [[Boquila|chameleon vine]] adapts its leaf shape and colour to match that of the plant it is climbing.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Gianoli |first=Ernesto |year=2014 |title=Leaf Mimicry in a Climbing Plant Protects against Herbivory |journal=Cell |volume=24 |issue=9 |pages=984–987 |doi=10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.010 |pmid=24768053 |doi-access=free}}</ref>
Aggressive mimicry describes predators (or [[parasite]]s) that share the same characteristics as a harmless species, allowing them to avoid detection by their prey (or [[host (biology)|host]]). The mimic may resemble the prey or host itself, or another organism that is either neutral or beneficial to the signal receiver. In this class of mimicry, the model may be affected negatively, positively or not at all. Just as parasites can be treated as a form of predator,<ref name=Ecology>Begon, M., Townsend, C., Harper, J. (1996) ''[[Ecology: Individuals, populations and communities]]'' (third edition) Blackwell Science, London</ref> host-parasite mimicry is treated here as a subclass of aggressive mimicry.


=== Müllerian ===
The mimic may have a particular significance for duped prey.<!-- anglerfish example (see Wickler and also The Compleat Angler: Aggressive Mimicry in an Antennariid Anglerfish. Theodore W. Pietsch; David B. Grobecker Science > New Series, Vol. 201, no. 4353 (July, 1978), pp. 369–370 --> One such case is [[spider]]s, amongst which aggressive mimicry is quite common both in luring prey and disguising stealthily approaching predators.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Jackson | first1=R. R. | year=1995 | title=Eight-legged tricksters: Spiders that specialize at catching other spiders | url=| journal=BioScience | volume=42 | issue=8| pages=590–98 |jstor=1311924 | doi=10.2307/1311924}}</ref> One case is the [[golden orb weaver]] (''Nephila clavipes''), which spins a conspicuous golden colored web in well-lit areas. Experiments show that bees are able to associate the webs with danger when the yellow pigment is not present, as occurs in less well-lit areas where the web is much harder to see. Other colors were also learned and avoided, but bees seemed least able to effectively associate yellow pigmented webs with danger. Yellow is the color of many nectar bearing flowers, however, so perhaps avoiding yellow is not worthwhile. Another form of mimicry is based not on [[color]] but pattern. Species such as the silver argiope (''[[Argiope argentata]]'') employ prominent patterns in the middle of their webs, such as zigzags. These may reflect ultraviolet light, and mimic the pattern seen in many flowers known as [[nectar guide]]s. Spiders change their web day to day, which can be explained by bee's ability to remember web patterns. Bees are able to associate a certain pattern with a spatial location, meaning the spider must spin a new pattern regularly or suffer diminishing prey capture.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Craig | first1=C. L. | year=1995 | title=Webs of Deceit | url=| journal=[[Natural History (magazine)|Natural History]] | volume=104 | issue=3| pages=32–35 }}</ref>


{{Main|Müllerian mimicry}}
{{listen
|filename=Chlorobalius leucoviridis mimicry1.ogg|title=''Chlorobalius leucoviridis'' mimicry of ''Kobonga oxleyi''|description=''Kobonga oxleyi'' cicada song with reply clicks from a ''Chlorobalius leucoviridis''|format=[[Ogg]]
|filename2=Chlorobalius leucoviridis mimicry2.ogg|title2=''Chlorobalius leucoviridis'' mimicry of ''Pauropsalta sp.''|description2=''Pauropsalta sp.'' "Sandstone" song with reply clicks from a ''Chlorobalius leucoviridis''|format2=Ogg
}}


[[File:Batesian vs Müllerian Mimicry.svg|thumb|upright=1.8|Comparison of Batesian and Müllerian mimicry, illustrated with a [[hoverfly]], a [[wasp]] and a [[bee]]]]
Another case is where males are lured towards what seems to be a [[sexually receptive]] female. The model in this situation is the same species as the dupe. Beginning in the 1960s, James E. Lloyd's investigation of female [[firefly|fireflies]] of the genus ''[[Photuris (genus)|Photuris]]'' revealed they emit the same light signals that females of the genus ''[[Photinus (beetle)|Photinus]]'' use as a mating signal.<ref>Lloyd, J. E. (1965) Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris: Firefly Femmes Fatales Science 149:653–654.</ref> Further research showed male fireflies from several different [[genus|genera]] are attracted to these "[[femmes fatales]]", and are subsequently captured and eaten. Female signals are based on that received from the male, each female having a repertoire of signals matching the delay and duration of the female of the corresponding species. This mimicry may have evolved from non-mating signals that have become modified for predation.<ref>{{cite journal | author=Lloyd, J. E. | year=1975 | title=Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris Fireflies: Signal Repertoires by Femmes Fatales | journal=Science | volume=187 | issue=4175| pages=452–453 | doi=10.1126/science.187.4175.452 | pmid=17835312 |bibcode=1975Sci...187..452L }}</ref>


In Müllerian mimicry, two or more species have similar warning or [[Aposematism|aposematic]] signals and both share genuine [[anti-predation]] attributes (e.g. being unpalatable), as first described in ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterflies.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Meyer |first=A. |year=2006 |title=Repeating Patterns of Mimicry |journal=[[PLOS Biology]] |volume=4 |issue=10|page=e341 |doi=10.1371/journal.pbio.0040341 |pmid=17048984 |pmc=1617347 |doi-access=free}}</ref> This type of mimicry is unique in several respects. Firstly, both the mimic and the model benefit from the interaction, which could thus be classified as [[Mutualism (biology)|mutualism]]. The signal receiver also benefits by this system, despite being deceived about species identity, as it is able to generalize the pattern to potentially harmful encounters. The distinction between mimic and model that is clear in [[Batesian mimicry]] is also blurred. Where one species is scarce and another abundant, the rare species can be said to be the mimic. When both are present in similar numbers, however, it makes more sense to speak of each as a ''co-mimic'' than of distinct 'mimic' and 'model' species, as their warning signals tend to converge.<ref name="Rowland Thalainen Lindström 2007">{{cite journal |last1=Rowland |first1=Hannah M. |last2=Ihalainen |first2=Eira |last3=Lindström |first3=Leena |last4=Mappes |first4=Johanna |last5=Speed |first5=Michael P. |title=Co-mimics have a mutualistic relationship despite unequal defences |journal=[[Nature (journal)|Nature]] |volume=448 |issue=7149 |date=2007 |issn=0028-0836 |doi=10.1038/nature05899 |pages=64–67}}</ref> Also, the mimetic species may exist on a continuum from harmless to highly noxious, so Batesian mimicry grades smoothly into Müllerian convergence.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Huheey |first=James E. |year=1976 |title=Studies in warning coloration and mimicry VII — Evolutionary consequences of a Batesian–Müllerian spectrum: A model for Müllerian mimicry |journal=Evolution |volume=30 |issue=1|pages=86–93 |doi=10.2307/2407675 |pmid=28565050 |jstor=2407675}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last=Benson |first=W. W. |year=1977 |title=On the Supposed Spectrum Between Batesian and Mullerian Mimicry |journal=Evolution |volume=31 |issue=2 |pages=454–455 |doi=10.2307/2407770 |pmid=28563231 |jstor=2407770}}</ref>
[[File:Marshall katydid QL.RIA small.jpg|left|thumb|The spotted predatory katydid (''Chlorobalius leucoviridis''), an acoustic aggressive mimic of cicadas]]
The listrosceline [[katydid]] ''Chlorobalius leucoviridis'' of inland [[Australia]] is capable of attracting male cicadas of the tribe Cicadettini by imitating the species-specific reply clicks of sexually receptive female cicadas. This example of acoustic aggressive mimicry is similar to the ''Photuris'' firefly case in that the predator's mimicry is remarkably versatile – playback experiments show that ''C. leucoviridis'' is able to attract males of many cicada species, including cicadettine cicadas from other continents, even though cicada mating signals are species-specific.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Marshall | first1=D. C. | last2=Hill | first2=K. B. R. | year=2009 | title=Versatile aggressive mimicry of cicadas by an Australian predatory katydid | journal=PLOS ONE | volume=4 | issue=1| page=e4185 |pmid=19142230 | doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0004185 | pmc=2615208|bibcode=2009PLoSO...4.4185M | editor1-last=Chippindale | editor1-first=Adam K }}</ref>


{{anchor|Emsleyan|Mertensian}}
Some [[carnivorous plant]]s may also be able to increase their rate of capture through mimicry.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Moran | first1=Jonathan A. | year=1996 | title=Pitcher dimorphism, prey composition and the mechanisms of prey attraction in the pitcher plant ''Nepenthes rafflesiana'' in Borneo | url=| journal=Journal of Ecology | volume=84 | issue=4| pages=515–525 | doi=10.2307/2261474 }}</ref>


=== Emsleyan/Mertensian ===
Luring is not a necessary condition however, as the predator still has a significant advantage simply by not being identified as such. They may resemble a mutualistic [[symbiont]] or a species of little relevance to the prey.


{{main|Emsleyan mimicry}}
[[File:Epinephelus tukula is cleaned by two Labroides dimidiatus.jpg|right|thumb|Two [[bluestreak cleaner wrasse]] cleaning a [[potato grouper]], ''Epinephelus tukula'']]
A case of the latter situation is a species of [[cleaner fish]] and its mimic, though in this example the model is greatly disadvantaged by the presence of the mimic. Cleaner fish are the allies of many other species, which allow them to eat their parasites and dead skin. Some allow the cleaner to venture inside their body to hunt these parasites. However, one species of cleaner, the [[bluestreak cleaner wrasse]] (''Labroides dimidiatus''), is the unknowing model of a mimetic species, the sabre-toothed blenny (''Aspidontus taeniatus''). This [[wrasse]] resides in [[coral reef]]s in the [[Indian Ocean|Indian]] and the [[Pacific Ocean|Pacific]] Oceans, and is recognized by other fishes that then let it clean them. Its imposter, a species of [[blenny]], lives in the [[Indian Ocean]]—and not only looks like it in terms of size and [[animal coloration|coloration]], but even mimics the cleaner's "dance". Having fooled its prey into letting its guard down, it then bites it, tearing off a piece of its fin before fleeing. Fish [[grazing|grazed]] on in this fashion soon learn to distinguish mimic from model, but because the similarity is close between the two they become much more cautious of the model as well, so both are affected. Due to victims' ability to discriminate between foe and helper, the blennies have evolved close similarity, right down to the regional level.<ref>{{cite journal | author=Wickler, W. | year=1966 | title=Mimicry in Tropical Fishes | journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences | volume=251 | issue=772| pages=473–474 | doi=10.1098/rstb.1966.0036 |bibcode=1966RSPTB.251..473W }}</ref>


Emsleyan or Mertensian mimicry describes the unusual case where a deadly prey mimics a less dangerous species.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/> It was first proposed by M. G. Emsley in 1966 as a possible explanation for how a predator can learn to avoid a very dangerous aposematic animal, such as a [[coral snake]], when the predator is very likely to die, making learning unlikely.<ref name=Emsley>{{cite journal |last1=Emsley |first1=M. G. |year=1966 |title=The mimetic significance of ''Erythrolamprus aesculapii ocellatus'' Peters from Tobago |journal=Evolution |volume=20 |issue=4 |pages=663–64 |doi=10.2307/2406599 |jstor=2406599 |pmid=28562911}}</ref> The theory was developed by the German biologist [[Wolfgang Wickler]] who named it after the [[Germany|German]] [[herpetologist]] [[Robert Mertens]].<ref name="Wickler"/><ref name=Mertens>{{cite journal |last1=Mertens |first1=Robert |author-link=Robert Mertens |year=1956 |title=Das Problem der Mimikry bei Korallenschlangen |language=de |journal=Zool. Jahrb. Syst |volume=84 |pages=541–76}}</ref><ref name=Hecht>{{cite journal |last1=Hecht |first1=M. K. |last2=Marien |first2=D. |year=1956 |title=The coral snake mimic problem: a reinterpretation |journal=Journal of Morphology |volume=98 |issue=2 |pages=335–365 |doi=10.1002/jmor.1050980207 |s2cid=83825414}}</ref><ref name=Sheppard>{{cite journal |last1=Sheppard |first1=P. M. |last2=Wickler |first2=Wolfgang |author2-link=Wolfgang Wickler |year=1969 |title=Review of ''Mimicry in plants and animals'' by Wolfgang Wickler |journal=Journal of Animal Ecology |volume=38 |issue=1 |page=243 |doi=10.2307/2762 |jstor=2762}}</ref> The scenario is unlike Müllerian mimicry, where the most harmful species is the model. But if a predator dies on its first encounter with a deadly snake, it has no occasion to [[learning|learn]] to recognize the snake's warning signals. There would then be no advantage for an extremely deadly snake in being aposematic: any predator that attacked it would be killed before it could learn to avoid the deadly prey, so the snake would be better off being camouflaged to avoid attacks. But if the predator first learnt to avoid a less deadly warning-coloured snake, the deadly species could profit by mimicking the less dangerous snake.<ref name=Hecht/><ref name=Sheppard/> Some harmless [[milk snake]]s (''Lampropeltis triangulum''), the moderately toxic false coral snakes (''[[Erythrolamprus aesculapii]]''), and the deadly [[coral snakes]] (''[[Micrurus]]'') all have a red background color with black and white/yellow rings. In this system, both the milk snakes and the deadly coral snakes are mimics, while the false coral snakes are the model.<ref name=Emsley/><!--please don't add material here, see the main article instead-->
Another interesting example that does not involve any luring is the [[zone-tailed hawk]], which resembles the [[turkey vulture]]. It flies amongst the vultures, suddenly breaking from the formation and ambushing its prey.<ref>{{cite journal | author=Willis, E. O. | year=1963 | title=Is the Zone-Tailed Hawk a Mimic of the Turkey Vulture? | journal=The Condor | volume=65 | issue=4| pages=313–317 | doi=10.2307/1365357 }}</ref> Here the hawk's presence is of no evident significance to the vultures, affecting them neither negatively or positively.


