Jump to content

Problem solving: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
m v2.05 - Repaired 1 link to disambiguation page - (You can help) - Unpredictability
 
(607 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Approaches to problem solving}}
{{redirect|Problem|the genus of skippers|Problema|other uses|problem (disambiguation)}}
{{redirect|Problem|other uses|Problem (disambiguation)}}
'''Problem solving''' consists of using generic or ''ad hoc'' methods, in an orderly manner, for finding solutions to problems. Some of the problem-solving techniques developed and used in [[artificial intelligence]], [[computer science]], [[engineering]], [[mathematics]], or [[medicine]] are related to mental problem-solving techniques studied in [[psychology]].
{{more citations needed|date=September 2018}}
== English Word ==
{{Cognitive}}
'Difficult' is a english word which means tough things.
{{Neuropsychology}}
{{Puzzles}}

'''Problem solving''' is the process of achieving a goal by overcoming obstacles, a frequent part of most activities. Problems in need of solutions range from simple personal tasks (e.g. how to turn on an appliance) to complex issues in business and technical fields. The former is an example of simple problem solving (SPS) addressing one issue, whereas the latter is complex problem solving (CPS) with multiple interrelated obstacles.<ref name="Complex Problem Solving">{{Cite book|date=2014-04-04|editor-last=Frensch|editor-first=Peter A.|editor2-last=Funke|editor2-first=Joachim|title=Complex Problem Solving|publisher=Psychology Press |doi=10.4324/9781315806723|isbn=978-1-315-80672-3}}</ref> Another classification of problem-solving tasks is into well-defined problems with specific obstacles and goals, and ill-defined problems in which the current situation is troublesome but it is not clear what kind of resolution to aim for.<ref name=":0" /> Similarly, one may distinguish formal or fact-based problems requiring [[G factor (psychometrics)|psychometric intelligence]], versus socio-emotional problems which depend on the changeable emotions of individuals or groups, such as [[Emotional intelligence|tactful]] behavior, fashion, or gift choices.<ref name="Blanchard-Fields">{{cite journal |author=[[Fredda Blanchard-Fields|Blanchard-Fields, F.]] |year=2007 |title=Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |volume=16 |issue=1 |pages=26–31 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x |ref=Reference-Blanchard |s2cid=145645352}}</ref>

Solutions require sufficient resources and knowledge to attain the goal. Professionals such as lawyers, doctors, programmers, and consultants are largely problem solvers for issues that require technical skills and knowledge beyond general competence. Many businesses have found profitable markets by recognizing a problem and creating a solution: the more widespread and inconvenient the problem, the greater the opportunity to develop a [[Scalability|scalable]] solution.

There are many specialized problem-solving techniques and methods in fields such as [[science]], [[engineering]], [[business]], [[medicine]], [[mathematics]], [[computer science]], [[philosophy]], and [[societies|social organization]]. The mental techniques to identify, analyze, and solve problems are studied in [[psychology]] and [[cognitive science]]s. Also widely researched are the mental obstacles that prevent people from finding solutions; problem-solving impediments include [[confirmation bias]], [[mental set]], and [[functional fixedness]].

== Definition ==
== Definition ==
The term ''problem-solving'' is used in many disciplines, sometimes with different perspectives, and often with different terminologies. For instance, it is a mental process in [[psychology]] and a computerized process in [[computer science]]. Problems can also be classified into two different types (ill-defined and well-defined) from which appropriate solutions are to be made. Ill-defined problems are those that do not have clear goals, solution paths, or expected solution. Well-defined problems have specific goals, clearly defined solution paths, and clear expected solutions. These problems also allow for more initial planning than ill-defined problems.<ref>[[Daniel Schacter|Schacter, D.L.]] et al. (2009). Psychology, Second Edition. New York: Worth Publishers. pp. 376</ref> Being able to solve problems sometimes involves dealing with pragmatics (logic) and semantics (interpretation of the problem). The ability to understand what the goal of the problem is and what rules could be applied represent the key to solving the problem. Sometimes the problem requires some abstract thinking and coming up with a creative solution.
The term ''problem solving'' has a slightly different meaning depending on the discipline. For instance, it is a mental process in [[psychology]] and a computerized process in [[computer science]]. There are two different types of problems: ill-defined and well-defined; different approaches are used for each. Well-defined problems have specific end goals and clearly expected solutions, while ill-defined problems do not. Well-defined problems allow for more initial planning than ill-defined problems.<ref name=":0">{{cite book|author-link1=Daniel Schacter|last1=Schacter|first1=D.L.|last2=Gilbert|first2= D.T.|last3=Wegner|first3=D.M.|title=Psychology|edition=2nd|location=New York|year=2011|publisher= Worth Publishers|page=376}}</ref> Solving problems sometimes involves dealing with [[pragmatics]] (the way that context contributes to meaning) and [[semantics]] (the interpretation of the problem). The ability to understand what the end goal of the problem is, and what rules could be applied, represents the key to solving the problem. Sometimes a problem requires [[abstract thinking]] or coming up with a creative solution.


Problem solving has two major domains: [[Mathematical problem|mathematical problem solving]] and personal problem solving. Each concerns some difficulty or barrier that is encountered.<ref name="Zimmermann">{{cite conference |first=Bernd |last=Zimmermann |year=2004 |title=On mathematical problem-solving processes and history of mathematics |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/238733375 |conference=ICME 10|location=Copenhagen}}</ref>
=== Psychology ===
In [[psychology]], problem solving refers to a state of desire for reaching a definite 'goal' from a present condition that either is not directly moving toward the goal, is far from it, or needs more [[complexity|complex]] logic for finding a missing description of conditions or steps toward the goal.<ref>"In each case "where you want to be" is an imagined (or written) state in which you would like to be. We might use the term 'Problem Identification' or analysis in order to figure out exactly what the problem is. After we have found a problem we need to define what the problem is. In other words, a distinguished feature of a problem is that there is a ''goal'' to be reached and how you get there is not immediately obvious.", What is a problem? in S. Ian Robertson, Problem solving, Psychology Press, 2001, p. 2.</ref> In [[psychology]], problem solving is the concluding part of a larger process that also includes [[problem finding]] and [[problem shaping]].


=== Psychology ===
Considered the most complex of all [[intelligence|intellectual]] functions, problem solving has been defined as a higher-order [[cognitive]] process that requires the modulation and control of more routine or fundamental skills.<ref name="G&L87">{{Wikicite | id= Goldstein | reference= Goldstein F. C., & Levin H. S. (1987). Disorders of reasoning and problem-solving ability. In M. Meier, A. Benton, & L. Diller (Eds.), ''Neuropsychological rehabilitation''. London: Taylor & Francis Group.}}</ref> Problem solving has two major domains: [[Mathematical problem|mathematical problem solving]] and personal problem solving where, in the second, some difficulty or barrier is encountered.<ref name="Zimmermann">Bernd Zimmermann, [http://www.icme-organisers.dk/tsg18/S12BerndZimmermann.pdf On mathematical problem solving processes and history of mathematics], University of Jena</ref> Further problem solving occurs when moving from a given state to a desired goal state is needed for either [[organism|living organisms]] or an [[artificial intelligence]] [[system]].
Problem solving in psychology refers to the process of finding solutions to problems encountered in life.<ref>{{cite book|first=Donald K.|last=Granvold|chapter=Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy with Adults|editor-first=Jerrold R.|editor-last=Brandell|title=Theory and Practice in Clinical Social Work|year=1997|publisher=Simon and Schuster|isbn=978-0-684-82765-0|pages=[https://books.google.com/books?id=K9Hm0UuFGJ0C&pg=PA189 189]}}</ref> Solutions to these problems are usually situation- or context-specific. The process starts with [[problem finding]] and [[problem shaping]], in which the problem is discovered and simplified. The next step is to generate possible solutions and evaluate them. Finally a solution is selected to be implemented and verified. Problems have an ''end goal'' to be reached; how you get there depends upon problem orientation (problem-solving coping style and skills) and systematic analysis.<ref>{{cite book|chapter=Introduction to the study of problem solving|title=Problem Solving|first=S. Ian|last=Robertson|publisher=Psychology Press|year=2001|isbn=0-415-20300-7}}</ref>


While problem solving accompanies the very beginning of human evolution and especially the history of mathematics,<ref name="Zimmermann"/> the nature of human problem solving processes and methods has been studied by [[psychologist]]s over the past hundred years. Methods of studying problem solving include [[introspection]], [[behaviorism]], [[simulation]], [[computer modeling]], and [[experiment]]. Social psychologists have recently distinguished between independent and interdependent problem-solving.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rubin | first1 = M. | last2 = Watt | first2 = S. E. | last3 = Ramelli | first3 = M. | year = 2012 | title = Immigrants' social integration as a function of approach-avoidance orientation and problem-solving style | url = | journal = International Journal of Intercultural Relations | volume = 36 | issue = | pages = 498–505 | doi = 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.12.009 }}</ref>
Mental health professionals study the human problem-solving processes using methods such as [[introspection]], [[behaviorism]], [[simulation]], [[computer modeling]], and [[experiment]]. Social psychologists look into the person-environment relationship aspect of the problem and independent and interdependent problem-solving methods.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rubin | first1 = M. | last2 = Watt | first2 = S. E. | last3 = Ramelli | first3 = M. | year = 2012 | title = Immigrants' social integration as a function of approach-avoidance orientation and problem-solving style | journal = International Journal of Intercultural Relations | volume = 36 | issue = 4| pages = 498–505 | doi = 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.12.009 | hdl = 1959.13/931119 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> Problem solving has been defined as a higher-order [[cognitive]] process and [[intelligence|intellectual function]] that requires the modulation and control of more routine or fundamental skills.<ref name="G&L87">{{cite book | author1= Goldstein F. C. |author2=Levin H. S. |year=1987 |chapter=Disorders of reasoning and problem-solving ability |editor1=M. Meier |editor2=A. Benton |editor3=L. Diller |title=Neuropsychological rehabilitation |place=London |publisher=Taylor & Francis Group.}}</ref>


Empirical research shows many different strategies and factors influence everyday problem solving.<ref>{{multiref2
=== Clinical psychology ===
|1={{cite book |last1=Vallacher |first1=Robin |last2=M. Wegner |first2=Daniel |chapter=Action Identification Theory |title=Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology |pages=327–348 |doi=10.4135/9781446249215.n17|year=2012 |isbn=978-0-85702-960-7 }}
Simple laboratory-based tasks can be useful solving; however, they usually omit the complexity and [[Valence (psychology)|emotional valence]] of "real-world" problems. In clinical psychology, researchers have focused on the role of emotions in problem solving (D'Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; D'Zurilla & Nezu, 1982), demonstrating that poor emotional control can disrupt focus on the target task and impede problem resolution (Rath, Langenbahn, Simon, Sherr, & Diller, 2004). In this conceptualization, human problem solving consists of two related processes: problem orientation, the motivational/attitudinal/affective approach to problematic situations and problem-solving skills. Working with individuals with frontal lobe injuries, [[Neuropsychology|neuropsychologists]] have discovered that deficits in emotional control and reasoning can be remediated, improving the capacity of injured persons to resolve everyday problems successfully (Rath, Simon, Langenbahn, Sherr, & Diller, 2003).
|2={{cite journal | doi = 10.1080/01650250143000319 | volume=26 | issue=1 | title=Gender differences in older adults' everyday cognitive collaboration | journal=International Journal of Behavioral Development | pages=45–59| pmc=2909137 |pmid=20657668| year=2002 | last1=Margrett | first1=J. A | last2=Marsiske | first2=M }}
|3={{cite journal | doi = 10.1093/geront/gnt118 | pmid=24142914 | volume=54 | issue=1 | title=The Convoy Model: Explaining Social Relations From a Multidisciplinary Perspective | journal=The Gerontologist | pages=82–92| pmc=3894851 | year=2013 | last1=Antonucci | first1=T. C | last2=Ajrouch | first2=K. J | last3=Birditt | first3=K. S }}
}}</ref> [[Rehabilitation psychology|Rehabilitation psychologists]] studying people with frontal lobe injuries have found that deficits in emotional control and reasoning can be re-mediated with effective rehabilitation and could improve the capacity of injured persons to resolve everyday problems.<ref name="Rath2003">{{cite journal |last1=Rath |first1=Joseph F. |last2=Simon |first2=Dvorah |last3=Langenbahn |first3=Donna M. |last4=Sherr |first4=Rose Lynn |last5=Diller |first5=Leonard |title=Group treatment of problem-solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury: A randomised outcome study |journal=Neuropsychological Rehabilitation |year= 2003 |volume=13 |issue=4 |pages=461–488 |doi=10.1080/09602010343000039 |s2cid=143165070 |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247514323}}</ref> Interpersonal everyday problem solving is dependent upon personal motivational and contextual components. One such component is the [[Valence (psychology)|emotional valence]] of "real-world" problems, which can either impede or aid problem-solving performance. Researchers have focused on the role of emotions in problem solving,<ref name=DZurilla>{{multiref2
|1={{cite journal | last1 = D'Zurilla | first1 = T. J. | last2 = Goldfried | first2 = M. R. | year=1971 | title = Problem solving and behavior modification | journal = Journal of Abnormal Psychology | volume = 78 | issue = 1| pages = 107–126 | doi=10.1037/h0031360 | pmid = 4938262 |ref=Reference-DZurilla1971}}
|2={{cite book |author1= D'Zurilla, T. J. |author2=Nezu, A. M. |year=1982 |chapter=Social problem solving in adults |editor=P. C. Kendall |title=Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy |volume=1 |pages=201–274 |place=New York |publisher=Academic Press}}
}}</ref> demonstrating that poor emotional control can disrupt focus on the target task, impede problem resolution, and lead to negative outcomes such as fatigue, depression, and inertia.<ref name="rath2004">{{cite journal |last1=Rath |first1=J. F. |last2=Langenbahn |first2=D. M. |last3=Simon |first3=D |last4=Sherr |first4=R. L. |last5=Fletcher |first5=J. |last6=Diller |first6=L. |year=2004 |title=The construct of problem solving in higher level neuropsychological assessment and rehabilitation*1 |journal=Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology |volume=19 |issue=5 |pages=613–635 |doi=10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.006 |pmid=15271407 |doi-access=free}}</ref> {{clarify|text=In conceptualization, |date=September 2023}}human problem solving consists of two related processes: problem orientation, and the motivational/attitudinal/affective approach to problematic situations and problem-solving skills.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Rath|first1=Joseph F.|last2=Hradil|first2=Amy L.|last3=Litke|first3=David R.|last4=Diller|first4=Leonard|date=2011|title=Clinical applications of problem-solving research in neuropsychological rehabilitation: Addressing the subjective experience of cognitive deficits in outpatients with acquired brain injury.|journal=Rehabilitation Psychology|language=en|volume=56|issue=4|pages=320–328|doi=10.1037/a0025817|pmid=22121939|pmc=9728040 |issn=1939-1544}}</ref> People's strategies cohere with their goals<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hoppmann |first1=Christiane A. |last2=Blanchard-Fields |first2=Fredda |author-link2=Fredda Blanchard-Fields |title=Goals and everyday problem solving: Manipulating goal preferences in young and older adults |journal=Developmental Psychology |year= 2010 |volume=46 |issue=6 |pages=1433–1443 |doi=10.1037/a0020676|pmid=20873926 }}</ref> and stem from the process of comparing oneself with others.


=== Cognitive sciences ===
=== Cognitive sciences ===
Among the first experimental psychologists to study problem solving were the [[Gestalt psychology|Gestaltists]] in [[Germany]], such as [[Karl Duncker]] in ''The Psychology of Productive Thinking'' (1935).<ref name=Duncker>{{cite book | last= Duncker|first=Karl |year=1935 |title=Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens |trans-title=The psychology of productive thinking |location=Berlin |publisher=Julius Springer |language=de}}</ref> Perhaps best known is the work of [[Allen Newell]] and [[Herbert A. Simon]].<ref name="Newell">{{cite book |last1=Newell|first1= Allen |last2=Simon|first2= Herbert A. |year=1972 |title=Human problem solving |location=Englewood Cliffs, N.J. |publisher=Prentice-Hall}}</ref>
The early experimental work of the [[Gestalt psychology|Gestaltist]]s in [[Germany]] placed the beginning of problem solving study (e.g., [[Karl Duncker]] in 1935 with his book ''The psychology of productive thinking''<ref name=Duncker>{{Wikicite | id= Duncker| reference= Duncker, K. (1935). ''Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens'' [The psychology of productive thinking]. Berlin: Julius Springer.}}</ref>). Later this experimental work continued through the 1960s and early 1970s with research conducted on relatively simple (but novel for participants) laboratory tasks of problem solving.<ref>For example Duncker's "X-ray" problem; Ewert & Lambert's "disk" problem in 1932, later known as [[Tower of Hanoi]].</ref><ref>{{Wikicite | id= Mayer| reference= Mayer, R. E. (1992). ''Thinking, problem solving, cognition''. Second edition. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.}}</ref> Choosing simple novel tasks was based on the clearly defined [[optimal solution]]s and their short time for solving, which made it possible for the researchers to trace participants' steps in problem-solving process. Researchers' underlying assumption was that simple tasks such as the [[Tower of Hanoi]] correspond to the main properties of "[[Reality|real world]]" problems and thus the characteristic [[cognitive process]]es within participants' attempts to solve simple problems are the same for "real world" problems too; simple problems were used for reasons of convenience and with the expectation that thought generalizations to more complex problems would become possible. Perhaps the best-known and most impressive example of this line of research is the work by [[Allen Newell]] and [[Herbert A. Simon]].<ref name=Newell>*{{Wikicite | id= Newell| reference= Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). ''Human problem solving''. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.}}</ref> Other experts have shown that the principle of [[Decomposition (computer science)|decomposition]] improves the ability of the problem solver to make good judgment.<ref>{{cite journal|url=http://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/ideas/pdf/armstrong2/DecompositionPrinciple.pdf | title =The Use of the Decomposition Principle in Making Judgments | author = J. Scott Armstrong, William B. Denniston, Jr. and Matt M. Gordon | journal = Organizational Behavior and Human Performance | volume = 14 | pages = 257–263 | year = 1975 | doi=10.1016/0030-5073(75)90028-8}}</ref>


Experiments in the 1960s and early 1970s asked participants to solve relatively simple, well-defined, but not previously seen laboratory tasks.<ref>For example:
=== Computer science and algorithmics ===
* X-ray problem, by {{cite book | last= Duncker|first= Karl |year=1935 |title=Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens |trans-title=The psychology of productive thinking |place=Berlin |publisher=Julius Springer |language=de}}
In [[computer science]] and in the part of [[artificial intelligence]] that deals with algorithms ("algorithmics"), problem solving encompasses a number of techniques known as [[algorithm]]s, [[heuristic]]s, [[root cause analysis]], etc. In these disciplines, problem solving is part of a larger process that encompasses problem determination, [[Data deduplication|de-duplication]], analysis, diagnosis, repair, etc.
* Disk problem, later known as [[Tower of Hanoi]], by {{cite journal | last1=Ewert | first1=P. H. | last2=Lambert | first2=J. F. | title=Part II: The Effect of Verbal Instructions upon the Formation of a Concept | journal=The Journal of General Psychology | publisher=Informa UK Limited | volume=6 | issue=2 | year=1932 | issn=0022-1309 | doi=10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880 | pages=400–413 | url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880 | url-access=subscription | access-date=2019-06-09 | archive-date=2020-08-06 | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200806135752/https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880 | url-status=live }}</ref><ref>{{cite book | last= Mayer|first= R. E. |year=1992 |title=Thinking, problem solving, cognition |edition=Second |location=New York |publisher=W. H. Freeman and Company}}</ref> These simple problems, such as the [[Tower of Hanoi]], admitted [[optimal solution]]s that could be found quickly, allowing researchers to observe the full problem-solving process. Researchers assumed that these model problems would elicit the characteristic [[cognitive process]]es by which more complex "real world" problems are solved.


An outstanding problem-solving technique found by this research is the principle of [[Decomposition (computer science)|decomposition]].<ref>{{cite journal |first1=J. Scott|last1=Armstrong|first2=William B. Jr.|last2=Denniston|first3=Matt M.|last3=Gordon |year=1975|title=The Use of the Decomposition Principle in Making Judgments |url=http://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/ideas/pdf/armstrong2/DecompositionPrinciple.pdf |journal=Organizational Behavior and Human Performance |volume=14 |issue=2 |pages=257–263 |doi=10.1016/0030-5073(75)90028-8 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100620221713/http://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/ideas/pdf/armstrong2/DecompositionPrinciple.pdf |archive-date=2010-06-20 |s2cid=122659209}}</ref>
=== Engineering ===
Problem solving is used in when products or processes fail, so corrective action can be taken to prevent further [[failure]]s. It can also be applied to a product or process prior to an actual fail event, i.e., when a potential problem can be predicted and analyzed, and mitigation applied so the problem never actually occurs. Techniques such as [[Failure mode and effects analysis|Failure Mode Effects Analysis]] can be used to proactively reduce the likelihood of problems occurring.


=== Computer science ===
[[Forensic engineering]] is an important technique of [[failure analysis]] that involves tracing product defects and flaws. Corrective action can then be taken to prevent further failures.


{{Expand section|date=September 2018}}
Reverse engineering attempts to discover the original problem-solving logic used in developing a product by taking it apart.


