Jump to content

Anne Treisman: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Cydebot (talk | contribs)
m Robot - Removing category Grawemeyer Award winners per CFD at Wikipedia:Categories for discussion/Log/2019 July 18.
 
(99 intermediate revisions by 48 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|English cognitive psychologist (1935–2018)}}
{{EngvarB|date=August 2014}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=August 2014}}
{{Use British English|date=August 2014}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=September 2023}}
{{Infobox scientist
{{Infobox scientist
|name = Anne M. Treisman
|name = Anne M. Treisman
|image = Honorary degree recipient Anne Treisman speaking at Congregation ceremony 2004.jpg
|image = Anne Treisman.jpg
|image_upright =
|caption = Treisman in 2004<br>at the [[University of British Columbia]]
|caption = Treisman in 2011, [[Princeton University]], recipient of the [[National Medal of Science]]
|birth_name = Anne Marie Taylor
|birth_name = Anne Marie Taylor
|birth_date = {{Birth date|df=yes|1935|02|27}}
|birth_date = {{Birth date|df=yes|1935|02|27}}
|birth_place = [[Wakefield]], [[Yorkshire]], England
|birth_place = [[Wakefield]], [[West Riding of Yorkshire]], England
|residence = [[New York City]], United States
|nationality =
|nationality =
|death_date = {{death date and age|df=yes|2018|02|9|1935|02|27}}
|death_date = {{death date and age|df=yes|2018|02|9|1935|02|27}}
|death_place =
|death_place = [[Manhattan]], [[New York City]], US
|field = [[Psychology]]
|field = [[Psychology]]
|work_institution = [[Princeton University]]
|work_institution = [[Princeton University]]
|alma_mater = [[Newnham College, Cambridge]]<br>[[University of Oxford]]
|alma_mater = [[Newnham College, Cambridge]]<br />[[Somerville College, Oxford]]
|doctoral_advisor = Richard C. Oldfield
|doctoral_advisor = Richard C. Oldfield
|notable_students = Postdoctoral fellows [[Nancy Kanwisher]] and [[Nilli Lavie]]
|notable_students = Postdoctoral fellows [[Nancy Kanwisher]] and [[Nilli Lavie]]
|known_for = [[Feature integration theory]], [[Attenuation theory]]
|known_for = [[Feature integration theory]], [[Attenuation theory]]
|prizes = [[Golden Brain Award]] <small>(1996)</small><br>[[Grawemeyer Award]] in Psychology <small>(2009)</small><br>[[National Medal of Science]] <small>(2011)</small>
|prizes = [[Golden Brain Award]] <small>(1996)</small><br />[[Grawemeyer Award]] in Psychology <small>(2009)</small><br />[[National Medal of Science]] <small>(2011)</small>
|spouse = {{plainlist|
|spouse = Michel Treisman (1960–1976, divorced)<br>[[Daniel Kahneman]] (1978–2018, her death)
* {{marriage|Michel Treisman|1960|1976|reason=divorced}}
* {{marriage|[[Daniel Kahneman]]|1978}}
}}
}}
|children = [[Deborah Treisman]], and three others}}


'''Anne Marie Treisman''' (née '''Taylor'''; 27 February 1935 – 9 February 2018) was an English [[psychologist]] who specialised in [[cognitive psychology]].
'''Anne Marie Treisman''' (née '''Taylor'''; 27 February 1935<ref name="obit nyt">{{cite web|url=https://www.nytimes.com/2018/02/13/obituaries/anne-treisman-who-studied-how-we-perceive-dies-at-82.html|title=Anne Treisman, Who Studied How We Perceive, Dies at 82|publisher=The New York Times|date=13 February 2018|author=Genzlinger, Neil}}</ref> – 9 February 2018)<ref name=DeanPrinceton>Dean of the Faculty. (2018). [Obituary] Anne Marie Treisman. Retrieved 12 Feb 2018, from https://dof.princeton.edu/about/clerk-faculty/emeritus/anne-marie-treisman
</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/observer/obsonline/anne-treisman-1935-2018.html|title=Anne Treisman, 1935-2018|publisher=|accessdate=12 February 2018}}</ref><ref name = KTCenter>Kahneman-Treisman Center for Behavioral Science & Public Policy. (2018). Anne Treisman 1935 - 2018. Retrieved 14 Feb 2018, from https://behavioralpolicy.princeton.edu/news/anne-treisman-1935-2018</ref> was an English [[psychologist]] who specialised in [[cognitive psychology]]. She researched visual [[attention]], object [[perception]], and [[memory]]. One of her most influential ideas is the [[feature integration theory|feature integration theory of attention]], first published with G. Gelade in 1980. Treisman taught at the [[University of Oxford]], [[University of British Columbia]], [[University of California, Berkeley]] and [[Princeton University]]. Notable postdoctoral fellows she supervised included [[Nancy Kanwisher]] and [[Nilli Lavie]]. In 2013, Treisman received the [[National Medal of Science]] from President [[Barack Obama]] for her pioneering work in the study of attention.<ref>{{cite web|title=Treisman wins National Medal of Science for psychology research |url=https://www.princeton.edu/main/news/archive/S35/72/22Q97/index.xml?section=topstories |publisher=Princeton University |date=3 January 2013 |accessdate=9 March 2013}}</ref> During her long career, Treisman experimentally and theoretically defined the issue of how information is selected and integrated to form meaningful objects that guide human thought and action.


Treisman researched visual [[attention]], object [[perception]], and [[memory]]. One of her most influential ideas is the [[Feature integration theory|feature integration theory of attention]], first published with Garry Gelade in 1980. Treisman taught at the [[University of Oxford]], [[University of British Columbia]], [[University of California, Berkeley]], and [[Princeton University]]. Notable postdoctoral fellows she supervised included [[Nancy Kanwisher]] and [[Nilli Lavie]].
== Life and education ==
Anne Treisman was born in [[Wakefield]], [[Yorkshire]], England. Two years later, her family moved to a village near [[Rochester, Kent]]{{sfn|Sheehy|2016|p=580}} where her father, Percy Taylor, worked as chief education officer during [[World War II]].<ref>{{cite web|title=One on one...with Anne Treisman |url=http://www.thepsychologist.org.uk/archive/archive_home.cfm?volumeID=23&editionID=194&ArticleID=1755|publisher=the psychologist |date=9 October 2010|accessdate=18 November 2013}}</ref> Her mother, Suzanne Touren, was French.<ref name="obit nyt"/> At the age of 11, Treisman moved with her family to [[Reading, Berkshire]] where she attended the girls’ grammar school [[Kendrick School]].<ref>{{cite web |url=https://www.sfn.org/-/media/SfN/Documents/TheHistoryofNeuroscience/Volume-8/AnneTreisman.ashx?la=en&hash=EDED3E84883883094054A02B99D0C69F97071694 |title=Anne Treisman |publisher= sfn.org |date=16 April 2014 |accessdate=12 February 2018}}</ref> The English educational system at the time forced Treisman to choose only three subjects in her last two years at secondary school, and Treisman focused on the language arts (French, Latin and History).


In 2013, Treisman received the [[National Medal of Science]] from President [[Barack Obama]] for her pioneering work in the study of attention.<ref>{{cite web |title=Treisman wins National Medal of Science for psychology research |url=https://www.princeton.edu/main/news/archive/S35/72/22Q97/index.xml?section=topstories |publisher=Princeton University |date=3 January 2013 |access-date=9 March 2013 |archive-date=13 April 2016 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160413202418/https://www.princeton.edu/main/news/archive/S35/72/22Q97/index.xml?section=topstories |url-status=live }}</ref> During her long career, Treisman experimentally and theoretically defined the issue of how information is selected and integrated to form meaningful objects that guide human thought and action.
Treisman received her [[Bachelor of Arts|BA]] in French Literature at [[Newnham College, Cambridge]] in 1954. She received a first class BA with distinction, which earned her a scholarship that she used to obtain a second BA in psychology. During this extra year, Treisman studied under the supervision of [[Richard Gregory]], who introduced her to various methods of exploring the mind through experiments in perception.<ref name="History of Research on Attention">[[Wikibooks:Applied History of Psychology/History of Research on Attention]]</ref> While at Cambridge, she was active in the [[folk music]] scene.<ref>{{cite journal|first=Dorian|last=Lynskey|title=Folk story|journal=Cam|year=2016|issue=77|pages=36–41}}</ref>


