Bracketing paradox: Difference between revisions
(9 intermediate revisions by 8 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
In [[Morphology (linguistics)|linguistic morphology]], the '''bracketing paradox''' concerns morphologically complex [[word]]s which have more than one analysis, or ''[[Bracketing (linguistics)|bracketing]]'', e.g., one for phonology and one for [[semantics]], and the two are not compatible, or brackets do not align. |
In [[Morphology (linguistics)|linguistic morphology]], the '''bracketing paradox''' concerns morphologically complex [[word]]s which have more than one analysis, or ''[[Bracketing (linguistics)|bracketing]]'', e.g., one for phonology and one for [[semantics]], and the two are not compatible, or brackets do not align. |
||
==English examples== |
|||
⚫ | One type of a bracketing paradox found in [[English language|English]] is exemplified by words like ''unhappier'' or ''uneasier''.<ref name=":0">Pesetsky, D. 1985. "Morphology and logical form." ''Linguistic Inquiry'' 16:193–246.</ref> The synthetic [[comparative]] [[suffix]] ''-er'' generally occurs with monosyllabic [[adjective]]s and a small class of disyllabic adjectives with the primary (and only) [[Stress (phonology)|stress]] on the first syllable. Other adjectives take the analytic comparative ''more''. Thus, we have ''older'' and ''grumpier'', but ''more correct'' and ''more restrictive''. From a phonological perspective, this suggests that a word like ''uneasier'' must be formed by combining the suffix ''er'' with the adjective ''easy'', since ''uneasy'' is a three syllable word: |
||
===Comparatives such as ''unhappier''=== |
|||
⚫ | One type of a bracketing paradox found in [[English language|English]] is exemplified by words like ''unhappier'' or ''uneasier''.<ref name=":0">[[David Pesetsky|Pesetsky, D.]] 1985. "Morphology and logical form." ''Linguistic Inquiry'' 16:193–246.</ref> The synthetic [[comparative]] [[suffix]] ''-er'' generally occurs with monosyllabic [[adjective]]s and a small class of disyllabic adjectives with the primary (and only) [[Stress (phonology)|stress]] on the first syllable. Other adjectives take the analytic comparative ''more''. Thus, we have ''older'' and ''grumpier'', but ''more correct'' and ''more restrictive''. From a phonological perspective, this suggests that a word like ''uneasier'' must be formed by combining the suffix ''er'' with the adjective ''easy'', since ''uneasy'' is a three syllable word: |
||
:<math>\Big[\mbox{un-}\Big] \Big[ \big[\mbox{easi}\big] \big[\mbox{-er}\big] \Big]</math> |
:<math>\Big[\mbox{un-}\Big] \Big[ \big[\mbox{easi}\big] \big[\mbox{-er}\big] \Big]</math> |
||
However, ''uneasier'' means "more uneasy", not "not more easy". Thus, from a [[Meaning (linguistics)|semantic]] perspective, ''uneasier'' must be a combination of ''er'' with the adjective ''uneasy'': |
However, ''uneasier'' means "more uneasy", not "not more easy". Thus, from a [[Meaning (linguistics)|semantic]] perspective, ''uneasier'' must be a combination of ''er'' with the adjective ''uneasy'': |
||
:<math>\Big [ \big[\mbox{un-}\big] \big[\mbox{easi}\big] \Big ] \Big[\mbox{-er}\Big] </math> |
:<math>\Big [ \big[\mbox{un-}\big] \big[\mbox{easi}\big] \Big ] \Big[\mbox{-er}\Big] </math> |
||
This, however, violates the morphophonological rules for the suffix ''-er''. Phenomena such as this have been argued to represent a mismatch between different levels of grammatical structure.<ref>Sproat, R. 1988. "Bracketing paradoxes, cliticization, and other topics: The mapping between syntactic and phonological structure." In Everaert et al. (eds), ''Morphology and Modularity.'' Amsterdam: North-Holland.</ref> |
This, however, violates the morphophonological rules for the suffix ''-er''. Phenomena such as this have been argued to represent a mismatch between different levels of grammatical structure.<ref>[[Richard Sproat|Sproat, R.]] 1988. "Bracketing paradoxes, cliticization, and other topics: The mapping between syntactic and phonological structure." In Everaert et al. (eds), ''Morphology and Modularity.'' Amsterdam: North-Holland.</ref> |
||
===Professions such as ''nuclear physicist''=== |
|||