Much of computer science and [[artificial intelligence]] involves designing automated systems to solve a specified type of problem: to accept input data and calculate a correct or adequate response, reasonably quickly. [[Algorithm]]s are recipes or instructions that direct such systems, written into [[computer program]]s.
Other problem solving tools are [[Linear and Nonlinear Programming]], [[Queuing Systems]], and [[Simulation]].<ref name='MalakootiMCDM'>{{cite book|last1=Malakooti|first1=Behnam|title=Operations and Production Systems with Multiple Objectives|date=2013|publisher=John Wiley & Sons|isbn=978-1-118-58537-5}}</ref>


Steps for designing such systems include problem determination, [[heuristic]]s, [[root cause analysis]], [[Data deduplication|de-duplication]], analysis, diagnosis, and repair. Analytic techniques include linear and nonlinear programming, [[queuing systems]], and simulation.<ref name="MalakootiMCDM">{{cite book |last1=Malakooti |first1=Behnam |title=Operations and Production Systems with Multiple Objectives |year=2013 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons |isbn=978-1-118-58537-5}}</ref> A large, perennial obstacle is to find and fix errors in computer programs: [[debugging]].
== Cognitive sciences: two schools ==
In [[cognitive sciences]], researchers' realization that problem-solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise (e.g. Sternberg, 1995) and that, consequently, findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to problem-solving situations outside the laboratory, has led to an emphasis on real-world problem solving since the 1990s. This emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North America and Europe, however. Whereas North American research has typically concentrated on studying problem solving in separate, natural knowledge domains, much of the European research has focused on novel, complex problems, and has been performed with computerized scenarios (see Funke, 1991, for an overview).


=== Europe ===
=== Logic ===
Formal [[logic]] concerns issues like validity, truth, inference, argumentation, and proof. In a problem-solving context, it can be used to formally represent a problem as a theorem to be proved, and to represent the knowledge needed to solve the problem as the premises to be used in a proof that the problem has a solution.
In Europe, two main approaches have surfaced, one initiated by [[Donald Broadbent]] (1977; see Berry & Broadbent, 1995) in the United Kingdom and the other one by [[Dietrich Dörner]] (1975, 1985; see Dörner & Wearing, 1995) in Germany. The two approaches share an emphasis on relatively complex, semantically rich, computerized laboratory tasks, constructed to resemble real-life problems. The approaches differ somewhat in their theoretical goals and methodology, however. The tradition initiated by Broadbent emphasizes the distinction between cognitive problem-solving processes that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness, and typically employs mathematically well-defined computerized systems. The tradition initiated by Dörner, on the other hand, has an interest in the interplay of the cognitive, motivational, and social components of problem solving, and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain up to 2,000 highly interconnected variables (e.g., Dörner, Kreuzig, Reither & Stäudel's 1983 LOHHAUSEN project; Ringelband, Misiak & Kluwe, 1990). Buchner (1995) describes the two traditions in detail.


The use of computers to prove mathematical theorems using formal logic emerged as the field of [[automated theorem proving]] in the 1950s. It included the use of [[heuristic]] methods designed to simulate human problem solving, as in the [[Logic Theory Machine]], developed by Allen Newell, Herbert A. Simon and J. C. Shaw, as well as algorithmic methods such as the [[Resolution (logic)|resolution]] principle developed by [[John Alan Robinson]].
=== North America ===
In North America, initiated by the work of [[Herbert A. Simon]] on "learning by doing" in [[semantic]]ally rich domains (e.g. [[#Reference-Anzai|Anzai & Simon, 1979]]; [[#Reference-Bhaskar|Bhaskar & Simon, 1977]]), researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural [[knowledge domain]]s – such as physics, writing, or [[chess]] playing – thus relinquishing their attempts to extract a global theory of problem solving (e.g. Sternberg & Frensch, 1991). Instead, these researchers have frequently focused on the development of problem solving within a certain domain, that is on the development of [[expertise]] (e.g. [[#Reference-Anderson|Anderson, Boyle & Reiser, 1985]]; [[#Reference-Chase|Chase & Simon, 1973]]; [[#Reference-Chi|Chi, Feltovich & Glaser, 1981]]).


In addition to its use for finding proofs of mathematical theorems, automated theorem-proving has also been used for [[program verification]] in computer science. In 1958, [[John McCarthy (computer scientist)|John McCarthy]] proposed the [[advice taker]], to represent information in formal logic and to derive answers to questions using automated theorem-proving. An important step in this direction was made by [[Cordell Green]] in 1969, who used a resolution theorem prover for question-answering and for such other applications in artificial intelligence as robot planning.
Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include:
*Reading ([[#Reference-Stanovich|Stanovich & Cunningham, 1991]])
*Writing ([[#Reference-Bryson|Bryson, Bereiter, Scardamalia & Joram, 1991]])
*Calculation ([[#Reference-Sokol|Sokol & McCloskey, 1991]])
*Political decision making ([[#Reference-Voss|Voss, Wolfe, Lawrence & Engle, 1991]])
*Problem Solving for Business ([[#Reference-Cornell|Cornell, 2010]])
*Managerial problem solving ([[#Reference-Wagner|Wagner, 1991]])
*Lawyers' reasoning ([[#Reference-Amsel|Amsel, Langer & Loutzenhiser, 1991]])
*Mechanical problem solving ([[#Reference-Hegarty|Hegarty, 1991]])
*Problem solving in electronics ([[#Reference-Lesgold|Lesgold & Lajoie, 1991]])
*Computer skills ([[#Reference-Kay|Kay, 1991]])
*Game playing ([[#Reference-Frensch|Frensch & Sternberg, 1991]])
*Personal problem solving ([[#Reference-Heppner|Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987]])
*Mathematical problem solving ([[George Pólya|Pólya]], 1945; [[#Reference-Schoenfeld|Schoenfeld, 1985]])
*Social problem solving (D'Zurilla & Goldfreid, 1971; D'Zurilla & Nezu, 1982)
*Problem solving for innovations and inventions: TRIZ (Altshuller, 1973, 1990, 1995)


The resolution theorem-prover used by Cordell Green bore little resemblance to human problem solving methods. In response to criticism of that approach from researchers at MIT, [[Robert Kowalski]] developed [[logic programming]] and [[SLD resolution]],<ref>{{cite journal|last=Kowalski|first=Robert|url=https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/IFIP%2074.pdf|title=Predicate Logic as a Programming Language|journal=Information Processing|volume=74|year=1974|access-date=2023-09-20|archive-date=2024-01-19|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20240119025430/https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/IFIP%2074.pdf|url-status=live}}</ref> which solves problems by problem decomposition. He has advocated logic for both computer and human problem solving<ref>{{cite book|last=Kowalski|first=Robert|url=https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/LogicForProblemSolving.pdf|title=Logic for Problem Solving|series=Artificial Intelligence Series|volume=7|publisher=Elsevier Science Publishing|year=1979|isbn=0-444-00368-1|access-date=2023-09-20|archive-date=2023-11-02|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20231102032823/https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/LogicForProblemSolving.pdf|url-status=live}}</ref> and computational logic to improve human thinking.<ref>{{cite book|last=Kowalski|first=Robert|url=https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/newbook.pdf|title=Computational Logic and Human Thinking: How to be Artificially Intelligent|publisher=Cambridge University Press|year=2011|access-date=2023-09-20|archive-date=2024-06-01|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20240601181910/https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/newbook.pdf|url-status=live}}</ref>
== Characteristics of complex problems ==


=== Engineering ===
As elucidated by [[Dietrich Dörner]] and later expanded upon by [[Joachim Funke]], complex problems have some typical characteristics that can be summarized as follows:
When products or processes fail, problem solving techniques can be used to develop corrective actions that can be taken to prevent further [[failure]]s. Such techniques can also be applied to a product or process prior to an actual failure event—to predict, analyze, and mitigate a potential problem in advance. Techniques such as [[failure mode and effects analysis]] can proactively reduce the likelihood of problems.

In either the reactive or the proactive case, it is necessary to build a causal explanation through a process of diagnosis. In deriving an explanation of effects in terms of causes, [[Abductive reasoning|abduction]] generates new ideas or hypotheses (asking "how?"); [[Deductive reasoning|deduction]] evaluates and refines hypotheses based on other plausible premises (asking "why?"); and [[Inductive reasoning|induction]] justifies a hypothesis with empirical data (asking "how much?").<ref name="Staat">{{cite journal|last=Staat|first=Wim|title=On abduction, deduction, induction and the categories|journal=Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society|volume=29|number=2|year=1993|pages=225–237}}</ref> The objective of abduction is to determine which hypothesis or proposition to test, not which one to adopt or assert.<ref name="Sullivan">{{cite journal|last=Sullivan|first=Patrick F.|title=On Falsificationist Interpretations of Peirce|journal=Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society|volume=27|number=2|year=1991|pages=197–219}}</ref> In the [[Charles S. Peirce|Peircean]] logical system, the logic of abduction and deduction contribute to our conceptual understanding of a phenomenon, while the logic of induction adds quantitative details (empirical substantiation) to our conceptual knowledge.<ref name="Yu">{{cite conference|last=Ho|first=Yu Chong|title=Abduction? Deduction? Induction? Is There a Logic of Exploratory Data Analysis?|year=1994|conference=Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association|location=New Orleans, La.|url=https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED376173.pdf|access-date=2023-09-20|archive-date=2023-11-02|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20231102041717/https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED376173.pdf|url-status=live}}</ref>

[[Forensic engineering]] is an important technique of [[failure analysis]] that involves tracing product defects and flaws. Corrective action can then be taken to prevent further failures.

Reverse engineering attempts to discover the original problem-solving logic used in developing a product by disassembling the product and developing a plausible pathway to creating and assembling its parts.<ref>{{Cite web |url=https://litemind.com/problem-definition/|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170621145314/https://litemind.com/problem-definition/|archive-date=2017-06-21|title=Einstein's Secret to Amazing Problem Solving (and 10 Specific Ways You Can Use It)|website=Litemind|language=en-US|access-date=2017-06-11|date=2008-11-04 |last1=Passuello |first1=Luciano }}</ref>

=== Military science ===
In [[military Science|military science]], problem solving is linked to the concept of "end-states", the conditions or situations which are the aims of the strategy.<ref name="USJFCOM">{{cite web|date=27 October 2009|title=Commander's Handbook for Strategic Communication and Communication Strategy|url=http://www.au.af.mil/au/awc/awcgate/jfcom/cc_handbook_sc_27oct2009.pdf|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110429051434/http://www.au.af.mil/au/awc/awcgate/jfcom/cc_handbook_sc_27oct2009.pdf|archive-date=April 29, 2011|access-date=10 October 2016|publisher=[[United States Joint Forces Command]], Joint Warfighting Center, Suffolk, Va.}}</ref>{{rp|xiii, E-2}} Ability to solve problems is important at any [[military rank]], but is essential at the [[command and control]] level. It results from deep qualitative and quantitative understanding of possible scenarios. ''Effectiveness'' in this context is an evaluation of results: to what extent the end states were accomplished.<ref name="USJFCOM"/>{{rp|IV-24}} ''Planning'' is the process of determining how to effect those end states.<ref name="USJFCOM"/>{{rp|IV-1}}


== Processes ==
*[[Complexity]] (large numbers of items, interrelations and decisions)
Some models of problem solving involve identifying a [[goal]] and then a sequence of subgoals towards achieving this goal. Andersson, who introduced the [[ACT-R]] model of cognition, modelled this collection of goals and subgoals as a [[Stack (abstract data type)|goal stack]] in which the mind contains a stack of goals and subgoals to be completed, and a single task being carried out at any time.<ref name=":1">{{Cite book |last=Robertson |first=S. Ian |title=Problem solving: perspectives from cognition and neuroscience |year=2017 |isbn=978-1-317-49601-4 |edition=2nd |location=London |publisher=Taylor & Francis |oclc=962750529}}</ref>{{rp|page=51}}
**[[enumerability]]
**[[Homogeneity and heterogeneity|heterogeneity]]
**[[:wikt:connectivity|connectivity]] (hierarchy relation, communication relation, allocation relation)
*[[Dynamics (physics)|Dynamics]] (time considerations)
**temporal constraints
**temporal sensitivity
**phase effects
**dynamic [[unpredictability]]
*Intransparency (lack of clarity of the situation)
**commencement opacity
**continuation opacity
*[[Polytely]] (multiple goals)
**inexpressiveness
**opposition
**transience


Knowledge of how to solve one problem can be applied to another problem, in a process known as [[Knowledge transfer|transfer]].{{r|:1|page=56}}
The resolution of complex problems requires a direct attack on each of these characteristics that are encountered
<ref>[http://resolver.scholarsportal.info.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/resolve/02692821/v34i0003/221_pstics]</ref>


== Problem-solving strategies ==
== Problem-solving strategies ==
{{See also|:Category:Problem solving skills}}
Problem-solving are the steps that one would use to find the problem(s) that are in the way to getting to one's own goal. Some would refer to this as the 'problem-solving cycle'. (Bransford & Stein, 1993) In this cycle one will recognize the problem, define the problem, develop a strategy to fix the problem, organize the knowledge of the problem cycle, figure-out the resources at the user's disposal, monitor one's progress, and evaluate the solution for accuracy. Although called a cycle, one does not have to do each step in order to fix the problem, in fact those who don't are usually better at problem solving.{{Citation needed |date=December 2012}} The reason it is called a cycle is that once one is completed with a problem another usually will pop up.
Problem-solving strategies are steps to overcoming the obstacles to achieving a goal. The iteration of such strategies over the course of solving a problem is the "problem-solving cycle".<ref name="Bransford1993">{{cite book | last1=Bransford|first1=J. D. |last2=Stein|first2=B. S | year = 1993 | title = The ideal problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity |edition=2nd |location = New York | publisher = W.H. Freeman. |ref=Reference-Bransford}}</ref>


Common steps in this cycle include recognizing the problem, defining it, developing a strategy to fix it, organizing knowledge and resources available, monitoring progress, and evaluating the effectiveness of the solution. Once a solution is achieved, another problem usually arises, and the cycle starts again.
Blanchard-Fields (2007) looks at problem solving from one of two facets. The first looking at those problems that only have one solution (like mathematical problems, or fact-based questions) which are grounded in psychometric intelligence. The other that is socioemotional in nature and are unpredictable with answers that are constantly changing (like what's your favorite color or what you should get someone for Christmas).


Insight is the sudden [[Aha! moment|a''ha!'']] solution to a problem, the birth of a new idea to simplify a complex situation. Solutions found through insight are often more incisive than those from step-by-step analysis. A quick solution process requires insight to select productive moves at different stages of the problem-solving cycle. Unlike Newell and Simon's formal definition of a ''move problem'', there is no consensus definition of an ''insight problem''.<ref>{{multiref2
The following techniques are usually called ''problem-solving strategies'<ref>[[Wang, Y., & Chiew, V.]] (2010). On the cognitive process of human problem solving. Cognitive Systems Research, 11(1), 81-92.</ref>
|1={{Cite journal|last1=Ash|first1=Ivan K.|last2=Jee|first2=Benjamin D.|last3=Wiley|first3=Jennifer|year=2012|title=Investigating Insight as Sudden Learning|journal=The Journal of Problem Solving|volume=4|issue=2|doi=10.7771/1932-6246.1123|issn=1932-6246|doi-access=free}}
|2={{Cite journal|last1=Chronicle|first1=Edward P.|last2=MacGregor|first2=James N.|last3=Ormerod|first3=Thomas C.|year=2004|title=What Makes an Insight Problem? The Roles of Heuristics, Goal Conception, and Solution Recoding in Knowledge-Lean Problems.|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition|volume=30|issue=1|pages=14–27|doi=10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.14|pmid=14736293|s2cid=15631498|issn=1939-1285|url=https://eprints.lancs.ac.uk/id/eprint/558/2/Chronicle_et_alJEP_LM%26C_03.pdf }}
|3={{Cite journal|last1=Chu|first1=Yun|last2=MacGregor|first2=James N.|year=2011|title=Human Performance on Insight Problem Solving: A Review|journal=The Journal of Problem Solving|volume=3|issue=2|doi=10.7771/1932-6246.1094|issn=1932-6246|doi-access=free}}
}}</ref>


Some problem-solving strategies include:<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Wang | first1=Y. | last2=Chiew | first2=V. | title=On the cognitive process of human problem solving | journal=Cognitive Systems Research | publisher=Elsevier BV | volume=11 | issue=1 | year=2010 | issn=1389-0417 | doi=10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.08.003 | pages=81–92 | s2cid=16238486 | url=https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Patricia_Ryser-Welch/post/Do_Machines_learn/attachment/59d6235b79197b8077981b28/AS:306908018216960@1450183981555/download/61-Elsevier-CogSys-ProblemSolving.pdf}}</ref>
* [[Abstraction]]: solving the problem in a model of the system before applying it to the real system
; [[Abstraction]]: solving the problem in a tractable model system to gain insight into the real system
* [[Analogy]]: using a solution that solves an analogous problem
* [[Brainstorming]]: (especially among groups of people) suggesting a large number of solutions or ideas and combining and developing them until an optimum solution is found
; [[Analogy]]: adapting the solution to a previous problem which has similar features or mechanisms
; [[Brainstorming]]: (especially among groups of people) suggesting a large number of solutions or ideas and combining and developing them until an optimum solution is found
; [[Wiktionary:bypass|Bypasses]]: transform the problem into another problem that is easier to solve, bypassing the barrier, then transform that solution back to a solution to the original problem.
* [[Analysis|Divide and conquer]]: breaking down a large, complex problem into smaller, solvable problems
; [[Critical thinking]]: analysis of available evidence and arguments to form a judgement via rational, skeptical, and unbiased evaluation
* [[Hypothesis testing]]: assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove (or, in some contexts, disprove) the assumption
; [[Divide and conquer algorithm|Divide and conquer]]: breaking down a large, complex problem into smaller, solvable problems
* [[Lateral thinking]]: approaching solutions indirectly and creatively
; [[Help-seeking]]: obtaining external assistance to deal with obstacles
* [[Means-ends analysis]]: choosing an action at each step to move closer to the goal
; [[Hypothesis testing]]: assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove (or, in some contexts, disprove) the assumption
* [[Method of focal objects]]: synthesizing seemingly non-matching characteristics of different objects into something new
; [[Lateral thinking]]: approaching solutions indirectly and creatively
* [[Morphological analysis (problem-solving)|Morphological analysis]]: assessing the output and interactions of an entire system
; [[Means-ends analysis]]: choosing an action at each step to move closer to the goal
* [[Proof (truth)|Proof]]: try to prove that the problem cannot be solved. The point where the proof fails will be the starting point for solving it
; [[Morphological analysis (problem-solving)|Morphological analysis]]: assessing the output and interactions of an entire system
* [[Reduction (complexity)|Reduction]]: transforming the problem into another problem for which solutions exist
; [[Observation]] / [[Question]]: in the [[natural sciences]] an observation is an act or instance of [[noticing]] or perceiving and the acquisition of [[information]] from a [[primary source]]. A question is an [[utterance]] which serves as a request for [[information]].{{citation needed|date=May 2024}}
* [[Research]]: employing existing ideas or adapting existing solutions to similar problems
; [[Proof (truth)|Proof of impossibility]]: try to prove that the problem cannot be solved. The point where the proof fails will be the starting point for solving it
* [[Root cause analysis]]: identifying the cause of a problem
; [[Reduction (complexity)|Reduction]]: transforming the problem into another problem for which solutions exist
* [[Trial-and-error]]: testing possible solutions until the right one is found
; [[Research]]: employing existing ideas or adapting existing solutions to similar problems
; [[Root cause analysis]]: identifying the cause of a problem
; [[Trial-and-error]]: testing possible solutions until the right one is found


== Problem-solving methods ==
== Problem-solving methods ==
{{See also|:Category:Problem solving methods|:Category:Problem structuring methods}}
* APS ([[Applied Problem Solving]])<ref>[[Ivan Fantin]] (2014). Applied Problem Solving. Method, Applications, Root Causes, Countermeasures, Poka-Yoke and A3. How to make things happen to solve problems. Milan, Italy: Createspace, an Amazon company. ISBN 978-1499122282</ref>
* {{annotated link|[[A3 problem solving]]}}
* [[Eight Disciplines Problem Solving]]
* {{annotated link|[[Design thinking]]}}
* [[GROW model]]
* {{annotated link|[[Eight Disciplines Problem Solving]]}}
* [[How to Solve It]]
* {{annotated link|[[GROW model]]}}
* [[Kepner-Tregoe|Kepner-Tregoe Problem Solving and Decision Making]]
* {{annotated link|[[Help-seeking]]}}
* [[OODA loop]] (observe, orient, decide, and act)
* {{annotated link|[[How to Solve It]]}}
* [[PDCA]] (plan–do–check–act)
* {{annotated link|[[Lateral thinking]]}}
* [[RPR Problem Diagnosis]] (rapid problem resolution)
* {{annotated link|[[OODA loop]]}}
* [[TRIZ]] (in [[russian language|Russian]]: ''Teoriya Resheniya Izobretatelskikh Zadatch'', "theory of solving inventor's problems")
* {{annotated link|[[PDCA]]}}
* [[A3 Problem Solving]]
* {{annotated link|[[Root cause analysis]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[RPR problem diagnosis]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[TRIZ]]}}
* [[Scientific method]] – is an [[Empirical evidence|empirical]] method for acquiring [[knowledge]] that has characterized the development of [[science]].
* {{annotated link|[[Swarm intelligence]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[System dynamics]]}}


==Common barriers to problem solving==
==Common barriers{{anchor|Common_barriers_to_problem_solving}}==
Common barriers to problem solving are mental constructs that impede our ability to correctly solve problems. These barriers prevent people from solving problems in the most efficient manner possible. Five of the most common processes and factors that researchers have identified as barriers to problem solving are ''[[confirmation bias]], [[mental set]], [[functional fixedness]], unnecessary constraints, and irrelevant information.''
Common barriers to problem solving include mental constructs that impede an efficient search for solutions. Five of the most common identified by researchers are: [[confirmation bias]], [[mental set]], [[functional fixedness]], unnecessary constraints, and irrelevant information.