== Early life and education ==
In 1957, Treisman attended the University of Oxford to work toward her [[DPhil]] under her advisor, Carolus Oldfield.<ref name="History of Research on Attention"/> Treisman conducted research on [[aphasia]], but soon pursued interest in non-clinical populations. Treisman's research was guided by [[Donald Broadbent]]'s book, ''Perception and Communication''.<ref name="Broadbent 1958">{{cite book|last=Broadbent|first=D. E.|authorlink=Donald Broadbent|title=Perception and Communication|publisher=Pergamon Press|location=Elmsford, NY|year=1958}}</ref> After three years of research, she married Michel Treisman in 1960, another Oxford graduate student.<ref name="obit nyt"/> Two years later, Treisman completed her thesis, "Selective Attention and Speech Perception" in 1962.<ref name="History of Research on Attention"/>
Anne Treisman was born in [[Wakefield]], [[West Riding of Yorkshire]], England on 27 February 1935.<ref name="obit nyt">{{cite web|url=https://www.nytimes.com/2018/02/13/obituaries/anne-treisman-who-studied-how-we-perceive-dies-at-82.html|title=Anne Treisman, Who Studied How We Perceive, Dies at 82|work=The New York Times|date=13 February 2018|last=Genzlinger|first=Neil|access-date=14 February 2018|archive-date=6 November 2018|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181106113810/https://www.nytimes.com/2018/02/13/obituaries/anne-treisman-who-studied-how-we-perceive-dies-at-82.html|url-status=live}}</ref> Two years later, her family moved to a village near [[Rochester, Kent]]{{sfn|Sheehy|Chapman|Conroy|2016|p=580}} where her father, Percy Taylor, worked as chief education officer during [[World War II]].<ref>{{cite web|title=One on one...with Anne Treisman|url=http://www.thepsychologist.org.uk/archive/archive_home.cfm?volumeID=23&editionID=194&ArticleID=1755|publisher=the psychologist|date=9 October 2010|access-date=18 November 2013|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130620121833/http://www.thepsychologist.org.uk/archive/archive_home.cfm?volumeID=23&editionID=194&ArticleID=1755|archive-date=20 June 2013|url-status=dead}}</ref> Her mother, Suzanne Touren, was French.<ref name="obit nyt" /> At the age of 11, Treisman moved with her family to [[Reading, Berkshire]] where she attended the girls' grammar school [[Kendrick School]].<ref>{{cite web |url=https://www.sfn.org/-/media/SfN/Documents/TheHistoryofNeuroscience/Volume-8/AnneTreisman.ashx?la=en&hash=EDED3E84883883094054A02B99D0C69F97071694 |title=Anne Treisman |publisher=sfn.org |date=16 April 2014 |access-date=12 February 2018 |archive-date=18 October 2018 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181018003019/https://www.sfn.org/-/media/SfN/Documents/TheHistoryofNeuroscience/Volume-8/AnneTreisman.ashx?la=en&hash=EDED3E84883883094054A02B99D0C69F97071694 |url-status=live }}</ref> The English educational system at the time forced Treisman to choose only three subjects in her last two years at secondary school, and Treisman focused on the language arts (French, Latin and History).


Treisman received her [[Bachelor of Arts|BA]] in French Literature at [[Newnham College, Cambridge]], in 1954. She received a first class BA with distinction, which earned her a scholarship that she used to obtain a second BA in psychology. During this extra year, Treisman studied under the supervision of [[Richard Gregory]], who introduced her to various methods of exploring the mind through experiments in perception.<ref name="History of Research on Attention">[[Wikibooks:Applied History of Psychology/History of Research on Attention]]</ref> While at Cambridge, she was active in the [[folk music]] scene.<ref>{{cite journal|first=Dorian|last=Lynskey|title=Folk story|journal=Cam|year=2016|issue=77|pages=36–41}}</ref>
In 1976, Treisman's marriage to Michel ended in a divorce.<ref name="obit nyt"/> She remarried in 1978 to [[Daniel Kahneman]], who won the [[Nobel Memorial Prize]] for Economics in 2002.<ref>{{cite web |title=Autobiography |url=https://www.nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/economics/laureates/2002/kahneman-autobio.html |publisher=[[nobelprize.org]] |last=Kahneman |first=Daniel |year=2002 |accessdate=14 October 2014}}</ref>


In 1957, Treisman attended [[Somerville College, Oxford]], to work toward her [[DPhil]] under her advisor, Carolus Oldfield.<ref name="History of Research on Attention" /> Treisman conducted research on [[aphasia]], but soon pursued interest in non-clinical populations. Treisman's research was guided by [[Donald Broadbent]]'s book, ''Perception and Communication''.<ref name="Broadbent 1958">{{cite book|last=Broadbent|first=D.&nbsp;E.|author-link=Donald Broadbent|title=Perception and Communication|publisher=Pergamon Press|location=Elmsford, NY|year=1958}}</ref>
She died on 9 February 2018 from a stroke.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/local/obituaries/anne-treisman-pathbreaking-psychologist-who-developed-a-theory-of-perception-dies-at-82/2018/02/14/4eaf0de2-1199-11e8-8ea1-c1d91fcec3fe_story.html|title=Anne Treisman, pathbreaking psychologist who developed a theory of perception, dies at 82|author=|date=|website=Washington Post}}</ref>

Treisman completed her thesis, "Attention and speech", in 1961.<ref name="History of Research on Attention" /><ref>{{cite thesis |title=Attention and speech |url=https://www.wikidata.org/wiki/Q59547331 |date=1 January 1961 |first=Anne |last=Treisman}}</ref>


== Career ==
== Career ==
[[File:Anne Treisman - National Medal of Science, 2011.webm|thumb|Treisman discussing her life and career]]
Around the time Treisman was working toward her DPhil, psychology was shifting from the traditional behaviorist view to that of an active processor of information.<ref>{{cite book | last = Goldstein | first = E.B. | authorlink = Bruce Goldstein | title = Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience | publisher = Wadsworth | location = Belmont, CA | year = 2005 }}</ref> Donald Broadbent and Colin Cherry had recently introduced selective listening (also known as the "[[cocktail party effect]]")<ref>{{cite journal | last = Cherry | first = Colin | title = Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and Two Ears | journal = The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | volume = 25 | issue = 5 | pages = 975–979 | year = 1953 | doi=10.1121/1.1907229}}</ref> Broadbent later proposed a Filter Model of selective attention which proposed that unattended auditory information is filtered out early in the process of perception and is not analysed.<ref name="Broadbent 1958"/> This theory was criticised because it could not explain why unattended information sometimes gets through the "filter".
Around the time Treisman was working toward her DPhil, psychology was shifting from a behaviorist point to view to the idea that behavior is the outcome of active information processing.<ref>{{cite book | last = Goldstein | first = E.&nbsp;B. | author-link = Bruce Goldstein | title = Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience | url = https://archive.org/details/cognitivepsychol00gold | url-access = registration | publisher = Wadsworth | location = Belmont, CA | year = 2005 | isbn = 978-0-534-57726-1 }}</ref> Donald Broadbent and [[Colin Cherry]] had recently introduced the idea of selective listening (often exemplified by the so-called "[[cocktail party effect]]")<ref>{{cite journal | last = Cherry | first = Colin | author-link = Colin Cherry | title = Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and Two Ears | journal = The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | volume = 25 | issue = 5 | pages = 975–979 | year = 1953 | doi = 10.1121/1.1907229 | hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-002A-F750-3 | url = http://pubman.mpdl.mpg.de/pubman/item/escidoc:2309493/component/escidoc:2309492/Cherry_1953-some-.experiments.pdf | hdl-access = free | access-date = 20 September 2019 | archive-date = 21 September 2017 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20170921233902/http://pubman.mpdl.mpg.de/pubman/item/escidoc:2309493/component/escidoc:2309492/Cherry_1953-some-.experiments.pdf | url-status = live }}</ref> Broadbent later proposed a [[Broadbent's filter model of attention|Filter Model of selective attention]] which states that unattended auditory information is not analysed but rather it is filtered out early in the process of perception.<ref name="Broadbent 1958" /> This theory was criticised because it could not explain why unattended information sometimes gets through the "filter".


