Jump to content

Shooting bias: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
First paragraph is not neutral to police work culture, deeming it "highly racist"
Citation bot (talk | contribs)
Added url. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by Grimes2 | #UCB_webform 450/753
 
(115 intermediate revisions by 63 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Form of implicit racial bias}}
{{Globalize|date=March 2019}}
{{Globalize|date=March 2019}}


The term '''''shooting bias''''', also known as "'''shooter bias'''", is a form of [[implicit bias|implicit]] racial [[bias]] which refers to the supposed tendency among the police to shoot black civilians rather than white civilians, even when they are unarmed.
The term '''''shooting bias''''', also known as "'''shooter bias'''", is a proposed form of [[implicit bias|implicit]] racial [[bias]] which refers to the apparent tendency among the police to shoot black civilians more often than white civilians, even when they are unarmed.<ref name=":6">{{Cite journal|last1=Feingold|first1=Jonathan|last2=Lorang|first2=Karen|date=2012-01-01|title=Defusing Implicit Bias|url=https://scholarship.law.bu.edu/faculty_scholarship/827/|journal=UCLA Law Review Discourse|volume=59|pages=212}}</ref> In countries where white people aren't the majority, shooting bias may still apply, with different minority groups facing discrimination.


The probability of being shot by the police depends on factors such as ethnicity, location, the income of the neighborhood and whether or not the person is carrying a weapon<ref name=":2" /> as well as the emotions shown by the victim.<ref name=":4">{{Cite journal|last=Kubota|first=Jennifer T.|last2=Ito|first2=Tiffany A.|title=The role of expression and race in weapons identification|journal=Emotion|language=en|volume=14|issue=6|pages=1115–1124|doi=10.1037/a0038214|pmc=4235656|pmid=25401289|year=2014}}</ref>
The concept proposes that the probability of being shot by the police depends on ethnicity in addition to the other known factors like location, the income of the neighborhood and whether or not the person is carrying a weapon<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal|last=Ross|first=Cody T.|date=2015-11-05|title=A Multi-Level Bayesian Analysis of Racial Bias in Police Shootings at the County-Level in the United States, 2011–2014|journal=PLOS ONE|volume=10|issue=11|pages=e0141854|bibcode=2015PLoSO..1041854R|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0141854|pmc=4634878|pmid=26540108|doi-access=free}}</ref> as well as the emotions shown by the victim.<ref name=":4">{{Cite journal|last1=Kubota|first1=Jennifer T.|last2=Ito|first2=Tiffany A.|title=The role of expression and race in weapons identification|journal=Emotion|language=en|volume=14|issue=6|pages=1115–1124|doi=10.1037/a0038214|pmc=4235656|pmid=25401289|year=2014}}</ref> Shooting bias is not limited to one race, as studies have shown that both black and white individuals demonstrated almost equivalent levels of shooting bias.<ref>{{Cite SSRN |last=Benforado|first=Adam|date=2010-10-29|title=Quick on the Draw: Implicit Bias and the Second Amendment |language=en |ssrn=1701089}}</ref>

== Key Questions in Literature ==

=== Benchmark ===
Violent crime is committed by a very small number of individuals. These individuals tend to come from very poor disadvantaged communities. Since minorities are overrepresented in these communities, minorities are also overrepresented in violent crime. Thus, any statistical analysis of police bias must take this into account. Using population, police-citizen interactions, or total arrests as a benchmark, we observe that black citizens appear more likely than white citizens to be fatally shot by police officers. Using violent crime arrests or weapons offense arrests, we observe that black citizens appear less likely to be fatally shot by police officers.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Tregle|first1=Brandon|last2=Nix|first2=Justin|last3=Alpert|first3=Geoffrey P.|date=2018-12-07|title=Disparity does not mean bias: making sense of observed racial disparities in fatal officer-involved shootings with multiple benchmarks|journal=Journal of Crime and Justice|volume=42|issue=1|pages=18–31|doi=10.1080/0735648x.2018.1547269|s2cid=150005614|issn=0735-648X}}</ref>