===Confirmation bias===
===Confirmation bias===
{{Main|Confirmation bias}}
Within the field of [[science]] there exists a fundamental standard, the [[scientific method]], which outlines the process of discovering facts or truths about the world through unbiased consideration of all pertinent information, and impartial observation of and/or experimentation with that information. According to this theory, one is able to most accurately find a solution to a perceived problem by performing the aforementioned steps. The scientific method is not a process that is limited to scientists, but rather it is one that all people can practice in their respective fields of work as well as in their personal lives. [[Confirmation bias]] can be described as one's unconscious or unintentional corruption of the scientific method. Thus when one demonstrates confirmation bias, he or she is formally or informally collecting data, and then subsequently observing and experimenting with that data in such a way that favors a preconceived notion that may or may not have ''[[motivation]]''.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Nickerson | first1 = R. S. | year = 1998 | title = Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises | url = | journal = Review of General Psychology | volume = 2 | issue = 2| page = 176 | doi = 10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175 }}</ref> Interestingly, research has found that professionals within scientific fields of study also experience confirmation bias. In Andreas Hergovich, Reinhard Schott, and Christoph Burger's experiment conducted online, for instance, it was discovered that professionals within the field of psychological research are likely to view scientific studies that are congruent with their preconceived understandings more favorably than studies that are incongruent with their established beliefs.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hergovich | first1 = Schott | last2 = Burger | first2 = | year = 2010 | title = Biased evaluation of abstracts depending on topic and conclusion: Further evidence of a confirmation bias within scientific psychology | url = | journal = Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues | volume = 29 | issue = 3| pages = 188–209 | doi=10.1007/s12144-010-9087-5}}</ref>
Confirmation bias is an unintentional tendency to collect and use data which favors preconceived notions. Such notions may be incidental rather than motivated by important personal beliefs: the desire to be right may be sufficient motivation.<ref name="Nickerson1998">{{cite journal |year=1998 |title=Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises |journal=Review of General Psychology |volume=2 |issue=2 |page=176 |doi=10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175 |last1=Nickerson |first1=Raymond S.|s2cid=8508954 }}</ref>


Motivation refers to one's desire to defend or find substantiation for beliefs (e.g., religious beliefs) that are important to him or her.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Nickerson | first1 = R. S. | year = 1998 | title = Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises | url = | journal = Review of General Psychology | volume = 2 | issue = 2| pages = 175–220 | doi = 10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175 }}</ref> According to Raymond Nickerson, one can see the consequences of confirmation bias in real life situations, which range in severity from inefficient government policies to genocide. With respect to the latter and most severe ramification of this cognitive barrier, Nickerson argued that those involved in committing genocide of persons accused of [[Witch-hunt|witchcraft]], an atrocity that occurred from the 15th to 17th centuries, demonstrated confirmation bias with motivation. Researcher Michael Allen found evidence for confirmation bias with motivation in school children who worked to manipulate their science experiments in such a way that would produce their hoped for results.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Allen | first1 = | year = 2011 | title = Theory-led confirmation bias and experimental persona | url = | journal = Research in Science & Technological Education | volume = 29 | issue = 1| pages = 107–127 | doi=10.1080/02635143.2010.539973}}</ref> However, confirmation bias does not necessarily require motivation. In 1960, [[Peter Cathcart Wason]] conducted an experiment in which participants first viewed three numbers and then created a hypothesis that proposed a rule that could have been used to create that triplet of numbers. When testing their hypotheses, participants tended to only create additional triplets of numbers that would confirm their hypotheses, and tended not to create triplets that would negate or disprove their hypotheses. Thus research also shows that people can and do work to confirm theories or ideas that do not support or engage personally significant beliefs.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Wason | first1 = P. C. | year = 1960 | title = On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task | url = | journal = Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 12 | issue = | pages = 129–140 | doi=10.1080/17470216008416717}}</ref>
Scientific and technical professionals also experience confirmation bias. One online experiment, for example, suggested that professionals within the field of psychological research are likely to view scientific studies that agree with their preconceived notions more favorably than clashing studies.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Hergovich | first1=Andreas | last2=Schott | first2=Reinhard | last3=Burger | first3=Christoph | title=Biased Evaluation of Abstracts Depending on Topic and Conclusion: Further Evidence of a Confirmation Bias Within Scientific Psychology | journal=Current Psychology | publisher=Springer Science and Business Media LLC | volume=29 | issue=3 | year= 2010 | issn=1046-1310 | doi=10.1007/s12144-010-9087-5 | pages=188–209| s2cid=145497196 }}</ref> According to Raymond Nickerson, one can see the consequences of confirmation bias in real-life situations, which range in severity from inefficient government policies to genocide. Nickerson argued that those who killed people accused of [[Witch-hunt|witchcraft]] demonstrated confirmation bias with motivation.{{cn|reason=|date=September 2023}} Researcher Michael Allen found evidence for confirmation bias with motivation in school children who worked to manipulate their science experiments to produce favorable results.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Allen | first=Michael | title=Theory-led confirmation bias and experimental persona | journal=Research in Science & Technological Education | publisher=Informa UK Limited | volume=29 | issue=1 | year=2011 | issn=0263-5143 | doi=10.1080/02635143.2010.539973|bibcode=2011RSTEd..29..107A | pages=107–127| s2cid=145706148 }}</ref>

However, confirmation bias does not necessarily require motivation. In 1960, [[Peter Cathcart Wason]] conducted an experiment in which participants first viewed three numbers and then created a hypothesis in the form of a rule that could have been used to create that triplet of numbers. When testing their hypotheses, participants tended to only create additional triplets of numbers that would confirm their hypotheses, and tended not to create triplets that would negate or disprove their hypotheses.<ref>{{cite journal|year=1960|title=On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task|journal=Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology|volume=12|issue=3|pages=129–140|doi=10.1080/17470216008416717|last1=Wason|first1=P. C.|s2cid=19237642}}
</ref>


===Mental set===
===Mental set===
{{Main|Mental set}}
Mental set was first articulated by [[Abraham S. Luchins|Abraham Luchins]] in the 1940s and demonstrated in his well-known water jug experiments.<ref>Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54 (Whole No. 248).</ref> In these experiments, participants were asked to fill one jug with a specific amount of water using only other jugs (typically three) with different maximum capacities as tools. After Luchins gave his participants a set of water jug problems that could all be solved by employing a single technique, he would then give them a problem that could either be solved using that same technique or a novel and simpler method. Luchins discovered that his participants tended to use the same technique that they had become accustomed to despite the possibility of using a simpler alternative.<ref>Öllinger, Jones, & Knoblich (2008). Investigating the effect of mental set on insight problem solving. ''Experimental Psychology',' 55(4), 269–270.</ref> Thus mental set describes one's inclination to attempt to solve problems in such a way that has proved successful in previous experiences. However, as Luchins' work revealed, such methods for finding a solution that have worked in the past may not be adequate or optimal for certain new but similar problems. Therefore, it is often necessary for people to move beyond their mental sets in order to find solutions. This was again demonstrated in [[Norman Maier]]'s 1931 experiment, which challenged participants to solve a problem by using a household object (pliers) in an unconventional manner. Maier observed that participants were often unable to view the object in a way that strayed from its typical use, a phenomenon regarded as a particular form of mental set (more specifically known as functional fixedness, which is the topic of the following section). When people cling rigidly to their mental sets, they are said to be experiencing ''fixation'', a seeming obsession or preoccupation with attempted strategies that are repeatedly unsuccessful.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Wiley | first1 = J | year = 1998 | title = Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving | url = | journal = Memory & Cognition | volume = 24 | issue = 4| pages = 716–730 | doi=10.3758/bf03211392}}</ref> In the late 1990s, researcher Jennifer Wiley worked to reveal that expertise can work to create a mental set in persons considered to be experts in certain fields, and she furthermore gained evidence that the mental set created by expertise could lead to the development of fixation.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Wiley | first1 = J | year = 1998 | title = Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving | url = http://search.ebscohost.com.ezproxy.biola.edu/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=1998-10386-011&login.asp&site=ehost-live | journal = Memory & Cognition | volume = 24 | issue = 4| pages = 716–730 | doi=10.3758/bf03211392}}</ref>
Mental set is the inclination to re-use a previously successful solution, rather than search for new and better solutions. It is a reliance on habit.

It was first articulated by [[Abraham S. Luchins]] in the 1940s with his well-known water jug experiments.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Luchins|first=Abraham S.|year=1942|title=Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung|journal=Psychological Monographs|volume=54|number=248|pages=i-95 |doi=10.1037/h0093502 }}</ref> Participants were asked to fill one jug with a specific amount of water by using other jugs with different maximum capacities. After Luchins gave a set of jug problems that could all be solved by a single technique, he then introduced a problem that could be solved by the same technique, but also by a novel and simpler method. His participants tended to use the accustomed technique, oblivious of the simpler alternative.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Öllinger | first1=Michael | last2=Jones | first2=Gary | last3=Knoblich | first3=Günther | title=Investigating the Effect of Mental Set on Insight Problem Solving | journal=Experimental Psychology | publisher=Hogrefe Publishing Group | volume=55 | issue=4 | year=2008 | issn=1618-3169 | doi=10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.269 | pages=269–282 | pmid=18683624 | url=http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/23048/1/193183_1563%20Jones%20Postprint.pdf | access-date=2023-01-31 | archive-date=2023-03-16 | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230316064717/http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/23048/1/193183_1563%20Jones%20Postprint.pdf | url-status=live }}</ref> This was again demonstrated in [[Norman Maier]]'s 1931 experiment, which challenged participants to solve a problem by using a familiar tool (pliers) in an unconventional manner. Participants were often unable to view the object in a way that strayed from its typical use, a type of mental set known as functional fixedness (see the following section).

Rigidly clinging to a mental set is called ''fixation'', which can deepen to an obsession or preoccupation with attempted strategies that are repeatedly unsuccessful.<ref name="Wiley1998">{{cite journal|year=1998|title=Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving|journal=Memory & Cognition|volume=24|issue=4|pages=716–730|doi=10.3758/bf03211392|pmid=9701964|last1=Wiley|first1=Jennifer|doi-access=free}}</ref> In the late 1990s, researcher Jennifer Wiley found that professional expertise in a field can create a mental set, perhaps leading to fixation.<ref name="Wiley1998" />

[[Groupthink]], in which each individual takes on the mindset of the rest of the group, can produce and exacerbate mental set.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Cottam|first1=Martha L.|last2=Dietz-Uhler|first2=Beth|last3=Mastors|first3=Elena|last4=Preston|first4=Thomas|year=2010|title=Introduction to Political Psychology|edition=2nd|location=New York|publisher=Psychology Press}}</ref> Social pressure leads to everybody thinking the same thing and reaching the same conclusions.


===Functional fixedness===
===Functional fixedness===
{{Main|Functional fixedness}}
[[Functional fixedness]] is a specific form of mental set and fixation, which was alluded to earlier in the Maier experiment, and furthermore it is another way in which cognitive bias can be seen throughout daily life. Tim German and Clark Barrett describe this barrier as the fixed design of an object hindering the individual's ability to see it serving other functions. In more technical terms, these researchers explained that "[s]ubjects become "fixed" on the design function of the objects, and problem solving suffers relative to control conditions in which the object's function is not demonstrated."<ref>German, Tim, P., and Barrett, Clark., H. Functional fixedness in a technologically sparse culture. University of California, Santa Barbara. American psychological society. 16 (1), 2005.</ref> Functional fixedness is defined as only having that primary function of the object itself hinder the ability of it serving another purpose other than its original function. In research that highlighted the primary reasons that young children are immune to functional fixedness, it was stated that "functional fixedness...[is when]subjects are hindered in reaching the solution to a problem by their knowledge of an object's conventional function."<ref>German, Tim, P., Defeyter, Margaret A. Immunity to functional fixedness in young children. University of Essex, Colchester, England. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 7 (4), 2000.</ref> Furthermore, it is important to note that functional fixedness can be easily expressed in commonplace situations. For instance, imagine the following situation: a man sees a bug on the floor that he wants to kill, but the only thing in his hand at the moment is a can of air freshener. If the man starts looking around for something in the house to kill the bug with instead of realizing that the can of air freshener could in fact be used not only as having its main function as to freshen the air, he is said to be experiencing functional fixedness. The man's knowledge of the can being served as purely an air freshener hindered his ability to realize that it too could have been used to serve another purpose, which in this instance was as an instrument to kill the bug. Functional fixedness can happen on multiple occasions and can cause us to have certain cognitive biases. If we only see an object as serving one primary focus than we fail to realize that the object can be used in various ways other than its intended purpose. This can in turn cause many issues with regards to problem solving. Common sense seems to be a plausible answer to functional fixedness. One could make this argument because it seems rather simple to consider possible alternative uses for an object. Perhaps using common sense to solve this issue could be the most accurate answer within this context. With the previous stated example, it seems as if it would make perfect sense to use the can of air freshener to kill the bug rather than to search for something else to serve that function but, as research shows, this is often not the case.


Functional fixedness is the tendency to view an object as having only one function, and to be unable to conceive of any novel use, as in the Maier pliers experiment described above. Functional fixedness is a specific form of mental set, and is one of the most common forms of cognitive bias in daily life.
Functional fixedness limits the ability for people to solve problems accurately by causing one to have a very narrow way of thinking. Functional fixedness can be seen in other types of learning behaviors as well. For instance, research has discovered the presence of functional fixedness in many educational instances. Researchers Furio, Calatayud, Baracenas, and Padilla stated that "... functional fixedness may be found in learning concepts as well as in solving chemistry problems."<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = [[Carlos Furió Más|Furio]] | first1 = C. | last2 = Calatayud | first2 = M. L. | last3 = Baracenas | first3 = S | last4 = Padilla | first4 = O | title = Functional fixedness and functional reduction as common sense reasonings in chemical equilibrium and in geometry and polarity of molecules. Valencia, Spain | url = | journal = Science Education | volume = 84 | issue = 5| year = 2000 }}</ref> There was more emphasis on this function being seen in this type of subject and others.


As an example, imagine a man wants to kill a bug in his house, but the only thing at hand is a can of air freshener. He may start searching for something to kill the bug instead of squashing it with the can, thinking only of its main function of deodorizing.
There are several hypotheses in regards to how functional fixedness relates to problem solving.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Adamson | first1 = Robert E | year = | title = Functional fixedness as related to problem solving: A repetition of three experiments. Stanford University. California | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 44 | issue = 4| page = 1952 | doi=10.1037/h0062487}}</ref> There are also many ways in which a person can run into problems while thinking of a particular object with having this function. If there is one way in which a person usually thinks of something rather than multiple ways then this can lead to a constraint in how the person thinks of that particular object. This can be seen as narrow minded thinking, which is defined as a way in which one is not able to see or accept certain ideas in a particular context. Functional fixedness is very closely related to this as previously mentioned. This can be done intentionally and or unintentionally, but for the most part it seems as if this process to problem solving is done in an unintentional way.


Tim German and Clark Barrett describe this barrier: "subjects become 'fixed' on the design function of the objects, and problem solving suffers relative to control conditions in which the object's function is not demonstrated."<ref>{{cite journal | last1=German | first1=Tim P. | last2=Barrett | first2=H. Clark | title=Functional Fixedness in a Technologically Sparse Culture | journal=Psychological Science | publisher=SAGE Publications | volume=16 | issue=1 | year=2005 | issn=0956-7976 | doi=10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00771.x | pages=1–5| pmid=15660843 | s2cid=1833823 }}</ref> Their research found that young children's limited knowledge of an object's intended function reduces this barrier<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = German | first1 = Tim P. | last2 = Defeyter | first2 = Margaret A. | year = 2000| title = Immunity to functional fixedness in young children | journal = Psychonomic Bulletin and Review | volume = 7 | issue = 4| pages = 707–712| doi = 10.3758/BF03213010 | pmid = 11206213 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Research has also discovered functional fixedness in educational contexts, as an obstacle to understanding: "functional fixedness may be found in learning concepts as well as in solving chemistry problems."<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Furio |first1=C. |last2=Calatayud |first2=M. L. |last3=Baracenas |first3=S. |last4=Padilla |first4=O. |year=2000 |title=Functional fixedness and functional reduction as common sense reasonings in chemical equilibrium and in geometry and polarity of molecules|journal=Science Education |volume=84 |issue=5 |pages=545–565 |doi=10.1002/1098-237X(200009)84:5<545::AID-SCE1>3.0.CO;2-1|bibcode=2000SciEd..84..545F }}</ref>
Functional fixedness can affect problem solvers in at least two particular ways. The first is with regards to time, as functional fixedness causes people to use more time than necessary to solve any given problem. Secondly, functional fixedness often causes solvers to make more attempts to solve a problem than they would have made if they were not experiencing this cognitive barrier. In the worst case, functional fixedness can completely prevent a person from realizing a solution to a problem. Functional fixedness is a commonplace occurrence, which affects the lives of many people.


There are several hypotheses in regards to how functional fixedness relates to problem solving.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Adamson |first1=Robert E |year=1952 |title=Functional fixedness as related to problem solving: A repetition of three experiments |journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology |volume=44 |issue=4 |pages=288–291 |doi=10.1037/h0062487|pmid=13000071 }}</ref> It may waste time, delaying or entirely preventing the correct use of a tool.
===Unnecessary constraints===
[[Unnecessary constraints]] is another very common barrier that people face while attempting to problem-solve. This particular phenomenon occurs when the subject, trying to solve the problem subconsciously, places boundaries on the task at hand, which in turn forces him or her to strain to be more innovative in their thinking. The solver hits a barrier when they become fixated on only one way to solve their problem, and it becomes increasingly difficult to see anything but the method they have chosen. Typically, the solver experiences this when attempting to use a method they have already experienced success from, and they can not help but try to make it work in the present circumstances as well, even if they see that it is counterproductive.<ref name="Kellogg, R. T. 2003">Kellogg, R. T. (2003). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). California: Sage Publications, Inc.</ref>


===Unnecessary constraints===
[[Groupthink]], or taking on the mindset of the rest of the group members, can also act as an unnecessary constraint while trying to solve problems.<ref>Cottam, Martha L., Dietz-Uhler, Beth, Mastors, Elena, & Preston, & Thomas. (2010). Introduction to Political Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.</ref> This is due to the fact that with everybody thinking the same thing, stopping on the same conclusions, and inhibiting themselves to think beyond this. This is very common, but the most well-known example of this barrier making itself present is in the famous example of the dot problem. In this example, there are nine dots lying in a square- three dots across, and three dots running up and down. The solver is then asked to draw no more than four lines, without lifting their pen or pencil from the paper. This series of lines should connect all of the dots on the paper. Then, what typically happens is the subject creates an assumption in their mind that they must connect the dots without letting his or her pen or pencil go outside of the square of dots. Standardized procedures like this can often bring mentally invented constraints of this kind,<ref>Meloy, J. R. (1998). The Psychology of Stalking, Clinical and Forensic Perspectives (2nd ed.). London, England: Academic Press.</ref> and researchers have found a 0% correct solution rate in the time allotted for the task to be completed.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = MacGregor | first1 = J.N. | last2 = Ormerod | first2 = T.C. | last3 = Chronicle | first3 = E.P. | year = 2001 | title = Information-processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume = 27 | issue = 1| pages = 176–201 | doi=10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.176}}</ref> The imposed constraint inhibits the solver to think beyond the bounds of the dots. It is from this phenomenon that the expression "think outside the box" is derived.<ref name="Weiten, Wayne 2011">Weiten, Wayne. (2011). Psychology: themes and variations (8th ed.). California: Wadsworth.</ref>
Unnecessary constraints are arbitrary boundaries imposed unconsciously on the task at hand, which foreclose a productive avenue of solution. The solver may become fixated on only one type of solution, as if it were an inevitable requirement of the problem. Typically, this combines with mental set—clinging to a previously successful method.<ref name="Kellogg, R. T. 2003">{{cite book|last=Kellogg|first=R. T.|year=2003|title=Cognitive psychology|edition=2nd|location=California|publisher=Sage Publications, Inc.}}</ref>{{page needed|date=September 2023}}


Visual problems can also produce mentally invented constraints.<ref>{{cite book|last=Meloy|first=J. R.|year=1998|title=The Psychology of Stalking, Clinical and Forensic Perspectives|edition=2nd|location=London, England|publisher=Academic Press}}</ref>{{page needed|date=September 2023}} A famous example is the dot problem: nine dots arranged in a three-by-three grid pattern must be connected by drawing four straight line segments, without lifting pen from paper or backtracking along a line. The subject typically assumes the pen must stay within the outer square of dots, but the solution requires lines continuing beyond this frame, and researchers have found a 0% solution rate within a brief allotted time.<ref>{{cite journal|last2=Ormerod|first2=T.C.|last3=Chronicle|first3=E.P.|year=2001|title=Information-processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition|volume=27|issue=1|pages=176–201|doi=10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.176|last1=MacGregor|first1=J.N.|pmid=11204097}}</ref>
This problem can be quickly solved with a dawning of realization, or ''insight''. A few minutes of struggling over a problem can bring these sudden insights, where the solver quickly sees the solution clearly. Problems such as this are most typically solved via insight and can be very difficult for the subject depending on either how they have structured the problem in their minds, how they draw on their past experiences, and how much they juggle this information in their working memories<ref name="Weiten, Wayne 2011"/> In the case of the nine-dot example, the solver has already been structured incorrectly in their minds because of the constraint that they have placed upon the solution. In addition to this, people experience struggles when they try to compare the problem to their prior knowledge, and they think they must keep their lines within the dots and not go beyond. They do this because trying to envision the dots connected outside of the basic square puts a strain on their working memory.<ref name="Weiten, Wayne 2011"/>