After receiving her DPhil, Treisman worked in the Medical Research Council Psycholinguistics Research Unit conducting research in selective listening.<ref name="History of Research on Attention"/> In 1964, Treisman proposed her Attenuation Theory, which countered Broadbent's Filter model and proposed that unattended information is attenuated rather than completely filtered out of consciousness. Treisman used a dichotic listening task during which participants heard multiple languages and different voices (male vs. female). She showed that a difference between two equally known languages allowed no more efficient selection than a difference in subject matter between two messages in the same language. Unknown foreign languages, however, produced less interference.<ref name="Treisman 1964 206–219">{{cite journal | last = Treisman | first = Anne | title = Verbal cues, language and meaning in selective attention | journal = The American Journal of Psychology| volume = 77 | issue = 2 | pages = 206–219 | year = 1964 | doi=10.2307/1420127}}</ref> It appeared that complete rejection or filtering of one language was almost impossible; with some degree of variability depending on physical characteristics and language of the message received. Treisman concluded that features of incoming messages are analysed successfully from the [[nervous system]] and that selection between messages in the same voice, intensity, and localisation takes place during, rather than before or after, the analysis, which results in the identification of their verbal content.<ref name="Treisman 1964 206–219"/> At this stage, the information-handling capacity becomes limited and can only handle one input at a time, either keeping to one message where possible, or switching between the two. Thus, Broadbent's suggestion that classes of words constitute separate "input channels" could be rejected. Her theory was also that physical characteristics are processed early, while semantic processing occurs at a later point.<ref name="Treisman 1964 206–219"/>
After receiving her DPhil, Treisman worked in the [[Medical Research Council (United Kingdom)|Medical Research Council]]'s Psycholinguistics Research Unit at Oxford conducting research in selective listening.<ref name="evans">{{cite journal|title=Anne Marie Treisman (1935–2018)|first=Karla&nbsp;K.|last=Evans|date=1 July 2018|journal=Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics|volume=80|issue=5|pages=1027–1029|doi=10.3758/s13414-018-1563-2|pmid=29934823|s2cid=49386674|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="History of Research on Attention" /> In 1964, Treisman proposed her Attenuation Theory, which modified Broadbent's Filter model by stating that unattended information is attenuated rather than completely filtered out. Treisman used a dichotic listening task during which participants heard multiple languages and different voices (male vs. female). She showed that a difference between two equally known languages allowed no more efficient selection than a difference in subject matter between two messages in the same language. Unknown foreign languages, however, produced less interference.<ref name="Treisman 1964 206–219">{{cite journal | last = Treisman | first = Anne | title = Verbal cues, language and meaning in selective attention | journal = The American Journal of Psychology| volume = 77 | issue = 2 | pages = 206–219 | year = 1964 | doi=10.2307/1420127| jstor = 1420127 | pmid = 14141474 }}</ref> It appeared that complete rejection of one language was almost impossible; with some degree of variability depending on physical characteristics and language of the message received. Treisman concluded that features of multiple incoming messages are successfully analysed, and that selection between messages in the same voice, intensity, and localisation takes place during, rather than before or after, this analysis, which results in the identification of their verbal content.<ref name="Treisman 1964 206–219" /> Information-handling capacity is limited following this analysis; the process handles one input at a time, either keeping to one message where possible, or switching between the two. Thus, Broadbent's suggestion that classes of words constitute separate "input channels" could be rejected. Her theory also indicated that physical characteristics are processed early, while semantic processing occurs at a later point.<ref name="Treisman 1964 206–219" /> Her work had an enormous impact on her field. For example, In 1967, while working as a visiting scientist at [[Bell Labs|Bell Telephone Laboratories]]' psychology department, she published a paper in ''[[Psychological Review]]'' that was "central to the development of selective attention as a scientific field of study".<ref>{{cite web|title=William James Fellow Award 2002|url=https://www.psychologicalscience.org/members/awards-and-honors/fellow-award/recipent-past-award-winners/anne_treisman|website=Association for Psychological Science|access-date=30 January 2022|archive-date=30 January 2022|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220130195027/https://www.psychologicalscience.org/members/awards-and-honors/fellow-award/recipent-past-award-winners/anne_treisman|url-status=live}}</ref>


Treisman and Kahneman accepted positions at the [[University of British Columbia]] shortly after their marriage. In 1980, Treisman and Gelade published their seminal paper on Feature Integration Theory (FIT).<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu">{{cite journal|last1=Treisman |first1=Anne |author1-link=Anne Treisman |last2=Gelade |first2=Garry |title=A Feature-Integration Theory of Attention |journal=Cognitive Psychology |volume=12 |pages=97–136 |year=1980 |issue=1 |url=http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/class/psy355/gilden/treisman.pdf |doi=10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 |pmid=7351125 |s2cid=353246 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080905025042/http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/class/Psy355/Gilden/treisman.pdf |archive-date=5 September 2008 }}</ref> One key element of FIT is that early stages of object perception encode features such as color, form, and orientation as separate entities; focused attention combines these distinct features into perceived objects.
In 1967, while working as a visiting scientist at [[Bell Labs|Bell Telephone Laboratories]]' psychology department, she published a paper in ''Psychological Review'' that was "central to the development of selective attention as a scientific field of study".<ref>{{cite web|website=Association for Psychological Science|url=https://www.psychologicalscience.org/awards/james/citations/treisman.cfm|title=William James Fellow Award 2002}}</ref>

Treisman later returned to Oxford, where she accepted a position in the Psychology Department as a University lecturer and was appointed a Fellow of [[St Anne's College, Oxford|St. Anne's College]] (Treisman, 2006). Here, she began exploring the idea that attention is involved in integrating distinct features to form a whole representation of an object.

Treisman and Kahneman accepted positions at the [[University of British Columbia]] shortly after their marriage. In 1980, Treisman and Gelade published their seminal classic proposing Feature Integration Theory (FIT).<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu">{{cite journal|last1=Treisman |first1=Anne |author1-link=Anne Treisman |last2=Gelade |first2=Garry |title=A Feature-Integration Theory of Attention |journal=Cognitive Psychology |volume=12 |pages=97–136 |year=1980 |url=http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/class/psy355/gilden/treisman.pdf |doi=10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 |pmid=7351125 |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20080905025042/http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/class/Psy355/Gilden/treisman.pdf |archivedate=5 September 2008 }}</ref> One key element of FIT is that early stages of object perception encode features such as color, form, and orientation as separate entities; focused attention combines the distinct features into object perception.


Treisman moved to the University of California, Berkeley, in 1986, where she and Kahneman ran a joint "Attention Lab" in the Psychology Department. From 1993 until her retirement, in 2010, Treisman was a member of the Psychology Department at Princeton University. She was named Princeton's James S. McDonnell Distinguished University Professor of Psychology in 1995. Her work has appeared in 29 book chapters and more than 80 journal articles and is heavily cited in the psychological literature, as well as prominently included in both introductory and advanced textbooks. Established with an anonymous gift in 2015, the Kahneman-Treisman Center for Behavioral Science & Public Policy, housed in Princeton's Woodrow Wilson School, honors the legacy of Daniel Kahneman and Anne Treisman.
Treisman moved to the University of California, Berkeley, in 1986, where she and Kahneman ran a joint "Attention Lab" in the Psychology Department. From 1993 until her retirement, in 2010, Treisman was a member of the Psychology Department at Princeton University. She was named Princeton's James S. McDonnell Distinguished University Professor of Psychology in 1995. Her work has appeared in 29 book chapters and more than 80 journal articles and is heavily cited in the psychological literature, as well as prominently included in both introductory and advanced textbooks. Established with an anonymous gift in 2015, the Kahneman-Treisman Center for Behavioral Science & Public Policy, housed in Princeton's Woodrow Wilson School, honors the legacy of Daniel Kahneman and Anne Treisman.