=== Resisting Arrest ===
Additionally, data on the percent of individuals who resist arrest is also needed to draw conclusions about police bias. That data was available for Texas and California. Researchers were able to analyze this data to determine that there was shooting bias in California but not in Texas.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Shjarback|first1=John A.|last2=Nix|first2=Justin|date=January 2020|title=Considering violence against police by citizen race/ethnicity to contextualize representation in officer-involved shootings|journal=Journal of Criminal Justice|volume=66|pages=101653|doi=10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2019.101653|issn=0047-2352|doi-access=}}</ref>


== Number of deaths ==
== Number of deaths ==


=== Problems with police reporting practices ===
=== Problems with police reporting practices ===
Police data could be biased due to police reporting practices.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Klinger|first=David A.|date=2011-12-13|title=On the Problems and Promise of Research on Lethal Police Violence|journal=Homicide Studies|language=en|volume=16|issue=1|pages=78–96|doi=10.1177/1088767911430861}}</ref> Departments can voluntarily include [[justifiable homicide]]s in the crime statistics of the [[Uniform Crime Reports|FBI's Uniform Crime Reports]], which means that a lot of departments don't provide data at all. Some cities haven't reported their data in years.<ref>{{Cite news|url=http://www.stltoday.com/news/local/crime-and-courts/nobodycounts-police-killings-in-the-u-s/article_8ec76c48-4414-5861-9183-134c75a4be10.html|title=Nobody counts police killings in the U.S.|last=Byers|first=Christine|last2=Moskop |first2=Walker|work=stltoday.com|access-date=2017-07-30|language=en|date=2014-11-09}}</ref> This means that the official data doesn't accurately reflect the number of civilians that are shot by the police.
Departments can voluntarily include [[justifiable homicide]]s in the crime statistics of the [[Uniform Crime Reports|FBI's Uniform Crime Reports]], which means that a lot of departments don't provide data at all. Some cities haven't reported their data in years.<ref>{{Cite news|url=http://www.stltoday.com/news/local/crime-and-courts/nobodycounts-police-killings-in-the-u-s/article_8ec76c48-4414-5861-9183-134c75a4be10.html|title=Nobody counts police killings in the U.S.|last1=Byers|first1=Christine|last2=Moskop |first2=Walker|work=stltoday.com|access-date=2017-07-30|language=en|date=2014-11-09}}</ref> This means that the official data doesn't accurately reflect the number of civilians that are shot by the police.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Klinger|first=David A.|date=2011-12-13|title=On the Problems and Promise of Research on Lethal Police Violence|journal=Homicide Studies|language=en|volume=16|issue=1|pages=78–96|doi=10.1177/1088767911430861|s2cid=145366158}}</ref>