Luckily, the solution to the problem becomes obvious as insight occurs following incremental movements made toward the solution. These tiny movements happen without the solver knowing. Then when the insight is realized fully, the "aha" moment happens for the subject.<ref>Novick, L. R., & Bassok, M. (2005). Problem solving. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (Ch. 14, pp. 321-349). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.</ref> These moments of insight can take a long while to manifest or not so long at other times, but the way that the solution is arrived at after toiling over these barriers stays the same.
This problem has produced the expression "[[think outside the box]]".<ref name="Weiten, Wayne 2011">{{cite book|last=Weiten|first=Wayne|year=2011|title=Psychology: themes and variations|edition=8th|location=California|publisher=Wadsworth}}</ref>{{page needed|date=September 2023}} Such problems are typically solved via a sudden insight which leaps over the mental barriers, often after long toil against them.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Novick|first1=L. R.|last2=Bassok|first2=M.|year=2005|chapter=Problem solving|editor-first1=K. J.|editor-last1=Holyoak|editor-first2=R. G.|editor-last2=Morrison|title=Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning|pages=321–349|location=New York, N.Y.|publisher=Cambridge University Press}}</ref> This can be difficult depending on how the subject has structured the problem in their mind, how they draw on past experiences, and how well they juggle this information in their working memory. In the example, envisioning the dots connected outside the framing square requires visualizing an unconventional arrangement, which is a strain on working memory.<ref name="Weiten, Wayne 2011" />


===Irrelevant information===
===Irrelevant information===
{{See also|Information overload|Mass media}}
[[Irrelevant information]] is information presented within a problem that is unrelated or unimportant to the specific problem.<ref name="Kellogg, R. T. 2003"/> Within the specific context of the problem, irrelevant information would serve no purpose in helping solve that particular problem. Often ''irrelevant information'' is detrimental to the problem solving process. It is a common barrier that many people have trouble getting through, especially if they are not aware of it. ''Irrelevant information'' makes solving otherwise relatively simple problems much harder.<ref>Walinga, Jennifer. (2010). From walls to windows: Using barriers as pathways to insightful solutions. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 44, 143-167. doi: 10.1002/j.2162- 6057.2010.tb01331.x</ref>
Irrelevant information is a specification or data presented in a problem that is unrelated to the solution.<ref name="Kellogg, R. T. 2003" /> If the solver assumes that all information presented needs to be used, this often derails the problem solving process, making relatively simple problems much harder.<ref>{{cite journal|year=2010|title=From walls to windows: Using barriers as pathways to insightful solutions|journal=The Journal of Creative Behavior|volume=44|issue=3|pages=143–167|doi=10.1002/j.2162-6057.2010.tb01331.x|last1=Walinga|first1=Jennifer}}</ref>


For example: "Fifteen percent of the people in Topeka have unlisted telephone numbers. You select 200 names at random from the Topeka phone book. How many of these people have unlisted phone numbers?"{{r|Weiten, Wayne 2011}}{{page needed|date=September 2023}} The "obvious" answer is 15%, but in fact none of the unlisted people would be listed among the 200. This kind of "[[trick question]]" is often used in aptitude tests or cognitive evaluations.<ref name="Walinga, Jennifer 2011">{{cite journal|last1=Walinga|first1=Jennifer|last2=Cunningham|first2=J. Barton|last3=MacGregor|first3=James N.|year=2011|title=Training insight problem solving through focus on barriers and assumptions|journal=The Journal of Creative Behavior|volume=45 |pages=47–58 |doi=10.1002/j.2162-6057.2011.tb01084.x }}</ref> Though not inherently difficult, they require independent thinking that is not necessarily common. Mathematical [[Word problem (mathematics education)|word problem]]s often include irrelevant qualitative or numerical information as an extra challenge.
For example:


=== Avoiding barriers by changing problem representation ===
"Fifteen percent of the people in Topeka have unlisted telephone numbers. You select 200 names at random from the Topeka phone book. How many of these people have unlisted phone numbers?"<ref>Weiten, Wayne. (2011). Psychology: themes and variations (8th ed.) California: Wadsworth.</ref>
The disruption caused by the above cognitive biases can depend on how the information is represented:<ref name="Walinga, Jennifer 2011" /> visually, verbally, or mathematically. A classic example is the Buddhist monk problem:


{{quote|A Buddhist monk begins at dawn one day walking up a mountain, reaches the top at sunset, meditates at the top for several days until one dawn when he begins to walk back to the foot of the mountain, which he reaches at sunset. Making no assumptions about his starting or stopping or about his pace during the trips, prove that there is a place on the path which he occupies at the same hour of the day on the two separate journeys.}}
The people that are not listed in the phone book would not be among the 200 names you selected. The individuals looking at this task would have naturally wanted to use the 15% given to them in the problem. They see that there is information present and they immediately think that it needs to be used. This of course is not true. These kinds of questions are often used to test students taking aptitude tests or cognitive evaluations.<ref name="Walinga, Jennifer 2011">Walinga, Jennifer, Cunningham, J. Barton, & MacGregor, James N. (2011). Training insight problem solving through focus on barriers and assumptions. The Journal of Creative Behavior.</ref> They aren't meant to be difficult but they are meant to require thinking that is not necessarily common. ''Irrelevant Information'' is commonly represented in math problems, word problems specifically, where numerical information is put for the purpose of challenging the individual.


The problem cannot be addressed in a verbal context, trying to describe the monk's progress on each day. It becomes much easier when the paragraph is represented mathematically by a function: one visualizes a [[Graph of a function|graph]] whose horizontal axis is time of day, and whose vertical axis shows the monk's position (or altitude) on the path at each time. Superimposing the two journey curves, which traverse opposite diagonals of a rectangle, one sees they must cross each other somewhere. The visual representation by graphing has resolved the difficulty.
One reason [[irrelevant information]] is so effective at keeping a person off topic and away from the relevant information, is in how it is represented.<ref name="Walinga, Jennifer 2011"/> The way information is represented can make a vast difference in how difficult the problem is to be overcome. Whether a problem is represented visually, verbally, spatially, or mathematically, irrelevant information can have a profound effect on how long a problem takes to be solved; or if it's even possible. The Buddhist monk problem is a classic example of [[irrelevant information]] and how it can be represented in different ways:


Similar strategies can often improve problem solving on tests.<ref name="Kellogg, R. T. 2003" /><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Vlamings |first1=Petra H. J. M. |last2=Hare |first2=Brian |last3=Call |first3=Joseph |year=2009 |title=Reaching around barriers: The performance of great apes and 3–5-year-old children |journal=Animal Cognition |volume=13 |issue=2 |pages=273–285 |doi=10.1007/s10071-009-0265-5 |pmc=2822225 |pmid=19653018}}</ref>
::A Buddhist monk begins at dawn one day walking up a mountain, reaches the top at sunset, meditates at the top for several days until one dawn when he begins to walk back to the foot of the mountain, which he reaches at sunset. Making no assumptions about his starting or stopping or about his pace during the trips, prove that there is a place on the path which he occupies at the same hour of the day on the two separate journeys.


=== Other barriers for individuals ===
This problem is near impossible to solve because of how the information is represented. Because it is written out in a way that represents the information verbally, it causes us to try and create a mental image of the paragraph. This is often very difficult to do especially with all the ''Irrelevant Information'' involved in the question. This example is made much easier to understand when the paragraph is represented visually. Now if the same problem was asked, but it was also accompanied by a corresponding graph, it would be far easier to answer this question; ''Irrelevant Information'' no longer serves as a road block. By representing the problem visually, there are no difficult words to understand or scenarios to imagine. The visual representation of this problem has removed the difficulty of solving it.
People who are engaged in problem solving tend to overlook subtractive changes, even those that are critical elements of efficient solutions.{{example needed|date=September 2023}} This tendency to solve by first, only, or mostly creating or adding elements, rather than by subtracting elements or processes is shown to intensify with higher [[cognitive load]]s such as [[information overload]].<ref>{{multiref2
|1={{cite news |first=Sujata |last=Gupta |title=People add by default even when subtraction makes more sense |url=https://www.sciencenews.org/article/psychology-numbers-people-add-default-subtract-better |access-date=10 May 2021 |work=Science News |date=7 April 2021 |archive-date=21 May 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210521134851/https://www.sciencenews.org/article/psychology-numbers-people-add-default-subtract-better |url-status=live }}
|2={{cite journal |last1=Adams |first1=Gabrielle S. |last2=Converse |first2=Benjamin A. |last3=Hales |first3=Andrew H. |last4=Klotz |first4=Leidy E. |title=People systematically overlook subtractive changes |journal=Nature |date=April 2021 |volume=592 |issue=7853 |pages=258–261 |doi=10.1038/s41586-021-03380-y |pmid=33828317 |bibcode=2021Natur.592..258A |s2cid=233185662 |url=https://www.nature.com/articles/s41586-021-03380-y |url-access=subscription |access-date=10 May 2021 |language=en |issn=1476-4687 |archive-date=10 May 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210510130853/https://www.nature.com/articles/s41586-021-03380-y |url-status=live }}
}}</ref>


== Dreaming: problem solving without waking consciousness ==
These types of representations are often used to make difficult problems easier.<ref>Vlamings, Petra H. J. M., Hare, Brian, & Call, Joseph. Reaching around barriers: The performance of great apes and 3-5-year-old children. ''Animal Cognition'', 13, 273-285. {{DOI|10.1007/s10071-009-0265-5}}</ref> They can be used on tests as a strategy to remove ''Irrelevant Information,'' which is one of the most common forms of barriers when discussing the issues of problem solving.<ref name="Kellogg, R. T. 2003"/> Identifying crucial information presented in a problem and then being able to correctly identify its usefulness is essential. Being aware of ''Irrelevant Information'' is the first step in overcoming this common barrier.
People can also solve problems while they are asleep. There are many reports of scientists and engineers who solved problems in their [[dream]]s. For example, [[Elias Howe]], inventor of the sewing machine, figured out the structure of the bobbin from a dream.<ref>{{cite book|last=Kaempffert|first=Waldemar B.|year=1924|title=A Popular History of American Invention|volume=2|location=New York|publisher=Charles Scribner's Sons|page=[https://archive.org/details/popularhistoryof02kaem/page/385/mode/1up 385]}}</ref>

The chemist [[August Kekulé]] was considering how benzene arranged its six carbon and hydrogen atoms. Thinking about the problem, he dozed off, and dreamt of dancing atoms that fell into a snakelike pattern, which led him to discover the benzene ring. As Kekulé wrote in his diary,

{{blockquote|One of the snakes seized hold of its own tail, and the form whirled mockingly before my eyes. As if by a flash of lightning I awoke; and this time also I spent the rest of the night in working out the consequences of the hypothesis.<ref>{{multiref2|1={{cite journal | last1 = Kekulé | first1 = August | year = 1890 | title = Benzolfest-Rede. | journal=Berichte der Deutschen Chemischen Gesellschaft|volume=23|pages= 1302–1311 }}
|2={{cite journal | last1 = Benfey | first1 = O. | year = 1958 | title = Kekulé and the birth of the structural theory of organic chemistry in 1858 | doi = 10.1021/ed035p21 | journal = Journal of Chemical Education | volume = 35 | issue = 1 | pages = 21–23 | bibcode = 1958JChEd..35...21B}} }}</ref>}}

There also are empirical studies of how people can think consciously about a problem before going to sleep, and then solve the problem with a dream image. Dream researcher [[William C. Dement]] told his undergraduate class of 500 students that he wanted them to think about an infinite series, whose first elements were OTTFF, to see if they could deduce the principle behind it and to say what the next elements of the series would be.<ref name="Dement 1972">{{cite book|last=Dement|first=W.C.|year=1972|title=Some Must Watch While Some Just Sleep|location=New York|publisher=Freeman}}</ref>{{page needed|date=September 2023}} He asked them to think about this problem every night for 15 minutes before going to sleep and to write down any dreams that they then had. They were instructed to think about the problem again for 15 minutes when they awakened in the morning.

The sequence OTTFF is the first letters of the numbers: one, two, three, four, five. The next five elements of the series are SSENT (six, seven, eight, nine, ten). Some of the students solved the puzzle by reflecting on their dreams. One example was a student who reported the following dream:<ref name="Dement 1972"/>{{page needed|date=September 2023}}

{{blockquote|I was standing in an art gallery, looking at the paintings on the wall. As I walked down the hall, I began to count the paintings: one, two, three, four, five. As I came to the sixth and seventh, the paintings had been ripped from their frames. I stared at the empty frames with a peculiar feeling that some mystery was about to be solved. Suddenly I realized that the sixth and seventh spaces were the solution to the problem!}}

With more than 500 undergraduate students, 87 dreams were judged to be related to the problems students were assigned (53 directly related and 34 indirectly related). Yet of the people who had dreams that apparently solved the problem, only seven were actually able to consciously know the solution. The rest (46 out of 53) thought they did not know the solution.

Mark Blechner conducted this experiment and obtained results similar to Dement's.<ref name="Blechner 2018">{{cite book|last=Blechner|first=Mark J.|year=2018|title=The Mindbrain and Dreams: An Exploration of Dreaming, Thinking, and Artistic Creation|location=New York|publisher=Routledge}}</ref>{{page needed|date=September 2023}} He found that while trying to solve the problem, people had dreams in which the solution appeared to be obvious from the dream, but it was rare for the dreamers to realize how their dreams had solved the puzzle. Coaxing or hints did not get them to realize it, although once they heard the solution, they recognized how their dream had solved it. For example, one person in that OTTFF experiment dreamed:<ref name="Blechner 2018"/>{{page needed|date=September 2023}}

{{blockquote|There is a big clock. You can see the movement. The big hand of the clock was on the number six. You could see it move up, number by number, six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven, twelve. The dream focused on the small parts of the machinery. You could see the gears inside.}}

In the dream, the person counted out the next elements of the series—six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven, twelve—yet he did not realize that this was the solution of the problem. His sleeping mindbrain{{jargon inline|date=September 2023}} solved the problem, but his waking mindbrain was not aware how.

[[Albert Einstein]] believed that much problem solving goes on unconsciously, and the person must then figure out and formulate consciously what the mindbrain{{jargon inline|date=September 2023}} has already solved. He believed this was his process in formulating the theory of relativity: "The creator of the problem possesses the solution."<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Fromm | first1 = Erika O. | year = 1998 | title = Lost and found half a century later: Letters by Freud and Einstein | journal = American Psychologist | volume = 53 | issue = 11| pages = 1195–1198 | doi = 10.1037/0003-066x.53.11.1195 }}</ref> Einstein said that he did his problem solving without words, mostly in images. "The words or the language, as they are written or spoken, do not seem to play any role in my mechanism of thought. The psychical entities which seem to serve as elements in thought are certain signs and more or less clear images which can be 'voluntarily' reproduced and combined."<ref>{{cite book|last=Einstein|first=Albert|year=1954|chapter=A Mathematician's Mind|title=Ideas and Opinions|location=New York|publisher=Bonanza Books|page=25}}</ref>

== Cognitive sciences: two schools ==
{{anchor|acrossDomainsExpertise}}
Problem-solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise.<ref>{{cite book|last=Sternberg|first=R. J.|year=1995|chapter=Conceptions of expertise in complex problem solving: A comparison of alternative conceptions|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=295–321|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}</ref> For this reason, [[cognitive sciences]] findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to problem-solving situations outside the laboratory. This has led to a research emphasis on real-world problem solving, since the 1990s. This emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North America and Europe, however. Whereas North American research has typically concentrated on studying problem solving in separate, natural knowledge domains, much of the European research has focused on novel, complex problems, and has been performed with computerized scenarios.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Funke |first1=J. |year=1991 |chapter=Solving complex problems: Human identification and control of complex systems |pages=185–222 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref>

=== Europe ===
In Europe, two main approaches have surfaced, one initiated by [[Donald Broadbent]]<ref>{{multiref2
|1={{cite journal|last=Broadbent|first=Donald E.|year=1977|url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1080/14640747708400596|url-access=subscription|title=Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control|journal=Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology|volume=29|issue=2|pages=181–201|doi=10.1080/14640747708400596|s2cid=144328372|access-date=2019-06-09|archive-date=2020-08-06|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200806214714/https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1080/14640747708400596|url-status=live}}
|2={{cite book|last1=Berry|first1=Dianne C.|last2=Broadbent|first2=Donald E.|year=1995|chapter-url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/200134353|chapter=Implicit learning in the control of complex systems: A reconsideration of some of the earlier claims|editor-first1=P.A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=131–150|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
}}</ref> in the United Kingdom and the other one by [[Dietrich Dörner]]<ref>{{multiref2
|1={{cite journal | last= Dörner|first= Dietrich|year=1975|title=Wie Menschen eine Welt verbessern wollten|trans-title=How people wanted to improve the world|journal=Bild der Wissenschaft|volume=12|pages=48–53|lang=de}}
|2={{cite book | last= Dörner|first= Dietrich|year=1985|chapter= Verhalten, Denken und Emotionen|trans-chapter=Behavior, thinking, and emotions|editor-first1=L. H.|editor-last1= Eckensberger |editor-first2= E. D.|editor-last2= Lantermann |title=Emotion und Reflexivität|pages=157–181|location=München, Germany|publisher=Urban & Schwarzenberg|lang=de}}
|3={{cite book | last1= Dörner|first1= Dietrich|last2= Wearing|first2= Alex J. |year=1995|chapter-url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/200134353 |chapter=Complex problem solving: Toward a (computer-simulated) theory|editor-first1=P.A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=65–99|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
}}</ref> in Germany. The two approaches share an emphasis on relatively complex, semantically rich, computerized laboratory tasks, constructed to resemble real-life problems. The approaches differ somewhat in their theoretical goals and methodology. The tradition initiated by Broadbent emphasizes the distinction between cognitive problem-solving processes that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness, and typically employs mathematically well-defined computerized systems. The tradition initiated by Dörner, on the other hand, has an interest in the interplay of the cognitive, motivational, and social components of problem solving, and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain up to 2,000 highly interconnected variables.<ref>{{multiref2
|1={{cite book | last= Buchner|first= A. |year=1995|chapter=Theories of complex problem solving|editor-first1=P.A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=27–63|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
|2={{cite book | editor-last1= Dörner|editor-first1=D.|editor-last2= Kreuzig|editor-first2= H. W.|editor-last3= Reither|editor-first3= F.|editor-last4= Stäudel|editor-first4=T.|year=1983|title=Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität|trans-title=Lohhausen. On dealing with uncertainty and complexity|location=Bern, Switzerland|publisher=Hans Huber|lang=de}}
|3={{cite book | last1= Ringelband|first1= O. J.|last2= Misiak|first2= C.|last3= Kluwe|first3= R. H.|year=1990|chapter=Mental models and strategies in the control of a complex system|editor-first1=D.|editor-last1=Ackermann|editor-first2=M. J.|editor-last2=Tauber|title=Mental models and human-computer interaction|volume=1|pages=151–164|location=Amsterdam|publisher=Elsevier Science Publishers}}
}}</ref>

=== North America ===
In North America, initiated by the work of Herbert A. Simon on "learning by doing" in [[semantic]]ally rich domains,<ref>{{multiref2
|1={{cite journal |last1=Anzai|first1=K. |last2=Simon|first2= H. A. |title=The theory of learning by doing |journal=Psychological Review |volume=86 |pages=124–140 |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.86.2.124 |year=1979 |pmid=493441 |issue=2 |ref=Reference-Anzai}}
|2={{cite journal | last1=Bhaskar | first1=R. | last2=Simon | first2=Herbert A. | title=Problem Solving in Semantically Rich Domains: An Example from Engineering Thermodynamics | journal=Cognitive Science | publisher=Wiley | volume=1 | issue=2 | year=1977 | issn=0364-0213 | doi=10.1207/s15516709cog0102_3 |doi-access=free | pages=193–215}}
}}</ref> researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural [[knowledge domain]]s—such as physics, writing, or [[chess]] playing—rather than attempt to extract a global theory of problem solving.<ref>e.g., {{cite book |year=1991 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref> These researchers have focused on the development of problem solving within certain domains, that is on the development of [[expertise]].<ref>{{multiref2
|1={{cite journal | last1= Chase|first1=W. G.|last2=Simon|first2=H. A.|year=1973|title=Perception in chess|journal=Cognitive Psychology|volume=4|pages=55–81|doi=10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2 }}
|2={{cite journal |last1=Chi|first1= M. T. H. |last2=Feltovich|first2= P. J. |last3=Glaser|first3= R. |year=1981 |title=Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices |journal=Cognitive Science |volume=5 |issue=2 |pages=121–152 |ref=Reference-Chi |doi=10.1207/s15516709cog0502_2|doi-access=free }}
|3={{cite journal |last1=Anderson|first1=J. R. |last2=Boyle|first2= C. B. |last3=Reiser|first3= B. J. | title = Intelligent tutoring systems | journal = Science | year = 1985 | volume = 228 | pages = 456–462 | doi = 10.1126/science.228.4698.456 | pmid = 17746875 | issue = 4698 |ref=Reference-Anderson|bibcode=1985Sci...228..456A |s2cid=62403455}}
}}</ref>

Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include:
* calculation<ref>{{cite book |last1=Sokol |first1=S. M. |last2=McCloskey |first2=M. |year=1991 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA85 |chapter-url-access=limited|chapter=Cognitive mechanisms in calculation |pages=85–116 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref>
* computer skills<ref>{{cite book |last1=Kay |first1=D. S. |year=1991 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA317 |chapter-url-access=limited |chapter=Computer interaction: Debugging the problems |pages=317–340 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443 |access-date=2022-12-04 |archive-date=2022-12-04 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20221204055601/https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA317 |url-status=live }}</ref>
* game playing<ref>{{cite book |last1=Frensch |first1=P. A. |last2=Sternberg |first2=R. J. |year=1991 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA343 |chapter-url-access=limited|chapter=Skill-related differences in game playing |pages=343–381 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J .|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref>
* lawyers' reasoning<ref name="Amsel1991">{{cite book |last1=Amsel |first1=E. |last2=Langer |first2=R. |last3=Loutzenhiser |first3=L. |year=1991 |chapter=Do lawyers reason differently from psychologists? A comparative design for studying expertise |pages=223–250 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref>
* managerial problem solving<ref name="Wagner">{{cite book |last1=Wagner |first1=R. K. |year=1991 |chapter=Managerial problem solving |pages=159–183 |id=[[PsycNET]]: [https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1991-98396-005 1991-98396-005] |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}</ref>
* mathematical problem solving<ref>{{multiref2|1={{cite book|author-link=George Pólya|last=Pólya|first=George|year=1945|title=How to Solve It|publisher=Princeton University Press}}|2={{cite book|last=Schoenfeld|first=A. H.|year=1985|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=0cbSBQAAQBAJ|url-access=limited|title=Mathematical Problem Solving|location=Orlando, Fla.|publisher=Academic Press|isbn=978-1-4832-9548-0|access-date=2019-06-09|archive-date=2023-10-23|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20231023053840/https://books.google.com/books?id=0cbSBQAAQBAJ|url-status=live}} }}</ref>
* mechanical problem solving<ref>{{cite book |last=Hegarty |first=M. |year=1991 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA253 |chapter-url-access=limited |chapter=Knowledge and processes in mechanical problem solving |pages=253–285 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443 |access-date=2022-12-04 |archive-date=2022-12-04 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20221204055603/https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA253 |url-status=live }}</ref>
* personal problem solving<ref>{{cite journal | last1= Heppner|first1= P. P.|last2= Krauskopf|first2= C. J. |year=1987|title= An information-processing approach to personal problem solving|journal=The Counseling Psychologist|volume=15|issue= 3|pages=371–447|doi= 10.1177/0011000087153001|s2cid= 146180007}}</ref>
* political decision making<ref>{{cite book |last1=Voss |first1=J. F. |last2=Wolfe |first2=C. R. |last3=Lawrence |first3=J. A. |last4=Engle |first4=R. A. |year=1991 |chapter=From representation to decision: An analysis of problem solving in international relations |pages=119–158 |id=[[PsycNET]]: [https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1991-98396-004 1991-98396-004] |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref>
* problem solving in electronics<ref>{{cite book |last1=Lesgold |first1=A. |last2=Lajoie |first2=S. |year=1991 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA287 |chapter-url-access=limited |chapter=Complex problem solving in electronics |pages=287–316 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443 |access-date=2022-12-04 |archive-date=2022-12-04 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20221204055601/https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA287 |url-status=live }}</ref>
* problem solving for innovations and inventions: [[TRIZ]]<ref name="Altshuller1994">{{cite book | last = Altshuller | first = Genrich | year = 1994 | title = And Suddenly the Inventor Appeared | translator = Lev Shulyak | location = Worcester, Mass. | publisher = Technical Innovation Center | isbn = 978-0-9640740-1-9 |ref=Reference-Altshuller1994}}</ref>
* reading<ref>{{cite book |last1=Stanovich |first1=K. E. |last2=Cunningham |first2=A. E. |year=1991 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA3 |chapter-url-access=limited |chapter=Reading as constrained reasoning |pages=3–60 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443 |access-date=2022-12-04 |archive-date=2023-09-03 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230903202339/https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA3 |url-status=live }}</ref>
* social problem solving<ref name=DZurilla />
* writing<ref>{{cite book |last1=Bryson |first1=M. |last2=Bereiter |first2=C. |last3=Scardamalia |first3=M. |last4=Joram |first4=E. |year=1991 |chapter=Going beyond the problem as given: Problem solving in expert and novice writers |pages=61–84 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, N.J. |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref>

== Characteristics of complex problems ==

Complex problem solving (CPS) is distinguishable from simple problem solving (SPS). In SPS there is a singular and simple obstacle. In CPS there may be multiple simultaneous obstacles. For example, a surgeon at work has far more complex problems than an individual deciding what shoes to wear. As elucidated by Dietrich Dörner, and later expanded upon by Joachim Funke, complex problems have some typical characteristics, which include:<ref name="Complex Problem Solving"/>

* [[complexity]] (large numbers of items, interrelations, and decisions)
* [[enumerability]]{{clarify|reason=what is enumerable? does this mean enumerable in the formal mathematical sense? what would it mean for a complex problem to be unenumerable?|date=September 2023}}
* [[Homogeneity and heterogeneity|heterogeneity]]{{Specify|reason=of what?|date=September 2023}}
* [[:wikt:connectivity|connectivity]] (hierarchy relation, communication relation, allocation relation){{clarify|reason=it's unclear what this refers to or what that parenthetical list means|date=September 2023}}
* [[Dynamics (physics)|dynamics]] (time considerations){{clarify|reason=what do either "dynamics" or "time considerations" mean in this context?|date=September 2023}}
** temporal constraints
** temporal sensitivity{{clarify|reason=how does this differ from "temporal constraints"?|date=September 2023}}
** phase effects{{Definition needed|date=September 2023}}
** dynamic [[Predictability|unpredictability]]{{Specify|reason=of what?|date=September 2023}}
* intransparency (lack of clarity of the situation)
** commencement opacity{{Definition needed|date=September 2023}}
** continuation opacity{{Definition needed|date=September 2023}}
* [[polytely]] (multiple goals)<ref>{{cite book |year=1991 |editor-first1=R. J. |editor-last1=Sternberg |editor-first2=P. A. |editor-last2=Frensch |title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms |place=Hillsdale, NJ |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |isbn=0-8058-0650-4 |oclc=23254443}}</ref>
** inexpressivenes{{Specify|reason=of what to what?|date=September 2023}}
** opposition{{Specify|reason=to what by what?|date=September 2023}}
** transience{{Specify|reason=of what|date=September 2023}}

==Collective problem solving==
{{See also|Crowdsolving|Collective action|Collaborative intelligence|Mass collaboration|Collective wisdom|The Wisdom of Crowds|Distributed knowledge|Online participation|Group decision-making}}
People solve problems on many different levels—from the individual to the civilizational. Collective problem solving refers to problem solving performed collectively. [[Social issue]]s and global issues can typically only be solved collectively.

The complexity of contemporary problems exceeds the cognitive capacity of any individual and requires different but complementary varieties of expertise and collective problem solving ability.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hung|first1=Woei|title=Team-based complex problem solving: a collective cognition perspective|journal=Educational Technology Research and Development|year=2013|volume=61|issue=3|pages=365–384 |doi=10.1007/s11423-013-9296-3 |s2cid=62663840}}</ref>

[[Collective intelligence]] is shared or group intelligence that emerges from the [[collaboration]], collective efforts, and competition of many individuals.

In collaborative problem solving people [[teamwork|work together]] to solve real-world problems. Members of problem-solving groups share a common concern, a similar passion, and/or a commitment to their work. Members can ask questions, wonder, and try to understand common issues. They share expertise, experiences, tools, and methods.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Jewett|first1=Pamela|first2=Deborah|last2= MacPhee|title=Adding Collaborative Peer Coaching to Our Teaching Identities|journal=The Reading Teacher|year=2012|volume=66|issue=2|pages=105–110|doi=10.1002/TRTR.01089}}</ref> Groups may be fluid based on need, may only occur temporarily to finish an assigned task, or may be more permanent depending on the nature of the problems.

For example, in the educational context, members of a group may all have input into the decision-making process and a role in the learning process. Members may be responsible for the thinking, teaching, and monitoring of all members in the group. Group work may be coordinated among members so that each member makes an equal contribution to the whole work. Members can identify and build on their individual strengths so that everyone can make a significant contribution to the task.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Wang|first=Qiyun|title=Design and Evaluation of a Collaborative Learning Environment|journal=Computers and Education|year=2009|volume=53|issue=4|pages=1138–1146|doi=10.1016/j.compedu.2009.05.023}}</ref> Collaborative group work has the ability to promote critical thinking skills, problem solving skills, [[social skills]], and [[self-esteem]]. By using collaboration and communication, members often learn from one another and construct meaningful knowledge that often leads to better learning outcomes than individual work.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Wang|first=Qiyan|title=Using online shared workspaces to support group collaborative learning|journal=Computers and Education|year=2010|volume=55|issue=3|pages=1270–1276|doi= 10.1016/j.compedu.2010.05.023}}</ref>

Collaborative groups require joint intellectual efforts between the members and involve [[social interaction]]s to solve problems together. The [[knowledge sharing|knowledge shared]] during these interactions is acquired during communication, negotiation, and production of materials.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Kai-Wai Chu|first1=Samuel|first2=David M.|last2=Kennedy|title=Using Online Collaborative tools for groups to Co-Construct Knowledge|journal=Online Information Review|year=2011|volume=35|issue=4|pages=581–597|doi=10.1108/14684521111161945 |s2cid=206388086|issn=1468-4527 }}</ref> Members actively seek information from others by asking questions. The capacity to use questions to acquire new information increases understanding and the ability to solve problems.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Legare|first1=Cristine|first2=Candice |last2=Mills |first3=Andre|last3= Souza |first4=Leigh |last4=Plummer |first5=Rebecca|last5= Yasskin |title=The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood|journal=Journal of Experimental Child Psychology|year=2013|volume=114|issue=1|pages=63–7 |doi=10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002 |pmid=23044374}}</ref>

In a 1962 research report, [[Douglas Engelbart]] linked collective intelligence to organizational effectiveness, and predicted that proactively "augmenting human intellect" would yield a multiplier effect in group problem solving: "Three people working together in this augmented mode [would] seem to be more than three times as effective in solving a complex problem as is one augmented person working alone".<ref>{{cite book|last=Engelbart|first=Douglas|year=1962|chapter-url=https://www.dougengelbart.org/pubs/augment-3906.html#3b9|chapter=Team Cooperation|title=Augmenting Human Intellect: A Conceptual Framework|publisher=Stanford Research Institute|volume=AFOSR-3223}}</ref>

[[Henry Jenkins]], a theorist of new media and media convergence, draws on the theory that collective intelligence can be attributed to media convergence and [[participatory culture]].<ref>{{cite book | last=Flew | first=Terry | year= 2008 | title=New Media: an introduction | publisher=Oxford University Press | location=Melbourne}}</ref> He criticizes contemporary education for failing to incorporate online trends of collective problem solving into the classroom, stating "whereas a collective intelligence community encourages ownership of work as a group, schools grade individuals". Jenkins argues that interaction within a knowledge community builds vital skills for young people, and teamwork through collective intelligence communities contributes to the development of such skills.<ref name=":25">{{Cite web|url=http://labweb.education.wisc.edu/curric606/readings/Jenkins2002.pdf|title=Interactive audiences? The 'collective intelligence' of media fans |last=Henry|first=Jenkins|access-date=December 11, 2016|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180426232104/https://labweb.education.wisc.edu/curric606/readings/Jenkins2002.pdf|archive-date=April 26, 2018}}</ref>

[[Collective impact]] is the commitment of a group of actors from different sectors to a common agenda for solving a specific social problem, using a structured form of collaboration.

After [[World War II]] the [[UN]], the [[Bretton Woods system|Bretton Woods organization]], and the [[WTO]] were created. Collective problem solving on the international level crystallized around these three types of organization from the 1980s onward. As these global institutions remain state-like or state-centric it is unsurprising that they perpetuate state-like or state-centric approaches to collective problem solving rather than alternative ones.<ref>{{cite book|last=Finger|first=Matthias|chapter=Which governance for sustainable development? An organizational and institutional perspective|editor-last1=Park|editor-first1=Jacob|editor-last2=Conca|editor-first2=Ken|editor-last3=Finger |editor-first3=Matthias |title=The Crisis of Global Environmental Governance: Towards a New Political Economy of Sustainability |publisher=Routledge |isbn=978-1-134-05982-9 |language=en|date=2008-03-27|page=[https://books.google.com/books?id=lrr3K50r144C&pg=PA48 48]}}</ref><!--https://books.google.com/books?id=CtXvBwAAQBAJ&pg=PA189-->

[[Crowdsourcing]] is a process of accumulating ideas, thoughts, or information from many independent participants, with aim of finding the best solution for a given challenge. Modern [[information technologies]] allow for many people to be involved and facilitate managing their suggestions in ways that provide good results.<ref>{{multiref2
|1={{cite journal|last1=Guazzini|first1=Andrea|last2=Vilone|first2=Daniele|last3=Donati|first3=Camillo|last4=Nardi|first4=Annalisa|last5=Levnajić|first5=Zoran|title=Modeling crowdsourcing as collective problem solving|journal=Scientific Reports|date=10 November 2015|volume=5|page=16557|doi=10.1038/srep16557 |pmid=26552943 |pmc=4639727 |bibcode=2015NatSR...516557G|arxiv=1506.09155}}
|2={{cite journal|last1=Boroomand|first1=A.|last2=Smaldino|first2=P.E.|year=2021|title=Hard Work, Risk-Taking, and Diversity in a Model of Collective Problem Solving|journal=Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation|volume=24|number=4|doi=10.18564/jasss.4704 |s2cid=240483312 |doi-access=free}}
}}</ref> The [[Internet]] allows for a new capacity of collective (including planetary-scale) problem solving.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Stefanovitch|first1=Nicolas|last2=Alshamsi |first2=Aamena |last3=Cebrian |first3=Manuel |last4=Rahwan|first4=Iyad|title=Error and attack tolerance of collective problem solving: The DARPA Shredder Challenge|journal=EPJ Data Science |date=30 September 2014|volume=3|issue=1|doi=10.1140/epjds/s13688-014-0013-1|doi-access=free|hdl=21.11116/0000-0002-D39F-D|hdl-access=free}}</ref>


==See also==
==See also==
{{Portal|Thinking}}
{{Portal|Philosophy|Psychology}}
{{Wikiquote}}
* [[Outline of thought]] - topic tree that identifies many types of thoughts, methods of problem solving, types of thinking, aspects of thought, related fields, and more.
* {{annotated link|[[Actuarial science]]}}
* [[Outline of human intelligence]] - topic tree presenting the traits, capacities, models, and research fields of human intelligence, and more.
* {{annotated link|[[Analytical skill]]}}
{{Div col}}
* [[creative problem solving]]
* {{annotated link|[[Creative problem-solving]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[Collective intelligence]]}}
* [[divergent thinking]]
* {{annotated link|[[Community of practice]]}}
* [[Eight Disciplines Problem Solving]]
* {{annotated link|[[Coworking]]}}
* [[Grey problem]]
* {{annotated link|[[Crowdsolving]]}}
* [[innovation]]
* {{annotated link|[[Divergent thinking]]}}
* [[instrumentalism]]
* {{annotated link|[[Grey problem]]}}
* [[problem statement]]
* {{annotated link|[[Innovation]]}}
* [[psychedelics in problem-solving experiment]]
* {{annotated link|[[Instrumentalism]]}}
* [[Subgoal labeling]]
*{{Annotated link|[[Problem-posing education]]}}
* [[troubleshooting]]
* {{annotated link|[[Problem statement]]}}
* [[wicked problem]]
* {{annotated link|[[Problem structuring methods]]}}
{{Div col end}}
* {{annotated link|[[Shared intentionality]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[Structural fix]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[Subgoal labeling]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[Troubleshooting]]}}
* {{annotated link|[[Wicked problem]]}}


==Notes==
==Notes==
{{reflist}}
{{reflist}}
Tahir muhdi-ul-din from GC university fsd