== Feature integration theory ==
=== Feature integration theory ===
{{main article|Feature integration theory}}
{{Main|Feature integration theory}}
Treisman's feature integration theory is a two-stage model of visual object perception:
Treisman's feature integration theory is a two-stage model of visual object perception:


;Pre-attentive stage
;Pre-attentive stage
The first stage is called "pre-attentive" because it happens automatically, or without effort or attention by the perceiver. In this stage, an object is broken down into its elementary features for processing (i.e., color, [[Surface texture|texture]], [[shape]], etc.). Treisman posits we are unaware of this stage of attention because it occurs quickly and early in perceptual processes (before conscious awareness).<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu"/>
The first stage is called "pre-attentive" because it happens automatically, or without effort or attention by the perceiver. In this stage, an object is broken down into its elementary features for processing (i.e., color, [[Surface texture|texture]], [[shape]], etc.). Treisman posits we are unaware of this stage of attention because it occurs quickly and early in perceptual processes (before conscious awareness).<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu" />
Evidence for the pre-attentive state comes from Treisman's own studies. Treisman created a display of four objects flanked by two black numbers. The display flashed on a computer screen for 1/5 of a second and followed by a random-dot masking field to eliminate residual perception of the stimuli after the stimuli were turned off. Participants were asked to first report on the black numbers, followed by what they saw at each of the four locations where the shapes had been.<ref name="Goldstein 2010">{{cite book | last = Goldstein | first = Bruce | title = Sensation and Perception | publisher = Cengage Learning | edition = 8th | location = Wadsworth | year = 2010 }}</ref> Under these conditions, participants reported seeing illusory conjunctions in 18% of trials. That is, participants reported seeing objects that consisted of a combination of features from two different stimuli.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Treisman | first = Anne | author1-link = Anne Treisman | title = Focused attention in the perception and retrieval of multidimensional stimuli | journal = Perception and Psychophysics | volume = 22 | pages = 1–11 | year = 1977 | doi=10.3758/bf03206074}}</ref> For example, after seeing a big yellow circle, a big blue triangle, a small red triangle, and a small green circle, a person might report seeing a small red circle and a small green triangle. The reason illusory conjunctions occurred is that stimuli were presented rapidly and the observers' attention was distracted from the target object by having them focus on the black numbers; thus, elementary features had not yet been grouped or bound to an object. Having participants attend to the target objects eliminated the illusory conjunction.<ref name="ReferenceA">{{cite journal | last1 = Treisman | first1 = Anne | author1-link = Anne Treisman | last2 = Schmidt | first2 = Hilary | title = Illusory Conjunctions in the Perception of Objects | journal = Cognitive Psychology | volume = 14 | pages = 107–141 | year = 1982 | doi=10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8 | pmid=7053925}}</ref>
Evidence for the pre-attentive state comes from Treisman's own studies. In a well-known study, Treisman created a display of four objects flanked by two black numbers. The display flashed on a computer screen for 1/5 of a second and followed by a random-dot masking field to eliminate residual perception of the stimuli after the stimuli were turned off. Participants were asked to first report on the black numbers, followed by what they saw at each of the four locations where the shapes had been.<ref name="Goldstein 2010">{{cite book | last = Goldstein | first = Bruce | title = Sensation and Perception | publisher = Cengage Learning | edition = 8th | location = Wadsworth | year = 2010 }}</ref> Under these conditions, participants reported seeing illusory conjunctions in 18% of trials. That is, participants reported seeing objects that consisted of a combination of features from two different stimuli.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Treisman | first = Anne | author1-link = Anne Treisman | title = Focused attention in the perception and retrieval of multidimensional stimuli | journal = Perception and Psychophysics | volume = 22 | pages = 1–11 | year = 1977 | doi=10.3758/bf03206074| doi-access = free }}</ref> For example, after seeing a big yellow circle, a big blue triangle, a small red triangle, and a small green circle, a person might report seeing a small red circle and a small green triangle. The reason illusory conjunctions occurred is that stimuli were presented rapidly and the observers' attention was distracted from the target object by having them focus on the black numbers; thus, elementary features had not yet been grouped or bound to an object. Having participants attend to the target objects eliminated the illusory conjunction.<ref name="ReferenceA">{{cite journal | last1 = Treisman | first1 = Anne | author1-link = Anne Treisman | last2 = Schmidt | first2 = Hilary | title = Illusory Conjunctions in the Perception of Objects | journal = Cognitive Psychology | volume = 14 | pages = 107–141 | year = 1982 | issue = 1 | doi=10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8 | pmid=7053925| s2cid = 11201516 }}</ref>


;Focused attention stage
;Focused attention stage
The second stage of processing depends on attention. In this stage, the features are combined, resulting in the perception of a whole object rather than individual features.<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu" /> Treisman linked this process of binding to neural activity, noting that an object causes activity in both the "what" and "where" areas of the cortex (see [[Two-streams hypothesis]]). Activity in the "what" processing stream would include information about color and form, while activity in the "where" stream would include information about location and motion. According to Treisman, attention is the "glue" that combines the information from both streams and causes us to perceive all the features of an object as combined at one specific location.<ref name="Goldstein 2010" /> Perceiving one object in isolation appears relatively straightforward, but when we confront multiple objects, numerous features may exist at different locations. The perceptual system's task is to associate each of these features with the object to which it belongs. Feature integration theory says that in order for this to occur, we need to focus our attention on each object in turn. Once we attend to a particular location, the features at that location are bound together and are associated with the object at that location.<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu" />
The second stage of processing depends on attention. In this stage, the features are recombined, so we perceive the whole object rather than individual features.<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu"/>


Treisman's FIT model uses three different spatially selective mechanisms to solve the binding problem: selection by a spatial attention window, inhibition of locations from feature maps containing unwanted features, and top-down activation of the location containing the attended object.<ref name="Treisman 1996 171–178">{{cite journal | last = Treisman | first = Anne | title = The binding problem | journal = Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 6 | pages = 171–178 | year = 1996 | doi=10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80070-5 | pmid=8725958 | issue=2| s2cid = 8643357 }}</ref>
Treisman linked the process of binding that occurs in the focused attention stage to physiology by noting that an object causes activity in both the "what" and "where" streams of the cortex (see [[Two-streams hypothesis]]). Activity in the "what" stream would include information about color and form, while activity in the "where" stream would include information about location and motion. According to Treisman, attention is the "glue" that combines the information from both streams and causes us to perceive all the features of an object as combined at one specific location.<ref name="Goldstein 2010"/> It is easy to consider perceiving one object in isolation, but when we consider multiple objects, numerous features exist at many locations. The perceptual system's task is to associate each of these features with the object to which it belongs. Feature integration theory proposes that in order for this to occur, we need to focus our attention on each object in turn. Once we attend to a particular location, the features at that location are bound together and are associated with the object at that location.<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu"/>


=== The binding problem ===
Treisman repeated the illusory conjunction experiment, but now instructed participants to ignore the flanking numbers and focus their attention on the four target objects. Focused attention eliminated illusory conjunctions.<ref name="ReferenceA"/>
[[William James]] discussed the connection between attention and mental processes, "Millions of items…are present to my senses which never properly enter my experience. Why? Because they have no interest for me. My experience is what I agree to attend to…Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought…. It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others."<ref name="Goldstein 2010" />


In the early 1980s, neuroscientists such as [[Torsten Wiesel]] and [[David H. Hubel]] were discovering that different areas of the primate visual cortex were finely tuned to selective features, such as line orientation, luminance, color, movement, etc. These findings prompted the question of how these distinct features are connected into a unified whole. This question has been called the [[binding problem]]. For example, when you see a red ball roll by, cells sensitive to movement fire in the medial temporal cortex, while cells sensitive to color, shape and location fire in other areas. Despite all this distinct neuronal firing, you don't perceive the ball as separated by shape, movement and color perceptions; you experience an integrated experience with all these components occurring together.<ref name="Goldstein 2010" /> The question of how these elements are combined is the essence of the binding problem, a central focus of research into the late 1990s. A number of possible mechanisms were envisaged, including [[grandmother cells]] responding to specific conjunctions of features that uniquely identify a particular object;<ref>{{cite journal | last = Barlow | first = H.&nbsp;B. | title = Single units and cognition: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology | journal = Perception | volume = 1 | pages = 371–394 | year = 1972 | issue = 4 | doi=10.1068/p010371 | pmid=4377168| s2cid = 17487970 | doi-access = free }}</ref> local cell assemblies onto which the pathways from different feature maps converge, perhaps with adjustable connections allowing flexible routing of signals;<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Olshausen | first1 = B.&nbsp;A. | last2 = Anderson | first2 = C.&nbsp;H. | last3 = Van Essen | first3 = D.&nbsp;C. | title = A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information | journal = Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 13 | pages = 4700–4719 | year = 1993 | issue = 11 | doi = 10.1523/jneurosci.13-11-04700.1993 | pmid = 8229193 | pmc = 6576339 | doi-access = free }}</ref> a serial scan of different spatial areas selected by an adjustable attention window, conjoining the features that each contains and excluding features from adjacent areas;<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu" /> detection of temporal contiguity – parts and properties whose onset, offset or motion coincide probably belong to the same object synchronised firing of cells responding to features of the same object, perhaps assisted by oscillatory neural activity.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Milner | first = P. | title = A model for visual shape recognition | journal = Psychological Review| volume = 15 | pages = 521–535 | year = 1974 | issue = 6 | doi = 10.1037/h0037149 | pmid = 4445414 }}</ref> Treisman used failures of binding to shed light on its underlying mechanisms. Specifically, she found that left-brain-damaged patients have increasing illusory conjunctions and decreased performance in a spatially cued attention task, which suggests a link between attentional binding and the [[parietal lobes]].<ref name="Treisman 1996 171–178" /> Treisman also cited corroborating evidence from [[positron emission tomography]] and [[event-related potential]] studies which were consistent with the spatial attention account of feature integration.
Treisman's FIT model now uses three different spatially selective mechanisms to solve the binding problem: selection by a spatial attention window, inhibition of locations from feature maps containing unwanted features, and top-down activation of the location containing the currently attended object.<ref name="Treisman 1996 171–178">{{cite journal | last = Treisman | first = Anne | title = The binding problem | journal = Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 6 | pages = 171–178 | year = 1996 | doi=10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80070-5 | pmid=8725958 | issue=2}}</ref>