Newspapers like ''[[The Guardian]]'' and ''[[The Washington Post]]'' have started gathering a database of [[Lists of killings by law enforcement officers|fatal police shootings]],<ref name=":1">{{Cite web|url=https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/ng-interactive/2015/jun/01/the-counted-police-killings-us-database|title=The Counted: people killed by police in the United States – interactive|last=Swaine|first=Jon|last2=Laughland|first2=Oliver|website=the Guardian|access-date=2017-07-30|last3=Lartey|first3=Jamiles|last4=Davis|first4=Kenan|last5=Harris|first5=Rich|last6=Popovich|first6=Nadja|last7=Powell|first7=Kenton|last8=team|first8=Guardian US interactive}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/graphics/national/police-shootings-2017/|title=Police shootings 2017 database|website=Washington Post|access-date=2017-07-30}}</ref> revealing that in 2015 twice as many civilians had been fatally shot than the FBI's data suggested.<ref>{{Cite news |url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/national/fatal-police-shootings-in-2015-approaching-400-nationwide/2015/05/30/d322256a-058e-11e5-a428-c984eb077d4e_story.html |title=Fatal police shootings in 2015 approaching 400 nationwide |last=Kindy |first=Kimberly |date=2015-05-30 |work=Washington Post |access-date=2019-12-08 |last2=Tate |first2=Julie |language=en-US |last3=Jenkins |first3=Jennifer |last4=Rich |first4=Steven |last5=Alexander |first5=Keith L. |last6=Lowery |first6=Wesley |display-authors=1}}</ref> An FBI working group has started working on a proposal for making the reports more accurate, but they would still rely on voluntary data and therefore wouldn't fix the main reporting issue.<ref>{{Cite news |url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/national/fbi-to-sharply-expand-system-for-tracking-fatal-police-shootings/2015/12/08/a60fbc16-9dd4-11e5-bce4-708fe33e3288_story.html |title=FBI to sharply expand system for tracking fatal police shootings |last=Kindy |first=Kimberly |date=2015-12-08 |work=Washington Post |access-date=2019-12-08 |language=en-US}}</ref>
Newspapers like ''[[The Guardian]]'' and ''[[The Washington Post]]'' have started compiling databases of [[Lists of killings by law enforcement officers|fatal police shootings]],<ref name=":1">{{Cite web|url=https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/ng-interactive/2015/jun/01/the-counted-police-killings-us-database|title=The Counted: people killed by police in the United States – interactive|last1=Swaine|first1=Jon|last2=Laughland|first2=Oliver|website=the Guardian|access-date=2017-07-30|last3=Lartey|first3=Jamiles|last4=Davis|first4=Kenan|last5=Harris|first5=Rich|last6=Popovich|first6=Nadja|last7=Powell|first7=Kenton|last8=team|first8=Guardian US interactive}}</ref><ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/graphics/national/police-shootings-2017/|title=Police shootings 2017 database|newspaper=Washington Post|access-date=2017-07-30}}</ref> revealing that in 2015 twice as many civilians had been fatally shot as the FBI's data suggested.<ref>{{Cite news |url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/national/fatal-police-shootings-in-2015-approaching-400-nationwide/2015/05/30/d322256a-058e-11e5-a428-c984eb077d4e_story.html |title=Fatal police shootings in 2015 approaching 400 nationwide |last1=Kindy |first1=Kimberly |date=2015-05-30 |newspaper=Washington Post |access-date=2019-12-08 |last2=Tate |first2=Julie |language=en-US |last3=Jenkins |first3=Jennifer |last4=Rich |first4=Steven |last5=Alexander |first5=Keith L. |last6=Lowery |first6=Wesley |display-authors=1}}</ref> An FBI working group has started working on a proposal for making the reports more accurate, but they would still rely on voluntary data and therefore wouldn't fix the main reporting issue.<ref>{{Cite news |url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/national/fbi-to-sharply-expand-system-for-tracking-fatal-police-shootings/2015/12/08/a60fbc16-9dd4-11e5-bce4-708fe33e3288_story.html |title=FBI to sharply expand system for tracking fatal police shootings |last=Kindy |first=Kimberly |date=2015-12-08 |newspaper=Washington Post |access-date=2019-12-08 |language=en-US}}</ref>


=== Alternative database by ''The Guardian'' ===
=== Alternative database by ''The Guardian'' ===
The database developed by ''[[The Guardian]]'' is currently the largest database on fatal shootings available.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Hehman|first=Eric|last2=Flake|first2=Jessica K.|last3=Calanchini|first3=Jimmy|date=2017-07-27|title=Disproportionate Use of Lethal Force in Policing Is Associated With Regional Racial Biases of Residents|journal=Social Psychological and Personality Science|volume=9|issue=4|language=en|pages=393–401|doi=10.1177/1948550617711229|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/75bdce1149945091a6465c0f276f758e6c830d98}}</ref> They gather data through police reports, monitoring of regional news, fact-checked witness statements and other crowdsourced police fatality databases.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/ng-interactive/2015/jun/01/about-the-counted|title=About The Counted: why and how the Guardian is counting US police killings|website=the Guardian|access-date=2017-07-30}}</ref>
The database developed by ''[[The Guardian]]'' is currently the largest database on fatal shootings available.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Hehman|first1=Eric|last2=Flake|first2=Jessica K.|last3=Calanchini|first3=Jimmy|date=2017-07-27|title=Disproportionate Use of Lethal Force in Policing Is Associated With Regional Racial Biases of Residents|journal=Social Psychological and Personality Science|volume=9|issue=4|language=en|pages=393–401|doi=10.1177/1948550617711229|s2cid=51773001|url=https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7wz2f23w }}</ref> They gather data through police reports, monitoring of regional news, fact-checked witness statements and other crowdsourced police fatality databases.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/ng-interactive/2015/jun/01/about-the-counted|title=About The Counted: why and how the Guardian is counting US police killings|website=the Guardian|access-date=2017-07-30}}</ref>