Another type of [[English language|English]] bracketing paradox is found in [[compound word]]s that are a name for a professional of a particular discipline, preceded by a modifier that narrows that discipline: ''nuclear physicist'', ''historical linguist'', ''political scientist'', etc.<ref>Williams, E. 1981. "On the notions 'lexically related' and 'head of a word.'" ''Linguistic Inquiry'' 12:245–274.</ref><ref>Spencer, A. 1988. "Bracketing paradoxes and the English lexicon." ''Language'' 64:663–682.</ref> Taking ''nuclear physicist'' as an example, we see that there are at least two reasonable ways that the compound word can be bracketed (ignoring the fact that ''nuclear'' itself is morphologically complex): |
Another type of [[English language|English]] bracketing paradox is found in [[compound word]]s that are a name for a professional of a particular discipline, preceded by a modifier that narrows that discipline: ''nuclear physicist'', ''historical linguist'', ''political scientist'', etc.<ref>Williams, E. 1981. "On the notions 'lexically related' and 'head of a word.'" ''Linguistic Inquiry'' 12:245–274.</ref><ref>Spencer, A. 1988. "Bracketing paradoxes and the English lexicon." ''Language'' 64:663–682.</ref> Taking ''nuclear physicist'' as an example, we see that there are at least two reasonable ways that the compound word can be bracketed (ignoring the fact that ''nuclear'' itself is morphologically complex): |
||
Line 16: | Line 22: | ||
=== Raising === |
=== Raising === |
||
Pesetsky (1985)<ref name=":0" /> accounts for the bracketing paradox by proposing that phonological bracketing occurs in syntax and semantic bracketing occurs after the output is sent to [[logical form (linguistics)|LF]]. This solution is parallel to [[Operator (linguistics)#Quantifier Raising .28QR.29|quantifier raising]]. For example, the sentence: ''Every farmer owns a donkey'' has two interpretations: |
[[David Pesetsky|Pesetsky]] (1985)<ref name=":0" /> accounts for the bracketing paradox by proposing that phonological bracketing occurs in syntax and semantic bracketing occurs after the output is sent to [[logical form (linguistics)|LF]]. This solution is parallel to [[Operator (linguistics)#Quantifier Raising .28QR.29|quantifier raising]]. For example, the sentence: ''Every farmer owns a donkey'' has two interpretations: |
||
# Every farmer owns their own donkey: ∀x[farmer(x) → ∃y[donkey(y) ∧ own(x,y)] ] |
# Every farmer owns their own donkey: ∀x[farmer(x) → ∃y[donkey(y) ∧ own(x,y)] ] |
||
Line 28: | Line 34: | ||
Syntax: [ un [ happy er ] ] → LF: [ [ un [ happy ''t<sub>1</sub>'' ] ] er<sub>1</sub> ] |
Syntax: [ un [ happy er ] ] → LF: [ [ un [ happy ''t<sub>1</sub>'' ] ] er<sub>1</sub> ] |
||
=== Late |
=== Late adjunction === |
||
An alternative account is proposed by Newell (2005).<ref>{{Cite journal|url = http://www.hum2.leidenuniv.nl/pdf/lucl/sole/console13/console13-newell.pdf|title = Bracketing paradoxes and particle verbs: a late adjunction analysis.|last = Newell|first = Heather|date = 2005|journal = Proceedings of ConSOLE |
An alternative account is proposed by Newell (2005).<ref>{{Cite journal|url = http://www.hum2.leidenuniv.nl/pdf/lucl/sole/console13/console13-newell.pdf|title = Bracketing paradoxes and particle verbs: a late adjunction analysis.|last = Newell|first = Heather|date = 2005|journal = Proceedings of ConSOLE Xiii|access-date = July 29, 2015}}</ref> She argues that ''un-'' adjoins at a late stage of the derivation in LF, possibly after the spell-out of [happy -er]. Under this interpretation the stages are: |
||
Syntax: [ happy er ] → Late Insertion: [ [ un happy ] er ] |
Syntax: [ happy er ] → Late Insertion: [ [ un happy ] er ] |
||
Line 47: | Line 53: | ||
=== Glomming === |
=== Glomming === |
||
A famous bracketing paradox of Russian verb complex<ref>{{Cite book|title = Problems in the theory of phonology: Russian phonology and Turkish phonology.|last = Lightner|first = Theodore|publisher = Linguistic Research|year = 1972 |
A famous bracketing paradox of Russian verb complex<ref>{{Cite book|title = Problems in the theory of phonology: Russian phonology and Turkish phonology.|last = Lightner|first = Theodore|publisher = Linguistic Research|year = 1972|location = Edmonton, Alberta}}</ref> such as ''razorvala'' 'she ripped apart' shows different phonological and semantic analyses: |
||
{{interlinear |indent=2 |glossing=no abbr |
|||
razo rv a |
|razo rv a la |
||
⚫ | |||
|}} |
|||
⚫ | |||
* morphophonology – [prefix [root suffixes] ] |
* morphophonology – [prefix [root suffixes] ] |
||
* morphosemantics – [ [prefix root] suffixes] |