==References==
==Further reading==
* {{cite book|last1=Beckmann|first1=Jens F.|last2=Guthke|first2=Jürgen|year=1995|chapter-url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/200134353|chapter=Complex problem solving, intelligence, and learning ability|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=177–200|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
{{Refbegin}}
* {{cite book | last= Brehmer|first= Berndt |year=1995|chapter= Feedback delays in dynamic decision making|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=103–130|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
* {{cite book
* {{cite journal | last1= Brehmer|first1= Berndt|last2= Dörner|first2= D. |year=1993|title=Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study|journal=Computers in Human Behavior|volume=9|issue= 2–3|pages=171–184|doi= 10.1016/0747-5632(93)90005-D}}
| last = Altshuller
* {{cite book | last= Dörner|first= D. |year=1992|chapter= Über die Philosophie der Verwendung von Mikrowelten oder 'Computerszenarios' in der psychologischen Forschung|trans-chapter=On the proper use of microworlds or "computer scenarios" in psychological research|editor-first=H.|editor-last=Gundlach|title=Psychologische Forschung und Methode: Das Versprechen des Experiments. Festschrift für Werner Traxel|pages=53–87|location=Passau, Germany|publisher=Passavia-Universitäts-Verlag|lang=de}}
| first = Genrich
* {{cite journal |last1=Eyferth |first1=K. |last2=Schömann |first2=M. |last3=Widowski |first3=D. |year=1986 |title=Der Umgang von Psychologen mit Komplexität |trans-title=On how psychologists deal with complexity |journal=Sprache & Kognition |volume=5 |pages=11–26 |language=de}}
| year = 1973
* {{cite book|last=Funke|first=Joachim|year=1993|chapter-url=https://web-archive.southampton.ac.uk/cogprints.org/1679/2/Funke_1993_micro.pdf|chapter=Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results|editor-first1=G.|editor-last1=Strube|editor-first2=K.-F.|editor-last2=Wender|title=The cognitive psychology of knowledge|pages=313–330|location=Amsterdam|publisher=Elsevier Science Publishers}}
| title = Innovation Algorithm
* {{cite book |last=Funke|first=Joachim|year=1995|chapter-url=https://web-archive.southampton.ac.uk/cogprints.org/3003/1/Funke_1995_CPS.pdf|chapter=Experimental research on complex problem solving|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=243–268|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
| location = Worcester, MA
* {{cite book |last=Funke|first=U.|year=1995|chapter=Complex problem solving in personnel selection and training|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=219–240|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
| publisher = Technical Innovation Center
* {{cite book | last1= Groner|first1= M.|last2= Groner|first2= R.|last3= Bischof|first3= W. F. |year=1983|chapter= Approaches to heuristics: A historical review|editor-first1= R.|editor-last1= Groner|editor-first2= M.|editor-last2= Groner|editor-first3= W. F.|editor-last3= Bischof|title=Methods of heuristics|pages=1–18|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
| isbn = 0-9640740-2-8
* {{cite book | last= Hayes|first=J.|year=1980|title=The complete problem solver|location=Philadelphia|publisher=The Franklin Institute Press}}
}}
* {{cite book | last= Huber|first= O. |year=1995|chapter= Complex problem solving as multistage decision making|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=151–173|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
* {{cite book
* {{cite journal | last= Hübner|first= Ronald |year=1989|url=https://www.cogpsych.uni-konstanz.de/pdf/Hubner_1989_Methoden.pdf|title=Methoden zur Analyse und Konstruktion von Aufgaben zur kognitiven Steuerung dynamischer Systeme|trans-title=Methods for the analysis and construction of dynamic system control tasks|journal=Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie|volume=36|pages=221–238|lang=de}}
| last = Altshuller
* {{cite book | last= Hunt|first= Earl|year=1991|chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA383|chapter-url-access=limited|chapter=Some comments on the study of complexity|editor-first1=R. J.|editor-last1=Sternberg|editor-first2=P. A.|editor-last2=Frensch|title=Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms|pages=383–395|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates|isbn= 978-1-317-78386-2}}
| first = Genrich
* {{cite journal |last= Hussy|first= W.|year=1985|title=Komplexes Problemlösen—Eine Sackgasse?|trans-title=Complex problem solving—a dead end?|journal=Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie|volume=32|pages=55–77|lang=de}}
| year = 1984
* {{cite book |last1=Kluwe|first1=R. H. |year=1993 |doi=10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62668-0|chapter=Chapter 19 Knowledge and Performance in Complex Problem Solving|title=The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge|volume=101|pages=401–423|series=Advances in Psychology |isbn=978-0-444-89942-2}}
| title = Creativity as an Exact Science
* {{cite book | last= Kluwe|first= R. H. |year=1995|chapter=Single case studies and models of complex problem solving|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=269–291|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
| location = New York, NY
* {{cite journal| last1= Kolb|first1=S.|last2= Petzing|first2= F.|last3=Stumpf|first3=S.|year=1992|title=Komplexes Problemlösen: Bestimmung der Problemlösegüte von Probanden mittels Verfahren des Operations Research—ein interdisziplinärer Ansatz|trans-title=Complex problem solving: determining the quality of human problem solving by operations research tools—an interdisciplinary approach|journal=Sprache & Kognition|volume=11|pages=115–128|lang=de}}
| publisher = Gordon & Breach
* {{cite book|last= Krems|first=Josef F. |year=1995 |chapter-url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/200134353|chapter=Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving|editor-first1=P. A.|editor-last1=Frensch|editor-first2=J.|editor-last2=Funke|title=Complex problem solving: The European Perspective|pages=201–218|location=Hillsdale, N.J.|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates}}
| isbn = 0-677-21230-5
* {{cite book | last= Melzak| first= Z. |year=1983|title=Bypasses: A Simple Approach to Complexity|location=London, UK|publisher=Wiley|lang=en}}
}}
* {{cite book | last= Müller| first= H. |year=1993|title=Komplexes Problemlösen: Reliabilität und Wissen|trans-title=Complex problem solving: Reliability and knowledge|location=Bonn, Germany|publisher=Holos|lang=de}}
* {{cite book
* {{cite book | last1= Paradies| first1= M.W.|last2= Unger|first2=L. W. |year=2000|title=TapRooT—The System for Root Cause Analysis, Problem Investigation, and Proactive Improvement|location=Knoxville, Tenn.|publisher= System Improvements}}
| last = Altshuller
* {{cite book |last1=Putz-Osterloh |first1=Wiebke |doi=10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62664-3 |chapter=Chapter 15 Strategies for Knowledge Acquisition and Transfer of Knowledge in Dynamic Tasks |title=The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge |volume=101 |pages=331–350 |series=Advances in Psychology |year=1993 |isbn=978-0-444-89942-2 }}
| first = Genrich
* {{cite journal | last1= Riefer| first1= David M.|last2=Batchelder|first2= William H. |year=1988|url=https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/9bf7/c2bb2a621983c0e590ea1f019bd1eb8029d4.pdf|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181125115507/https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/9bf7/c2bb2a621983c0e590ea1f019bd1eb8029d4.pdf|archive-date=2018-11-25|title=Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes|journal=Psychological Review|volume=95| issue= 3|pages=318–339| doi= 10.1037/0033-295x.95.3.318| s2cid= 14994393}}
| year = 1994
* {{cite book | last= Schaub|first= H.|year=1993|title=Modellierung der Handlungsorganisation|location=Bern, Switzerland|publisher= Hans Huber|lang=de}}
| title = And Suddenly the Inventor Appeared
* {{cite book | last= Strauß|first= B. |year=1993|title=Konfundierungen beim Komplexen Problemlösen. Zum Einfluß des Anteils der richtigen Lösungen (ArL) auf das Problemlöseverhalten in komplexen Situationen|trans-title=Confoundations in complex problem solving. On the influence of the degree of correct solutions on problem solving in complex situations|location=Bonn, Germany|publisher=Holos|lang=de}}
| others = translated by Lev Shulyak
* {{cite journal | last= Strohschneider|first=S.|year=1991|title=Kein System von Systemen! Kommentar zu dem Aufsatz 'Systemmerkmale als Determinanten des Umgangs mit dynamischen Systemen' von Joachim Funke|trans-title=No system of systems! Reply to the paper 'System features as determinants of behavior in dynamic task environments' by Joachim Funke|journal=Sprache & Kognition|volume=10|pages=109–113|lang=de}}
| location = Worcester, MA
* {{cite book | last=Tonelli | first=Marcello | title=Unstructured Processes of Strategic Decision-Making | year=2011 |location=Saarbrücken, Germany|publisher= Lambert Academic Publishing| isbn=978-3-8465-5598-9}}
| publisher = Technical Innovation Center
* {{cite book | last= Van Lehn|first=Kurt |year=1989|url=https://apps.dtic.mil/dtic/tr/fulltext/u2/a218905.pdf|chapter= Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition|editor-first=M. I.|editor-last=Posner|title=Foundations of cognitive science|pages=527–579|location=Cambridge, Mass.|publisher=MIT Press}}
| isbn = 0-9640740-1-X
* {{citation|author= Wisconsin Educational Media Association|year=1993|url=https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED376817|title=Information literacy: A position paper on information problem-solving|location=Madison, Wis.|series=WEMA Publications|volume=ED 376 817}} (Portions adapted from [[Michigan State Board of Education]]'s Position Paper on Information Processing Skills, 1992.)
}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Amsel| reference= Amsel, E., Langer, R., & Loutzenhiser, L. (1991). Do lawyers reason differently from psychologists? A comparative design for studying expertise. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 223-250). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 978-0-8058-1783-6}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Anderson| reference= {{cite journal
| author = Anderson, J. R., Boyle, C. B., & Reiser, B. J.
| title = Intelligent tutoring systems
| journal = Science
| year = 1985
| volume = 228
| pages = 456–462
| doi = 10.1126/science.228.4698.456
| pmid = 17746875
| issue = 4698
}}
}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Anzai| reference= {{cite journal
|author=Anzai, K., & Simon, H. A. (1979)
|title=The theory of learning by doing
|journal=Psychological Review
|volume=86
|pages=124–140
|doi=10.1037/0033-295X.86.2.124
|year=1979
|pmid=493441
|issue=2}}
}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Beckmann| reference= Beckmann, J. F., & Guthke, J. (1995). Complex problem solving, intelligence, and learning ability. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 177-200). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Berry| reference= Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1995). Implicit learning in the control of complex systems: A reconsideration of some of the earlier claims. In P.A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 131-150). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Bhaskar| reference= Bhaskar, R., & Simon, H. A. (1977). Problem solving in semantically rich domains: An example from engineering thermodynamics. ''Cognitive Science'', 1, 193-215.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Brehmer1995| reference= Brehmer, B. (1995). Feedback delays in dynamic decision making. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 103-130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Brehmer1993| reference= Brehmer, B., & Dörner, D. (1993). Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study. ''Computers in Human Behavior'', 9, 171-184.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Broadbent| reference= Broadbent, D. E. (1977). Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control. ''Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology'', 29, 181-201.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Bryson| reference= Bryson, M., Bereiter, C., Scardamalia, M., & Joram, E. (1991). Going beyond the problem as given: Problem solving in expert and novice writers. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 61-84). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Buchner1995a| reference= Buchner, A. (1995). Theories of complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 27-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Buchner1995b| reference= Buchner, A., Funke, J., & Berry, D. C. (1995). Negative correlations between control performance and verbalizable knowledge: Indicators for implicit learning in process control tasks? ''Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology'', 48A, 166-187.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Chase| reference= Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. ''Cognitive Psychology'', 4, 55-81.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Chi| reference= {{cite journal
|author=Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R.
|year=1981
|title=Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices
|journal=Cognitive Science
|volume=5
|issue=2
|pages=121–152
|url=http://www.usabilityviews.com/uv007206.html
|doi=10.1207/s15516709cog0502_2}}
}}
* {{cite book
| last = Cornell
| first = Kate
| year = 2010
| title = WebKaizen, Better Faster Cheaper Problem Solving for Business
| location = Omaha NE
| publisher = Prevail Digital Publishing
| isbn = 978-0-9831102-1-7
}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Dörner1975| reference= Dörner, D. (1975). Wie Menschen eine Welt verbessern wollten [How people wanted to improve the world]. ''Bild der Wissenschaft'', 12, 48-53.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Dörner1985| reference= Dörner, D. (1985). Verhalten, Denken und Emotionen [Behavior, thinking, and emotions]. In L. H. Eckensberger & E. D. Lantermann (Eds.), ''Emotion und Reflexivität'' (pp. 157-181). München, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Dörner1992| reference= Dörner, D. (1992). Über die Philosophie der Verwendung von Mikrowelten oder "Computerszenarios" in der psychologischen Forschung [On the proper use of microworlds or "computer scenarios" in psychological research]. In H. Gundlach (Ed.), ''Psychologische Forschung und Methode: Das Versprechen des Experiments. [[Festschrift]] für Werner Traxel'' (pp. 53-87). Passau, Germany: Passavia-Universitäts-Verlag.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Dörner1983| reference= Dörner, D., Kreuzig, H. W., Reither, F., & Stäudel, T. (Eds.). (1983). ''Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität'' [Lohhausen. On dealing with uncertainty and complexity]. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Dörner1995| reference= Dörner, D., & Wearing, A. (1995). Complex problem solving: Toward a (computer-simulated) theory. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 65-99). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. }}
*{{Wikicite | id= Duncker| reference= Duncker, K. (1935). ''Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens'' [The psychology of productive thinking]. Berlin: Julius Springer.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Ewert| reference= Ewert, P. H., & Lambert, J. F. (1932). Part II: The effect of verbal instructions upon the formation of a concept. ''Journal of General Psychology'', 6, 400-411.}}
* {{cite journal | last1 = D'Zurilla | first1 = T. J. | last2 = Goldfried | first2 = M. R. | year = 1971 | title = Problem solving and behavior modification | url = | journal = Journal of Abnormal Psychology | volume = 78 | issue = | pages = 107–126 | doi=10.1037/h0031360}}
* D'Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1982). Social problem solving in adults. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (Vol. 1, pp.&nbsp;201–274). New York: Academic Press.
*{{Wikicite | id= Eyferth| reference= Eyferth, K., Schömann, M., & Widowski, D. (1986). Der Umgang von Psychologen mit Komplexität [On how psychologists deal with complexity]. ''Sprache & Kognition'', 5, 11-26.}}
* {{cite book | last = Fantin | first = Ivan | year = 2014 | title = Applied Problem Solving. Method, Applications, Root Causes, Countermeasures, Poka-Yoke and A3. How to make things happen to solve problems. | location = Milan, Italy | publisher = Createspace, an Amazon company | isbn = 978-1499122282}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Frensch1995| reference= Frensch, P. A., & Funke, J. (Eds.). (1995). ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective''. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Frensch1991| reference= Frensch, P. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1991). Skill-related differences in game playing. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 343-381). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Funke1991| reference= Funke, J. (1991). Solving complex problems: Human identification and control of complex systems. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 185-222). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Funke1993| reference= Funke, J. (1993). Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results. In G. Strube & K.-F. Wender (Eds.), ''The cognitive psychology of knowledge'' (pp. 313-330). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Funke1995a| reference= Funke, J. (1995). Experimental research on complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 243-268). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Funke1995b| reference= Funke, U. (1995). Complex problem solving in personnel selection and training. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 219-240). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Goldstein | reference= Goldstein F. C., & Levin H. S. (1987). Disorders of reasoning and problem-solving ability. In M. Meier, A. Benton, & L. Diller (Eds.), ''Neuropsychological rehabilitation''. London: Taylor & Francis Group.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Groner| reference= Groner, M., Groner, R., & Bischof, W. F. (1983). Approaches to heuristics: A historical review. In R. Groner, M. Groner, & W. F. Bischof (Eds.), ''Methods of heuristics'' (pp. 1-18). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id=Halpern| reference= Halpern, Diane F. (2002). Thought & Knowledge. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}} [http://worldcat.org/oclc/50065032&tab=holdings Worldcat Library Catalog]
*{{Wikicite | id= Hayes| reference= Hayes, J. (1980). ''The complete problem solver''. Philadelphia: The Franklin Institute Press.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Hegarty| reference= Hegarty, M. (1991). Knowledge and processes in mechanical problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 253-285). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Heppner| reference= Heppner, P. P., & Krauskopf, C. J. (1987). An information-processing approach to personal problem solving. ''The Counseling Psychologist'', 15, 371-447.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Huber| reference= Huber, O. (1995). Complex problem solving as multi stage decision making. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 151-173). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Hübner| reference= Hübner, R. (1989). Methoden zur Analyse und Konstruktion von Aufgaben zur kognitiven Steuerung dynamischer Systeme [Methods for the analysis and construction of dynamic system control tasks]. ''Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie'', 36, 221-238.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Hunt| reference= Hunt, E. (1991). Some comments on the study of complexity. In R. J. Sternberg, & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 383-395). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Hussy| reference= Hussy, W. (1985). Komplexes Problemlösen - Eine Sackgasse? [Complex problem solving - a dead end?]. ''Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie'', 32, 55-77.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Kay| reference= Kay, D. S. (1991). Computer interaction: Debugging the problems. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 317-340). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Kluwe1993| reference= Kluwe, R. H. (1993). Knowledge and performance in complex problem solving. In G. Strube & K.-F. Wender (Eds.), ''The cognitive psychology of knowledge'' (pp. 401-423). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Kluwe1995| reference= Kluwe, R. H. (1995). Single case studies and models of complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 269-291). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Kolb| reference= Kolb, S., Petzing, F., & Stumpf, S. (1992). Komplexes Problemlösen: Bestimmung der Problemlösegüte von Probanden mittels Verfahren des Operations Research ? ein interdisziplinärer Ansatz [Complex problem solving: determining the quality of human problem solving by operations research tools - an interdisciplinary approach]. ''Sprache & Kognition'', 11, 115-128.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Krems| reference= Krems, J. F. (1995). Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 201-218). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Lesgold| reference= Lesgold, A., & Lajoie, S. (1991). Complex problem solving in electronics. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 287-316). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Mayer| reference= Mayer, R. E. (1992). ''Thinking, problem solving, cognition''. Second edition. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Müller| reference= Müller, H. (1993). ''Komplexes Problemlösen: Reliabilität und Wissen'' [Complex problem solving: Reliability and knowledge]. Bonn, Germany: Holos.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Newell| reference= Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). ''Human problem solving''. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.}}
* {{cite book
| last = Offord
| first = Paul
| year = 2011
| title = RPR: A Problem Diagnosis Method for IT Professionals
| location = Essex, England
| publisher = Advance Seven Limited
| isbn = 978-1-4478-4443-3
}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Paradies| reference= Paradies, M.W., & Unger, L. W. (2000). ''TapRooT - The System for Root Cause Analysis, Problem Investigation, and Proactive Improvement''. Knoxville, TN: System Improvements.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Putz| reference= Putz-Osterloh, W. (1993). Strategies for knowledge acquisition and transfer of knowledge in dynamic tasks. In G. Strube & K.-F. Wender (Eds.), ''The cognitive psychology of knowledge'' (pp. 331-350). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.}}
* Rath J. F.; Langenbahn D. M.; Simon D.; Sherr R. L.; Fletcher J.; Diller L. (2004). The construct of problem solving in higher level neuropsychological assessment and rehabilitation. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 613-635. {{doi|10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.006}}
* Rath, J. F.; Simon, D.; Langenbahn, D. M.; Sherr, R. L.; Diller, L. (2003). Group treatment of problem-solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury: A randomised outcome study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 13, 461-488.
*{{Wikicite | id= Riefer| reference= Riefer, D.M., & Batchelder, W.H. (1988). Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes. ''Psychological Review'', 95, 318-339.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Ringelband| reference= Ringelband, O. J., Misiak, C., & Kluwe, R. H. (1990). Mental models and strategies in the control of a complex system. In D. Ackermann, & M. J. Tauber (Eds.), ''Mental models and human-computer interaction'' (Vol. 1, pp. 151-164). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Schaub| reference= Schaub, H. (1993). ''Modellierung der Handlungsorganisation''. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Schoenfeld| reference= Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). ''Mathematical Problem Solving''. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Sokol| reference= Sokol, S. M., & McCloskey, M. (1991). Cognitive mechanisms in calculation. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 85-116). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Stanovich| reference= Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1991). Reading as constrained reasoning. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 3-60). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Sternberg1995| reference= Sternberg, R. J. (1995). Conceptions of expertise in complex problem solving: A comparison of alternative conceptions. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: The European Perspective'' (pp. 295-321). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Sternberg1991| reference= Sternberg, R. J., & Frensch, P. A. (Eds.). (1991). ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms''. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Strauß| reference= Strauß, B. (1993). ''Konfundierungen beim Komplexen Problemlösen. Zum Einfluß des Anteils der richtigen Lösungen (ArL) auf das Problemlöseverhalten in komplexen Situationen'' [Confoundations in complex problem solving. On the influence of the degree of correct solutions on problem solving in complex situations]. Bonn, Germany: Holos.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Strohschneider| reference= Strohschneider, S. (1991). Kein System von Systemen! Kommentar zu dem Aufsatz "Systemmerkmale als Determinanten des Umgangs mit dynamischen Systemen" von Joachim Funke [No system of systems! Reply to the paper "System features as determinants of behavior in dynamic task environments" by Joachim Funke]. ''Sprache & Kognition'', 10, 109-113.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Van | reference= Van Lehn, K. (1989). Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), ''Foundations of cognitive science'' (pp. 527-579). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Voss| reference= Voss, J. F., Wolfe, C. R., Lawrence, J. A., & Engle, R. A. (1991). From representation to decision: An analysis of problem solving in international relations. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 119-158). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Wagner| reference= Wagner, R. K. (1991). Managerial problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), ''Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms'' (pp. 159-183). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.}}
*{{Wikicite | id= Wisconsin | reference= Wisconsin Educational Media Association. (1993). "Information literacy: A position paper on information problem-solving." Madison, WI: WEMA Publications. (ED 376 817). (Portions adapted from Michigan State Board of Education's Position Paper on Information Processing Skills, 1992).}}
* {{cite journal
|author=Blanchard-Fields, F.
|year=2007
|title= Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science
|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science
|volume=16
|issue=1
|pages=26–31
|doi= 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x}}
* {{cite book
| author=Bransford, J. D., & Stein, B. S
| year = 1993
| title = The ideal problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity (2nd ed.)
}}
* {{cite journal
|author=Ghasempour,Z., Md Nor Bakar and Jahanshahloo G. R.
|year = 2013
|title=Innovation in Teaching and Learning through Problem Posing Tasks and Metacognitive Strategies
| url=http://naturalspublishing.com/files/published/vw6qz9526444la.pdf
}}
*{{cite journal|last=Condell|author2=Wade,Galway,McBride,Gormley,Brennan,Somasundram|journal=Springer Science+Business Media|year=2010|volume=2010|pages=223–233|url=http://journals1.scholarsportal.info.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/tmp/7323702971461253957.pdf}}{{Refend}}


==External links==
==External links==
*{{Wikiversity inline|Solving Problems}}

{{wikiquote}}
* [http://suresolv.com/ SureSolv - A place for learning, critical thinking and...problem solving]
* [http://www.ericdigests.org/1996-4/skills.htm Computer skills for information problem-solving: Learning and teaching technology in context]
* [http://www.sethchernoff.com/spirituality/change-embrace-spiritual-guide-problem-solving/ Own it, Change it, and Embrace it – Your Spiritual Guide to Problem Solving]
* [http://wik.ed.uiuc.edu/articles/p/r/o/Problem_solving-Elementary_level_320d.html Problem solving - Elementary level]


{{Human intelligence topics}}
{{Human intelligence topics}}
{{Footer Neuropsychology}}
{{Virtues}}


{{Authority control}}
{{Authority control}}


[[Category:Problem solving| ]]
[[Category:Problem solving| ]]
[[Category:Reasoning]]
[[Category:Artificial intelligence]]
[[Category:Artificial intelligence]]
[[Category:Educational psychology]]
[[Category:Educational psychology]]
[[Category:Mental processes]]
[[Category:Cognitive psychology]]
[[Category:Neuropsychological assessment]]
[[Category:Neuropsychological assessment]]
[[Category:Psychology articles needing expert attention]]
[[Category:Psychology articles needing expert attention]]

Latest revision as of 06:13, 1 January 2025

Problem solving is the process of achieving a goal by overcoming obstacles, a frequent part of most activities. Problems in need of solutions range from simple personal tasks (e.g. how to turn on an appliance) to complex issues in business and technical fields. The former is an example of simple problem solving (SPS) addressing one issue, whereas the latter is complex problem solving (CPS) with multiple interrelated obstacles.[1] Another classification of problem-solving tasks is into well-defined problems with specific obstacles and goals, and ill-defined problems in which the current situation is troublesome but it is not clear what kind of resolution to aim for.[2] Similarly, one may distinguish formal or fact-based problems requiring psychometric intelligence, versus socio-emotional problems which depend on the changeable emotions of individuals or groups, such as tactful behavior, fashion, or gift choices.[3]

Solutions require sufficient resources and knowledge to attain the goal. Professionals such as lawyers, doctors, programmers, and consultants are largely problem solvers for issues that require technical skills and knowledge beyond general competence. Many businesses have found profitable markets by recognizing a problem and creating a solution: the more widespread and inconvenient the problem, the greater the opportunity to develop a scalable solution.

There are many specialized problem-solving techniques and methods in fields such as science, engineering, business, medicine, mathematics, computer science, philosophy, and social organization. The mental techniques to identify, analyze, and solve problems are studied in psychology and cognitive sciences. Also widely researched are the mental obstacles that prevent people from finding solutions; problem-solving impediments include confirmation bias, mental set, and functional fixedness.

Definition

[edit]

The term problem solving has a slightly different meaning depending on the discipline. For instance, it is a mental process in psychology and a computerized process in computer science. There are two different types of problems: ill-defined and well-defined; different approaches are used for each. Well-defined problems have specific end goals and clearly expected solutions, while ill-defined problems do not. Well-defined problems allow for more initial planning than ill-defined problems.[2] Solving problems sometimes involves dealing with pragmatics (the way that context contributes to meaning) and semantics (the interpretation of the problem). The ability to understand what the end goal of the problem is, and what rules could be applied, represents the key to solving the problem. Sometimes a problem requires abstract thinking or coming up with a creative solution.

Problem solving has two major domains: mathematical problem solving and personal problem solving. Each concerns some difficulty or barrier that is encountered.[4]

Psychology

[edit]

Problem solving in psychology refers to the process of finding solutions to problems encountered in life.[5] Solutions to these problems are usually situation- or context-specific. The process starts with problem finding and problem shaping, in which the problem is discovered and simplified. The next step is to generate possible solutions and evaluate them. Finally a solution is selected to be implemented and verified. Problems have an end goal to be reached; how you get there depends upon problem orientation (problem-solving coping style and skills) and systematic analysis.[6]

Mental health professionals study the human problem-solving processes using methods such as introspection, behaviorism, simulation, computer modeling, and experiment. Social psychologists look into the person-environment relationship aspect of the problem and independent and interdependent problem-solving methods.[7] Problem solving has been defined as a higher-order cognitive process and intellectual function that requires the modulation and control of more routine or fundamental skills.[8]

Empirical research shows many different strategies and factors influence everyday problem solving.[9] Rehabilitation psychologists studying people with frontal lobe injuries have found that deficits in emotional control and reasoning can be re-mediated with effective rehabilitation and could improve the capacity of injured persons to resolve everyday problems.[10] Interpersonal everyday problem solving is dependent upon personal motivational and contextual components. One such component is the emotional valence of "real-world" problems, which can either impede or aid problem-solving performance. Researchers have focused on the role of emotions in problem solving,[11] demonstrating that poor emotional control can disrupt focus on the target task, impede problem resolution, and lead to negative outcomes such as fatigue, depression, and inertia.[12] In conceptualization,[clarification needed]human problem solving consists of two related processes: problem orientation, and the motivational/attitudinal/affective approach to problematic situations and problem-solving skills.[13] People's strategies cohere with their goals[14] and stem from the process of comparing oneself with others.

Cognitive sciences

[edit]

Among the first experimental psychologists to study problem solving were the Gestaltists in Germany, such as Karl Duncker in The Psychology of Productive Thinking (1935).[15] Perhaps best known is the work of Allen Newell and Herbert A. Simon.[16]

Experiments in the 1960s and early 1970s asked participants to solve relatively simple, well-defined, but not previously seen laboratory tasks.[17][18] These simple problems, such as the Tower of Hanoi, admitted optimal solutions that could be found quickly, allowing researchers to observe the full problem-solving process. Researchers assumed that these model problems would elicit the characteristic cognitive processes by which more complex "real world" problems are solved.

An outstanding problem-solving technique found by this research is the principle of decomposition.[19]

Computer science

[edit]

Much of computer science and artificial intelligence involves designing automated systems to solve a specified type of problem: to accept input data and calculate a correct or adequate response, reasonably quickly. Algorithms are recipes or instructions that direct such systems, written into computer programs.