=== Number of deaths ===
=== Number of deaths ===
In 2016, ''The Guardian'' counted 1093 people<ref name=":1" /> who were killed by the police in the United States. Out of these 574 were white and 266 were black. 95 of the white victims were unarmed, 42 of the black victims were unarmed.
In 2016, ''The Guardian'' counted 1093 people<ref name=":1" /> who were killed by the police in the United States. Out of these, 574 were white and 266 were black. 95 of the white victims were unarmed, while 42 of the black victims were unarmed.


More white than black people are shot. It is important to distinguish to differentiate between the number of deaths of an ethnic group and the likelihood of being shot by police. The likelihood of being shot as a black rather than a white person is higher, whether the victim is armed or not.<ref name=":2" />
More white than black people are shot. It is important to distinguish to differentiate between the number of deaths of an ethnic group and the likelihood of being shot by police. The likelihood of being shot as a black rather than a white person is higher, whether the victim is armed or not.<ref name=":2" />


== Likelihood of being shot: Black vs. White ==
== Likelihood of being shot with respect to race ==
===Observational studies===
A study carried out at the [[University of California]] found "evidence of a significant bias in the killing of unarmed black Americans compared to unarmed white Americans".<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal|last=Ross|first=Cody T.|date=2015-11-05|title=A Multi-Level Bayesian Analysis of Racial Bias in Police Shootings at the County-Level in the United States, 2011–2014|journal=PLoS ONE|volume=10|issue=11|pages=e0141854|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0141854|pmc=4634878|pmid=26540108|bibcode=2015PLoSO..1041854R}}</ref> In this study, the probability of being shot by the police as a black, unarmed person versus as a white, unarmed person was 3.49 times higher. Unarmed Hispanics' likelihood to be shot was 1.67 times higher than for unarmed Whites.
A study carried out at the University of California found "evidence of a significant bias in the killing of unarmed black Americans compared to unarmed white Americans".<ref name=":2"/> In this study, the probability of being shot by the police as a black, unarmed person versus as a white, unarmed person was 3.49 times higher. Unarmed Hispanics' likelihood to be shot was 1.67 times higher than for unarmed Whites. Black people have been 28% of those killed by police since 2013 despite being only 13% of the population.<ref>https://mappingpoliceviolence.org/ {{Bare URL inline|date=August 2024}}</ref>