<gallery class=center mode=nolines widths=220>
====Parasites====
File:Micrurus tener.jpg|The deadly Texas coral snake,<br/>''[[Micrurus tener]]'',<br/>(the Emsleyan/Mertensian mimic)<!--yes, surprising but true-->
[[File:European Cuckoo Mimics Sparrowhawk.jpg|thumb|upright|left|Mimicry in a [[parasite]]: [[common cuckoo|Cuckoo]] adult mimics [[Eurasian sparrowhawk|sparrowhawk]], alarming small birds enough to give female cuckoo time to lay eggs in their nests.]]
File:Erythrolamprus aesculapii (cropped).jpg|The moderately toxic<br/>''[[Erythrolamprus aesculapii]]''<br/>(the model for both types of mimicry)
File:Lampropeltis triangulum annulata.jpg|The harmless Mexican milk snake,<br/>''[[Lampropeltis triangulum annulata]]''<br/>(the Batesian mimic)<!--this is not a typo-->
</gallery>


=== Wasmannian ===
Parasites can also be aggressive mimics, though the situation is somewhat different from those outlined previously. Some predators have a feature that draws prey; parasites can also mimic their host's natural prey, but are eaten themselves, a pathway into their host. ''[[Leucochloridium]]'', a genus of [[flatworm]], matures in the digestive system of [[songbird]]s, their eggs then passing out of the bird via the [[feces]]. They are then taken up by ''[[Succinea]]'', a terrestrial snail. The eggs develop in this [[intermediate host]], and then must find a suitable bird to mature in. Since the host birds do not eat snails, the [[sporocyst]] has another strategy to reach its host's intestine. They are brightly colored and move in a pulsating fashion. A sporocyst-sac pulsates in the snail's eye stalks,<ref>[http://www.semioticon.com/seo/M/images/mimicry_2.jpg See here for a photo.]</ref><ref>Moore J., 2002. Parasites and the behavior of animals. Oxford University
Press, Oxford.</ref> coming to resemble an irresistible meal for a songbird. In this way, it can bridge the gap between hosts, allowing it to complete its life cycle.<ref name=Wickler /> A nematode (''[[Myrmeconema neotropicum]]'') changes the colour of the abdomen of workers of the canopy ant ''Cephalotes atratus'' to make it appear like the ripe fruits of ''Hyeronima alchorneoides''. It also changes the behaviour of the ant so that the [[gaster (insect anatomy)|gaster]] (rear part) is held raised. This presumably increases the chances of the ant being eaten by birds. The droppings of birds are collected by other ants and fed to their brood, thereby helping to spread the nematode.<ref>{{cite journal | pmid=18279076 | year=2008 | last1=Yanoviak | first1=SP | last2=Kaspari | first2=M | last3=Dudley | first3=R | last4=Poinar Jr | first4=G | title=Parasite-induced fruit mimicry in a tropical canopy ant | volume=171 | issue=4 | pages=536–44 | doi=10.1086/528968 | journal=The American Naturalist |url=http://www.canopyants.com/2008_AmNat.pdf | format= PDF }}</ref>


{{further|Ant mimicry}}
In an unusual case, [[planidium]] larvae of some beetles of the genus ''[[Meloe]]'' form a group and produce a [[pheromone]] that mimics the sex attractant of its host [[Apidae|bee]] species. When a male bee arrives and attempts to mate with the mass of larvae, they climb onto his abdomen. From there, they transfer to a female bee, and from there to the bee nest to parasitize the bee larvae.<ref>{{cite journal | doi=10.1073/pnas.0603901103 | title=Phoretic nest parasites use sexual deception to obtain transport to their host's nest | year=2006 | last1=Saul-Gershenz | first1=L. S. | journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences | volume=103 | issue=38 | pages=14039–14044 |pmc=1599908 | pmid=16966608|bibcode=2006PNAS..10314039S | last2=Millar | first2=J. G. }}</ref>


In [[Wasmannian mimicry]], the mimic resembles a model that it [[inquiline|lives along with]] in a nest or colony. Most of the models here are [[eusociality|eusocial]] insects, principally ants.<ref name="Wasmann 1894">{{cite book |last=Wasmann |first=Erich |author-link=Erich Wasmann |year=1894 |title=Kritisches Verzeichniss der myrmecophilin und termitophilen Arthropoden |language=de |trans-title=Critical Inventory of Myrmecophile and Termitophile Arthropods |publisher=Felix Dames |location=Berlin}}</ref><ref name="HölldoblerWilson1990">{{cite book |last1=Hölldobler |first1=Bert |last2=Wilson |first2=Edward O. |author2-link=Edward O. Wilson |title=The Ants |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ljxV4h61vhUC&pg=PA512 |year=1990 |publisher=[[Harvard University Press]] |isbn=978-0-674-04075-5 |pages=511–514}}</ref>
[[File:Cuckoo Eggs Mimicking Reed Warbler Eggs.JPG|thumb|upright|[[Brood parasitism]]: cuckoo eggs (larger) mimic many species of host birds' eggs, in this case of [[Eurasian reed warbler|reed warbler]].]]


=== Gilbertian ===
Host-parasite mimicry is a two species system where a parasite mimics its own host. [[Cuckoo]]s are a canonical example of [[brood parasitism]], a form of [[kleptoparasitism]] where the mother has its offspring raised by another unwitting organism, cutting down the biological mother's [[parental investment]] in the process. The ability to lay eggs that mimic the host eggs is the key [[adaptation]]. The adaptation to different hosts is inherited through the female line in so-called [[Polymorphism (biology)#The cuckoo|gentes]]. Cases of ''intraspecific'' brood parasitism, where a female lays in conspecific's nest, as illustrated by the [[goldeneye (duck)|goldeneye]] duck (''Bucephala clangula''),<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Andersson | first1=M. | last2=Eriksson | first2=M. O. G. | year=1982 | title=Nest parasitism in Goldeneyes ''Bucephala clangula'': some evolutionary aspects | url=| journal=American Naturalist | volume=120 | issue=| pages=1–16 | doi=10.1086/283965 }}</ref> do not represent a case of mimicry. Another example is that of chemical mimicry, in which the parasitic butterfly ''[[Phengaris rebeli]]'', which parasitizes the ant species ''[[Myrmica schencki]]'' by releasing chemicals that fool the worker ants to believe that the caterpillar larvae are ant larvae, and enable the ''P. rebeli'' larvae to be brought directly into the ''M. schencki'' nest.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Barbero |first=Francesca |author2=Thomas JA |author3=Bonelli S |author4=Balletto E |author5=Schonrogge K |title=Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants |journal=Journal of Experimental Biology |year=2009 |volume=212 |pages=4084–4090 |url=http://www.researchgate.net/publication/40039224_Acoustical_mimicry_in_a_predatory_social_parasite_of_ants |accessdate=28 September 2013 |doi=10.1242/jeb.032912 |pmid=19946088 |issue=Pt 24}}</ref> Parasitic (cuckoo) bumblebees (formerly ''[[Psithyrus]]'', now included in ''[[Bombus]]'') resemble their hosts more closely than would be expected by chance, at least in areas like Europe where parasite-host co-speciation is common. However, this is explainable as Müllerian mimicry, rather than requiring the parasite's coloration to deceive the host and thus constitute aggressive mimicry.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Williams |first1=Paul H. |title=Do the parasitic Psithyrus resemble their host bumblebees in colour pattern? |journal=Apidologie |date=2008 |volume=39 |issue=6 |pages=637–649 |doi=10.1051/apido:2008048 |url=http://www.apidologie.org/articles/apido/pdf/2008/06/m08010.pdf}}</ref>


{{main|Gilbertian mimicry}}
===Reproductive===


[[File:Heliconiinae - Heliconius numata.JPG|thumb|Some ''[[Passiflora]]'' flower species use Gilbertian mimicry, defending against being eaten by larvae of ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterflies with leaf [[stipule]]s (not shown) that resemble the butterfly's eggs.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref name=Campbell/>]]
Reproductive mimicry occurs when the actions of the dupe directly aid in the mimic's [[reproduction]]. This is common in plants with deceptive flowers that do not provide the reward they seem to offer. Other forms of mimicry have a reproductive component, such as [[Vavilovian mimicry]] involving seeds, and [[brood parasitism]], which can also involve aggressive mimicry.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Feeney |first1=W. E. |last2=Troscianko |first2=J. |last3=Langmore |first3=N. E. |last4=Spottiswoode |first4=C. N.|title=Evidence for aggressive mimicry in an adult brood parasitic bird, and generalized defences in its host |journal=Proceedings B of the Royal Society |date=July 2015 |volume=282 |issue=1810 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2015.0795 |url=http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/282/1810/20150795 |pages=20150795}}</ref>


Gilbertian mimicry is bipolar, involving only two species. The potential host (or prey) drives away its parasite (or predator) by mimicking it, the reverse of host-parasite aggressive mimicry. It was coined by Pasteur as a phrase for such rare mimicry systems,<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/> and is named after the American [[ecologist]] [[Lawrence E. Gilbert]] who described it in 1975.<ref>{{cite book |last=Gilbert |first=Lawrence E. |year=1975 |chapter=Ecological consequences of a coevolved mutualism between butterflies and plants |editor1=L. E. Gilbert |editor2=P. H. Raven |title=Coevolution of Animals and Plants |pages=210–240 |publisher=[[University of Texas Press]] |oclc=636384400}}</ref> The classical instance of Gilbertian mimicry is in the plant genus ''[[Passiflora]]'', which is grazed by the [[micropredator]] larvae of some ''[[Heliconius]]'' butterflies. The host plants have evolved [[stipules]] that mimic mature ''Heliconius'' eggs near the point of hatching. The butterflies avoid laying eggs near existing ones, reducing [[intraspecific competition]] between caterpillars, which are also [[cannibalism (zoology)|cannibalistic]], so those that lay on vacant leaves provide their offspring with a greater chance of survival. The stipules thus appear to have evolved as Gilbertian mimics of butterfly eggs, under selection pressure from these caterpillars.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><!--Pasteur page 186--><ref name=Campbell>{{cite book |last=Campbell |first=N. A. |year=1996 |title=Biology |edition=4th |at=Chapter 50 |publisher=Benjamin Cummings |isbn=0-8053-1957-3}}</ref>
====Mimicry of flowers====
{{Main|Mimicry in plants}}
<!--
[[File:Cardinal flower20010810.PNG|right|thumb|''[[Lobelia cardinalis]]'' is believed to be a Bakerian mimic, attracting [[humming bird]]s without producing [[nectar]]<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Williamson | first1=G. B. | last2=Black | first2=E. M. | year=1981 | title=Mimicry in Hummingbird-Pollinated Plants? | url=| journal=Ecology | volume=62 | issue=2| pages=494–496 | doi=10.2307/1936724 }}</ref>]] Is this correct? Or is it a Dodsonian mimic?
-->
Bakerian mimicry, named after Herbert G. Baker,<ref>Baker H. G. 1976. "Mistake" pollination as a reproductive system, with special reference to the Caricaceae. Pp 161–169 in J. Burley and B. T. Styles, eds. {{Clarify|date=July 2013}} ''Variation, breeding, and conservation of tropical trees''. Academic Press, London, U.K.</ref> is a form of automimicry where female [[flower]]s mimic male flowers of their own species, cheating pollinators out of a reward. This reproductive mimicry may not be readily apparent as members of the same species may still exhibit some degree of [[sexual dimorphism]]. It is common in many species of [[Caricaceae]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Bawa | first1=K. S. | year=1980 | title=Mimicry of male by female flowers and intrasexual competition for pollinators in ''Jacaratia dolichaula'' (D. Smith) Woodson (Caricaceae) | url=| journal=[[Evolution (journal)|Evolution]] | volume=34 | issue=3| pages=467–74 | doi=10.2307/2408216 }}</ref>


=== Browerian ===
Like Bakerian mimicry, Dodsonian mimicry is a form of reproductive floral mimicry, but the model belongs to a different species than the mimic. The name refers to [[Calaway H. Dodson]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Dodson | first1=C. H. | last2=Frymire | first2=G. P. | year=1961 | title=Natural pollination of orchids | url=| journal=Missouri Botanical Garden Bulletin | volume=49 | issue=| pages=133–39 }}</ref> By providing similar sensory signals as the model flower, it can lure its pollinators. Like Bakerian mimics, no nectar is provided. ''[[Epidendrum ibaguense]]'' ([[Orchidaceae]]) resembles flowers of ''[[Lantana camara]]'' and ''[[Asclepias curassavica]]'', and is pollinated by monarch butterflies and perhaps [[hummingbird]]s.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Boyden | first1=T. C. | year=1980 | title=Floral mimicry by Epidendrurn ibaguense (Orchidaceae) in Panama | url=| journal=Evolution | volume=34 | issue=1| pages=135–36 | doi=10.2307/2408322 }}</ref> Similar cases are seen in some other species of the same family. The mimetic species may still have pollinators of its own though. For example, a [[Lamellicornia|lamellicorn beetle]], which usually pollinates correspondingly colored ''[[Cistus]]'' flowers, is also known to aid in pollination of ''[[Ophrys]]'' species that are normally pollinated by bees.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Kullenberg | first1=B. | year=1961 | title=Studies in ''Ophrys'' pollination | url=| journal=Zool. Bidr. Uppsala | volume=34 | issue=| pages=1–340 }}</ref>


{{main|Automimicry}}
====Pseudocopulation====
{{Further2|[[Pseudocopulation]]}}


Browerian mimicry, named after [[Lincoln P. Brower]] and Jane Van Zandt Brower who first described it in 1967,<ref>{{cite book |last=Brower |first=Lincoln P. |author-link=Lincoln P. Brower |year=1970 |chapter=Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain and implications for mimicry theory |editor-first=K. L. |editor-last=Chambers |title=Biochemical Coevolution |location=Corvallis, Oregon, USA |publisher=Oregon State Univ. |pages=69–82}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Brower |first1=Lincoln P. |author-link=Lincoln P. Brower |last2=Van Brower |first2=J. V. Z. |last3=Corvino |first3=J. M. |year=1967 |title=Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |volume=57 |issue=4|pages=893–98 |doi=10.1073/pnas.57.4.893 |pmid=5231352 |pmc=224631 |bibcode=1967PNAS...57..893B|doi-access=free}}</ref> is a postulated form of ''automimicry''; where the model belongs to the same species as the mimic. This is the analogue of Batesian mimicry within a single species, and occurs when there is a palatability spectrum within a population. Examples include the [[Monarch butterfly|monarch]] and the [[Queen (butterfly)|queen]] from the subfamily [[Milkweed butterfly|Danainae]], which feed on [[milkweed]] species of varying toxicity. These species store toxins from its host plant, which are maintained even in the adult. As levels of toxin vary depending on diet, some individuals are more toxic than the rest, which profit from the toxicity of those individuals, just as hoverflies benefit from mimicking well-defended wasps.<ref name="Pasteur 1982" />
[[File:Ophrys insectifera Saarland 05.jpg|upright|thumb|The [[fly orchid]] (''Ophrys insectifera'')]]


=== Misdirection by automimicry ===
Pseudocopulation occurs when a flower mimics a [[female]] of a certain [[insect]] species, inducing the [[male]]s to try to copulate with the flower. This is much like the aggressive mimicry in fireflies described previously, but with a more benign outcome for the pollinator. This form of mimicry has been called ''Pouyannian mimicry'',<ref name=Pasteur /> after [[Maurice-Alexandre Pouyanne]], who first described the phenomenon.<ref>Correvon H., Pouyanne M. (1916) {{lang|fr|Un curieux cas de mimetisme chez les Ophrydées}}. ''J. Soc. Nat. Hortic. Fr.'' '''17''': 29–31, 41–42, 84.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Pouyanne | first1=M. | year=1917 | title=La fécondation des ''Ophrys'' par les insectes | url=| journal=Bull. Soc. Hist. Nat. Afr. Nord | volume=8 | issue=| pages=1–2 }}</ref> It is most common in orchids, which mimic females of the order [[Hymenoptera]] (generally bees and wasps), and may account for around 60% of pollinations.<ref name=Orchid>Van der Pijl, L., Dodson, C. H. (1966) ''Orchid Flowers; Their Pollination and Evolution''. Coral Gables, Florida, USA, Univ. Miami Press.</ref> Depending on the morphology of the flower, a pollen sac called a [[pollinia]] is attached to the head or abdomen of the male. This is then transferred to the [[Stigma (flower)|stigma]] of the next flower the male tries to inseminate, resulting in pollination. Visual mimicry is the most obvious sign of this deception for humans, but the visual aspect may be minor or non-existent. It is the senses of [[touch]] and [[olfaction]] that are most important.<ref name=Orchid />