Steps for designing such systems include problem determination, heuristics, root cause analysis, de-duplication, analysis, diagnosis, and repair. Analytic techniques include linear and nonlinear programming, queuing systems, and simulation.[20] A large, perennial obstacle is to find and fix errors in computer programs: debugging.

Logic

[edit]

Formal logic concerns issues like validity, truth, inference, argumentation, and proof. In a problem-solving context, it can be used to formally represent a problem as a theorem to be proved, and to represent the knowledge needed to solve the problem as the premises to be used in a proof that the problem has a solution.

The use of computers to prove mathematical theorems using formal logic emerged as the field of automated theorem proving in the 1950s. It included the use of heuristic methods designed to simulate human problem solving, as in the Logic Theory Machine, developed by Allen Newell, Herbert A. Simon and J. C. Shaw, as well as algorithmic methods such as the resolution principle developed by John Alan Robinson.

In addition to its use for finding proofs of mathematical theorems, automated theorem-proving has also been used for program verification in computer science. In 1958, John McCarthy proposed the advice taker, to represent information in formal logic and to derive answers to questions using automated theorem-proving. An important step in this direction was made by Cordell Green in 1969, who used a resolution theorem prover for question-answering and for such other applications in artificial intelligence as robot planning.

The resolution theorem-prover used by Cordell Green bore little resemblance to human problem solving methods. In response to criticism of that approach from researchers at MIT, Robert Kowalski developed logic programming and SLD resolution,[21] which solves problems by problem decomposition. He has advocated logic for both computer and human problem solving[22] and computational logic to improve human thinking.[23]

Engineering

[edit]

When products or processes fail, problem solving techniques can be used to develop corrective actions that can be taken to prevent further failures. Such techniques can also be applied to a product or process prior to an actual failure event—to predict, analyze, and mitigate a potential problem in advance. Techniques such as failure mode and effects analysis can proactively reduce the likelihood of problems.

In either the reactive or the proactive case, it is necessary to build a causal explanation through a process of diagnosis. In deriving an explanation of effects in terms of causes, abduction generates new ideas or hypotheses (asking "how?"); deduction evaluates and refines hypotheses based on other plausible premises (asking "why?"); and induction justifies a hypothesis with empirical data (asking "how much?").[24] The objective of abduction is to determine which hypothesis or proposition to test, not which one to adopt or assert.[25] In the Peircean logical system, the logic of abduction and deduction contribute to our conceptual understanding of a phenomenon, while the logic of induction adds quantitative details (empirical substantiation) to our conceptual knowledge.[26]

Forensic engineering is an important technique of failure analysis that involves tracing product defects and flaws. Corrective action can then be taken to prevent further failures.

Reverse engineering attempts to discover the original problem-solving logic used in developing a product by disassembling the product and developing a plausible pathway to creating and assembling its parts.[27]

Military science

[edit]

In military science, problem solving is linked to the concept of "end-states", the conditions or situations which are the aims of the strategy.[28]: xiii, E-2  Ability to solve problems is important at any military rank, but is essential at the command and control level. It results from deep qualitative and quantitative understanding of possible scenarios. Effectiveness in this context is an evaluation of results: to what extent the end states were accomplished.[28]: IV-24  Planning is the process of determining how to effect those end states.[28]: IV-1 

Processes

[edit]

Some models of problem solving involve identifying a goal and then a sequence of subgoals towards achieving this goal. Andersson, who introduced the ACT-R model of cognition, modelled this collection of goals and subgoals as a goal stack in which the mind contains a stack of goals and subgoals to be completed, and a single task being carried out at any time.[29]: 51 

Knowledge of how to solve one problem can be applied to another problem, in a process known as transfer.[29]: 56 

Problem-solving strategies

[edit]

Problem-solving strategies are steps to overcoming the obstacles to achieving a goal. The iteration of such strategies over the course of solving a problem is the "problem-solving cycle".[30]

Common steps in this cycle include recognizing the problem, defining it, developing a strategy to fix it, organizing knowledge and resources available, monitoring progress, and evaluating the effectiveness of the solution. Once a solution is achieved, another problem usually arises, and the cycle starts again.

Insight is the sudden aha! solution to a problem, the birth of a new idea to simplify a complex situation. Solutions found through insight are often more incisive than those from step-by-step analysis. A quick solution process requires insight to select productive moves at different stages of the problem-solving cycle. Unlike Newell and Simon's formal definition of a move problem, there is no consensus definition of an insight problem.[31]

Some problem-solving strategies include:[32]

Abstraction
solving the problem in a tractable model system to gain insight into the real system
Analogy
adapting the solution to a previous problem which has similar features or mechanisms
Brainstorming
(especially among groups of people) suggesting a large number of solutions or ideas and combining and developing them until an optimum solution is found
Bypasses
transform the problem into another problem that is easier to solve, bypassing the barrier, then transform that solution back to a solution to the original problem.
Critical thinking
analysis of available evidence and arguments to form a judgement via rational, skeptical, and unbiased evaluation
Divide and conquer
breaking down a large, complex problem into smaller, solvable problems
Help-seeking
obtaining external assistance to deal with obstacles
Hypothesis testing
assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove (or, in some contexts, disprove) the assumption
Lateral thinking
approaching solutions indirectly and creatively
Means-ends analysis
choosing an action at each step to move closer to the goal
Morphological analysis
assessing the output and interactions of an entire system
Observation / Question
in the natural sciences an observation is an act or instance of noticing or perceiving and the acquisition of information from a primary source. A question is an utterance which serves as a request for information.[citation needed]
Proof of impossibility
try to prove that the problem cannot be solved. The point where the proof fails will be the starting point for solving it
Reduction
transforming the problem into another problem for which solutions exist
Research
employing existing ideas or adapting existing solutions to similar problems
Root cause analysis
identifying the cause of a problem
Trial-and-error
testing possible solutions until the right one is found

Problem-solving methods

[edit]

Common barriers

[edit]

Common barriers to problem solving include mental constructs that impede an efficient search for solutions. Five of the most common identified by researchers are: confirmation bias, mental set, functional fixedness, unnecessary constraints, and irrelevant information.

Confirmation bias

[edit]

Confirmation bias is an unintentional tendency to collect and use data which favors preconceived notions. Such notions may be incidental rather than motivated by important personal beliefs: the desire to be right may be sufficient motivation.[33]

Scientific and technical professionals also experience confirmation bias. One online experiment, for example, suggested that professionals within the field of psychological research are likely to view scientific studies that agree with their preconceived notions more favorably than clashing studies.[34] According to Raymond Nickerson, one can see the consequences of confirmation bias in real-life situations, which range in severity from inefficient government policies to genocide. Nickerson argued that those who killed people accused of witchcraft demonstrated confirmation bias with motivation.[citation needed] Researcher Michael Allen found evidence for confirmation bias with motivation in school children who worked to manipulate their science experiments to produce favorable results.[35]

However, confirmation bias does not necessarily require motivation. In 1960, Peter Cathcart Wason conducted an experiment in which participants first viewed three numbers and then created a hypothesis in the form of a rule that could have been used to create that triplet of numbers. When testing their hypotheses, participants tended to only create additional triplets of numbers that would confirm their hypotheses, and tended not to create triplets that would negate or disprove their hypotheses.[36]

Mental set

[edit]

Mental set is the inclination to re-use a previously successful solution, rather than search for new and better solutions. It is a reliance on habit.

It was first articulated by Abraham S. Luchins in the 1940s with his well-known water jug experiments.[37] Participants were asked to fill one jug with a specific amount of water by using other jugs with different maximum capacities. After Luchins gave a set of jug problems that could all be solved by a single technique, he then introduced a problem that could be solved by the same technique, but also by a novel and simpler method. His participants tended to use the accustomed technique, oblivious of the simpler alternative.[38] This was again demonstrated in Norman Maier's 1931 experiment, which challenged participants to solve a problem by using a familiar tool (pliers) in an unconventional manner. Participants were often unable to view the object in a way that strayed from its typical use, a type of mental set known as functional fixedness (see the following section).

Rigidly clinging to a mental set is called fixation, which can deepen to an obsession or preoccupation with attempted strategies that are repeatedly unsuccessful.[39] In the late 1990s, researcher Jennifer Wiley found that professional expertise in a field can create a mental set, perhaps leading to fixation.[39]

Groupthink, in which each individual takes on the mindset of the rest of the group, can produce and exacerbate mental set.[40] Social pressure leads to everybody thinking the same thing and reaching the same conclusions.

Functional fixedness

[edit]

Functional fixedness is the tendency to view an object as having only one function, and to be unable to conceive of any novel use, as in the Maier pliers experiment described above. Functional fixedness is a specific form of mental set, and is one of the most common forms of cognitive bias in daily life.

As an example, imagine a man wants to kill a bug in his house, but the only thing at hand is a can of air freshener. He may start searching for something to kill the bug instead of squashing it with the can, thinking only of its main function of deodorizing.

Tim German and Clark Barrett describe this barrier: "subjects become 'fixed' on the design function of the objects, and problem solving suffers relative to control conditions in which the object's function is not demonstrated."[41] Their research found that young children's limited knowledge of an object's intended function reduces this barrier[42] Research has also discovered functional fixedness in educational contexts, as an obstacle to understanding: "functional fixedness may be found in learning concepts as well as in solving chemistry problems."[43]

There are several hypotheses in regards to how functional fixedness relates to problem solving.[44] It may waste time, delaying or entirely preventing the correct use of a tool.

Unnecessary constraints

[edit]

Unnecessary constraints are arbitrary boundaries imposed unconsciously on the task at hand, which foreclose a productive avenue of solution. The solver may become fixated on only one type of solution, as if it were an inevitable requirement of the problem. Typically, this combines with mental set—clinging to a previously successful method.[45][page needed]

Visual problems can also produce mentally invented constraints.[46][page needed] A famous example is the dot problem: nine dots arranged in a three-by-three grid pattern must be connected by drawing four straight line segments, without lifting pen from paper or backtracking along a line. The subject typically assumes the pen must stay within the outer square of dots, but the solution requires lines continuing beyond this frame, and researchers have found a 0% solution rate within a brief allotted time.[47]

This problem has produced the expression "think outside the box".[48][page needed] Such problems are typically solved via a sudden insight which leaps over the mental barriers, often after long toil against them.[49] This can be difficult depending on how the subject has structured the problem in their mind, how they draw on past experiences, and how well they juggle this information in their working memory. In the example, envisioning the dots connected outside the framing square requires visualizing an unconventional arrangement, which is a strain on working memory.[48]

Irrelevant information

[edit]

Irrelevant information is a specification or data presented in a problem that is unrelated to the solution.[45] If the solver assumes that all information presented needs to be used, this often derails the problem solving process, making relatively simple problems much harder.[50]

For example: "Fifteen percent of the people in Topeka have unlisted telephone numbers. You select 200 names at random from the Topeka phone book. How many of these people have unlisted phone numbers?"[48][page needed] The "obvious" answer is 15%, but in fact none of the unlisted people would be listed among the 200. This kind of "trick question" is often used in aptitude tests or cognitive evaluations.[51] Though not inherently difficult, they require independent thinking that is not necessarily common. Mathematical word problems often include irrelevant qualitative or numerical information as an extra challenge.

Avoiding barriers by changing problem representation

[edit]

The disruption caused by the above cognitive biases can depend on how the information is represented:[51] visually, verbally, or mathematically. A classic example is the Buddhist monk problem:

A Buddhist monk begins at dawn one day walking up a mountain, reaches the top at sunset, meditates at the top for several days until one dawn when he begins to walk back to the foot of the mountain, which he reaches at sunset. Making no assumptions about his starting or stopping or about his pace during the trips, prove that there is a place on the path which he occupies at the same hour of the day on the two separate journeys.

The problem cannot be addressed in a verbal context, trying to describe the monk's progress on each day. It becomes much easier when the paragraph is represented mathematically by a function: one visualizes a graph whose horizontal axis is time of day, and whose vertical axis shows the monk's position (or altitude) on the path at each time. Superimposing the two journey curves, which traverse opposite diagonals of a rectangle, one sees they must cross each other somewhere. The visual representation by graphing has resolved the difficulty.

Similar strategies can often improve problem solving on tests.[45][52]

Other barriers for individuals

[edit]

People who are engaged in problem solving tend to overlook subtractive changes, even those that are critical elements of efficient solutions.[example needed] This tendency to solve by first, only, or mostly creating or adding elements, rather than by subtracting elements or processes is shown to intensify with higher cognitive loads such as information overload.[53]

Dreaming: problem solving without waking consciousness

[edit]

People can also solve problems while they are asleep. There are many reports of scientists and engineers who solved problems in their dreams. For example, Elias Howe, inventor of the sewing machine, figured out the structure of the bobbin from a dream.[54]

The chemist August Kekulé was considering how benzene arranged its six carbon and hydrogen atoms. Thinking about the problem, he dozed off, and dreamt of dancing atoms that fell into a snakelike pattern, which led him to discover the benzene ring. As Kekulé wrote in his diary,

One of the snakes seized hold of its own tail, and the form whirled mockingly before my eyes. As if by a flash of lightning I awoke; and this time also I spent the rest of the night in working out the consequences of the hypothesis.[55]

There also are empirical studies of how people can think consciously about a problem before going to sleep, and then solve the problem with a dream image. Dream researcher William C. Dement told his undergraduate class of 500 students that he wanted them to think about an infinite series, whose first elements were OTTFF, to see if they could deduce the principle behind it and to say what the next elements of the series would be.[56][page needed] He asked them to think about this problem every night for 15 minutes before going to sleep and to write down any dreams that they then had. They were instructed to think about the problem again for 15 minutes when they awakened in the morning.

The sequence OTTFF is the first letters of the numbers: one, two, three, four, five. The next five elements of the series are SSENT (six, seven, eight, nine, ten). Some of the students solved the puzzle by reflecting on their dreams. One example was a student who reported the following dream:[56][page needed]

I was standing in an art gallery, looking at the paintings on the wall. As I walked down the hall, I began to count the paintings: one, two, three, four, five. As I came to the sixth and seventh, the paintings had been ripped from their frames. I stared at the empty frames with a peculiar feeling that some mystery was about to be solved. Suddenly I realized that the sixth and seventh spaces were the solution to the problem!

With more than 500 undergraduate students, 87 dreams were judged to be related to the problems students were assigned (53 directly related and 34 indirectly related). Yet of the people who had dreams that apparently solved the problem, only seven were actually able to consciously know the solution. The rest (46 out of 53) thought they did not know the solution.

Mark Blechner conducted this experiment and obtained results similar to Dement's.[57][page needed] He found that while trying to solve the problem, people had dreams in which the solution appeared to be obvious from the dream, but it was rare for the dreamers to realize how their dreams had solved the puzzle. Coaxing or hints did not get them to realize it, although once they heard the solution, they recognized how their dream had solved it. For example, one person in that OTTFF experiment dreamed:[57][page needed]

There is a big clock. You can see the movement. The big hand of the clock was on the number six. You could see it move up, number by number, six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven, twelve. The dream focused on the small parts of the machinery. You could see the gears inside.

In the dream, the person counted out the next elements of the series—six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven, twelve—yet he did not realize that this was the solution of the problem. His sleeping mindbrain[jargon] solved the problem, but his waking mindbrain was not aware how.

Albert Einstein believed that much problem solving goes on unconsciously, and the person must then figure out and formulate consciously what the mindbrain[jargon] has already solved. He believed this was his process in formulating the theory of relativity: "The creator of the problem possesses the solution."[58] Einstein said that he did his problem solving without words, mostly in images. "The words or the language, as they are written or spoken, do not seem to play any role in my mechanism of thought. The psychical entities which seem to serve as elements in thought are certain signs and more or less clear images which can be 'voluntarily' reproduced and combined."[59]

Cognitive sciences: two schools

[edit]

Problem-solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise.[60] For this reason, cognitive sciences findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to problem-solving situations outside the laboratory. This has led to a research emphasis on real-world problem solving, since the 1990s. This emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North America and Europe, however. Whereas North American research has typically concentrated on studying problem solving in separate, natural knowledge domains, much of the European research has focused on novel, complex problems, and has been performed with computerized scenarios.[61]

Europe

[edit]

In Europe, two main approaches have surfaced, one initiated by Donald Broadbent[62] in the United Kingdom and the other one by Dietrich Dörner[63] in Germany. The two approaches share an emphasis on relatively complex, semantically rich, computerized laboratory tasks, constructed to resemble real-life problems. The approaches differ somewhat in their theoretical goals and methodology. The tradition initiated by Broadbent emphasizes the distinction between cognitive problem-solving processes that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness, and typically employs mathematically well-defined computerized systems. The tradition initiated by Dörner, on the other hand, has an interest in the interplay of the cognitive, motivational, and social components of problem solving, and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain up to 2,000 highly interconnected variables.[64]

North America

[edit]

In North America, initiated by the work of Herbert A. Simon on "learning by doing" in semantically rich domains,[65] researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural knowledge domains—such as physics, writing, or chess playing—rather than attempt to extract a global theory of problem solving.[66] These researchers have focused on the development of problem solving within certain domains, that is on the development of expertise.[67]

Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include:

  • calculation[68]
  • computer skills[69]
  • game playing[70]
  • lawyers' reasoning[71]
  • managerial problem solving[72]
  • mathematical problem solving[73]
  • mechanical problem solving[74]
  • personal problem solving[75]
  • political decision making[76]
  • problem solving in electronics[77]
  • problem solving for innovations and inventions: TRIZ[78]
  • reading[79]
  • social problem solving[11]
  • writing[80]

Characteristics of complex problems

[edit]

Complex problem solving (CPS) is distinguishable from simple problem solving (SPS). In SPS there is a singular and simple obstacle. In CPS there may be multiple simultaneous obstacles. For example, a surgeon at work has far more complex problems than an individual deciding what shoes to wear. As elucidated by Dietrich Dörner, and later expanded upon by Joachim Funke, complex problems have some typical characteristics, which include:[1]

Collective problem solving

[edit]

People solve problems on many different levels—from the individual to the civilizational. Collective problem solving refers to problem solving performed collectively. Social issues and global issues can typically only be solved collectively.

The complexity of contemporary problems exceeds the cognitive capacity of any individual and requires different but complementary varieties of expertise and collective problem solving ability.[82]

Collective intelligence is shared or group intelligence that emerges from the collaboration, collective efforts, and competition of many individuals.

In collaborative problem solving people work together to solve real-world problems. Members of problem-solving groups share a common concern, a similar passion, and/or a commitment to their work. Members can ask questions, wonder, and try to understand common issues. They share expertise, experiences, tools, and methods.[83] Groups may be fluid based on need, may only occur temporarily to finish an assigned task, or may be more permanent depending on the nature of the problems.

For example, in the educational context, members of a group may all have input into the decision-making process and a role in the learning process. Members may be responsible for the thinking, teaching, and monitoring of all members in the group. Group work may be coordinated among members so that each member makes an equal contribution to the whole work. Members can identify and build on their individual strengths so that everyone can make a significant contribution to the task.[84] Collaborative group work has the ability to promote critical thinking skills, problem solving skills, social skills, and self-esteem. By using collaboration and communication, members often learn from one another and construct meaningful knowledge that often leads to better learning outcomes than individual work.[85]

Collaborative groups require joint intellectual efforts between the members and involve social interactions to solve problems together. The knowledge shared during these interactions is acquired during communication, negotiation, and production of materials.[86] Members actively seek information from others by asking questions. The capacity to use questions to acquire new information increases understanding and the ability to solve problems.[87]

In a 1962 research report, Douglas Engelbart linked collective intelligence to organizational effectiveness, and predicted that proactively "augmenting human intellect" would yield a multiplier effect in group problem solving: "Three people working together in this augmented mode [would] seem to be more than three times as effective in solving a complex problem as is one augmented person working alone".[88]

Henry Jenkins, a theorist of new media and media convergence, draws on the theory that collective intelligence can be attributed to media convergence and participatory culture.[89] He criticizes contemporary education for failing to incorporate online trends of collective problem solving into the classroom, stating "whereas a collective intelligence community encourages ownership of work as a group, schools grade individuals". Jenkins argues that interaction within a knowledge community builds vital skills for young people, and teamwork through collective intelligence communities contributes to the development of such skills.[90]

Collective impact is the commitment of a group of actors from different sectors to a common agenda for solving a specific social problem, using a structured form of collaboration.