On the other hand a 2016–2018 study by the [[National Bureau of Economic Research]] ({{abbr|NBER|National Bureau of Economic Research}}) of twenty-first century data recorded by 12 police departments across the U.S., which took into account various factors such as type of confrontation, whether the suspect was armed or not, and whether or not they drew a gun, found that while overall "blacks are 21 percent more likely than whites to be involved in an interaction with police in which at least a weapon is drawn" and that in the raw data from [[Stop-and-frisk in New York City|New York City's Stop and Frisk program]] "[[Race and ethnicity in the United States Census|blacks and Hispanics]] are more than fifty percent more likely to have an interaction with police which involves any use of force" after "<nowiki>[p]</nowiki>artitioning the data in myriad ways, we find no evidence of racial discrimination in officer-involved shootings."{{refn|name=fryer1|{{cite report|surname=Fryer|given=Roland Gerhard|author-link=Roland G. Fryer Jr.|title=An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force|series=NBER Working Papers|id=W22399|date=July 2016|edition=Revised January 2018|institution=[[National Bureau of Economic Research]]|url=https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/w22399/w22399.pdf|doi=10.3386/w22399|doi-access=free|oclc=956328193|s2cid=158634577|docket=[[JEL classification codes|{{abbr|JEL|Journal of Economic Literature}}]] № [[JEL classification codes#J. Labor and Demographic Economics|{{abbr|J01|Labor and Demographic Economics—Labor Economics: General}}]], [[JEL classification codes#K. Law and Economics|{{abbr|K0|Law and Economics—General}}]]|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201031080410/https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/w22399/w22399.pdf|archive-date=2020-10-31|url-status=live}}<ref name="fryer1_pub">{{cite journal|surname=Fryer|given=Roland Gerhard|author-link=Roland G. Fryer Jr.|title=An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force|journal=[[Journal of Political Economy]]|date=June 2019|volume=127|number=3|pages=1210–1261|publisher=[[University of Chicago]]|doi=10.1086/701423|s2cid=158634577|url=https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/full/10.1086/701423|oclc=8118094562|issn=0022-3808}}</ref>}} The study did find bias against blacks and Hispanics in non-lethal and less-extreme lethal violence, stating that "as the intensity of force increases (e.g. handcuffing civilians without arrest, drawing or pointing a weapon, or using pepper spray or a baton), the probability that any civilian is subjected to such treatment is small, but the racial difference remains surprisingly constant", and noted that "<nowiki>[u]</nowiki>ntil recently, data on officer-involved shootings were extremely rare and contained little information on the details surrounding an incident".<ref name="fryer1"/>
The number vary greatly depending on the county, sometimes reaching a probability of 20 to 1 or more for unarmed blacks to be shot. There are several maps that showcase the distribution of fatal shootings across the country.<ref>{{Cite web | url=https://mappingpoliceviolence.org | title=Mapping Police Violence}}</ref>


After the NBER study was published in the [[Peer review|peer-reviewed]] ''[[Journal of Political Economy]]'', a comment on it by [[Steven Durlauf]] and ([[List of Nobel Memorial Prize laureates in Economics|Nobel Memorial Prize in Economics recipient]]) [[James Heckman]] of the [[Harris School of Public Policy Studies]] at the [[University of Chicago]] stated, "<nowiki>[i]</nowiki>n our judgment, this paper does ''not'' establish credible evidence on the presence or absence of discrimination against African Americans in police shootings."<ref name="fryer1_com">{{cite journal|surname=Durlauf|given=Steven Neil|author-link=Steven Durlauf|surname2=Heckman|given2=James Joseph|author-link2=James Heckman|title=An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force: A Comment|journal=[[Journal of Political Economy]]|date=2020-07-21|volume=128|number=10|pages=3998–4002|publisher=[[University of Chicago]]|url=https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/full/10.1086/710976|doi=10.1086/710976|s2cid=222811199|oclc=8672021465|issn=0022-3808|archive-url=https://archive.today/20201108063306/https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/full/10.1086/710976|archive-date=2020-11-08}}</ref> The NBER study's author, [[Roland G. Fryer Jr.]], responded by saying Durlauf and Heckman erroneously claim that his sample is "based on stops". Further, he states that the "vast majority of the data...is gleaned from 911 calls for service in which a civilian requests police presence."<ref name="fryer1_re">{{cite journal|surname=Fryer|given=Roland Gerhard|author-link=Roland G. Fryer Jr.|title=An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force: A Response|journal=[[Journal of Political Economy]]|date=2020-07-21|volume=128|number=10|pages=4003–4008|publisher=[[University of Chicago]]|url=https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/full/10.1086/710977|doi=10.1086/710977|s2cid=222813143|oclc=8672034484|issn=0022-3808|archive-url=https://archive.today/20201108065748/https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/full/10.1086/710977|archive-date=2020-11-08}}</ref>
Crime rate was not associated with likelihood of being shot in this study.
A 2018 study in the journal ''[[Social Psychological and Personality Science]]'' sought to "argue for more reasonable benchmarks to compare fatal shooting rates across racial groups" than "comparing the group's raw shooting numbers against each group's overall representation in the population". On the premise that "the more [racial] group members are involved in criminal activity, the more exposure they have to situations in which police shootings would be likely to occur" the authors analyzed [[#Alternative database by The Guardian|''The Guardian''{{'s}} database]] through their own measurements of each race's involvement in criminal activity, calculated from the [[Federal Bureau of Investigation]]'s [[National Incident-Based Reporting System#Similarities and differences between SRS and NIBRS|Summary Reporting System (SRS) and National Incident-Based Reporting System (NIBRS)]], the [[National Crime Victimization Survey]] (NCVS), and the [[Centers for Disease Control and Prevention]]'s [https://wonder.cdc.gov WONDER database]. Once the data were weighted by these measurements, the authors were able to conclude that, for example, although "<nowiki>[o]</nowiki>dds were 3.7 times higher for Blacks relative to Whites to be fatally shot ''given'' population proportions" while holding or reaching for a harmless object, this was appropriate given higher rates of average criminal involvement among blacks. They propose, "<nowiki>[i]</nowiki>f officers are more likely to misidentify a harmless object in the hands of a Black citizen due to stereotypes, the cause of officers holding those stereotypes may rest with (the very small percentage of) those who are more likely to engage in criminal activity."<ref name="jcesario">{{cite journal |last1=Cesario |first1=Joseph |last2=Johnson |first2=David |last3=Terrill |first3=William |title=Is There Evidence of Racial Disparity in Police Use of Deadly Force? Analyses of Officer-Involved Fatal Shootings in 2015–2016. |journal=[[Social Psychological and Personality Science]] |date=July 2019 |volume=10 |issue=5 |pages=586–595 |doi=10.1177/1948550618775108 |s2cid=54057840 |url=https://osf.io/2btyn/ |access-date=November 8, 2020 |archive-date=November 8, 2020 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201108181622/https://files.osf.io/v1/resources/2btyn/providers/osfstorage/5b263d7bc9cb1b000f4ac380?action=download&direct=&version=1 |url-status=dead }}</ref>