[[File:Chaetodon capistratus2.jpg|thumb|upright=0.6|[[Eyespot (mimicry)|Eyespots]] of [[foureye butterflyfish]] (''Chaetodon capistratus'') mimic its own eyes, deflecting attacks from the vulnerable head.]]
====Inter-sexual mimicry====
{{main|Sexual mimicry}}
Inter-sexual mimicry occurs when individuals of one sex in a species mimic members of the opposite sex. An example is the three male forms of the marine [[isopod]] ''[[Paracerceis sculpta]]''. Alpha males are the largest and guard a [[harem]] of females. Beta males mimic females and manage to enter the harem of females [[Passing (gender)|without being detected]] by the alpha males allowing them to mate. Gamma males are the smallest males and mimic juveniles. This also allows them to mate with the females without the alpha males detecting them.<ref name="Shuster">{{cite journal |last=Shuster |first=Stephen |date=May 1987 |title=Alternative Reproductive Behaviors: Three Discrete Male Morphs in Paracerceis sculpta, an Intertidal Isopod from the Northern Gulf of California |journal=Journal of Crustacean Biology |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=318–327 |doi=10.2307/1548612 |jstor=1548612}}</ref> Similarly, among [[common side-blotched lizard]]s, some males mimic the yellow throat coloration and even mating rejection behavior of the other sex to sneak matings with guarded females. These males look and behave like unreceptive females. This [[evolutionary game theory|strategy]] is effective against "usurper" males with orange throats, but ineffective against blue throated "guarder" males, which chase them away.<ref name="S&L1996">{{cite journal |last=Sinervo |first=B. |author2=C. M. Lively| year=1996 |title=The rock–paper–scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies |journal=Nature |volume=380 |issue=6571 |pages=240–243 |doi=10.1038/380240a0 |bibcode=1996Natur.380..240S }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Sinervo | first1=B. | last2=Miles | first2=D. B. | last3=Frankino | first3=W. A. | last4=Klukowski | first4=M. | last5=Denardo | first5=D. F. | year=2000 | title=Testosterone, Endurance, and Darwinian Fitness: Natural and Sexual Selection on the Physiological Bases of Alternative Male Behaviors in Side-Blotched Lizards | url=| journal=Hormones and Behavior | volume=38 | issue=4| pages=222–233 | doi=10.1006/hbeh.2000.1622 | pmid=11104640 }}</ref> Female [[spotted hyena]]s have [[Spotted hyena#Female genitalia|pseudo-penis]]es that make them look like males.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Muller | first1=M. N. | last2=Wrangham | first2=R. | year=2002 | title=Sexual Mimicry in Hyenas | url=| journal=The Quarterly Review of Biology | volume=77 | issue=1| pages=3–16 | doi=10.1086/339199 | pmid=11963460 }}</ref>


===Automimicry===
{{further|Automimicry}}
[[File:Chaetodon capistratus2.jpg|thumb|left|upright|Eyespots of [[foureye butterflyfish]] (''Chaetodon capistratus'') mimic its own eyes, deflecting attacks from the vulnerable head.]]


'''Automimicry''' or '''intraspecific mimicry''' occurs within a single species. One form of such mimicry is where one part of an organism's body resembles another part. For example, the tails of some snakes resemble their heads and they show behavior such as moving backwards when threatened and presenting the predator with the tail, thereby improving their chances of escape without fatal harm. Some species of fishes have [[eyespot (mimicry)|eyespot]]s near their tails, and when mildly alarmed swim slowly backwards, presenting the tail as a head. Some insects too, have tail patterns and appendages of various degrees of sophistication that promote attacks at the rear rather than at the head. Several species of [[pygmy owl]] bear "false eyes" on the back of the head, misleading predators into reacting as though they were the subject of an aggressive stare.<ref>{{cite web|title=NORTHERN PYGMY OWL (Glaucidium californicum)|url=http://www.owlinstitute.org/northern-pygmy-owl.html|publisher=Owl Research Institute|accessdate=23 August 2015}}</ref>
One form of automimicry is where one part of an organism's body resembles another part. For example, the tails of some snakes resemble their heads; they move backwards when threatened and present the predator with the tail, improving their chances of escape without fatal harm. Some fishes have [[eyespot (mimicry)|eyespot]]s near their tails, and when mildly alarmed swim slowly backwards, presenting the tail as a head. Some insects such as some [[Lycaenidae|lycaenid]] butterflies have tail patterns and appendages of various degrees of sophistication that promote attacks at the rear rather than at the head. Several species of [[pygmy owl]] bear "false eyes" on the back of the head, misleading predators into reacting as though they were the subject of an aggressive stare.<ref>{{cite web |title=Northern Pygmy Owl (Glaucidium californicum) |url=http://www.owlinstitute.org/northern-pygmy-owl.html |publisher=Owl Research Institute |access-date=23 August 2015 |archive-date=28 December 2015 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20151228222405/http://www.owlinstitute.org/northern-pygmy-owl.html |url-status=dead}}</ref> Many insects have filamentous "tails" at the ends of their wings and patterns of markings on the wings themselves. These combine to create a "false head". This misdirects predators such as birds and [[Salticidae|jumping spiders]]. Spectacular examples occur in the [[Theclinae|hairstreak]] butterflies; when perching on a twig or flower, they commonly do so upside down and shift their rear wings repeatedly, causing antenna-like movements of the "tails" on their wings. Studies of rear-wing damage support the hypothesis that this strategy is effective in deflecting attacks from the insect's head.<ref>Sourakov, Andrei (2013): Two heads are better than one: false head allows Calycopis cecrops (Lycaenidae) to escape predation by a Jumping Spider, ''Phidippus pulcherrimus'' (Salticidae), Journal of Natural History, 47:15-16, 1047–1054</ref><ref>Robbins, Robert K. The "False Head" Hypothesis: Predation and Wing Pattern Variation of Lycaenid Butterflies. The American Naturalist Vol. 118, No. 5 (Nov., 1981), pp. 770–775</ref>


== Aggressive ==
[[File:Glaucidium californicum Verdi Sierra Pines 2.jpg|thumb|upright|Pygmy owl (''[[Glaucidium californicum]]'') showing eyespots behind head]]


{{Main|Aggressive mimicry}}
Some writers use the term "automimicry" when the mimic imitates other morphs within the same species. For example, in a species where males mimic females or vice versa, this may be an instance of [[sexual mimicry]] in [[Evolutionary game theory#The side-blotched lizard|evolutionary game theory]]. Examples are found in some species of birds, fishes, and lizards.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Plaistow | first1 = Stewart J. | last2 = Johnstone | first2 = Rufus A. | last3 = Colegrave | first3 = Nick | last4 = Spencer | first4 = Matthew | year = 2004 | title = Evolution of alternative mating tactics: conditional versus mixed strategies | url = | journal = Behavioral Ecology | volume = 15 | issue = 4| pages = 534–542 | doi = 10.1093/beheco/arh029 }}</ref> Quite elaborate strategies along these lines are known, such as the well-known "scissors, paper, rock" mimicry in ''[[Common side-blotched lizard|Uta stansburiana]]'',<ref>Schell, Robert & Dettman, Jessica. Ecology and breeding colors of the side-blotched lizard (Uta stansburiana) in the Grand Canyon.[https://watershed.ucdavis.edu/education/classes/files/content/flogs/lizard.pdf]</ref> but there are qualitatively different examples in many other species, such as some ''[[Platysaurus]]''.<ref>Lewis, Belinda Ann. Sexual Selection and Signalling in the Lizard Platysaurus minor. Thesis [http://wiredspace.wits.ac.za/bitstream/handle/10539/4740/Lewis_MSc.pdf]</ref>


=== Predators ===
Many species of insects are toxic or distasteful when they have fed on certain plants that contain chemicals of particular classes, but not when they have fed on plants that lack those chemicals. For instance, some species of the [[subfamily]] [[Milkweed butterfly|Danainae]] feed on various species of the [[Asclepiadoideae]] in the family [[Apocynaceae]], which render them poisonous and emetic to most predators. Such insects frequently are [[Aposematism|aposematically]] coloured and patterned. When feeding on innocuous plants however, they are harmless and nutritious, but a bird that once has sampled a toxic specimen is unlikely to eat harmless specimens that have the same aposematic coloration. When regarded as mimicry of toxic members of the same species, this too may be seen as automimicry.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Svennungsen | first1 = Thomas Owens | last2 = Holen | first2 = Øistein Haugsten | year = 2007 | title = The evolutionary stability of automimicry| url = | journal = Proc. R. Soc. B | volume = 274 | issue = 1621| pages = 2055–2063 | doi = 10.1098/rspb.2007.0456 }}</ref>


[[Aggressive mimicry]] is found in predators or [[parasite]]s that share some of the characteristics of a harmless species, allowing them to avoid detection by their prey or [[host (biology)|host]]; the strategy resembles a [[wolf in sheep's clothing]], though no conscious deceptive intent is involved. The mimic may resemble the prey or host itself, or another organism that does not threaten the prey or host. <ref name=Ecology>Begon, M.; Townsend, C.; Harper, J. (1996) ''[[Ecology: Individuals, populations and communities]]'' (third edition) Blackwell Science, London</ref>
[[File:Mittlerer Weinschwärmer Deilephila elpenor-004.jpg|thumb|left|Larva of elephant hawkmoth (''[[Deilephila elpenor]]'', [[Sphingidae]]), displaying eye-spots when alarmed]]
<!--[[File:Chenille de Deilephila elpenor 02.jpg|thumb|Larva of ''[[Deilephila elpenor]]'', showing eye-spots in unprovoked state]]-->


Several [[spider]]s use aggressive mimicry to lure prey.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Jackson |first1=R. R. |year=1995 |title=Eight-legged tricksters: Spiders that specialize at catching other spiders |journal=BioScience |volume=42 |issue=8 |pages=590–98 |jstor=1311924 |doi=10.2307/1311924}}</ref> Species such as the silver argiope (''[[Argiope argentata]]'') employ prominent patterns in the middle of their webs, such as zigzags. These may reflect ultraviolet light, and mimic the pattern seen in many flowers known as [[nectar guide]]s. Spiders change their web day to day, which can be explained by the ability of bees to remember web patterns.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Craig |first1=C. L. |year=1995 |title=Webs of Deceit |journal=[[Natural History (magazine)|Natural History]] |volume=104 |issue=3 |pages=32–35}}</ref>
Some species of caterpillar, such as many hawkmoths ([[Sphingidae]]), have [[Eyespot (mimicry)|eyespot]]s on their anterior abdominal segments. When alarmed, they retract the head and the thoracic segments into the body, leaving the apparently threatening large eyes at the front of the visible part of the body.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Martin Stevens |year=2005 |title=The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally demonstrated in the Lepidoptera |journal=[[Biological Reviews]] |volume=80 |issue=4 |pages=573–588 |doi=10.1017/S1464793105006810 |pmid=16221330}}</ref>


Another case is where males are lured towards what seems to be a [[sexually receptive]] female. The model in this situation is the same species as the dupe. Female [[firefly|fireflies]] of the genus ''[[Photuris (genus)|Photuris]]'' emit light signals that mimic the mating signals of females of the genus ''[[Photinus (beetle)|Photinus]]''.<ref>Lloyd, J. E. (1965) Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris: Firefly Femmes Fatales Science 149:653–654.</ref> Male fireflies from several different [[genus|genera]] are attracted to these "[[femmes fatales]]", and are captured and eaten. Each female has a repertoire of signals matching the delay and duration of the flashes of the female of the corresponding species.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Lloyd |first=J. E. |year=1975 |title=Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris Fireflies: Signal Repertoires by Femmes Fatales |journal=Science |volume=187 |issue=4175 |pages=452–453 |doi=10.1126/science.187.4175.452 |pmid=17835312 |bibcode=1975Sci...187..452L |s2cid=26761854}}</ref>
[[File:Gray Hairstreak (One more time...) (6222138633).jpg|thumb|upright|Automimicry: gray hairstreak (''[[Strymon melinus]]''), showing the false head at the rear, held upwards at rest]]


Some [[carnivorous plant]]s may be able to increase their rate of capturing insect prey through mimicry.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Moran |first1=Jonathan A. |year=1996 |title=Pitcher dimorphism, prey composition and the mechanisms of prey attraction in the pitcher plant ''Nepenthes rafflesiana'' in Borneo |journal=Journal of Ecology |volume=84 |issue=4 |pages=515–525 |doi=10.2307/2261474 |jstor=2261474 |bibcode=1996JEcol..84..515M}}</ref>
Many insects have filamentous "tails" at the ends of their wings and patterns of markings on the wings themselves. These combine to create a "false head". This misdirects predators such as birds and jumping spiders ([[Salticidae]]). Spectacular examples occur in the [[Theclinae|hairstreak]] butterflies; when perching on a twig or flower, they commonly do so upside down and shift their rear wings repeatedly, causing antenna-like movements of the "tails" on their wings. Studies of rear-wing damage support the hypothesis that this strategy is effective in deflecting attacks from the insect's head.<ref>Andrei Sourakov (2013): Two heads are better than one: false head allows Calycopis cecrops (Lycaenidae) to escape predation by a Jumping Spider, Phidippus pulcherrimus (Salticidae), Journal of Natural History, 47:15-16, 1047-1054</ref><ref>Robbins, Robert K. The "False Head" Hypothesis: Predation and Wing Pattern Variation of Lycaenid Butterflies. The American Naturalist Vol. 118, No. 5 (Nov., 1981), pp. 770-775</ref>


[[File:Epinephelus tukula is cleaned by two Labroides dimidiatus.jpg|thumb|Two [[bluestreak cleaner wrasse]] cleaning a [[potato grouper]], ''Epinephelus tukula'']]
===Others===


A different aggressive strategy is to mimic a mutualistic [[symbiont]] of the prey. [[Cleaner fish]] eat parasites and dead skin from client fish. Some allow the cleaner to venture inside their body to hunt these parasites. However, the sabre-toothed blenny or [[false cleanerfish]] (''Aspidontus taeniatus'') mimics the [[bluestreak cleaner wrasse]] (''Labroides dimidiatus''), which is recognized by other fishes as a cleaner. The false cleanerfish resembles the cleaner, and mimics the cleaner's "dance". Once it is allowed close to the client, it attacks, biting off a piece of its fin before fleeing. Fish wounded in this fashion soon learn to distinguish mimic from model, but because the similarity is close they also become much more cautious of the model.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Wickler |first=W. |author-link=Wolfgang Wickler |year=1966 |title=Mimicry in Tropical Fishes |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences |volume=251 |issue=772 |pages=473–474 |doi=10.1098/rstb.1966.0036 |bibcode=1966RSPTB.251..473W |s2cid=83609965}}</ref>
Some forms of mimicry do not fit easily within the classification given previously.
<!-- if this is not mimicry then it's only tangentially relevant here, probably not worth including?
[[Owl butterflies]] (genus ''Caligo'') bear eye-spots on the underside of their wings; if turned upside-down, their undersides resemble the face of an [[owl]] (such as the [[short-eared owl]] or the [[tropical screech owl]]), which could intimidate butterfly predators such as small [[lizard]]s and birds.<ref>[http://www.livewild.org/CostaRica/Pics/a6024.jpg See here for a photo]</ref> Thus it has been supposed that the eye-spots are a form of Batesian mimicry. However, the pose in which the butterfly resembles an owl's head is not one it normally adopts. Research suggests that eye-spots are not a form of mimicry and do not deter predators because they look like eyes. Rather the conspicuous contrast in the patterns on the wings deter predators.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Stevens | first1 = Martin | last2 = Hardman | first2 = Chloe J. | last3 = Stubbins | first3 = Claire L. | year = | title = Conspicuousness, not eye mimicry, makes ""eyespots"" effective antipredator signals| url = | journal = Behavioral Ecology | volume = 19 | issue = 3| pages = 525–531 | doi = 10.1093/beheco/arm162 }}</ref> -->