* morphosemantics – [ [prefix root] suffixes] |
||
One of the proposed solutions is parallel to the proposal for [[Navajo language|Navajo verbs]] with multiple prefixes.<ref>Harley, Heidi (2010). Affixation and the mirror principle. In Interfaces in linguistics: New research perspectives, ed. Rafaella Folli and Christiane Ulbrich. Oxford University Press.</ref> It includes head movement and Merger Under Adjacency, also called Glomming. For Russian, the derivation starts with [<sub>TP</sub> T [<sub>AspP</sub> Asp [<sub>vP</sub> v [<sub>VP</sub> √V [<sub>SC</sub> LP DP''<sub>obj</sub>'']]]]] It allows for the semantic bracketing as √V and LP are next to each other. Next, √V ''rv'' merges with v –''a'' via head movement and further v complex merges with Asp also via head movement. Lexical Prefix ''razo-'' is phrasal<ref>{{Cite |
One of the proposed solutions is parallel to the proposal for [[Navajo language|Navajo verbs]] with multiple prefixes.<ref>Harley, Heidi (2010). Affixation and the mirror principle. In Interfaces in linguistics: New research perspectives, ed. Rafaella Folli and Christiane Ulbrich. Oxford University Press.</ref> It includes head movement and Merger Under Adjacency, also called Glomming. For Russian, the derivation starts with [<sub>TP</sub> T [<sub>AspP</sub> Asp [<sub>vP</sub> v [<sub>VP</sub> √V [<sub>SC</sub> LP DP''<sub>obj</sub>'']]]]] It allows for the semantic bracketing as √V and LP are next to each other. Next, √V ''rv'' merges with v –''a'' via head movement and further v complex merges with Asp also via head movement. Lexical Prefix ''razo-'' is phrasal<ref>{{Cite web |last=Svenonius |first=Peter |date=2004 |title=Russian prefixes are phrasal |website=LingBuzz |s2cid=201079922 |url=https://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/000043/current.pdf}}</ref> and does not participate in the head movement. Thus, the complex structure emerging as a result of head movement to the exclusion of LP allows for the phonological bracketing. Finally, Glomming or Merger Under Adjacency takes place resulting in one verbal complex. |
||
== See also == |
== See also == |
Latest revision as of 05:09, 3 July 2024
In linguistic morphology, the bracketing paradox concerns morphologically complex words which have more than one analysis, or bracketing, e.g., one for phonology and one for semantics, and the two are not compatible, or brackets do not align.
English examples
[edit]Comparatives such as unhappier
[edit]One type of a bracketing paradox found in English is exemplified by words like unhappier or uneasier.[1] The synthetic comparative suffix -er generally occurs with monosyllabic adjectives and a small class of disyllabic adjectives with the primary (and only) stress on the first syllable. Other adjectives take the analytic comparative more. Thus, we have older and grumpier, but more correct and more restrictive. From a phonological perspective, this suggests that a word like uneasier must be formed by combining the suffix er with the adjective easy, since uneasy is a three syllable word:
However, uneasier means "more uneasy", not "not more easy". Thus, from a semantic perspective, uneasier must be a combination of er with the adjective uneasy:
This, however, violates the morphophonological rules for the suffix -er. Phenomena such as this have been argued to represent a mismatch between different levels of grammatical structure.[2]
Professions such as nuclear physicist
[edit]Another type of English bracketing paradox is found in compound words that are a name for a professional of a particular discipline, preceded by a modifier that narrows that discipline: nuclear physicist, historical linguist, political scientist, etc.[3][4] Taking nuclear physicist as an example, we see that there are at least two reasonable ways that the compound word can be bracketed (ignoring the fact that nuclear itself is morphologically complex):
- – one who studies physics, and who happens also to be nuclear (phonological bracketing)
- – one who studies nuclear physics, a subfield of physics that deals with nuclear phenomena (semantic bracketing)
What is interesting to many morphologists about this type of bracketing paradox in English is that the correct bracketing 2 (correct in the sense that this is the way that a native speaker would understand it) does not follow the usual bracketing pattern 1 typical for most compound words in English.