Treisman's work formed the basis for thousands of experiments in [[cognitive psychology]], vision sciences, [[cognitive science]], [[cognitive neuropsychology]] and [[cognitive neuroscience]].
==Impact==
[[William James]] discussed the connection between attention and mental processes, "Millions of items…are present to my senses which never properly enter my experience. Why? Because they have no interest for me. My experience is what I agree to attend to…Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought…. It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others."<ref name="Goldstein 2010"/>

In the early 1980s, neuroscientists such as [[Torston Wiesel]] and [[David H. Hubel]] were discovering that different areas of the primate visual cortex were finely tuned to selective features, such as line orientation, luminance, color, movement, etc. These findings prompted the question of how these distinct features are connected into a unified whole, e.g., the [[binding problem]]. For example, when you see a red ball roll by, cells sensitive to movement fire in the medial temporal cortex, while cells sensitive to color, shape and location fire in other areas. Despite all this distinct neuronal firing, you don't perceive the ball as separated by shape, movement and color perceptions; you experience an integrated experience with all these components occurring together.<ref name="Goldstein 2010"/> The question of how these elements are combined is the essence of the binding problem and continued into the late 1990s. A number of possible mechanisms were envisaged, including [[grandmother cells]] responding to specific conjunctions of features that uniquely identify a particular object;<ref>{{cite journal | last = Barlow | first = HB | title = Single units and cognition: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology | journal = Perception | volume = 1 | pages = 371–394 | year = 1972 | doi=10.1068/p010371}}</ref> local cell assemblies onto which the pathways from different feature maps converge, perhaps with adjustable connections allowing flexible routing of signals;<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Olshausen | first1 = BA | last2 = Anderson | first2 = CH | last3 = Van Essen | first3 = DC | title = A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information | journal = Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 13 | pages = 4700–4719 | year = 1993 }}</ref> a serial scan of different spatial areas selected by an adjustable attention window, conjoining the features that each contains and excluding features from adjacent areas;<ref name="homepage.psy.utexas.edu"/> detection of temporal contiguity – parts and properties whose onset, offset or motion coincide probably belong to the same object synchronised firing of cells responding to features of the same object, perhaps assisted by oscillatory neural activity.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Milner | first = P | title = A model for visual shape recognition | journal = J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept Perform] | volume = 15 | pages = 521–535 | year = 1974 }}</ref> Treisman used failures of binding to shed light on its underlying mechanisms. Specifically, she found that left-brain-damaged patients have increasing illusory conjunctions and decreased performance in a spatially cued attention task, which suggests a link between attentional binding and the [[parietal lobes]].<ref name="Treisman 1996 171–178"/> Treisman also cited corroborating evidence from [[positron emission tomography]] and [[event-related potential]] studies which were consistent with the spatial attention account of feature integration.

Treisman's work formed the basis for thousands of experiments in [[cognitive psychology]], vision sciences, [[cognitive science]], [[neuropsychology]] and [[cognitive neuroscience]].


== Honors ==
== Honors ==


Treisman was elected to the [[Royal Society of London]] in 1989, the [[US National Academy of Sciences]] in 1994, and the [[American Academy of Arts and Sciences]] in 1995, as well as a William James Fellow of the [[American Psychological Society]] in 2002. Treisman was the recipient of the 2009 [[University of Louisville]] [[Grawemeyer Award]] in Psychology for her explanation of how our brains build meaningful images from what we see.<ref name="grawemeyer.org">{{cite web|url=http://grawemeyer.org/psychology/previous-winners/2009.html|title=2009– Anne Treisman|last=|first=|date=|website=grawemeyer.org|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131105021513/http://grawemeyer.org/psychology/previous-winners/2009.html|archive-date=5 November 2013|dead-url=yes|access-date=|df=dmy-all}}</ref> In 2013, Treisman received the National Medal of Science from President Barack Obama for her pioneering work in the study of attention.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.nationalmedals.org/laureates/anne-treisman#|title=Anne Treisman|last=Dunn|first=Sydni|date=|website=National Science & Technology Medals Foundation|access-date=}}</ref>
Treisman was elected to the [[Royal Society of London]] in 1989,<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Kahneman|first1=Daniel|last2=Treisman|first2=Deborah|date=2020|title=Anne Marie Treisman. 27 February 1935 – 9 February 2018|journal=Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society|volume=68|pages=407–430|doi=10.1098/rsbm.2019.0035|doi-access=free}}</ref> the [[US National Academy of Sciences]] in 1994, the [[American Academy of Arts and Sciences]] in 1995, and the [[American Philosophical Society]] in 2005,<ref>{{cite web|title=APS Member History|url=https://search.amphilsoc.org/memhist/search?creator=Anne+M.+Treisman&title=&subject=&subdiv=&mem=&year=&year-max=&dead=&keyword=&smode=advanced|access-date=27 May 2021|website=search.amphilsoc.org|archive-date=6 February 2022|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220206224752/https://search.amphilsoc.org/memhist/search?creator=Anne+M.+Treisman&title=&subject=&subdiv=&mem=&year=&year-max=&dead=&keyword=&smode=advanced|url-status=live}}</ref> as well as a William James Fellow of the [[American Psychological Society]] in 2002. Treisman was the recipient of the 2009 [[University of Louisville]] [[Grawemeyer Award]] in Psychology for her explanation of how our brains build meaningful images from what we see.<ref name="grawemeyer.org">{{cite web|url=http://grawemeyer.org/psychology/previous-winners/2009.html|title=2009– Anne Treisman|website=grawemeyer.org|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131105021513/http://grawemeyer.org/psychology/previous-winners/2009.html|archive-date=5 November 2013|url-status=dead}}</ref> In 2013, Treisman received the National Medal of Science from President Barack Obama for her pioneering work in the study of attention.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.nationalmedals.org/laureates/anne-treisman|title=Anne Treisman|last=Dunn|first=Sydni|website=National Science & Technology Medals Foundation|access-date=3 April 2018|archive-date=14 August 2020|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200814024847/https://www.nationalmedals.org/laureates/anne-treisman|url-status=live}}</ref>


== Selected publications ==
== Selected publications ==
Key works include:
Key works include:
*{{cite journal | last1 = Treisman | first1 = A. | last2 = Gelade | first2 = G. | year = 1980 | title = A feature integration theory of attention | url = | journal = Cognitive Psychology | volume = 12 | issue = | pages = 97–136 | doi=10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 | pmid=7351125}}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Treisman | first1 = A. | last2 = Gelade | first2 = G. | year = 1980 | title = A feature integration theory of attention | journal = Cognitive Psychology | volume = 12 | issue = 1 | pages = 97–136 | doi=10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 | pmid=7351125| s2cid = 353246 }}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Treisman | first1 = A | year = 1991 | title = Search, similarity and the integration of features between and within dimensions | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | volume = 17 | issue = | pages = 652–676 | doi=10.1037/0096-1523.17.3.652}}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Treisman | first1 = A. | year = 1991 | title = Search, similarity and the integration of features between and within dimensions | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | volume = 17 | issue = 3| pages = 652–676 | doi=10.1037/0096-1523.17.3.652| pmid = 1834783 }}
*{{cite journal|first=A.|last=Treisman|title=Features and Objects in Visual Processing|url=http://www-psychology.concordia.ca/fac/deAlmeida/PSYC352/Pages/Treisman-1986-Features.pdf|journal=[[Scientific American]]|location=New York City|date=November 1986|volume=255|issue=5|pages=114–125|format=pdf|doi=10.1038/scientificamerican1186-114B|access-date=6 July 2013|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161020180239/http://www-psychology.concordia.ca/fac/deAlmeida/PSYC352/Pages/Treisman-1986-Features.pdf|archive-date=20 October 2016|dead-url=yes}}
*{{cite journal|first=A.|last=Treisman|title=Features and Objects in Visual Processing|url=http://www-psychology.concordia.ca/fac/deAlmeida/PSYC352/Pages/Treisman-1986-Features.pdf|journal=[[Scientific American]]|location=New York City|date=November 1986|volume=255|issue=5|pages=114–125|doi=10.1038/scientificamerican1186-114B|bibcode=1986SciAm.255e.114T|access-date=6 July 2013|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161020180239/http://www-psychology.concordia.ca/fac/deAlmeida/PSYC352/Pages/Treisman-1986-Features.pdf|archive-date=20 October 2016|url-status=dead}}