A mechanism that does not involve any luring is seen in the [[zone-tailed hawk]], which resembles the [[turkey vulture]]. It flies amongst the vultures, effectively camouflaged as a vulture which poses no threat to the hawk's prey. It hunts by suddenly breaking from the formation and ambushing its prey.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Willis |first=E. O. |year=1963 |title=Is the Zone-Tailed Hawk a Mimic of the Turkey Vulture? |journal=The Condor |volume=65 |issue=4 |pages=313–317 |doi=10.2307/1365357 |jstor=1365357}}</ref>
Floral mimicry is induced by the [[discomycete fungus]] ''[[Monilinia vaccinii-corymbosi]]''.<ref name=Batra>{{cite journal | last1=Batra | first1=L. R. | last2=Batra | first2=S. | year=1985 | title=Floral Mimicry Induced by Mummy-Berry Fungus Exploits Host's Pollinators as Vectors | url=| journal=Science | volume=228 | issue=4702| pages=1011–1013 | doi=10.1126/science.228.4702.1011 | pmid=17797664 |bibcode=1985Sci...228.1011B }}</ref> In this unusual case, a fungal [[plant pathogen]] infects [[leaf|leaves]] of [[blueberries]], causing them to secrete sugars, in effect mimicking the [[nectar]] of flowers. To the naked eye the leaves do not look like flowers, yet they still attract pollinating insects like bees using an ultraviolet signal. This case is unusual, in that the fungus benefits from the deception but it is the leaves that act as mimics, being harmed in the process. It is similar to host-parasite mimicry, but the host does not receive the signal. It has a little in common with automimicry, but the plant does not benefit from the mimicry, and the action of the pathogen is required to produce it.<ref name=Batra/>


==Evolution==
=== Parasites ===


Parasites can be aggressive mimics, though the situation is somewhat different from those outlined previously. They can mimic their hosts' natural prey, allowing themselves to be eaten as a pathway into their host. ''[[Leucochloridium]]'', a genus of [[flatworm]], matures in the digestive system of [[songbird]]s, their eggs then passing out of the bird in the [[faeces]]. They are then taken up by ''[[Succinea]]'', a terrestrial snail. The eggs develop in this [[intermediate host]], and must then find a suitable bird to mature in. Since the host birds do not eat snails, the [[Trematode life cycle stages|sporocyst]] has another strategy to reach its host's intestine. They are brightly coloured and move in a pulsating fashion. A sporocyst-sac pulsates in the snail's eye stalks,<ref>[http://www.semioticon.com/seo/M/images/mimicry_2.jpg See here for a photo.]</ref><ref>{{cite book |last=Moore |first=J. |date=2002 |title=Parasites and the behavior of animals |publisher=[[Oxford University Press]]}}</ref> coming to resemble an irresistible meal for a songbird. In this way, it can bridge the gap between hosts, allowing it to complete its life cycle.<ref name=Wickler/> A nematode (''[[Myrmeconema neotropicum]]'') changes the colour of the abdomen of workers of the canopy ant ''Cephalotes atratus'' to make it appear like the ripe fruits of ''Hyeronima alchorneoides''. It also changes the behaviour of the ant so that the [[gaster (insect anatomy)|gaster]] (rear part) is held raised. This presumably increases the chances of the ant being eaten by birds.<ref>{{cite journal |pmid=18279076 |year=2008 |last1=Yanoviak |first1=S. P. |last2=Kaspari |first2=M. |last3=Dudley |first3=R. |last4=Poinar |first4=G. Jr |title=Parasite-induced fruit mimicry in a tropical canopy ant |volume=171 |issue=4 |pages=536–44 |doi=10.1086/528968 |journal=The American Naturalist |s2cid=23857167 |url=http://www.canopyants.com/2008_AmNat.pdf}}</ref>
It is widely accepted that mimicry [[evolution|evolves]] as a positive adaptation. The lepidopterist and [[novelist]] [[Vladimir Nabokov]] argued that although natural selection might stabilize a "mimic" form, it would not be necessary to create it.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Alexander | first1=Victoria N. | year=2002 | title=Nabokov, Teleology and Insect Mimicry | url=http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/nabokov_studies/summary/v007/7.alexander.html | journal=Nabokov Studies | volume=7 | issue=| pages=177–213 | doi=10.1353/nab.2010.0004 }}</ref>


== Reproductive ==
The most widely accepted model used to explain the evolution of mimicry in butterflies is the two-step hypothesis. The first step involves [[mutation]] in [[modifier gene]]s that regulate a complex cluster of linked genes that cause large changes in morphology. The second step consists of selections on genes with smaller [[phenotype|phenotypic]] effects, creating an increasingly close resemblance. This model is supported by empirical evidence that suggests that a few single point mutations cause large phenotypic effects, while numerous others produce smaller effects. Some regulatory elements collaborate to form a [[supergene]] for the development of butterfly color patterns. The model is supported by computational [[simulation]]s of [[population genetics]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Holmgren | first1=N. M. A. | last2=Enquist | first2=M. | year=1999 | title=Dynamics of mimicry evolution | url=http://www.comp.leeds.ac.uk/biosystems/reading/paper/dynamics.pdf | format=PDF | journal=Biological Journal of the Linnean Society | volume=66 | issue=2| pages=145–158 | doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01880.x }}</ref> The Batesian mimicry in ''Papilio polytes'' is controlled by the ''[[doublesex]]'' gene.<ref>{{cite journal | authors=Kunte, K., Zhang, W., Tenger-Trolander, A., Palmer, D. H., Martin, A., Reed, R. D., ... & Kronforst, M. R. | date=2014 | title=Doublesex is a mimicry supergene | journal=Nature | volume=507 | issue=7491 | pages=229–232 | doi=10.1038/nature13112}}</ref>


Reproductive mimicry occurs when the actions of the dupe directly aid in the mimic's [[reproduction]]. This is common in plants with deceptive flowers that do not provide the reward they seem to offer and it may occur in Papua New Guinea fireflies, in which the signal of ''Pteroptyx effulgens'' is used by ''P. tarsalis'' to form aggregations to attract females.<ref name="Ohba & Shimoyama 2009">{{cite book |last1=Ohba |first1=N. |last2=Shimoyama |first2=Ayu|title=Bioluminescence in Focus - a collection of illuminating essays|year=2009|editor=Meyer-Rochow, V. B. |publisher=Research Signpost; Trivandrum, Kerala, India |pages=229–242}}</ref> Other forms of mimicry have a reproductive component, such as [[Vavilovian mimicry]] involving seeds, vocal mimicry in birds,<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Dalziell |first1=Anastasia H. |last2=Welbergen |first2=Justin A. |last3=Igic |first3=Branislav |last4=Magrath |first4=Robert D. |date=2014-07-30 |title=Avian vocal mimicry: a unified conceptual framework |journal=Biological Reviews |volume=90 |issue=2 |pages=643–668 |doi=10.1111/brv.12129 |pmid=25079896 |s2cid=207101926}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |date=1 September 2008 |title=Vocal mimicry in songbirds |journal=Animal Behaviour |volume=76 |issue=3 |pages=521–528 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.012 |last1=Kelley |first1=Laura A. |last2=Coe |first2=Rebecca L. |last3=Madden |first3=Joah R. |last4=Healy |first4=Susan D. |s2cid=53192695}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Goller |first1=Maria |last2=Shizuka |first2=Daizaburo |date=22 June 2018 |title=Evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry in songbirds |journal=Evolution Letters |volume=2 |issue=4 |pages=417–426 |doi=10.1002/evl3.62 |pmc=6121844 |pmid=30283692}}</ref> and aggressive and Batesian mimicry in brood parasite-host systems.<ref>{{cite book |url=https://www.bloomsbury.com/uk/cuckoo-9781408856567/ |title=Cuckoo: Cheating by Nature |last=Davies |first=Nick |date=2015 |publisher=Bloomsbury |isbn=978-1-4088-5656-7 |access-date=8 November 2018 |archive-date=28 February 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210228094002/https://www.bloomsbury.com/uk/cuckoo-9781408856567/ |url-status=dead }}</ref>
Some mimicry is imperfect. Natural selection drives mimicry only far enough to deceive predators. For example, when predators avoid a mimic that imperfectly resembles a coral snake, the mimic is sufficiently protected.<ref name=Wilson>{{cite journal | author=Wilson, J., Jahner, J., Williams, K., & Forister, M. | date=2013 | title=Ecological and Evolutionary Processes Drive the Origin and Maintenance of Imperfect Mimicry | journal=PLOS ONE | volume=8 | issue=4 | page=1}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | author=Kikuchi, D., & Pfenning, D. | date=2010 | title=Predator Cognition Permits Imperfect Coral Snake Mimicry | journal=The American Naturalist | volume=176 | issue=6 | pages=830–834 | doi=10.1086/657041 | pmid=20950143}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | author=Howse, P. E., & Allen, J. A. | date=1994 | title=Satyric Mimicry: The Evolution of Apparent Imperfection | journal=Proceedings of The Royal Society B | volume=257 | issue=1349 | pages=111–114 | doi=10.1098/rspb.1994.0102}}</ref>


=== Bakerian and Dodsonian ===
[[Convergent evolution]] is an alternative explanation for why organisms such as coral reef fish<ref>{{cite journal |title=Who resembles whom? Mimetic and coincidental look-alikes among tropical reef fishes |last=Robertson |first=D.R. |date=2013 |journal=PLOS ONE |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0054939 |pages=e54939 |volume=8}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |title=Coincidental resemblances among coral reef fishes from different oceans |last=Robertson |first=D. Ross |url=http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00338-015-1309-8 |journal=Coral Reefs |date=2015 |pages=977-977 |volume=34 |issue=3 |doi=10.1007/s00338-015-1309-8}}</ref> and benthic marine [[invertebrate]]s such as [[sponge]]s and [[nudibranch]]s have come to resemble each other.<ref>{{cite book |title=Antipredatory defensive roles of natural products from marine invertebrates |last=Pawlik |first=J.R. |publisher=Springer |year=2012 |isbn= |pages=677–710 |editor-first=E.|editor-last=Fattorusso |editor-last2=Gerwick |editor-first2=W.H. |editor-first3=O. |editor-last3=Taglialatela-Scafati |chapter=12}}</ref><!--Conflict of interest by editor Pawlikj-->


{{main|Mimicry in plants}}
==See also==
{{portal|Evolution|Biology|Ecology|Colours}}


Bakerian mimicry, named after [[Herbert G. Baker]],<ref>[[Herbert G. Baker|Baker, Herbert G.]] 1976. "Mistake" pollination as a reproductive system, with special reference to the Caricaceae. Pp 161–169 in J. Burley and B. T. Styles, eds. {{Clarify|date=July 2013}} ''Variation, breeding, and conservation of tropical trees''. Academic Press, London, U.K.</ref> is a form of automimicry where female [[flower]]s mimic male flowers of their own species, cheating pollinators out of a reward. This reproductive mimicry may not be readily apparent as members of the same species may still exhibit some degree of [[sexual dimorphism]]. It is common in many species of [[Caricaceae]].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bawa |first1=K. S. |year=1980 |title=Mimicry of male by female flowers and intrasexual competition for pollinators in ''Jacaratia dolichaula'' (D. Smith) Woodson (Caricaceae) |journal=[[Evolution (journal)|Evolution]] |volume=34 |issue=3|pages=467–74 |doi=10.2307/2408216 |jstor=2408216 |pmid=28568703}}</ref>
*[[Biomimicry]]

*[[Molecular mimicry]]
In Dodsonian mimicry, named after [[Calaway H. Dodson]], the model belongs to a different species than the mimic.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Dodson |first1=C. H. |last2=Frymire |first2=G. P. |year=1961 |title=Natural pollination of orchids |journal=Missouri Botanical Garden Bulletin |volume=49 |pages=133–39}}</ref> By resembling the model, a flower can lure its pollinators without offering nectar. The mechanism occurs in several orchids, including ''[[Epidendrum ibaguense]]'' which mimics flowers of ''[[Lantana camara]]'' and ''[[Asclepias curassavica]]'', and is pollinated by monarch butterflies and perhaps [[hummingbird]]s.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Boyden |first1=T. C. |year=1980 |title=Floral mimicry by Epidendrurn ibaguense (Orchidaceae) in Panama |journal=Evolution |volume=34 |issue=1|pages=135–36 |doi=10.2307/2408322 |jstor=2408322 |pmid=28563205}}</ref>
*[[Preadaptation]]

*[[Semiotics]]
=== Kirbyan mimicry, brood parasitism ===

{{main|Brood parasitism}}

[[Brood parasitism]] or Kirbyan mimicry is a two species system where a brood parasite mimics its host. [[Cuckoo]]s are a canonical example; the female cuckoo has its offspring raised by a bird of a different species, cutting down the biological mother's [[parental investment]]. The ability to lay eggs that mimic the host eggs is the key [[adaptation]]. The adaptation to different hosts is inherited through the female line in so-called [[Gens (behaviour)|gentes]] (gens, singular). ''Intraspecific'' brood parasitism, where a female lays in a conspecific's nest, as illustrated by the [[goldeneye (duck)|goldeneye]] duck (''Bucephala clangula''), do not involve mimicry<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Andersson |first1=M. |last2=Eriksson |first2=M. O. G. |year=1982 |title=Nest parasitism in Goldeneyes ''Bucephala clangula'': some evolutionary aspects |journal=American Naturalist |volume=120 |pages=1–16 |doi=10.1086/283965 |s2cid=86699716}}</ref> The parasitic butterfly ''[[Phengaris rebeli]]'' parasitizes the ant species ''[[Myrmica schencki]]'' by releasing chemicals that fool the worker ants to believe that the caterpillar larvae are ant larvae. This enables the larvae to be brought directly into the ant's nest.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Barbero |first=Francesca |author2=Thomas, J.A. |author3=Bonelli, S. |author4=Balletto, E. |author5=Schonrogge, K. |title=Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants |journal=Journal of Experimental Biology |year=2009 |volume=212 |pages=4084–4090 |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/40039224 |access-date=28 September 2013 |doi=10.1242/jeb.032912 |pmid=19946088 |issue=Pt 24 |doi-access=free}}</ref>

<gallery mode=packed heights=180>
File:European Cuckoo Mimics Sparrowhawk.jpg|Mimicry in a brood [[parasite]]: [[common cuckoo|Cuckoo]] mimics [[Eurasian sparrowhawk|sparrowhawk]], alarming small birds enough to give time to lay eggs.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Welbergen |first1=Justin A. |last2=Davies |first2=Nicholas B. |date=2011 |title=A parasite in wolf's clothing: hawk mimicry reduces mobbing of cuckoos by hosts |journal=Behavioral Ecology |volume=22 |issue=3 |pages=574–579 |doi=10.1093/beheco/arr008|doi-access=free}}</ref>
File:FinnBirdMimic.jpg|[[Hierococcyx varius|Common hawk-cuckoo]] resembles a predator, the [[shikra]].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Davies |first1=N. B. |last2=Welbergen |first2=J. A. |title=Cuckoo–hawk mimicry? An experimental test |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B |volume=275 |issue=1644 |pages=1817–1822 |year=2008 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.0331 |pmid=18467298 |pmc=2587796}}</ref>
File:Cuckoo Eggs Mimicking Reed Warbler Eggs.JPG|[[Brood parasitism|Egg mimicry]]: cuckoo eggs (larger) mimic many species of host birds' eggs, in this case of [[Eurasian reed warbler|reed warbler]].
</gallery>

=== Pouyannian ===

{{main|Pouyannian mimicry}}

[[File:Dasyscolia_ciliata.jpg|thumb|''[[Dasyscolia ciliata]]'', a scoliid wasp, [[Pseudocopulation|attempting to copulate]] with a flower of the orchid ''[[Ophrys speculum]]'']]

In Pouyannian mimicry, a flower mimics a [[female]] of a certain [[insect]] species, inducing the [[male]]s of that species to try to copulate with the flower. This is much like aggressive mimicry in fireflies, but with a more benign outcome for the pollinator. The mechanism is named after [[Maurice-Alexandre Pouyanne]], who first described the phenomenon.<ref name="Pasteur 1982"/><ref>Correvon H., Pouyanne M. (1916) {{lang|fr|Un curieux cas de mimetisme chez les Ophrydées}}. ''J. Soc. Nat. Hortic. Fr.'' '''17''': 29–31, 41–42, 84.</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Pouyanne |first1=M.-A. |year=1917 |title=La fécondation des ''Ophrys'' par les insectes |journal=Bulletin de la Société d'histoire naturelle de l'Afrique du Nord |volume=8 |pages=1–2}}</ref> It is most common in orchids, which mimic females of the order [[Hymenoptera]] (generally bees and wasps), and may account for around 60% of pollinations. Depending on the morphology of the flower, a pollen sac called a [[pollinium]] is attached to the head or abdomen of the male. This is then transferred to the [[Stigma (flower)|stigma]] of the next flower the male tries to inseminate, resulting in pollination. The mimicry is a combination of visual, [[chemical mimicry|by olfaction]], and by touch.<ref name="Pramanik Dorst Meesters Spaans 2020">{{cite journal |last1=Pramanik |first1=Dewi |last2=Dorst |first2=Nemi |last3=Meesters |first3=Niels |last4=Spaans |first4=Marlies |last5=Smets |first5=Erik |last6=Welten |first6=Monique |last7=Gravendeel |first7=Barbara |display-authors=3 |title=Evolution and development of three highly specialized floral structures of bee-pollinated Phalaenopsis species |journal=EvoDevo |volume=11 |issue=1 |date=2020 |page=16 |pmid=32793330 |pmc=7418404 |doi=10.1186/s13227-020-00160-z |doi-access=free }}</ref>

=== Vavilovian ===

{{Main|Vavilovian mimicry}}

[[File:Secale cereale.jpg|right|thumb|[[Rye]] is a secondary crop, originally being a mimetic weed of [[wheat]].]]