Proposed solutions
[edit]Raising
[edit]Pesetsky (1985)[1] accounts for the bracketing paradox by proposing that phonological bracketing occurs in syntax and semantic bracketing occurs after the output is sent to LF. This solution is parallel to quantifier raising. For example, the sentence: Every farmer owns a donkey has two interpretations:
- Every farmer owns their own donkey: ∀x[farmer(x) → ∃y[donkey(y) ∧ own(x,y)] ]
- There exists one donkey such that every farmer owns it: ∃y[donkey(y) ∧ ∀x[farmer(x) → own(x,y)] ]
- The structure for 1 is: [IP [DP1 every farmer ] [ [DP2 a donkey ] [t1 [VP owns t2 ] ] ] ]
- The structure for 2 is: [IP [DP2 a donkey ] [ [DP1 every farmer ] [t1 [VP owns t2 ] ] ] ]
Depending on which quantifier expression is higher, the meaning is shifted, but because this movement does not occur until LF, the structures are pronounced identically. Similarly to this account for scopal ambiguity in quantifier raising, Pesetsky proposes that in the structure of unhappier, happy and the comparative suffix -er are the first to combine, since -er may not attach to adjectives that are longer than two syllables. It is then fed to PF before the next phase, at which the negative prefix un- is then attached. At LF in the following phase, -er undergoes raising, forcing the interpretation of the word to be "more unhappy" and not "not happier".
Syntax: [ un [ happy er ] ] → LF: [ [ un [ happy t1 ] ] er1 ]
Late adjunction
[edit]An alternative account is proposed by Newell (2005).[5] She argues that un- adjoins at a late stage of the derivation in LF, possibly after the spell-out of [happy -er]. Under this interpretation the stages are:
Syntax: [ happy er ] → Late Insertion: [ [ un happy ] er ]
Contrasting with un-, the prefix in-, which also has negative meaning, is not allowed at late insertion. There are various pieces of evidence that in- is closer to the root.
- Selectional Restrictions: in- may only combine with Latinate roots, while un- is nonrestrictive
- Bound Morphemes: in- attaches to some bound morphemes, while un- only attaches to free morphemes (inept → *ept, inane → *ane)
- Nasal Assimilation: in- assimilates phonologically with the first phoneme of the morpheme to which it attaches, whereas the /n/ in un- is preserved
- Impossible: /in-/ + /ˈpasɪbl̩/ → [imˈpʰasɪbl̩] (*[inˈpʰasɪbl̩])
- Unpopular: /un-/ + /ˈpɒpjulr̩/ → [unˈpʰˈɒpjulr̩] (*[umˈpʰˈɒpjulr̩])
In order to participate in these processes, in- must attach at an earlier level directly to the root in order to adjectivize it. Thus, because it attaches early, if it creates a three-syllable word, -er may not attach, as -er attaches at a later stage above the root derivation.
- [ [ in √polite ] -er ] → crashes at PF
Glomming
[edit]A famous bracketing paradox of Russian verb complex[6] such as razorvala 'she ripped apart' shows different phonological and semantic analyses:
razo
PFX
rv
Root
a
THEME
la
3SG.PST.F
- morphophonology – [prefix [root suffixes] ]
- morphosemantics – [ [prefix root] suffixes]
One of the proposed solutions is parallel to the proposal for Navajo verbs with multiple prefixes.[7] It includes head movement and Merger Under Adjacency, also called Glomming. For Russian, the derivation starts with [TP T [AspP Asp [vP v [VP √V [SC LP DPobj]]]]] It allows for the semantic bracketing as √V and LP are next to each other. Next, √V rv merges with v –a via head movement and further v complex merges with Asp also via head movement. Lexical Prefix razo- is phrasal[8] and does not participate in the head movement. Thus, the complex structure emerging as a result of head movement to the exclusion of LP allows for the phonological bracketing. Finally, Glomming or Merger Under Adjacency takes place resulting in one verbal complex.
See also
[edit]References
[edit]- ^ a b Pesetsky, D. 1985. "Morphology and logical form." Linguistic Inquiry 16:193–246.
- ^ Sproat, R. 1988. "Bracketing paradoxes, cliticization, and other topics: The mapping between syntactic and phonological structure." In Everaert et al. (eds), Morphology and Modularity. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
- ^ Williams, E. 1981. "On the notions 'lexically related' and 'head of a word.'" Linguistic Inquiry 12:245–274.
- ^ Spencer, A. 1988. "Bracketing paradoxes and the English lexicon." Language 64:663–682.
- ^ Newell, Heather (2005). "Bracketing paradoxes and particle verbs: a late adjunction analysis" (PDF). Proceedings of ConSOLE Xiii. Retrieved July 29, 2015.
- ^ Lightner, Theodore (1972). Problems in the theory of phonology: Russian phonology and Turkish phonology. Edmonton, Alberta: Linguistic Research.
- ^ Harley, Heidi (2010). Affixation and the mirror principle. In Interfaces in linguistics: New research perspectives, ed. Rafaella Folli and Christiane Ulbrich. Oxford University Press.
- ^ Svenonius, Peter (2004). "Russian prefixes are phrasal" (PDF). LingBuzz. S2CID 201079922.