== Personal life ==
Treisman married Michel Treisman in 1960, another Oxford graduate student.<ref name="obit nyt" /> They divorced in 1976.<ref name="obit nyt" /> She remarried in 1978 to [[Daniel Kahneman]], who won the [[Nobel Memorial Prize]] for Economics in 2002.<ref>{{cite web |title=Autobiography |url=https://www.nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/economics/laureates/2002/kahneman-autobio.html |publisher=[[nobelprize.org]] |last=Kahneman |first=Daniel |year=2002 |access-date=14 October 2014 |archive-date=30 May 2013 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130530215603/http://www.nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/economics/laureates/2002/kahneman-autobio.html |url-status=live }}</ref>

She had four children: [https://med.nyu.edu/faculty/jessica-e-treisman Jessica Treisman] (b. 1963), a professor of cell biology at NYU School of Medicine; [https://polisci.ucla.edu/person/daniel-treisman/ Daniel Treisman] (b. 1964), a professor of political science at UCLA; Stephen Treisman (b. 1968), who lives in Berkshire; and [[Deborah Treisman]] (b. 1970), a fiction editor at ''[[The New Yorker]].<ref name=":0">{{cite news |last=Finn |first=Robin |date=28 January 2003 |title=PUBLIC LIVES; A Bookworm as a Child, Now the Talk of the Town |work=The New York Times |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2003/01/28/nyregion/public-lives-a-bookworm-as-a-child-now-the-talk-of-the-town.html |access-date=29 August 2022 |issn=0362-4331 |archive-date=8 January 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230108111337/https://www.nytimes.com/2003/01/28/nyregion/public-lives-a-bookworm-as-a-child-now-the-talk-of-the-town.html |url-status=live }}</ref><ref name=":2">{{cite news |last1=Carr |first1=David |last2=Kirkpatrick |first2=David&nbsp;D. |date=21 October 2002 |title=The Gatekeeper For Literature Is Changing At New Yorker |work=The New York Times |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2002/10/21/business/the-gatekeeper-for-literature-is-changing-at-new-yorker.html |access-date=29 August 2022 |issn=0362-4331 |archive-date=8 January 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230108111337/https://www.nytimes.com/2002/10/21/business/the-gatekeeper-for-literature-is-changing-at-new-yorker.html |url-status=live }}</ref>

She died on 9 February 2018, from a [[stroke]], at her home in [[Manhattan]].<ref>{{cite news |url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/local/obituaries/anne-treisman-pathbreaking-psychologist-who-developed-a-theory-of-perception-dies-at-82/2018/02/14/4eaf0de2-1199-11e8-8ea1-c1d91fcec3fe_story.html |title=Anne Treisman, pathbreaking psychologist who developed a theory of perception, dies at 82 |newspaper=The Washington Post |last=Smith |first=Harrison |date=14 February 2018 |access-date=6 January 2022 |url-status=live |archive-date=2 July 2018 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180702093341/https://www.washingtonpost.com/local/obituaries/anne-treisman-pathbreaking-psychologist-who-developed-a-theory-of-perception-dies-at-82/2018/02/14/4eaf0de2-1199-11e8-8ea1-c1d91fcec3fe_story.html }}</ref><ref name=DeanPrinceton>{{cite web |publisher=Princeton University Dean of the Faculty |date=2018 |title=Anne Marie Treisman |access-date=12 February 2018 |url=https://dof.princeton.edu/about/clerk-faculty/emeritus/anne-marie-treisman |url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161216071940/http://dof.princeton.edu/about/clerk-faculty/emeritus/anne-marie-treisman |archive-date=16 December 2016 }}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/observer/obsonline/anne-treisman-1935-2018.html|title=Anne Treisman, 1935–2018 |publisher=Association for Psychological Science |access-date=12 February 2018 |url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180213080906/http://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/observer/obsonline/anne-treisman-1935-2018.html |archive-date=13 February 2018 }}</ref><ref name = KTCenter>{{cite web |publisher=Princeton University Kahneman-Treisman Center for Behavioral Science & Public Policy |date=2018 |title=Anne Treisman 1935–2018 |access-date=14 February 2018 |url=https://behavioralpolicy.princeton.edu/news/anne-treisman-1935-2018 |url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20191110115625/https://behavioralpolicy.princeton.edu/news/anne-treisman-1935-2018 |archive-date=10 November 2019 }}</ref>


==See also==
== See also ==
*[[Attention]]
*[[Subitizing]]
*[[Cognitive neuropsychology]]
*[[Subitizing and counting]]


== References ==
== References ==
Line 92: Line 95:


== Further reading ==
== Further reading ==
*{{cite book|editor1-last=Sheehy|editor1-first=Noel|editor2-first=Anthony J.|editor2-last=Chapman|editor3-first=Wenday A.|editor3-last=Conroy|url=https://books.google.com.my/books?id=5CJZCwAAQBAJ&pg=PA580&dq=anne+treisman+born&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwi4yZ3mzaXZAhUIG5QKHWE7A7EQ6AEIKDAB#v=onepage&q&f=false|year=2016|series=World Reference Series|title=Biographical Dictionary of Psychology|publisher=Routledge|edition=Reprint|isbn=978-1-136-79885-6|ref=harv}}
*{{cite book|editor1-last=Sheehy|editor1-first=Noel|editor2-first=Anthony&nbsp;J.|editor2-last=Chapman|editor3-first=Wenday&nbsp;A.|editor3-last=Conroy|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=5CJZCwAAQBAJ&q=anne+treisman+born&pg=PA580|year=2016|series=World Reference Series|title=Biographical Dictionary of Psychology|publisher=Routledge|edition=Reprint|isbn=978-1-136-79885-6}}


== External links ==
== External links ==
Line 98: Line 101:
* [http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~lynnlab/people/atreisman_cv.pdf Treisman's Academic CV]
* [http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~lynnlab/people/atreisman_cv.pdf Treisman's Academic CV]
* [https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GDSPbKGDMY4 Video of Treisman talking about her work], from the National Science & Technology Medals Foundation
* [https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GDSPbKGDMY4 Video of Treisman talking about her work], from the National Science & Technology Medals Foundation
* [https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-23/edition-11/one-one-anne-treisman "One on one... with Anne Treisman"] at ''[[The Psychologist (magazine)|The Psychologist]]'', published November 2010


{{Winners of the National Medal of Science|behav-social}}
{{Winners of the National Medal of Science|behav-social}}
Line 109: Line 113:
[[Category:English psychologists]]
[[Category:English psychologists]]
[[Category:2018 deaths]]
[[Category:2018 deaths]]
[[Category:Cognitive scientists]]
[[Category:Princeton University faculty]]
[[Category:Princeton University faculty]]
[[Category:Members of the United States National Academy of Sciences]]
[[Category:Members of the United States National Academy of Sciences]]
[[Category:Fellows of the Society of Experimental Psychologists]]
[[Category:Fellows of the Society of Experimental Psychologists]]
[[Category:Fellows of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences]]
[[Category:Fellows of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences]]
[[Category:Female Fellows of the Royal Society]]
[[Category:Female fellows of the Royal Society]]
[[Category:Fellows of the Royal Society]]
[[Category:Fellows of the Royal Society]]
[[Category:Cognitive psychologists]]
[[Category:Women cognitive scientists]]
[[Category:Women cognitive scientists]]
[[Category:Fellows of the Cognitive Science Society]]
[[Category:Fellows of the Cognitive Science Society]]
[[Category:National Medal of Science laureates]]
[[Category:National Medal of Science laureates]]
[[Category:Alumni of Somerville College, Oxford]]
[[Category:Members of the American Philosophical Society]]
[[Category:Corresponding fellows of the British Academy]]
[[Category:APA Distinguished Scientific Award for an Early Career Contribution to Psychology recipients]]

Latest revision as of 08:32, 13 September 2024

Anne M. Treisman
Treisman in 2011, Princeton University, recipient of the National Medal of Science
Born
Anne Marie Taylor

(1935-02-27)27 February 1935
Died9 February 2018(2018-02-09) (aged 82)
Alma materNewnham College, Cambridge
Somerville College, Oxford
Known forFeature integration theory, Attenuation theory
Spouses
Michel Treisman
(m. 1960; div. 1976)
(m. 1978)
ChildrenDeborah Treisman, and three others
AwardsGolden Brain Award (1996)
Grawemeyer Award in Psychology (2009)
National Medal of Science (2011)
Scientific career
FieldsPsychology
InstitutionsPrinceton University
Doctoral advisorRichard C. Oldfield
Notable studentsPostdoctoral fellows Nancy Kanwisher and Nilli Lavie

Anne Marie Treisman (née Taylor; 27 February 1935 – 9 February 2018) was an English psychologist who specialised in cognitive psychology.