Vavilovian mimicry is found in [[weed]]s that come to share characteristics with a [[Crop|domesticated plant]] through [[natural selection|unintentional selection]].<ref name="Pasteur 1982" /> It is named after Russian [[botanist]] and [[geneticist]] [[Nikolai Vavilov]].<ref name=Vavilov>{{cite journal |last1=Vavilov |first1=N. I. |year=1951 |title=The origin, variation, immunity and breeding of cultivated plants (translation by K. S. Chester) |journal=Chronica Botanica |volume=13 |pages=1–366}}</ref> Selection against the weed may occur either by manually killing the weed, or by separating its seeds from those of the crop by [[winnowing]]. Vavilovian mimicry illustrates unintentional [[selective breeding|selection by man]]. Weeders do not want to select weeds and their seeds that look increasingly like cultivated plants, yet there is no other option. For example, early barnyard grass, ''[[Echinochloa oryzoides]]'', is a weed in [[rice]] fields and looks similar to rice; its seeds are often mixed in rice and have become difficult to separate through Vavilovian mimicry.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Barrett |first1=S. |year=1983 |title=Mimicry in Plants |journal=[[Scientific American]] |volume=257 |issue=3|pages=76–83 |doi=10.1038/scientificamerican0987-76 |bibcode=1987SciAm.257c..76B}}</ref> Vavilovian mimics may eventually be domesticated themselves, as in the case of rye in wheat; Vavilov called these weed-crops ''secondary crops''.<ref name=Vavilov/>

=== Inter-sexual mimicry ===

{{main|Sexual mimicry}}

Inter-sexual mimicry (a type of automimicry, as it is within a single species) occurs when individuals of one sex in a species mimic members of the opposite sex to facilitate [[Alternative mating strategy#Sneaking|sneak mating]]. An example is the three male forms of the marine [[isopod]] ''[[Paracerceis sculpta]]''. Alpha males are the largest and guard a [[Harem (zoology)|harem]] of females. Beta males mimic females and manage to enter the harem of females without being detected by the alpha males allowing them to mate. Gamma males are the smallest males and mimic juveniles. This also allows them to mate with the females without the alpha males detecting them.<ref name="Shuster">{{cite journal |last=Shuster |first=Stephen |date=May 1987 |title=Alternative Reproductive Behaviors: Three Discrete Male Morphs in Paracerceis sculpta, an Intertidal Isopod from the Northern Gulf of California |journal=Journal of Crustacean Biology |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=318–327 |doi=10.2307/1548612 |jstor=1548612}}</ref> Similarly, among [[common side-blotched lizard]]s, some males mimic the yellow throat coloration and even mating rejection behaviour of the other sex to sneak matings with guarded females. These males look and behave like unreceptive females. This [[evolutionary game theory|strategy]] is effective against "usurper" males with orange throats, but ineffective against blue throated "guarder" males, which chase them away.<ref name="S&L1996">{{cite journal |last=Sinervo |first=B. |author2=C. M. Lively |year=1996 |title=The rock–paper–scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies |journal=Nature |volume=380 |issue=6571 |pages=240–243 |doi=10.1038/380240a0 |bibcode=1996Natur.380..240S |s2cid=205026253}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Sinervo |first1=B. |last2=Miles |first2=D. B. |last3=Frankino |first3=W. A. |last4=Klukowski |first4=M. |last5=Denardo |first5=D. F. |year=2000 |title=Testosterone, Endurance, and Darwinian Fitness: Natural and Sexual Selection on the Physiological Bases of Alternative Male Behaviors in Side-Blotched Lizards |journal=Hormones and Behavior |volume=38 |issue=4|pages=222–233 |doi=10.1006/hbeh.2000.1622 |pmid=11104640 |s2cid=5759575}}</ref> Female [[spotted hyena]]s have [[Spotted hyena#Female genitalia|pseudo-penis]]es that make them look like males.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Muller |first1=M. N. |last2=Wrangham |first2=R. |year=2002 |title=Sexual Mimicry in Hyenas |journal=[[The Quarterly Review of Biology]] |volume=77 |issue=1|pages=3–16 |doi=10.1086/339199 |pmid=11963460 |s2cid=43440407 |url=http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:41467428}}</ref>

== See also ==

* [[Biomimicry]]
* [[Chemical mimicry]]
* [[Locomotor mimicry]]
* [[Mimic octopus]]
* [[Molecular mimicry]]
* [[Preadaptation]]
* [[Semiotics]]

== Notes ==


==Notes==
{{notelist}}
{{notelist}}


==References==
== References ==

{{reflist|28em}}
{{reflist|28em}}


==Further reading==
== Further reading ==

* Brower, L.P. (editor) 1988. ''Mimicry and the evolutionary process''. Chicago, the University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-07608-3 (a supplement of volume 131 of the journal ''[[American Naturalist]]'' dedicated to [[E. B. Ford]]).
* {{cite book |editor-last=Brower |editor-first=L. P. |editor-link=Lincoln Brower |year=1988 |title=Mimicry and the evolutionary process |location=Chicago |publisher=University of Chicago Press |isbn=0-226-07608-3 |ref=none}} (a supplement of volume 131 of the journal ''[[American Naturalist]]'' dedicated to [[E. B. Ford]]).
* [[Hugh B. Cott|Cott, H. B.]] (1940) ''[[Adaptive Coloration in Animals]]''. Methuen and Co, London, ISBN 0-416-30050-2
* {{cite book |last1=Carpenter |first1=G. D. Hale |author-link=Geoffrey Douglas Hale Carpenter |last2=Ford |first2=E. B. |author2-link=E.B. Ford |year=1933 |title=Mimicry |publisher=Methuen |location=London |ref=none}} <!--pioneering evolutionary genetics-->
* {{cite journal | last1=Dafni | first1=A. | year=1984 | title=Mimicry and Deception in Pollination | url=| journal=Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics | volume=15 | issue=| pages=259–278 | doi=10.1146/annurev.es.15.110184.001355 }}
* Edmunds, M. 1974. ''Defence in Animals: a survey of anti-predator defences''. Harlow, Essex and New York, Longman. ISBN 0-582-44132-3.
* [[Hugh B. Cott|Cott, H. B.]] (1940) ''[[Adaptive Coloration in Animals]]''. Methuen and Co, London, {{ISBN|0-416-30050-2}}
* {{cite journal | last1=Evans | first1=M. A. | year=1965 | title=Mimicry and the Darwinian Heritage | url=| journal=Journal of the History of Ideas | volume=26 | issue=2| pages=211–220 | doi=10.2307/2708228 }}
* {{cite journal |last=Dafni |first=A. |year=1984 |title=Mimicry and Deception in Pollination |journal=Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics |volume=15 |pages=259–278 |doi=10.1146/annurev.es.15.110184.001355 |ref=none}}
* Edmunds, M. 1974. ''Defence in Animals: a survey of anti-predator defences''. Harlow, Essex and New York, [[Longman]]. {{ISBN|0-582-44132-3}}.
* Owen, D. (1980) ''Camouflage and Mimicry''. Oxford University Press, ISBN 0-19-217683-8.
* {{cite journal | last1=Pasteur | first1=Georges | year=1982 | title=A classificatory review of mimicry systems | url=| journal=Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics | volume=13 | issue=| pages=169–199 | doi=10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125 }}
* {{cite journal |last1=Evans |first1=M. A. |year=1965 |title=Mimicry and the Darwinian Heritage |journal=Journal of the History of Ideas |volume=26 |issue=2 |pages=211–220 |doi=10.2307/2708228 |jstor=2708228 |ref=none}}
* [[Graeme Ruxton|Ruxton, G. D.]]; Speed, M. P.; Sherratt, T. N. (2004). ''Avoiding Attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry''. Oxford, Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-852860-4.
* Owen, D. (1980) ''Camouflage and Mimicry''. [[Oxford University Press]], {{ISBN|0-19-217683-8}}.
* {{cite journal | last1=Wiens | first1=D. | year=1978 | title=Mimicry in Plants | url=| journal=Evolutionary Biology | volume=11 | issue=| pages=365–403 | doi=10.1007/978-1-4615-6956-5_6}}
* {{cite journal |last1=Pasteur |first1=Georges |year=1982 |title=A classificatory review of mimicry systems |journal=Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics |volume=13 |pages=169–199 |doi=10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125 |ref=none}}
* Stevens, M. (2016). ''Cheats and deceits: how animals and plants exploit and mislead''. [[Oxford University Press]], {{ISBN|978-0-19-870789-9}}
* {{cite journal | last1=Vane-Wright | first1=R.I. | year=1976 | title=A unified classification of mimetic resemblances | url=| journal=Biol. J. Linn. Soc. | volume=8 | issue=| pages=25–56 | doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1976.tb00240.x }}
* {{Cite book |last1=Wiens |first1=D. |chapter=Mimicry in Plants |editor=Max K. Hecht |editor2=William C. Steere |editor3=Bruce Wallace |title=Evolutionary Biology |journal=BMC Evolutionary Biology |year=1978 |volume=11 |pages=365–403 |doi=10.1007/978-1-4615-6956-5_6|pmid=22182416 |pmc=3282713 |isbn=978-1-4615-6958-9 |ref=none}}
* [[Wolfgang Wickler|Wickler, W.]] (1968) ''Mimicry in Plants and Animals'' (translated from the German), McGraw-Hill, New York. ISBN 0-07-070100-8.
* {{cite journal |last1=Vane-Wright |first1=R. I. |year=1976 |title=A unified classification of mimetic resemblances |journal=Biol. J. Linn. Soc. |volume=8 |pages=25–56 |doi=10.1111/j.1095-8312.1976.tb00240.x |ref=none}}
* [[Wolfgang Wickler|Wickler, W.]] (1968) ''Mimicry in Plants and Animals'' (translated from the German), McGraw-Hill, New York. {{ISBN|0-07-070100-8}}.


===Children's===
===Children's===

* Hoff, M. K. (2003) ''Mimicry and Camouflage''. Creative Education. Mankato, Minnesota, USA, Great Britain. ISBN 1-58341-237-9.
* Hoff, M. K. (2003) ''Mimicry and Camouflage''. Creative Education. Mankato, Minnesota, USA, Great Britain. {{ISBN|1-58341-237-9}}.


==External links==
==External links==

{{Commons category|Mimicry}}
{{Commons category|Mimicry}}

{{Wikisource|Mimicry in Butterflies}}
{{Americana Poster|Mimicry in Animals}}
{{Americana Poster|Imitation in Animals}}
* [http://www.ucl.ac.uk/~ucbhdjm/courses/b242/Mimic/Mimic.html Warning colour and mimicry] • Lecture outline from [[University College London]]
* [http://www.ucl.ac.uk/~ucbhdjm/courses/b242/Mimic/Mimic.html Warning colour and mimicry] • Lecture outline from [[University College London]]
* [http://www.mprinstitute.org/vaclav/Camouflage.htm Camouflage and Mimicry in Fossils]
* [http://www.mprinstitute.org/vaclav/Camouflage.htm Camouflage and Mimicry in Fossils]
Line 240: Line 294:
{{evo ecol}}
{{evo ecol}}
{{camouflage}}
{{camouflage}}
{{Patterns in nature}}


{{Authority control}}
{{Authority control}}


[[Category:Mimicry|*]]
[[Category:Mimicry|Mimicry]]
[[Category:Polymorphism]]
[[Category:Polymorphism (biology)]]
[[Category:Camouflage]]
[[Category:Camouflage mechanisms]]
[[Category:Warning coloration]]
[[Category:Warning coloration]]

Latest revision as of 05:33, 2 December 2024

Many insects including hoverflies (C, D, E) and the wasp beetle (F) are Batesian mimics of stinging wasps (A, B), which are Müllerian mimics of each other.

In evolutionary biology, mimicry is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. In the simplest case, as in Batesian mimicry, a mimic resembles a model, so as to deceive a dupe, all three being of different species. A Batesian mimic, such as a hoverfly, is harmless, while its model, such as a wasp, is harmful, and is avoided by the dupe, such as an insect-eating bird. Birds hunt by sight, so the mimicry in that case is visual, but in other cases mimicry may make use of any of the senses. Most types of mimicry, including Batesian, are deceptive, as the mimics are not harmful, but Müllerian mimicry, where different harmful species resemble each other, is honest, as when species of wasps and of bees all have genuinely aposematic warning coloration. More complex types may be bipolar, involving only two species, such as when the model and the dupe are the same; this occurs for example in aggressive mimicry, where a predator in wolf-in-sheep's-clothing style resembles its prey, allowing it to hunt undetected. Mimicry is not limited to animals; in Pouyannian mimicry, an orchid flower is the mimic, resembling a female bee, its model; the dupe is the male bee of the same species, which tries to copulate with the flower, enabling it to transfer pollen, so the mimicry is again bipolar. In automimicry, another bipolar system, model and mimic are the same, as when blue lycaenid butterflies have 'tails' or eyespots on their wings that mimic their own heads, misdirecting predator dupes to strike harmlessly. Many other types of mimicry exist.

Etymology

[edit]

Use of the word mimicry dates to 1637. It derives from the Greek term mimetikos, "imitative", in turn from mimetos, the verbal adjective of mimeisthai, "to imitate".[1] "Mimicry" was first used in zoology by the English entomologists William Kirby and William Spence in 1823.[2][3] Originally used to describe people, "mimetic" was used in zoology from 1851.[1]

History

[edit]

Ancient

[edit]

Aristotle wrote in his History of Animals that partridges use a deceptive distraction display to lure predators away from their flightless young:[2][4]

When a man comes by chance upon a young brood [of partridges], and tries to catch them, the hen-bird rolls in front of the hunter, pretending to be lame: the man every moment thinks he is on the point of catching her, and so she draws him on and on, until every one of her brood has had time to escape; hereupon she returns to the nest and calls the young back.