After World War II the UN, the Bretton Woods organization, and the WTO were created. Collective problem solving on the international level crystallized around these three types of organization from the 1980s onward. As these global institutions remain state-like or state-centric it is unsurprising that they perpetuate state-like or state-centric approaches to collective problem solving rather than alternative ones.[91]

Crowdsourcing is a process of accumulating ideas, thoughts, or information from many independent participants, with aim of finding the best solution for a given challenge. Modern information technologies allow for many people to be involved and facilitate managing their suggestions in ways that provide good results.[92] The Internet allows for a new capacity of collective (including planetary-scale) problem solving.[93]

See also

[edit]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Frensch, Peter A.; Funke, Joachim, eds. (2014-04-04). Complex Problem Solving. Psychology Press. doi:10.4324/9781315806723. ISBN 978-1-315-80672-3.
  2. ^ a b Schacter, D.L.; Gilbert, D.T.; Wegner, D.M. (2011). Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. p. 376.
  3. ^ Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). "Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (1): 26–31. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x. S2CID 145645352.
  4. ^ Zimmermann, Bernd (2004). On mathematical problem-solving processes and history of mathematics. ICME 10. Copenhagen.
  5. ^ Granvold, Donald K. (1997). "Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy with Adults". In Brandell, Jerrold R. (ed.). Theory and Practice in Clinical Social Work. Simon and Schuster. pp. 189. ISBN 978-0-684-82765-0.
  6. ^ Robertson, S. Ian (2001). "Introduction to the study of problem solving". Problem Solving. Psychology Press. ISBN 0-415-20300-7.
  7. ^ Rubin, M.; Watt, S. E.; Ramelli, M. (2012). "Immigrants' social integration as a function of approach-avoidance orientation and problem-solving style". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 36 (4): 498–505. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.12.009. hdl:1959.13/931119.
  8. ^ Goldstein F. C.; Levin H. S. (1987). "Disorders of reasoning and problem-solving ability". In M. Meier; A. Benton; L. Diller (eds.). Neuropsychological rehabilitation. London: Taylor & Francis Group.
  9. ^
  10. ^ Rath, Joseph F.; Simon, Dvorah; Langenbahn, Donna M.; Sherr, Rose Lynn; Diller, Leonard (2003). "Group treatment of problem-solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury: A randomised outcome study". Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 13 (4): 461–488. doi:10.1080/09602010343000039. S2CID 143165070.
  11. ^ a b
    • D'Zurilla, T. J.; Goldfried, M. R. (1971). "Problem solving and behavior modification". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 78 (1): 107–126. doi:10.1037/h0031360. PMID 4938262.
    • D'Zurilla, T. J.; Nezu, A. M. (1982). "Social problem solving in adults". In P. C. Kendall (ed.). Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press. pp. 201–274.
  12. ^ Rath, J. F.; Langenbahn, D. M.; Simon, D; Sherr, R. L.; Fletcher, J.; Diller, L. (2004). "The construct of problem solving in higher level neuropsychological assessment and rehabilitation*1". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 19 (5): 613–635. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.006. PMID 15271407.
  13. ^ Rath, Joseph F.; Hradil, Amy L.; Litke, David R.; Diller, Leonard (2011). "Clinical applications of problem-solving research in neuropsychological rehabilitation: Addressing the subjective experience of cognitive deficits in outpatients with acquired brain injury". Rehabilitation Psychology. 56 (4): 320–328. doi:10.1037/a0025817. ISSN 1939-1544. PMC 9728040. PMID 22121939.
  14. ^ Hoppmann, Christiane A.; Blanchard-Fields, Fredda (2010). "Goals and everyday problem solving: Manipulating goal preferences in young and older adults". Developmental Psychology. 46 (6): 1433–1443. doi:10.1037/a0020676. PMID 20873926.
  15. ^ Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.
  16. ^ Newell, Allen; Simon, Herbert A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
  17. ^ For example:
  18. ^ Mayer, R. E. (1992). Thinking, problem solving, cognition (Second ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
  19. ^ Armstrong, J. Scott; Denniston, William B. Jr.; Gordon, Matt M. (1975). "The Use of the Decomposition Principle in Making Judgments" (PDF). Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 14 (2): 257–263. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(75)90028-8. S2CID 122659209. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2010-06-20.
  20. ^ Malakooti, Behnam (2013). Operations and Production Systems with Multiple Objectives. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-118-58537-5.
  21. ^ Kowalski, Robert (1974). "Predicate Logic as a Programming Language" (PDF). Information Processing. 74. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2024-01-19. Retrieved 2023-09-20.
  22. ^ Kowalski, Robert (1979). Logic for Problem Solving (PDF). Artificial Intelligence Series. Vol. 7. Elsevier Science Publishing. ISBN 0-444-00368-1. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2023-11-02. Retrieved 2023-09-20.
  23. ^ Kowalski, Robert (2011). Computational Logic and Human Thinking: How to be Artificially Intelligent (PDF). Cambridge University Press. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2024-06-01. Retrieved 2023-09-20.
  24. ^ Staat, Wim (1993). "On abduction, deduction, induction and the categories". Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society. 29 (2): 225–237.
  25. ^ Sullivan, Patrick F. (1991). "On Falsificationist Interpretations of Peirce". Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society. 27 (2): 197–219.
  26. ^ Ho, Yu Chong (1994). Abduction? Deduction? Induction? Is There a Logic of Exploratory Data Analysis? (PDF). Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. New Orleans, La. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2023-11-02. Retrieved 2023-09-20.
  27. ^ Passuello, Luciano (2008-11-04). "Einstein's Secret to Amazing Problem Solving (and 10 Specific Ways You Can Use It)". Litemind. Archived from the original on 2017-06-21. Retrieved 2017-06-11.
  28. ^ a b c "Commander's Handbook for Strategic Communication and Communication Strategy" (PDF). United States Joint Forces Command, Joint Warfighting Center, Suffolk, Va. 27 October 2009. Archived from the original (PDF) on April 29, 2011. Retrieved 10 October 2016.
  29. ^ a b Robertson, S. Ian (2017). Problem solving: perspectives from cognition and neuroscience (2nd ed.). London: Taylor & Francis. ISBN 978-1-317-49601-4. OCLC 962750529.
  30. ^ Bransford, J. D.; Stein, B. S (1993). The ideal problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity (2nd ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman.
  31. ^
  32. ^ Wang, Y.; Chiew, V. (2010). "On the cognitive process of human problem solving" (PDF). Cognitive Systems Research. 11 (1). Elsevier BV: 81–92. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.08.003. ISSN 1389-0417. S2CID 16238486.
  33. ^ Nickerson, Raymond S. (1998). "Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises". Review of General Psychology. 2 (2): 176. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175. S2CID 8508954.
  34. ^ Hergovich, Andreas; Schott, Reinhard; Burger, Christoph (2010). "Biased Evaluation of Abstracts Depending on Topic and Conclusion: Further Evidence of a Confirmation Bias Within Scientific Psychology". Current Psychology. 29 (3). Springer Science and Business Media LLC: 188–209. doi:10.1007/s12144-010-9087-5. ISSN 1046-1310. S2CID 145497196.
  35. ^ Allen, Michael (2011). "Theory-led confirmation bias and experimental persona". Research in Science & Technological Education. 29 (1). Informa UK Limited: 107–127. Bibcode:2011RSTEd..29..107A. doi:10.1080/02635143.2010.539973. ISSN 0263-5143. S2CID 145706148.
  36. ^ Wason, P. C. (1960). "On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 12 (3): 129–140. doi:10.1080/17470216008416717. S2CID 19237642.
  37. ^ Luchins, Abraham S. (1942). "Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung". Psychological Monographs. 54 (248): i-95. doi:10.1037/h0093502.
  38. ^ Öllinger, Michael; Jones, Gary; Knoblich, Günther (2008). "Investigating the Effect of Mental Set on Insight Problem Solving" (PDF). Experimental Psychology. 55 (4). Hogrefe Publishing Group: 269–282. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.269. ISSN 1618-3169. PMID 18683624. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2023-03-16. Retrieved 2023-01-31.
  39. ^ a b Wiley, Jennifer (1998). "Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving". Memory & Cognition. 24 (4): 716–730. doi:10.3758/bf03211392. PMID 9701964.
  40. ^ Cottam, Martha L.; Dietz-Uhler, Beth; Mastors, Elena; Preston, Thomas (2010). Introduction to Political Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.
  41. ^ German, Tim P.; Barrett, H. Clark (2005). "Functional Fixedness in a Technologically Sparse Culture". Psychological Science. 16 (1). SAGE Publications: 1–5. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00771.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 15660843. S2CID 1833823.
  42. ^ German, Tim P.; Defeyter, Margaret A. (2000). "Immunity to functional fixedness in young children". Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 7 (4): 707–712. doi:10.3758/BF03213010. PMID 11206213.
  43. ^ Furio, C.; Calatayud, M. L.; Baracenas, S.; Padilla, O. (2000). "Functional fixedness and functional reduction as common sense reasonings in chemical equilibrium and in geometry and polarity of molecules". Science Education. 84 (5): 545–565. Bibcode:2000SciEd..84..545F. doi:10.1002/1098-237X(200009)84:5<545::AID-SCE1>3.0.CO;2-1.
  44. ^ Adamson, Robert E (1952). "Functional fixedness as related to problem solving: A repetition of three experiments". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 44 (4): 288–291. doi:10.1037/h0062487. PMID 13000071.
  45. ^ a b c Kellogg, R. T. (2003). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). California: Sage Publications, Inc.
  46. ^ Meloy, J. R. (1998). The Psychology of Stalking, Clinical and Forensic Perspectives (2nd ed.). London, England: Academic Press.
  47. ^ MacGregor, J.N.; Ormerod, T.C.; Chronicle, E.P. (2001). "Information-processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 27 (1): 176–201. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.176. PMID 11204097.
  48. ^ a b c Weiten, Wayne (2011). Psychology: themes and variations (8th ed.). California: Wadsworth.
  49. ^ Novick, L. R.; Bassok, M. (2005). "Problem solving". In Holyoak, K. J.; Morrison, R. G. (eds.). Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. New York, N.Y.: Cambridge University Press. pp. 321–349.
  50. ^ Walinga, Jennifer (2010). "From walls to windows: Using barriers as pathways to insightful solutions". The Journal of Creative Behavior. 44 (3): 143–167. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2010.tb01331.x.
  51. ^ a b Walinga, Jennifer; Cunningham, J. Barton; MacGregor, James N. (2011). "Training insight problem solving through focus on barriers and assumptions". The Journal of Creative Behavior. 45: 47–58. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2011.tb01084.x.
  52. ^ Vlamings, Petra H. J. M.; Hare, Brian; Call, Joseph (2009). "Reaching around barriers: The performance of great apes and 3–5-year-old children". Animal Cognition. 13 (2): 273–285. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0265-5. PMC 2822225. PMID 19653018.
  53. ^
  54. ^ Kaempffert, Waldemar B. (1924). A Popular History of American Invention. Vol. 2. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons. p. 385.
  55. ^
    • Kekulé, August (1890). "Benzolfest-Rede". Berichte der Deutschen Chemischen Gesellschaft. 23: 1302–1311.
    • Benfey, O. (1958). "Kekulé and the birth of the structural theory of organic chemistry in 1858". Journal of Chemical Education. 35 (1): 21–23. Bibcode:1958JChEd..35...21B. doi:10.1021/ed035p21.
  56. ^ a b Dement, W.C. (1972). Some Must Watch While Some Just Sleep. New York: Freeman.
  57. ^ a b Blechner, Mark J. (2018). The Mindbrain and Dreams: An Exploration of Dreaming, Thinking, and Artistic Creation. New York: Routledge.
  58. ^ Fromm, Erika O. (1998). "Lost and found half a century later: Letters by Freud and Einstein". American Psychologist. 53 (11): 1195–1198. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.53.11.1195.
  59. ^ Einstein, Albert (1954). "A Mathematician's Mind". Ideas and Opinions. New York: Bonanza Books. p. 25.
  60. ^ Sternberg, R. J. (1995). "Conceptions of expertise in complex problem solving: A comparison of alternative conceptions". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 295–321.
  61. ^ Funke, J. (1991). "Solving complex problems: Human identification and control of complex systems". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 185–222. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  62. ^
  63. ^
    • Dörner, Dietrich (1975). "Wie Menschen eine Welt verbessern wollten" [How people wanted to improve the world]. Bild der Wissenschaft (in German). 12: 48–53.
    • Dörner, Dietrich (1985). "Verhalten, Denken und Emotionen" [Behavior, thinking, and emotions]. In Eckensberger, L. H.; Lantermann, E. D. (eds.). Emotion und Reflexivität (in German). München, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg. pp. 157–181.
    • Dörner, Dietrich; Wearing, Alex J. (1995). "Complex problem solving: Toward a (computer-simulated) theory". In Frensch, P.A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 65–99.
  64. ^
    • Buchner, A. (1995). "Theories of complex problem solving". In Frensch, P.A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 27–63.
    • Dörner, D.; Kreuzig, H. W.; Reither, F.; Stäudel, T., eds. (1983). Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität [Lohhausen. On dealing with uncertainty and complexity] (in German). Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
    • Ringelband, O. J.; Misiak, C.; Kluwe, R. H. (1990). "Mental models and strategies in the control of a complex system". In Ackermann, D.; Tauber, M. J. (eds.). Mental models and human-computer interaction. Vol. 1. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. pp. 151–164.
  65. ^
  66. ^ e.g., Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A., eds. (1991). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  67. ^
  68. ^ Sokol, S. M.; McCloskey, M. (1991). "Cognitive mechanisms in calculation". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 85–116. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  69. ^ Kay, D. S. (1991). "Computer interaction: Debugging the problems". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 317–340. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443. Archived from the original on 2022-12-04. Retrieved 2022-12-04.
  70. ^ Frensch, P. A.; Sternberg, R. J. (1991). "Skill-related differences in game playing". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J .: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 343–381. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  71. ^ Amsel, E.; Langer, R.; Loutzenhiser, L. (1991). "Do lawyers reason differently from psychologists? A comparative design for studying expertise". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 223–250. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  72. ^ Wagner, R. K. (1991). "Managerial problem solving". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 159–183. PsycNET: 1991-98396-005.
  73. ^
  74. ^ Hegarty, M. (1991). "Knowledge and processes in mechanical problem solving". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 253–285. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443. Archived from the original on 2022-12-04. Retrieved 2022-12-04.
  75. ^ Heppner, P. P.; Krauskopf, C. J. (1987). "An information-processing approach to personal problem solving". The Counseling Psychologist. 15 (3): 371–447. doi:10.1177/0011000087153001. S2CID 146180007.
  76. ^ Voss, J. F.; Wolfe, C. R.; Lawrence, J. A.; Engle, R. A. (1991). "From representation to decision: An analysis of problem solving in international relations". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 119–158. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443. PsycNET: 1991-98396-004.
  77. ^ Lesgold, A.; Lajoie, S. (1991). "Complex problem solving in electronics". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 287–316. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443. Archived from the original on 2022-12-04. Retrieved 2022-12-04.
  78. ^ Altshuller, Genrich (1994). And Suddenly the Inventor Appeared. Translated by Lev Shulyak. Worcester, Mass.: Technical Innovation Center. ISBN 978-0-9640740-1-9.
  79. ^ Stanovich, K. E.; Cunningham, A. E. (1991). "Reading as constrained reasoning". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 3–60. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443. Archived from the original on 2023-09-03. Retrieved 2022-12-04.
  80. ^ Bryson, M.; Bereiter, C.; Scardamalia, M.; Joram, E. (1991). "Going beyond the problem as given: Problem solving in expert and novice writers". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 61–84. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  81. ^ Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A., eds. (1991). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  82. ^ Hung, Woei (2013). "Team-based complex problem solving: a collective cognition perspective". Educational Technology Research and Development. 61 (3): 365–384. doi:10.1007/s11423-013-9296-3. S2CID 62663840.
  83. ^ Jewett, Pamela; MacPhee, Deborah (2012). "Adding Collaborative Peer Coaching to Our Teaching Identities". The Reading Teacher. 66 (2): 105–110. doi:10.1002/TRTR.01089.
  84. ^ Wang, Qiyun (2009). "Design and Evaluation of a Collaborative Learning Environment". Computers and Education. 53 (4): 1138–1146. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2009.05.023.
  85. ^ Wang, Qiyan (2010). "Using online shared workspaces to support group collaborative learning". Computers and Education. 55 (3): 1270–1276. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2010.05.023.
  86. ^ Kai-Wai Chu, Samuel; Kennedy, David M. (2011). "Using Online Collaborative tools for groups to Co-Construct Knowledge". Online Information Review. 35 (4): 581–597. doi:10.1108/14684521111161945. ISSN 1468-4527. S2CID 206388086.
  87. ^ Legare, Cristine; Mills, Candice; Souza, Andre; Plummer, Leigh; Yasskin, Rebecca (2013). "The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 114 (1): 63–7. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002. PMID 23044374.
  88. ^ Engelbart, Douglas (1962). "Team Cooperation". Augmenting Human Intellect: A Conceptual Framework. Vol. AFOSR-3223. Stanford Research Institute.
  89. ^ Flew, Terry (2008). New Media: an introduction. Melbourne: Oxford University Press.
  90. ^ Henry, Jenkins. "Interactive audiences? The 'collective intelligence' of media fans" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on April 26, 2018. Retrieved December 11, 2016.
  91. ^ Finger, Matthias (2008-03-27). "Which governance for sustainable development? An organizational and institutional perspective". In Park, Jacob; Conca, Ken; Finger, Matthias (eds.). The Crisis of Global Environmental Governance: Towards a New Political Economy of Sustainability. Routledge. p. 48. ISBN 978-1-134-05982-9.
  92. ^
  93. ^ Stefanovitch, Nicolas; Alshamsi, Aamena; Cebrian, Manuel; Rahwan, Iyad (30 September 2014). "Error and attack tolerance of collective problem solving: The DARPA Shredder Challenge". EPJ Data Science. 3 (1). doi:10.1140/epjds/s13688-014-0013-1. hdl:21.11116/0000-0002-D39F-D.

Further reading

[edit]
  • Beckmann, Jens F.; Guthke, Jürgen (1995). "Complex problem solving, intelligence, and learning ability". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 177–200.
  • Brehmer, Berndt (1995). "Feedback delays in dynamic decision making". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 103–130.
  • Brehmer, Berndt; Dörner, D. (1993). "Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study". Computers in Human Behavior. 9 (2–3): 171–184. doi:10.1016/0747-5632(93)90005-D.
  • Dörner, D. (1992). "Über die Philosophie der Verwendung von Mikrowelten oder 'Computerszenarios' in der psychologischen Forschung" [On the proper use of microworlds or "computer scenarios" in psychological research]. In Gundlach, H. (ed.). Psychologische Forschung und Methode: Das Versprechen des Experiments. Festschrift für Werner Traxel (in German). Passau, Germany: Passavia-Universitäts-Verlag. pp. 53–87.
  • Eyferth, K.; Schömann, M.; Widowski, D. (1986). "Der Umgang von Psychologen mit Komplexität" [On how psychologists deal with complexity]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 5: 11–26.
  • Funke, Joachim (1993). "Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results" (PDF). In Strube, G.; Wender, K.-F. (eds.). The cognitive psychology of knowledge. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. pp. 313–330.
  • Funke, Joachim (1995). "Experimental research on complex problem solving" (PDF). In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 243–268.
  • Funke, U. (1995). "Complex problem solving in personnel selection and training". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 219–240.
  • Groner, M.; Groner, R.; Bischof, W. F. (1983). "Approaches to heuristics: A historical review". In Groner, R.; Groner, M.; Bischof, W. F. (eds.). Methods of heuristics. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 1–18.
  • Hayes, J. (1980). The complete problem solver. Philadelphia: The Franklin Institute Press.
  • Huber, O. (1995). "Complex problem solving as multistage decision making". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 151–173.
  • Hübner, Ronald (1989). "Methoden zur Analyse und Konstruktion von Aufgaben zur kognitiven Steuerung dynamischer Systeme" [Methods for the analysis and construction of dynamic system control tasks] (PDF). Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie (in German). 36: 221–238.
  • Hunt, Earl (1991). "Some comments on the study of complexity". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 383–395. ISBN 978-1-317-78386-2.
  • Hussy, W. (1985). "Komplexes Problemlösen—Eine Sackgasse?" [Complex problem solving—a dead end?]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie (in German). 32: 55–77.
  • Kluwe, R. H. (1993). "Chapter 19 Knowledge and Performance in Complex Problem Solving". The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge. Advances in Psychology. Vol. 101. pp. 401–423. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62668-0. ISBN 978-0-444-89942-2.
  • Kluwe, R. H. (1995). "Single case studies and models of complex problem solving". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 269–291.
  • Kolb, S.; Petzing, F.; Stumpf, S. (1992). "Komplexes Problemlösen: Bestimmung der Problemlösegüte von Probanden mittels Verfahren des Operations Research—ein interdisziplinärer Ansatz" [Complex problem solving: determining the quality of human problem solving by operations research tools—an interdisciplinary approach]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 11: 115–128.
  • Krems, Josef F. (1995). "Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 201–218.
  • Melzak, Z. (1983). Bypasses: A Simple Approach to Complexity. London, UK: Wiley.
  • Müller, H. (1993). Komplexes Problemlösen: Reliabilität und Wissen [Complex problem solving: Reliability and knowledge] (in German). Bonn, Germany: Holos.
  • Paradies, M.W.; Unger, L. W. (2000). TapRooT—The System for Root Cause Analysis, Problem Investigation, and Proactive Improvement. Knoxville, Tenn.: System Improvements.
  • Putz-Osterloh, Wiebke (1993). "Chapter 15 Strategies for Knowledge Acquisition and Transfer of Knowledge in Dynamic Tasks". The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge. Advances in Psychology. Vol. 101. pp. 331–350. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62664-3. ISBN 978-0-444-89942-2.
  • Riefer, David M.; Batchelder, William H. (1988). "Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes" (PDF). Psychological Review. 95 (3): 318–339. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.95.3.318. S2CID 14994393. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-11-25.
  • Schaub, H. (1993). Modellierung der Handlungsorganisation (in German). Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
  • Strauß, B. (1993). Konfundierungen beim Komplexen Problemlösen. Zum Einfluß des Anteils der richtigen Lösungen (ArL) auf das Problemlöseverhalten in komplexen Situationen [Confoundations in complex problem solving. On the influence of the degree of correct solutions on problem solving in complex situations] (in German). Bonn, Germany: Holos.
  • Strohschneider, S. (1991). "Kein System von Systemen! Kommentar zu dem Aufsatz 'Systemmerkmale als Determinanten des Umgangs mit dynamischen Systemen' von Joachim Funke" [No system of systems! Reply to the paper 'System features as determinants of behavior in dynamic task environments' by Joachim Funke]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 10: 109–113.
  • Tonelli, Marcello (2011). Unstructured Processes of Strategic Decision-Making. Saarbrücken, Germany: Lambert Academic Publishing. ISBN 978-3-8465-5598-9.
  • Van Lehn, Kurt (1989). "Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition". In Posner, M. I. (ed.). Foundations of cognitive science (PDF). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. 527–579.
  • Wisconsin Educational Media Association (1993), Information literacy: A position paper on information problem-solving, WEMA Publications, vol. ED 376 817, Madison, Wis.{{citation}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) (Portions adapted from Michigan State Board of Education's Position Paper on Information Processing Skills, 1992.)
[edit]