Treisman researched visual attention, object perception, and memory. One of her most influential ideas is the feature integration theory of attention, first published with Garry Gelade in 1980. Treisman taught at the University of Oxford, University of British Columbia, University of California, Berkeley, and Princeton University. Notable postdoctoral fellows she supervised included Nancy Kanwisher and Nilli Lavie.

In 2013, Treisman received the National Medal of Science from President Barack Obama for her pioneering work in the study of attention.[1] During her long career, Treisman experimentally and theoretically defined the issue of how information is selected and integrated to form meaningful objects that guide human thought and action.

Early life and education

[edit]

Anne Treisman was born in Wakefield, West Riding of Yorkshire, England on 27 February 1935.[2] Two years later, her family moved to a village near Rochester, Kent[3] where her father, Percy Taylor, worked as chief education officer during World War II.[4] Her mother, Suzanne Touren, was French.[2] At the age of 11, Treisman moved with her family to Reading, Berkshire where she attended the girls' grammar school Kendrick School.[5] The English educational system at the time forced Treisman to choose only three subjects in her last two years at secondary school, and Treisman focused on the language arts (French, Latin and History).

Treisman received her BA in French Literature at Newnham College, Cambridge, in 1954. She received a first class BA with distinction, which earned her a scholarship that she used to obtain a second BA in psychology. During this extra year, Treisman studied under the supervision of Richard Gregory, who introduced her to various methods of exploring the mind through experiments in perception.[6] While at Cambridge, she was active in the folk music scene.[7]

In 1957, Treisman attended Somerville College, Oxford, to work toward her DPhil under her advisor, Carolus Oldfield.[6] Treisman conducted research on aphasia, but soon pursued interest in non-clinical populations. Treisman's research was guided by Donald Broadbent's book, Perception and Communication.[8]

Treisman completed her thesis, "Attention and speech", in 1961.[6][9]

Career

[edit]
Treisman discussing her life and career

Around the time Treisman was working toward her DPhil, psychology was shifting from a behaviorist point to view to the idea that behavior is the outcome of active information processing.[10] Donald Broadbent and Colin Cherry had recently introduced the idea of selective listening (often exemplified by the so-called "cocktail party effect")[11] Broadbent later proposed a Filter Model of selective attention which states that unattended auditory information is not analysed but rather it is filtered out early in the process of perception.[8] This theory was criticised because it could not explain why unattended information sometimes gets through the "filter".

After receiving her DPhil, Treisman worked in the Medical Research Council's Psycholinguistics Research Unit at Oxford conducting research in selective listening.[12][6] In 1964, Treisman proposed her Attenuation Theory, which modified Broadbent's Filter model by stating that unattended information is attenuated rather than completely filtered out. Treisman used a dichotic listening task during which participants heard multiple languages and different voices (male vs. female). She showed that a difference between two equally known languages allowed no more efficient selection than a difference in subject matter between two messages in the same language. Unknown foreign languages, however, produced less interference.[13] It appeared that complete rejection of one language was almost impossible; with some degree of variability depending on physical characteristics and language of the message received. Treisman concluded that features of multiple incoming messages are successfully analysed, and that selection between messages in the same voice, intensity, and localisation takes place during, rather than before or after, this analysis, which results in the identification of their verbal content.[13] Information-handling capacity is limited following this analysis; the process handles one input at a time, either keeping to one message where possible, or switching between the two. Thus, Broadbent's suggestion that classes of words constitute separate "input channels" could be rejected. Her theory also indicated that physical characteristics are processed early, while semantic processing occurs at a later point.[13] Her work had an enormous impact on her field. For example, In 1967, while working as a visiting scientist at Bell Telephone Laboratories' psychology department, she published a paper in Psychological Review that was "central to the development of selective attention as a scientific field of study".[14]

Treisman and Kahneman accepted positions at the University of British Columbia shortly after their marriage. In 1980, Treisman and Gelade published their seminal paper on Feature Integration Theory (FIT).[15] One key element of FIT is that early stages of object perception encode features such as color, form, and orientation as separate entities; focused attention combines these distinct features into perceived objects.

Treisman moved to the University of California, Berkeley, in 1986, where she and Kahneman ran a joint "Attention Lab" in the Psychology Department. From 1993 until her retirement, in 2010, Treisman was a member of the Psychology Department at Princeton University. She was named Princeton's James S. McDonnell Distinguished University Professor of Psychology in 1995. Her work has appeared in 29 book chapters and more than 80 journal articles and is heavily cited in the psychological literature, as well as prominently included in both introductory and advanced textbooks. Established with an anonymous gift in 2015, the Kahneman-Treisman Center for Behavioral Science & Public Policy, housed in Princeton's Woodrow Wilson School, honors the legacy of Daniel Kahneman and Anne Treisman.

Feature integration theory

[edit]

Treisman's feature integration theory is a two-stage model of visual object perception:

Pre-attentive stage

The first stage is called "pre-attentive" because it happens automatically, or without effort or attention by the perceiver. In this stage, an object is broken down into its elementary features for processing (i.e., color, texture, shape, etc.). Treisman posits we are unaware of this stage of attention because it occurs quickly and early in perceptual processes (before conscious awareness).[15] Evidence for the pre-attentive state comes from Treisman's own studies. In a well-known study, Treisman created a display of four objects flanked by two black numbers. The display flashed on a computer screen for 1/5 of a second and followed by a random-dot masking field to eliminate residual perception of the stimuli after the stimuli were turned off. Participants were asked to first report on the black numbers, followed by what they saw at each of the four locations where the shapes had been.[16] Under these conditions, participants reported seeing illusory conjunctions in 18% of trials. That is, participants reported seeing objects that consisted of a combination of features from two different stimuli.[17] For example, after seeing a big yellow circle, a big blue triangle, a small red triangle, and a small green circle, a person might report seeing a small red circle and a small green triangle. The reason illusory conjunctions occurred is that stimuli were presented rapidly and the observers' attention was distracted from the target object by having them focus on the black numbers; thus, elementary features had not yet been grouped or bound to an object. Having participants attend to the target objects eliminated the illusory conjunction.[18]

Focused attention stage

The second stage of processing depends on attention. In this stage, the features are combined, resulting in the perception of a whole object rather than individual features.[15] Treisman linked this process of binding to neural activity, noting that an object causes activity in both the "what" and "where" areas of the cortex (see Two-streams hypothesis). Activity in the "what" processing stream would include information about color and form, while activity in the "where" stream would include information about location and motion. According to Treisman, attention is the "glue" that combines the information from both streams and causes us to perceive all the features of an object as combined at one specific location.[16] Perceiving one object in isolation appears relatively straightforward, but when we confront multiple objects, numerous features may exist at different locations. The perceptual system's task is to associate each of these features with the object to which it belongs. Feature integration theory says that in order for this to occur, we need to focus our attention on each object in turn. Once we attend to a particular location, the features at that location are bound together and are associated with the object at that location.[15]

Treisman's FIT model uses three different spatially selective mechanisms to solve the binding problem: selection by a spatial attention window, inhibition of locations from feature maps containing unwanted features, and top-down activation of the location containing the attended object.[19]

The binding problem

[edit]

William James discussed the connection between attention and mental processes, "Millions of items…are present to my senses which never properly enter my experience. Why? Because they have no interest for me. My experience is what I agree to attend to…Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought…. It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others."[16]

In the early 1980s, neuroscientists such as Torsten Wiesel and David H. Hubel were discovering that different areas of the primate visual cortex were finely tuned to selective features, such as line orientation, luminance, color, movement, etc. These findings prompted the question of how these distinct features are connected into a unified whole. This question has been called the binding problem. For example, when you see a red ball roll by, cells sensitive to movement fire in the medial temporal cortex, while cells sensitive to color, shape and location fire in other areas. Despite all this distinct neuronal firing, you don't perceive the ball as separated by shape, movement and color perceptions; you experience an integrated experience with all these components occurring together.[16] The question of how these elements are combined is the essence of the binding problem, a central focus of research into the late 1990s. A number of possible mechanisms were envisaged, including grandmother cells responding to specific conjunctions of features that uniquely identify a particular object;[20] local cell assemblies onto which the pathways from different feature maps converge, perhaps with adjustable connections allowing flexible routing of signals;[21] a serial scan of different spatial areas selected by an adjustable attention window, conjoining the features that each contains and excluding features from adjacent areas;[15] detection of temporal contiguity – parts and properties whose onset, offset or motion coincide probably belong to the same object synchronised firing of cells responding to features of the same object, perhaps assisted by oscillatory neural activity.[22] Treisman used failures of binding to shed light on its underlying mechanisms. Specifically, she found that left-brain-damaged patients have increasing illusory conjunctions and decreased performance in a spatially cued attention task, which suggests a link between attentional binding and the parietal lobes.[19] Treisman also cited corroborating evidence from positron emission tomography and event-related potential studies which were consistent with the spatial attention account of feature integration.