— Aristotle, translated by D'Arcy Wentworth Thompson[4]

The behaviour is recognised as a form of mimicry by biologists.[2]

19th century

[edit]
In 1879, Fritz Müller created the first mathematical model of mimicry to explain why distasteful species should evolve similar appearances.[5]

In 1823, Kirby and Spence, in their book An Introduction to Entomology, used the term "mimicry" informally to depict the way that the structure and coloration of some insects resembled objects in their environments:[2][3]

A jumping bug, very similar to the one figured by Schellenberg, also much resembles the lichens of the oak on which I took it. The spectre tribe (Phasma) go still further in this mimicry, representing a small branch with its spray.[3]

The English naturalist Henry Walter Bates worked for several years on butterflies in the Amazon rainforest. Returning home, he described multiple forms of mimicry in an 1862 paper at the Linnean Society in London,[6] and then in his 1863 book The Naturalist on the River Amazons.[2][7] The term "Batesian mimicry" has since been used in his honour, its usage becoming restricted to the situation in which a harmless mimic gains protection from its predators by resembling a distasteful model.[2] Among the observations in Bates's 1862 paper is the statement:

I was never able to distinguish the Leptalides from the species they imitated, although they belong to a family totally different in structure and metamorphosis from the Heliconidae, without examining them closely after capture.[6]

The German naturalist Fritz Müller also spent many years studying butterflies in the Amazon rainforest. He first published a journal article on mimicry in German in 1878,[8] followed in 1879 by a paper to the Entomological Society of London (translated and presented by Ralph Meldola).[9] He described a situation where different species were each unpalatable to predators, and shared similar, genuine, warning signals. Bates found it hard to explain why this should be so, asking why they should need to mimic each other if both were harmful and could warn off predators on their own. Müller put forward the first mathematical model of mimicry for this phenomenon: if a common predator confuses the two species, individuals in both those species are more likely to survive, as fewer individuals of either species are killed by the predator. The term Müllerian mimicry, named in his honour, has since been used for this mutualistic form of mimicry.[5][10]

Müller wrote that

The resemblance of the genera named [Ituna and Thyridia] is the more worthy of notice since it occurs between insects both belonging to the group of butterflies which are protected by distastefulness. The explanation which applies in ordinary cases of [Batesian] mimicry—and no other has, so far as I know, been offered—cannot obtain for this imitation among protected species.[9]

Overview

[edit]

Evolved resemblance

[edit]
Batesian vs Müllerian mimicry: the former is deceptive, the latter honest.

Mimicry is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. Often, mimicry functions to protect from predators.[11] Mimicry systems have three basic roles: a mimic, a model, and a dupe. When these correspond to three separate species, the system is called disjunct; when the roles are taken by just two species, the system is called bipolar.[2][12] Mimicry evolves if a dupe (such as a predator) perceives a mimic (such as a palatable prey) as a model (the organism it resembles), and is deceived to change its behaviour to the mimic's selective advantage.[13] The resemblances can be via any sensory modality, including any combination of visual, acoustic, chemical, tactile, or electric.[13][14] Mimicry may be to the advantage of both organisms that share a resemblance, in which case it is mutualistic; or it can be to the detriment of one, making it parasitic or competitive. The evolutionary convergence between groups is driven by the selective action of a dupe.[2][15] Birds, for example, use sight to identify palatable insects,[16] whilst avoiding noxious ones. Over time, palatable insects may evolve to resemble noxious ones, making them mimics and the noxious ones models. Models do not have to be more abundant than mimics.[17] In the case of mutualism, each model is also a mimic; all such species can be called "co-mimics".[17] Many harmless species such as hoverflies are Batesian mimics of strongly defended species such as wasps, while many such well-defended species form Müllerian mimicry rings of co-mimics.[2] In the evolution of wasp-like appearance, it has been argued that insects evolve to masquerade wasps since predatory wasps do not attack each other, and that this mimetic resemblance has had the useful side-effect of deterring vertebrate predators.[18]

Mimicry can result in an evolutionary arms race if mimicry negatively affects the model, in which case the model can evolve a different appearance from the mimic.[17]p161 Mimics may have different models for different life cycle stages, or they may be polymorphic, with different individuals imitating different models, as occurs in Heliconius butterflies. Models tend to be relatively closely related to their mimics,[19] but mimicry can be of vastly different species, for example when spiders mimic ants. Most known mimics are insects,[14] though many other examples including vertebrates, plants, and fungi exist.[20][21][22]

Evolutionary explanations

[edit]

It is widely accepted that mimicry evolves as a positive adaptation. The lepidopterist and novelist Vladimir Nabokov however argued that although natural selection might stabilize a "mimic" form, it would not be necessary to create it.[23] The most widely accepted model used to explain the evolution of mimicry in butterflies is the two-step hypothesis. The first step involves mutation in modifier genes that regulate a complex cluster of linked genes that cause large changes in morphology. The second step consists of selections on genes with smaller phenotypic effects, creating an increasingly close resemblance. This model is supported by empirical evidence that suggests that a few single point mutations cause large phenotypic effects, while numerous others produce smaller effects. Some regulatory elements collaborate to form a supergene for the development of butterfly color patterns. The model is supported by computational simulations of population genetics.[24] The Batesian mimicry in Papilio polytes is controlled by the doublesex gene.[25]

Some mimicry is imperfect. Natural selection drives mimicry only far enough to deceive predators. For example, when predators avoid a mimic that imperfectly resembles a coral snake, the mimic is sufficiently protected.[26][27][28]

Convergent evolution is an alternative explanation for why coral reef fish have come to resemble each other;[29][30] the same applies to benthic marine invertebrates such as sponges and nudibranchs.[31]

Living and non-living models

[edit]

In its broadest definition, mimicry can include non-living models. The specific terms masquerade and mimesis are sometimes used when the models are inanimate, and the mimicry's purpose is crypsis.[32][14][2] For example, animals such as flower mantises, planthoppers, comma and geometer moth caterpillars resemble twigs, bark, leaves, bird droppings or flowers.[14][17][33][34] In addition, predators may make use of resemblance to harmless objects in aggressive masquerade, to enable them to approach prey.[35] This wolf in sheep's clothing strategy differs from the more specific resemblance to the prey in aggressive mimicry, where the prey is both model and dupe.[35]

Many animals bear eyespots, which are hypothesized to resemble the eyes of larger animals. They may not resemble any specific organism's eyes, and whether or not animals respond to them as eyes is also unclear.[36][37][38][39] The model is usually another species, except in automimicry, where members of the species mimic other members, or other parts of their own bodies, and in inter-sexual mimicry, where members of one sex mimic members of the other.[17]

Types

[edit]

Many types of mimicry have been described. An overview of each follows, highlighting the similarities and differences between the various forms. Classification is often based on function with respect to the mimic (e.g., avoiding harm). Some cases may belong to more than one class, e.g., automimicry and aggressive mimicry are not mutually exclusive, as one describes the species relationship between model and mimic, while the other describes the function for the mimic (obtaining food). The terminology used has been debated, as classifications have differed or overlapped; attempts to clarify definitions have led to the partial replacement of old terms with new ones.[2][40][41]

Some kinds of mimicry classified by Pasteur 1982[2]
Name No. of
spp.
Function Dupe finds
Model
Deception Description (mimic, model, dupe)
Aristotelian 2 Protective Agreeable Deceptive Brooding bird mimics itself with broken wing, luring predator away from nest[42]
Automimicry 1 or 2 Protective Agreeable Deceptive Multiple forms, e.g. one sex mimics the other, tail mimics head, etc.
Bakerian 2 Reproductive Forbidding Deceptive Female flower resembles male flower, cheating pollinator
Batesian 3 Protective Forbidding Deceptive Palatable mimic resembles distasteful model, deceives dupe
Browerian 2 Protective Forbidding Deceptive Palatable butterfly resembles toxic member of same species
Emsleyan 3 Protective Forbidding Deceptive Deadly snake resembles less deadly species, predators get chance to learn to avoid them
Gilbertian 2 Protective Forbidding Deceptive Host/prey mimics and so repels parasite/predator
Kirbyan 2 Aggressive Agreeable Deceptive Brood parasite adult or egg mimics host which raises the young as its own
Müllerian 3 or more Protective Forbidding Honest Distasteful co-mimics resemble each other, aposematically warning off predators[43]
Pouyannian 2 Reproductive Agreeable Deceptive Plant mimic resembles female bee, deceives male, gets itself pollinated
Vavilovian 3 Reproductive Agreeable Deceptive Mimic resembles crop, deceives farmer
Wasmannian 2 Commensalist Agreeable Deceptive Mimic resembles and deceives ant, lives in ant nest
Wicklerian 2 Aggressive Agreeable Deceptive Predator or parasite resembles and attacks prey or host; parasite may get itself swallowed
Camouflage 2 Protective Uninteresting Deceptive Mimic resembles background (plant parts, or inanimate)

Defensive

[edit]

Mimicry is defensive or protective when organisms are able to avoid harmful encounters by deceiving enemies into treating them as something else.

Batesian

[edit]
Macroxiphus, a harmless bush cricket, mimics a well-defended ant.

In Batesian mimicry, the mimic resembles the model, but does not have the attribute that makes it unprofitable to predators (e.g., unpalatability, or the ability to sting). In other words, a Batesian mimic is a sheep in wolf's clothing. Mimics are less likely to be found out (for example by predators) when in low proportion to their model. Such negative frequency-dependent selection applies in most forms of mimicry. Specifically, Batesian mimicry can only be maintained if the harm caused to the predator by eating a model outweighs the benefit of eating a mimic. The nature of learning is weighted in favor of the mimics, for a predator that has a bad first experience with a model tends to avoid anything that looks like it for a long time, and does not re-sample soon to see whether the initial experience was a false negative. However, if mimics become more abundant than models, then the probability of a young predator having a first experience with a mimic increases. Batesian systems are therefore most likely to be stable where the model is more abundant than the mimic.[44]

There are many Batesian mimics among butterflies and moths. Consul fabius and Eresia eunice imitate unpalatable Heliconius butterflies such as H. ismenius.[45] Limenitis arthemis imitate the poisonous pipevine swallowtail (Battus philenor). Several palatable moths produce ultrasonic click calls to mimic unpalatable tiger moths.[46][47][48][49] Octopuses of the genus Thaumoctopus (the mimic octopus) are able to intentionally alter their body shape and coloration to resemble dangerous sea snakes or lionfish.[50] In the Amazon, the helmeted woodpecker (Dryocopus galeatus), a rare species which lives in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil, Paraguay, and Argentina, has a similar red crest, black back, and barred underside to two larger woodpeckers: Dryocopus lineatus and Campephilus robustus. This mimicry reduces attacks on D. galeatus.[51] Batesian mimicry occurs in the plant kingdom, where the chameleon vine adapts its leaf shape and colour to match that of the plant it is climbing.[52]

Müllerian

[edit]
Comparison of Batesian and Müllerian mimicry, illustrated with a hoverfly, a wasp and a bee

In Müllerian mimicry, two or more species have similar warning or aposematic signals and both share genuine anti-predation attributes (e.g. being unpalatable), as first described in Heliconius butterflies.[53] This type of mimicry is unique in several respects. Firstly, both the mimic and the model benefit from the interaction, which could thus be classified as mutualism. The signal receiver also benefits by this system, despite being deceived about species identity, as it is able to generalize the pattern to potentially harmful encounters. The distinction between mimic and model that is clear in Batesian mimicry is also blurred. Where one species is scarce and another abundant, the rare species can be said to be the mimic. When both are present in similar numbers, however, it makes more sense to speak of each as a co-mimic than of distinct 'mimic' and 'model' species, as their warning signals tend to converge.[43] Also, the mimetic species may exist on a continuum from harmless to highly noxious, so Batesian mimicry grades smoothly into Müllerian convergence.[54][55]

Emsleyan/Mertensian

[edit]

Emsleyan or Mertensian mimicry describes the unusual case where a deadly prey mimics a less dangerous species.[2] It was first proposed by M. G. Emsley in 1966 as a possible explanation for how a predator can learn to avoid a very dangerous aposematic animal, such as a coral snake, when the predator is very likely to die, making learning unlikely.[56] The theory was developed by the German biologist Wolfgang Wickler who named it after the German herpetologist Robert Mertens.[14][57][58][59] The scenario is unlike Müllerian mimicry, where the most harmful species is the model. But if a predator dies on its first encounter with a deadly snake, it has no occasion to learn to recognize the snake's warning signals. There would then be no advantage for an extremely deadly snake in being aposematic: any predator that attacked it would be killed before it could learn to avoid the deadly prey, so the snake would be better off being camouflaged to avoid attacks. But if the predator first learnt to avoid a less deadly warning-coloured snake, the deadly species could profit by mimicking the less dangerous snake.[58][59] Some harmless milk snakes (Lampropeltis triangulum), the moderately toxic false coral snakes (Erythrolamprus aesculapii), and the deadly coral snakes (Micrurus) all have a red background color with black and white/yellow rings. In this system, both the milk snakes and the deadly coral snakes are mimics, while the false coral snakes are the model.[56]

Wasmannian

[edit]

In Wasmannian mimicry, the mimic resembles a model that it lives along with in a nest or colony. Most of the models here are eusocial insects, principally ants.[60][61]

Gilbertian

[edit]
Some Passiflora flower species use Gilbertian mimicry, defending against being eaten by larvae of Heliconius butterflies with leaf stipules (not shown) that resemble the butterfly's eggs.[2][19]

Gilbertian mimicry is bipolar, involving only two species. The potential host (or prey) drives away its parasite (or predator) by mimicking it, the reverse of host-parasite aggressive mimicry. It was coined by Pasteur as a phrase for such rare mimicry systems,[2] and is named after the American ecologist Lawrence E. Gilbert who described it in 1975.[62] The classical instance of Gilbertian mimicry is in the plant genus Passiflora, which is grazed by the micropredator larvae of some Heliconius butterflies. The host plants have evolved stipules that mimic mature Heliconius eggs near the point of hatching. The butterflies avoid laying eggs near existing ones, reducing intraspecific competition between caterpillars, which are also cannibalistic, so those that lay on vacant leaves provide their offspring with a greater chance of survival. The stipules thus appear to have evolved as Gilbertian mimics of butterfly eggs, under selection pressure from these caterpillars.[2][19]

Browerian

[edit]

Browerian mimicry, named after Lincoln P. Brower and Jane Van Zandt Brower who first described it in 1967,[63][64] is a postulated form of automimicry; where the model belongs to the same species as the mimic. This is the analogue of Batesian mimicry within a single species, and occurs when there is a palatability spectrum within a population. Examples include the monarch and the queen from the subfamily Danainae, which feed on milkweed species of varying toxicity. These species store toxins from its host plant, which are maintained even in the adult. As levels of toxin vary depending on diet, some individuals are more toxic than the rest, which profit from the toxicity of those individuals, just as hoverflies benefit from mimicking well-defended wasps.[2]

Misdirection by automimicry

[edit]
Eyespots of foureye butterflyfish (Chaetodon capistratus) mimic its own eyes, deflecting attacks from the vulnerable head.

One form of automimicry is where one part of an organism's body resembles another part. For example, the tails of some snakes resemble their heads; they move backwards when threatened and present the predator with the tail, improving their chances of escape without fatal harm. Some fishes have eyespots near their tails, and when mildly alarmed swim slowly backwards, presenting the tail as a head. Some insects such as some lycaenid butterflies have tail patterns and appendages of various degrees of sophistication that promote attacks at the rear rather than at the head. Several species of pygmy owl bear "false eyes" on the back of the head, misleading predators into reacting as though they were the subject of an aggressive stare.[65] Many insects have filamentous "tails" at the ends of their wings and patterns of markings on the wings themselves. These combine to create a "false head". This misdirects predators such as birds and jumping spiders. Spectacular examples occur in the hairstreak butterflies; when perching on a twig or flower, they commonly do so upside down and shift their rear wings repeatedly, causing antenna-like movements of the "tails" on their wings. Studies of rear-wing damage support the hypothesis that this strategy is effective in deflecting attacks from the insect's head.[66][67]

Aggressive

[edit]

Predators

[edit]

Aggressive mimicry is found in predators or parasites that share some of the characteristics of a harmless species, allowing them to avoid detection by their prey or host; the strategy resembles a wolf in sheep's clothing, though no conscious deceptive intent is involved. The mimic may resemble the prey or host itself, or another organism that does not threaten the prey or host. [68]

Several spiders use aggressive mimicry to lure prey.[69] Species such as the silver argiope (Argiope argentata) employ prominent patterns in the middle of their webs, such as zigzags. These may reflect ultraviolet light, and mimic the pattern seen in many flowers known as nectar guides. Spiders change their web day to day, which can be explained by the ability of bees to remember web patterns.[70]

Another case is where males are lured towards what seems to be a sexually receptive female. The model in this situation is the same species as the dupe. Female fireflies of the genus Photuris emit light signals that mimic the mating signals of females of the genus Photinus.[71] Male fireflies from several different genera are attracted to these "femmes fatales", and are captured and eaten. Each female has a repertoire of signals matching the delay and duration of the flashes of the female of the corresponding species.[72]

Some carnivorous plants may be able to increase their rate of capturing insect prey through mimicry.[73]

Two bluestreak cleaner wrasse cleaning a potato grouper, Epinephelus tukula

A different aggressive strategy is to mimic a mutualistic symbiont of the prey. Cleaner fish eat parasites and dead skin from client fish. Some allow the cleaner to venture inside their body to hunt these parasites. However, the sabre-toothed blenny or false cleanerfish (Aspidontus taeniatus) mimics the bluestreak cleaner wrasse (Labroides dimidiatus), which is recognized by other fishes as a cleaner. The false cleanerfish resembles the cleaner, and mimics the cleaner's "dance". Once it is allowed close to the client, it attacks, biting off a piece of its fin before fleeing. Fish wounded in this fashion soon learn to distinguish mimic from model, but because the similarity is close they also become much more cautious of the model.[74]

A mechanism that does not involve any luring is seen in the zone-tailed hawk, which resembles the turkey vulture. It flies amongst the vultures, effectively camouflaged as a vulture which poses no threat to the hawk's prey. It hunts by suddenly breaking from the formation and ambushing its prey.[75]

Parasites

[edit]

Parasites can be aggressive mimics, though the situation is somewhat different from those outlined previously. They can mimic their hosts' natural prey, allowing themselves to be eaten as a pathway into their host. Leucochloridium, a genus of flatworm, matures in the digestive system of songbirds, their eggs then passing out of the bird in the faeces. They are then taken up by Succinea, a terrestrial snail. The eggs develop in this intermediate host, and must then find a suitable bird to mature in. Since the host birds do not eat snails, the sporocyst has another strategy to reach its host's intestine. They are brightly coloured and move in a pulsating fashion. A sporocyst-sac pulsates in the snail's eye stalks,[76][77] coming to resemble an irresistible meal for a songbird. In this way, it can bridge the gap between hosts, allowing it to complete its life cycle.[14] A nematode (Myrmeconema neotropicum) changes the colour of the abdomen of workers of the canopy ant Cephalotes atratus to make it appear like the ripe fruits of Hyeronima alchorneoides. It also changes the behaviour of the ant so that the gaster (rear part) is held raised. This presumably increases the chances of the ant being eaten by birds.[78]

Reproductive

[edit]

Reproductive mimicry occurs when the actions of the dupe directly aid in the mimic's reproduction. This is common in plants with deceptive flowers that do not provide the reward they seem to offer and it may occur in Papua New Guinea fireflies, in which the signal of Pteroptyx effulgens is used by P. tarsalis to form aggregations to attract females.[79] Other forms of mimicry have a reproductive component, such as Vavilovian mimicry involving seeds, vocal mimicry in birds,[80][81][82] and aggressive and Batesian mimicry in brood parasite-host systems.[83]

Bakerian and Dodsonian

[edit]

Bakerian mimicry, named after Herbert G. Baker,[84] is a form of automimicry where female flowers mimic male flowers of their own species, cheating pollinators out of a reward. This reproductive mimicry may not be readily apparent as members of the same species may still exhibit some degree of sexual dimorphism. It is common in many species of Caricaceae.[85]

In Dodsonian mimicry, named after Calaway H. Dodson, the model belongs to a different species than the mimic.[86] By resembling the model, a flower can lure its pollinators without offering nectar. The mechanism occurs in several orchids, including Epidendrum ibaguense which mimics flowers of Lantana camara and Asclepias curassavica, and is pollinated by monarch butterflies and perhaps hummingbirds.[87]

Kirbyan mimicry, brood parasitism

[edit]

Brood parasitism or Kirbyan mimicry is a two species system where a brood parasite mimics its host. Cuckoos are a canonical example; the female cuckoo has its offspring raised by a bird of a different species, cutting down the biological mother's parental investment. The ability to lay eggs that mimic the host eggs is the key adaptation. The adaptation to different hosts is inherited through the female line in so-called gentes (gens, singular). Intraspecific brood parasitism, where a female lays in a conspecific's nest, as illustrated by the goldeneye duck (Bucephala clangula), do not involve mimicry[88] The parasitic butterfly Phengaris rebeli parasitizes the ant species Myrmica schencki by releasing chemicals that fool the worker ants to believe that the caterpillar larvae are ant larvae. This enables the larvae to be brought directly into the ant's nest.[89]

Pouyannian

[edit]
Dasyscolia ciliata, a scoliid wasp, attempting to copulate with a flower of the orchid Ophrys speculum

In Pouyannian mimicry, a flower mimics a female of a certain insect species, inducing the males of that species to try to copulate with the flower. This is much like aggressive mimicry in fireflies, but with a more benign outcome for the pollinator. The mechanism is named after Maurice-Alexandre Pouyanne, who first described the phenomenon.[2][92][93] It is most common in orchids, which mimic females of the order Hymenoptera (generally bees and wasps), and may account for around 60% of pollinations. Depending on the morphology of the flower, a pollen sac called a pollinium is attached to the head or abdomen of the male. This is then transferred to the stigma of the next flower the male tries to inseminate, resulting in pollination. The mimicry is a combination of visual, by olfaction, and by touch.[94]

Vavilovian

[edit]
Rye is a secondary crop, originally being a mimetic weed of wheat.

Vavilovian mimicry is found in weeds that come to share characteristics with a domesticated plant through unintentional selection.[2] It is named after Russian botanist and geneticist Nikolai Vavilov.[95] Selection against the weed may occur either by manually killing the weed, or by separating its seeds from those of the crop by winnowing. Vavilovian mimicry illustrates unintentional selection by man. Weeders do not want to select weeds and their seeds that look increasingly like cultivated plants, yet there is no other option. For example, early barnyard grass, Echinochloa oryzoides, is a weed in rice fields and looks similar to rice; its seeds are often mixed in rice and have become difficult to separate through Vavilovian mimicry.[96] Vavilovian mimics may eventually be domesticated themselves, as in the case of rye in wheat; Vavilov called these weed-crops secondary crops.[95]

Inter-sexual mimicry

[edit]

Inter-sexual mimicry (a type of automimicry, as it is within a single species) occurs when individuals of one sex in a species mimic members of the opposite sex to facilitate sneak mating. An example is the three male forms of the marine isopod Paracerceis sculpta. Alpha males are the largest and guard a harem of females. Beta males mimic females and manage to enter the harem of females without being detected by the alpha males allowing them to mate. Gamma males are the smallest males and mimic juveniles. This also allows them to mate with the females without the alpha males detecting them.[97] Similarly, among common side-blotched lizards, some males mimic the yellow throat coloration and even mating rejection behaviour of the other sex to sneak matings with guarded females. These males look and behave like unreceptive females. This strategy is effective against "usurper" males with orange throats, but ineffective against blue throated "guarder" males, which chase them away.[98][99] Female spotted hyenas have pseudo-penises that make them look like males.[100]

See also

[edit]