Treisman's work formed the basis for thousands of experiments in cognitive psychology, vision sciences, cognitive science, cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience.

Honors

[edit]

Treisman was elected to the Royal Society of London in 1989,[23] the US National Academy of Sciences in 1994, the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1995, and the American Philosophical Society in 2005,[24] as well as a William James Fellow of the American Psychological Society in 2002. Treisman was the recipient of the 2009 University of Louisville Grawemeyer Award in Psychology for her explanation of how our brains build meaningful images from what we see.[25] In 2013, Treisman received the National Medal of Science from President Barack Obama for her pioneering work in the study of attention.[26]

Selected publications

[edit]

Key works include:

  • Treisman, A.; Gelade, G. (1980). "A feature integration theory of attention". Cognitive Psychology. 12 (1): 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. PMID 7351125. S2CID 353246.
  • Treisman, A. (1991). "Search, similarity and the integration of features between and within dimensions". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 17 (3): 652–676. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.17.3.652. PMID 1834783.
  • Treisman, A. (November 1986). "Features and Objects in Visual Processing" (PDF). Scientific American. 255 (5). New York City: 114–125. Bibcode:1986SciAm.255e.114T. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican1186-114B. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 October 2016. Retrieved 6 July 2013.

Personal life

[edit]

Treisman married Michel Treisman in 1960, another Oxford graduate student.[2] They divorced in 1976.[2] She remarried in 1978 to Daniel Kahneman, who won the Nobel Memorial Prize for Economics in 2002.[27]

She had four children: Jessica Treisman (b. 1963), a professor of cell biology at NYU School of Medicine; Daniel Treisman (b. 1964), a professor of political science at UCLA; Stephen Treisman (b. 1968), who lives in Berkshire; and Deborah Treisman (b. 1970), a fiction editor at The New Yorker.[28][29]

She died on 9 February 2018, from a stroke, at her home in Manhattan.[30][31][32][33]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ "Treisman wins National Medal of Science for psychology research". Princeton University. 3 January 2013. Archived from the original on 13 April 2016. Retrieved 9 March 2013.
  2. ^ a b c d Genzlinger, Neil (13 February 2018). "Anne Treisman, Who Studied How We Perceive, Dies at 82". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 6 November 2018. Retrieved 14 February 2018.
  3. ^ Sheehy, Chapman & Conroy 2016, p. 580.
  4. ^ "One on one...with Anne Treisman". the psychologist. 9 October 2010. Archived from the original on 20 June 2013. Retrieved 18 November 2013.
  5. ^ "Anne Treisman". sfn.org. 16 April 2014. Archived from the original on 18 October 2018. Retrieved 12 February 2018.
  6. ^ a b c d Wikibooks:Applied History of Psychology/History of Research on Attention
  7. ^ Lynskey, Dorian (2016). "Folk story". Cam (77): 36–41.
  8. ^ a b Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and Communication. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.
  9. ^ Treisman, Anne (1 January 1961). Attention and speech (Thesis).
  10. ^ Goldstein, E. B. (2005). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. ISBN 978-0-534-57726-1.
  11. ^ Cherry, Colin (1953). "Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and Two Ears" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 25 (5): 975–979. doi:10.1121/1.1907229. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002A-F750-3. Archived (PDF) from the original on 21 September 2017. Retrieved 20 September 2019.
  12. ^ Evans, Karla K. (1 July 2018). "Anne Marie Treisman (1935–2018)". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 80 (5): 1027–1029. doi:10.3758/s13414-018-1563-2. PMID 29934823. S2CID 49386674.
  13. ^ a b c Treisman, Anne (1964). "Verbal cues, language and meaning in selective attention". The American Journal of Psychology. 77 (2): 206–219. doi:10.2307/1420127. JSTOR 1420127. PMID 14141474.
  14. ^ "William James Fellow Award 2002". Association for Psychological Science. Archived from the original on 30 January 2022. Retrieved 30 January 2022.
  15. ^ a b c d e Treisman, Anne; Gelade, Garry (1980). "A Feature-Integration Theory of Attention" (PDF). Cognitive Psychology. 12 (1): 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. PMID 7351125. S2CID 353246. Archived from the original (PDF) on 5 September 2008.
  16. ^ a b c d Goldstein, Bruce (2010). Sensation and Perception (8th ed.). Wadsworth: Cengage Learning.
  17. ^ Treisman, Anne (1977). "Focused attention in the perception and retrieval of multidimensional stimuli". Perception and Psychophysics. 22: 1–11. doi:10.3758/bf03206074.
  18. ^ Treisman, Anne; Schmidt, Hilary (1982). "Illusory Conjunctions in the Perception of Objects". Cognitive Psychology. 14 (1): 107–141. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8. PMID 7053925. S2CID 11201516.
  19. ^ a b Treisman, Anne (1996). "The binding problem". Cognitive Neuroscience. 6 (2): 171–178. doi:10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80070-5. PMID 8725958. S2CID 8643357.
  20. ^ Barlow, H. B. (1972). "Single units and cognition: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology". Perception. 1 (4): 371–394. doi:10.1068/p010371. PMID 4377168. S2CID 17487970.
  21. ^ Olshausen, B. A.; Anderson, C. H.; Van Essen, D. C. (1993). "A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information". Journal of Neuroscience. 13 (11): 4700–4719. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.13-11-04700.1993. PMC 6576339. PMID 8229193.
  22. ^ Milner, P. (1974). "A model for visual shape recognition". Psychological Review. 15 (6): 521–535. doi:10.1037/h0037149. PMID 4445414.
  23. ^ Kahneman, Daniel; Treisman, Deborah (2020). "Anne Marie Treisman. 27 February 1935 – 9 February 2018". Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society. 68: 407–430. doi:10.1098/rsbm.2019.0035.
  24. ^ "APS Member History". search.amphilsoc.org. Archived from the original on 6 February 2022. Retrieved 27 May 2021.
  25. ^ "2009– Anne Treisman". grawemeyer.org. Archived from the original on 5 November 2013.
  26. ^ Dunn, Sydni. "Anne Treisman". National Science & Technology Medals Foundation. Archived from the original on 14 August 2020. Retrieved 3 April 2018.
  27. ^ Kahneman, Daniel (2002). "Autobiography". nobelprize.org. Archived from the original on 30 May 2013. Retrieved 14 October 2014.
  28. ^ Finn, Robin (28 January 2003). "PUBLIC LIVES; A Bookworm as a Child, Now the Talk of the Town". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Archived from the original on 8 January 2023. Retrieved 29 August 2022.
  29. ^ Carr, David; Kirkpatrick, David D. (21 October 2002). "The Gatekeeper For Literature Is Changing At New Yorker". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Archived from the original on 8 January 2023. Retrieved 29 August 2022.
  30. ^ Smith, Harrison (14 February 2018). "Anne Treisman, pathbreaking psychologist who developed a theory of perception, dies at 82". The Washington Post. Archived from the original on 2 July 2018. Retrieved 6 January 2022.
  31. ^ "Anne Marie Treisman". Princeton University Dean of the Faculty. 2018. Archived from the original on 16 December 2016. Retrieved 12 February 2018.
  32. ^ "Anne Treisman, 1935–2018". Association for Psychological Science. Archived from the original on 13 February 2018. Retrieved 12 February 2018.
  33. ^ "Anne Treisman 1935–2018". Princeton University Kahneman-Treisman Center for Behavioral Science & Public Policy. 2018. Archived from the original on 10 November 2019. Retrieved 14 February 2018.

Further reading

[edit]
[edit]