Notes

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Harper, Douglas. "Online Etymology Dictionary". Retrieved 23 February 2022.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s Pasteur, G. (1982). "A Classificatory Review of Mimicry Systems". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 13: 169–199. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125. JSTOR 2097066.
  3. ^ a b c Kirby, William; Spence, William (1823). An Introduction to Entomology. Vol. 2 (3rd ed.). Longman, Hurst, Rees, Orme & Brown. p. 405.
  4. ^ a b Aristotle, History of Animals, book 9, chapter 8.
  5. ^ a b Mallet, James. "Fritz Müller in 1891". Archived from the original on 30 June 2024. Retrieved 18 November 2017.
  6. ^ a b c Bates, Henry W. (1862). "Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera: Heliconidae". Transactions of the Linnean Society. 23 (3): 495–566. doi:10.1111/j.1096-3642.1860.tb00146.x.
  7. ^ Bates, Henry W. (1863). The naturalist on the river Amazons. Murray.
  8. ^ Müller, Fritz (1878). "Ueber die Vortheile der Mimicry bei Schmetterlingen" [On the Advantages of Mimicry in Butterflies]. Zoologischer Anzeiger (in German). 1: 54–55.
  9. ^ a b Müller, Fritz (1879). "Ituna and Thyridia; a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies" (PDF). Proclamations of the Entomological Society of London. 1879. Translated by R. Meldola: 20–29. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2 March 2024.
  10. ^ Sherratt, Thomas (2008). "The Evolution of Müllerian Mimicry". Die Naturwissenschaften. 95 (8): 681–695. Bibcode:2008NW.....95..681S. doi:10.1007/s00114-008-0403-y. PMC 2443389. PMID 18542902.
  11. ^ King, R. C.; Stansfield, W. D.; Mulligan, P. K. (2006). A dictionary of genetics (7th ed.). Oxford University Press. p. 278. ISBN 978-0-19-530762-7.
  12. ^ Kikuchi, D. W.; Pfennig, D. W. (2013). "Imperfect Mimicry and the Limits of Natural Selection". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 88 (4): 297–315. doi:10.1086/673758. PMID 24552099. S2CID 11436992.
  13. ^ a b Dalziell, Anastasia H.; Welbergen, Justin A. (27 April 2016). "Mimicry for all modalities". Ecology Letters. 19 (6): 609–619. Bibcode:2016EcolL..19..609D. doi:10.1111/ele.12602. PMID 27117779.
  14. ^ a b c d e f Wickler, Wolfgang (1968). Mimicry in plants and animals. McGraw-Hill.
  15. ^ Wickler, Wolfgang (1965). "Mimicry and the Evolution of Animal Communication". Nature. 208 (5010): 519–21. Bibcode:1965Natur.208..519W. doi:10.1038/208519a0. S2CID 37649827.
  16. ^ Radford, Benjamin; Frazier, Kendrick (January 2017). "Cheats and Deceits: How Animals and Plants Exploit and Mislead". Skeptical Inquirer. 41 (1): 60.
  17. ^ a b c d e Ruxton, Graeme D.; Sherratt, Thomas N.; Speed, M. P. (2004). Avoiding Attack: the Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals, and Mimicry. Oxford University Press.
  18. ^ Boppré, Michael; Vane-Wright, Richard I.; Wickler, Wolfgang (1 January 2017). "A hypothesis to explain accuracy of wasp resemblances". Ecology and Evolution. 7 (1): 73–81. Bibcode:2017EcoEv...7...73B. doi:10.1002/ece3.2586. PMC 5214283. PMID 28070276.
  19. ^ a b c Campbell, N. A. (1996). Biology (4th ed.). Benjamin Cummings. Chapter 50. ISBN 0-8053-1957-3.
  20. ^ Boyden, T. C. (1980). "Floral mimicry by Epidendrum ibaguense (Orchidaceae) in Panama". Evolution. 34 (1): 135–136. doi:10.2307/2408322. JSTOR 2408322. PMID 28563205.
  21. ^ Roy, B. A. (1994). "The effects of pathogen-induced pseudoflowers and buttercups on each other's insect visitation". Ecology. 75 (2): 352–358. Bibcode:1994Ecol...75..352R. doi:10.2307/1939539. JSTOR 1939539.
  22. ^ Johnson, Steven D.; Schiestl, Florian P. (2016). Floral Mimicry. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-104723-7.
  23. ^ Alexander, Victoria N. (2002). "Nabokov, Teleology and Insect Mimicry". Nabokov Studies. 7: 177–213. doi:10.1353/nab.2010.0004. S2CID 42675699.
  24. ^ Holmgren, N. M. A.; Enquist, M. (1999). "Dynamics of mimicry evolution" (PDF). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 66 (2): 145–158. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01880.x.
  25. ^ Kunte, K.; Zhang, W.; Tenger-Trolander, A.; Palmer, D. H.; Martin, A.; Reed, R. D.; Mullen, S. P.; Kronforst, M. R. (2014). "doublesex is a mimicry supergene". Nature. 507 (7491): 229–232. Bibcode:2014Natur.507..229K. doi:10.1038/nature13112. PMID 24598547. S2CID 4448793.
  26. ^ Wilson, J.; Jahner, J.; Williams, K.; Forister, M. (2013). "Ecological and Evolutionary Processes Drive the Origin and Maintenance of Imperfect Mimicry". PLOS ONE. 8 (4): e61610. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...861610W. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061610. PMC 3625143. PMID 23593490.
  27. ^ Kikuchi, D.; Pfenning, D. (2010). "Predator Cognition Permits Imperfect Coral Snake Mimicry". The American Naturalist. 176 (6): 830–834. doi:10.1086/657041. PMID 20950143. S2CID 35411437.
  28. ^ Howse, P. E.; Allen, J. A. (1994). "Satyric Mimicry: The Evolution of Apparent Imperfection". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 257 (1349): 111–114. Bibcode:1994RSPSB.257..111H. doi:10.1098/rspb.1994.0102. S2CID 84458742.
  29. ^ Robertson, D. Ross (2013). "Who resembles whom? Mimetic and coincidental look-alikes among tropical reef fishes". PLOS ONE. 8 (1): e54939. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...854939R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054939. PMC 3556028. PMID 23372795.
  30. ^ Robertson, D. Ross (2015). "Coincidental resemblances among coral reef fishes from different oceans". Coral Reefs. 34 (3): 977. Bibcode:2015CorRe..34..977R. doi:10.1007/s00338-015-1309-8.
  31. ^ Pawlik, J.R. (2012). "12". In Fattorusso, E.; Gerwick, W.H.; Taglialatela-Scafati, O. (eds.). Antipredatory defensive roles of natural products from marine invertebrates. Springer. pp. 677–710. ISBN 978-90-481-3833-3.
  32. ^ Skelhorn, John; Rowland, Hannah M.; Ruxton, Graeme D. (2010). "The Evolution and Ecology of Masquerade". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 99: 1–8. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2009.01347.x.
  33. ^ Wiklund, Christer; Tullberg, Birgitta S. (September 2004). "Seasonal polyphenism and leaf mimicry in the comma butterfly". Animal Behaviour. 68 (3): 621–627. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.12.008. S2CID 54270418.
  34. ^ Endler, John A. (August 1981). "An Overview of the Relationships Between Mimicry and Crypsis". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 16 (1): 25–31. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x.
  35. ^ a b Pembury Smith, Matilda Q. R.; Ruxton, Graeme D. (2020). "Camouflage in predators". Biological Reviews. 95 (5): 1325–1340. doi:10.1111/brv.12612. hdl:10023/19948.
  36. ^ Stevens, Martin; Hopkins, Elinor; Hinde, William; Adcock, Amabel; Connolly, Yvonne; Troscianko, Tom; Cuthill, Innes C. (November 2007). "Field Experiments on the effectiveness of 'eyespots' as predator deterrents". Animal Behaviour. 74 (5): 1215–1227. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.031. S2CID 53186893.
  37. ^ Stevens, Martin (22 June 2007). "Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 274 (1617): 1457–1464. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0220. PMC 1950298. PMID 17426012.
  38. ^ Stevens, Martin; Stubbins, Claire L.; Hardman, Chloe J. (30 May 2008). "The anti-predator function of 'eyespots' on camouflaged and conspicuous prey". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 62 (11): 1787–1793. doi:10.1007/s00265-008-0607-3. S2CID 28288920.
  39. ^ Hossie, Thomas John; Sherratt, Thomas N. (August 2013). "Defensive posture and eyespots deter avian predators from attacking caterpillar models". Animal Behaviour. 86 (2): 383–389. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.029. S2CID 53263767.
  40. ^ Endler, John A. (1981). "An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 16: 25–31. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x.
  41. ^ Allen, J. A.; Cooper, J. M. (2010). "Crypsis and masquerade". Journal of Biological Education. 19 (4): 268. doi:10.1080/00219266.1985.9654747.
  42. ^ Pasteur cites Aristotle, History of Animals, book 9, chapter 9.
  43. ^ a b Rowland, Hannah M.; Ihalainen, Eira; Lindström, Leena; Mappes, Johanna; Speed, Michael P. (2007). "Co-mimics have a mutualistic relationship despite unequal defences". Nature. 448 (7149): 64–67. doi:10.1038/nature05899. ISSN 0028-0836.
  44. ^ Stearns, S. C.; Hoekstra, Rolf F. (2000). Evolution: An Introduction (5th ed.). Oxford University Press. p. 464. ISBN 978-0-19-854968-0.
  45. ^ Pinheiro, Carlos E. G. (1996). "Palatability and escaping ability in Neotropical butterflies: tests with wild kingbirds (Tyrannus melancholicus, Tyrannidae)". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 59 (4): 351–365. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1996.tb01471.x.
  46. ^ Barber, J. R.; Conner, W. E. (2007). "Acoustic mimicry in a predator–prey interaction". PNAS. 104 (22): 9331–9334. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.9331B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0703627104. PMC 1890494. PMID 17517637.
  47. ^ Barber, Jesse R.; Chadwell, Brad A.; Garrett, Nick; Schmidt-French, Barbara; Conner, William E. (July 2009). "Naïve bats discriminate arctiid moth warning sounds but generalize their aposematic meaning". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (Pt 14): 2141–2148. doi:10.1242/jeb.029991. ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 19561203. S2CID 1303252.
  48. ^ Barber, Jesse R.; Plotkin, David; Rubin, Juliette J.; Homziak, Nicholas T.; Leavell, Brian C.; et al. (21 June 2022). "Anti-bat ultrasound production in moths is globally and phylogenetically widespread". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 119 (25): e2117485119. Bibcode:2022PNAS..11917485B. doi:10.1073/pnas.2117485119. PMC 9231501. PMID 35704762.
  49. ^ Kawahara, Akito Y.; Barber, Jesse R. (19 May 2015). "Tempo and mode of antibat ultrasound production and sonar jamming in the diverse hawkmoth radiation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112 (20): 6407–6412. Bibcode:2015PNAS..112.6407K. doi:10.1073/pnas.1416679112. PMC 4443353. PMID 25941377.
  50. ^ "Mimic Octopus, Thaumoctopus mimicus at MarineBio.org". Archived from the original on 18 July 2017. Retrieved 9 June 2007.
  51. ^ "Deceptive Woodpecker Uses Mimicry to Avoid Competition". AMNH. Retrieved 12 August 2015.
  52. ^ Gianoli, Ernesto (2014). "Leaf Mimicry in a Climbing Plant Protects against Herbivory". Cell. 24 (9): 984–987. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.010. PMID 24768053.
  53. ^ Meyer, A. (2006). "Repeating Patterns of Mimicry". PLOS Biology. 4 (10): e341. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040341. PMC 1617347. PMID 17048984.
  54. ^ Huheey, James E. (1976). "Studies in warning coloration and mimicry VII — Evolutionary consequences of a Batesian–Müllerian spectrum: A model for Müllerian mimicry". Evolution. 30 (1): 86–93. doi:10.2307/2407675. JSTOR 2407675. PMID 28565050.
  55. ^ Benson, W. W. (1977). "On the Supposed Spectrum Between Batesian and Mullerian Mimicry". Evolution. 31 (2): 454–455. doi:10.2307/2407770. JSTOR 2407770. PMID 28563231.
  56. ^ a b Emsley, M. G. (1966). "The mimetic significance of Erythrolamprus aesculapii ocellatus Peters from Tobago". Evolution. 20 (4): 663–64. doi:10.2307/2406599. JSTOR 2406599. PMID 28562911.
  57. ^ Mertens, Robert (1956). "Das Problem der Mimikry bei Korallenschlangen". Zool. Jahrb. Syst (in German). 84: 541–76.
  58. ^ a b Hecht, M. K.; Marien, D. (1956). "The coral snake mimic problem: a reinterpretation". Journal of Morphology. 98 (2): 335–365. doi:10.1002/jmor.1050980207. S2CID 83825414.
  59. ^ a b Sheppard, P. M.; Wickler, Wolfgang (1969). "Review of Mimicry in plants and animals by Wolfgang Wickler". Journal of Animal Ecology. 38 (1): 243. doi:10.2307/2762. JSTOR 2762.
  60. ^ Wasmann, Erich (1894). Kritisches Verzeichniss der myrmecophilin und termitophilen Arthropoden [Critical Inventory of Myrmecophile and Termitophile Arthropods] (in German). Berlin: Felix Dames.
  61. ^ Hölldobler, Bert; Wilson, Edward O. (1990). The Ants. Harvard University Press. pp. 511–514. ISBN 978-0-674-04075-5.
  62. ^ Gilbert, Lawrence E. (1975). "Ecological consequences of a coevolved mutualism between butterflies and plants". In L. E. Gilbert; P. H. Raven (eds.). Coevolution of Animals and Plants. University of Texas Press. pp. 210–240. OCLC 636384400.
  63. ^ Brower, Lincoln P. (1970). "Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain and implications for mimicry theory". In Chambers, K. L. (ed.). Biochemical Coevolution. Corvallis, Oregon, USA: Oregon State Univ. pp. 69–82.
  64. ^ Brower, Lincoln P.; Van Brower, J. V. Z.; Corvino, J. M. (1967). "Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 57 (4): 893–98. Bibcode:1967PNAS...57..893B. doi:10.1073/pnas.57.4.893. PMC 224631. PMID 5231352.
  65. ^ "Northern Pygmy Owl (Glaucidium californicum)". Owl Research Institute. Archived from the original on 28 December 2015. Retrieved 23 August 2015.
  66. ^ Sourakov, Andrei (2013): Two heads are better than one: false head allows Calycopis cecrops (Lycaenidae) to escape predation by a Jumping Spider, Phidippus pulcherrimus (Salticidae), Journal of Natural History, 47:15-16, 1047–1054
  67. ^ Robbins, Robert K. The "False Head" Hypothesis: Predation and Wing Pattern Variation of Lycaenid Butterflies. The American Naturalist Vol. 118, No. 5 (Nov., 1981), pp. 770–775
  68. ^ Begon, M.; Townsend, C.; Harper, J. (1996) Ecology: Individuals, populations and communities (third edition) Blackwell Science, London
  69. ^ Jackson, R. R. (1995). "Eight-legged tricksters: Spiders that specialize at catching other spiders". BioScience. 42 (8): 590–98. doi:10.2307/1311924. JSTOR 1311924.
  70. ^ Craig, C. L. (1995). "Webs of Deceit". Natural History. 104 (3): 32–35.
  71. ^ Lloyd, J. E. (1965) Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris: Firefly Femmes Fatales Science 149:653–654.
  72. ^ Lloyd, J. E. (1975). "Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris Fireflies: Signal Repertoires by Femmes Fatales". Science. 187 (4175): 452–453. Bibcode:1975Sci...187..452L. doi:10.1126/science.187.4175.452. PMID 17835312. S2CID 26761854.
  73. ^ Moran, Jonathan A. (1996). "Pitcher dimorphism, prey composition and the mechanisms of prey attraction in the pitcher plant Nepenthes rafflesiana in Borneo". Journal of Ecology. 84 (4): 515–525. Bibcode:1996JEcol..84..515M. doi:10.2307/2261474. JSTOR 2261474.
  74. ^ Wickler, W. (1966). "Mimicry in Tropical Fishes". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 251 (772): 473–474. Bibcode:1966RSPTB.251..473W. doi:10.1098/rstb.1966.0036. S2CID 83609965.
  75. ^ Willis, E. O. (1963). "Is the Zone-Tailed Hawk a Mimic of the Turkey Vulture?". The Condor. 65 (4): 313–317. doi:10.2307/1365357. JSTOR 1365357.
  76. ^ See here for a photo.
  77. ^ Moore, J. (2002). Parasites and the behavior of animals. Oxford University Press.
  78. ^ Yanoviak, S. P.; Kaspari, M.; Dudley, R.; Poinar, G. Jr (2008). "Parasite-induced fruit mimicry in a tropical canopy ant" (PDF). The American Naturalist. 171 (4): 536–44. doi:10.1086/528968. PMID 18279076. S2CID 23857167.
  79. ^ Ohba, N.; Shimoyama, Ayu (2009). Meyer-Rochow, V. B. (ed.). Bioluminescence in Focus - a collection of illuminating essays. Research Signpost; Trivandrum, Kerala, India. pp. 229–242.
  80. ^ Dalziell, Anastasia H.; Welbergen, Justin A.; Igic, Branislav; Magrath, Robert D. (30 July 2014). "Avian vocal mimicry: a unified conceptual framework". Biological Reviews. 90 (2): 643–668. doi:10.1111/brv.12129. PMID 25079896. S2CID 207101926.
  81. ^ Kelley, Laura A.; Coe, Rebecca L.; Madden, Joah R.; Healy, Susan D. (1 September 2008). "Vocal mimicry in songbirds". Animal Behaviour. 76 (3): 521–528. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.012. S2CID 53192695.
  82. ^ Goller, Maria; Shizuka, Daizaburo (22 June 2018). "Evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry in songbirds". Evolution Letters. 2 (4): 417–426. doi:10.1002/evl3.62. PMC 6121844. PMID 30283692.
  83. ^ Davies, Nick (2015). Cuckoo: Cheating by Nature. Bloomsbury. ISBN 978-1-4088-5656-7. Archived from the original on 28 February 2021. Retrieved 8 November 2018.
  84. ^ Baker, Herbert G. 1976. "Mistake" pollination as a reproductive system, with special reference to the Caricaceae. Pp 161–169 in J. Burley and B. T. Styles, eds. [clarification needed] Variation, breeding, and conservation of tropical trees. Academic Press, London, U.K.
  85. ^ Bawa, K. S. (1980). "Mimicry of male by female flowers and intrasexual competition for pollinators in Jacaratia dolichaula (D. Smith) Woodson (Caricaceae)". Evolution. 34 (3): 467–74. doi:10.2307/2408216. JSTOR 2408216. PMID 28568703.
  86. ^ Dodson, C. H.; Frymire, G. P. (1961). "Natural pollination of orchids". Missouri Botanical Garden Bulletin. 49: 133–39.
  87. ^ Boyden, T. C. (1980). "Floral mimicry by Epidendrurn ibaguense (Orchidaceae) in Panama". Evolution. 34 (1): 135–36. doi:10.2307/2408322. JSTOR 2408322. PMID 28563205.
  88. ^ Andersson, M.; Eriksson, M. O. G. (1982). "Nest parasitism in Goldeneyes Bucephala clangula: some evolutionary aspects". American Naturalist. 120: 1–16. doi:10.1086/283965. S2CID 86699716.
  89. ^ Barbero, Francesca; Thomas, J.A.; Bonelli, S.; Balletto, E.; Schonrogge, K. (2009). "Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants". Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (Pt 24): 4084–4090. doi:10.1242/jeb.032912. PMID 19946088. Retrieved 28 September 2013.
  90. ^ Welbergen, Justin A.; Davies, Nicholas B. (2011). "A parasite in wolf's clothing: hawk mimicry reduces mobbing of cuckoos by hosts". Behavioral Ecology. 22 (3): 574–579. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr008.
  91. ^ Davies, N. B.; Welbergen, J. A. (2008). "Cuckoo–hawk mimicry? An experimental test". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 275 (1644): 1817–1822. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0331. PMC 2587796. PMID 18467298.
  92. ^ Correvon H., Pouyanne M. (1916) Un curieux cas de mimetisme chez les Ophrydées. J. Soc. Nat. Hortic. Fr. 17: 29–31, 41–42, 84.
  93. ^ Pouyanne, M.-A. (1917). "La fécondation des Ophrys par les insectes". Bulletin de la Société d'histoire naturelle de l'Afrique du Nord. 8: 1–2.
  94. ^ Pramanik, Dewi; Dorst, Nemi; Meesters, Niels; et al. (2020). "Evolution and development of three highly specialized floral structures of bee-pollinated Phalaenopsis species". EvoDevo. 11 (1): 16. doi:10.1186/s13227-020-00160-z. PMC 7418404. PMID 32793330.
  95. ^ a b Vavilov, N. I. (1951). "The origin, variation, immunity and breeding of cultivated plants (translation by K. S. Chester)". Chronica Botanica. 13: 1–366.
  96. ^ Barrett, S. (1983). "Mimicry in Plants". Scientific American. 257 (3): 76–83. Bibcode:1987SciAm.257c..76B. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0987-76.
  97. ^ Shuster, Stephen (May 1987). "Alternative Reproductive Behaviors: Three Discrete Male Morphs in Paracerceis sculpta, an Intertidal Isopod from the Northern Gulf of California". Journal of Crustacean Biology. 7 (2): 318–327. doi:10.2307/1548612. JSTOR 1548612.
  98. ^ Sinervo, B.; C. M. Lively (1996). "The rock–paper–scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies". Nature. 380 (6571): 240–243. Bibcode:1996Natur.380..240S. doi:10.1038/380240a0. S2CID 205026253.
  99. ^ Sinervo, B.; Miles, D. B.; Frankino, W. A.; Klukowski, M.; Denardo, D. F. (2000). "Testosterone, Endurance, and Darwinian Fitness: Natural and Sexual Selection on the Physiological Bases of Alternative Male Behaviors in Side-Blotched Lizards". Hormones and Behavior. 38 (4): 222–233. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2000.1622. PMID 11104640. S2CID 5759575.
  100. ^ Muller, M. N.; Wrangham, R. (2002). "Sexual Mimicry in Hyenas". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 77 (1): 3–16. doi:10.1086/339199. PMID 11963460. S2CID 43440407.

Further reading

[edit]

Children's

[edit]
  • Hoff, M. K. (2003) Mimicry and Camouflage. Creative Education. Mankato, Minnesota, USA, Great Britain. ISBN 1-58341-237-9.
[edit]