A more recent study was conducted by [[Michigan State University]] and the [[University of Maryland, Baltimore|University of Maryland]], compiling a list of more than 900 fatal U.S. police shootings in 2015 using crowdsourced databases from The [[The Washington Post|Washington Post]] and The [[The Guardian|Guardian]].[https://www.pnas.org/content/116/32/15877] Then, they asked police departments for information about the race of the officers responsible for the shootings. They found black police were more likely to kill black civilians than white civilians. However, the same held true for white and Hispanic officers: Each group of police was likelier to shoot civilians of their own race. That’s likely true, the researchers say, because police tend to be drawn from the communities they work in and are thus [[Probability|more likely]] to have deadly encounters with civilians of the same race. They concluded there were no antiblack or anti-Hispanic disparities across police shootings.[https://www.npr.org/2019/07/26/745731839/new-study-says-white-police-officers-are-not-more-likely-to-shoot-minority-suspe]
A more recent study was conducted by [[Michigan State University]] and the [[University of Maryland, Baltimore|University of Maryland]], compiling a list of more than 900 fatal U.S. police shootings in 2015 using crowdsourced databases from The [[The Washington Post|Washington Post]] and The [[The Guardian|Guardian]].[https://www.pnas.org/content/116/32/15877] Then, they asked police departments for information about the race of the officers responsible for the shootings. They found black police were more likely to kill black civilians than white civilians. However, the same held true for white and Hispanic officers: Each group of police was more likely to shoot civilians of their own race. Researchers claimed this is true because police tend to be drawn from the communities they work in and are thus [[Probability|more likely]] to have deadly encounters with civilians of the same race. They conclude that "increasing diversity among officers by itself is unlikely to reduce racial disparity in police shootings".

===Experimental studies===
Several experimental studies by social psychologists, in which college students are tested playing computer game simulations, have uncovered racial bias in their decisions to shoot.<ref name="agreenwald">{{cite journal |last1=Greenwald |first1=Anthony, G. |last2=Oakes |first2=Mark, A. |last3=Hoffman |first3=Hunter, G. |title=Targets of discrimination: Effects of race on responses to weapons holders |journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology |date=11 April 2003 |volume=39 |issue=4 |pages=399–405 |doi=10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00020-9 |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0022103103000209}}</ref><ref name="jcorrell">{{cite journal |last1=Correll |first1=Joshua |last2=Wittenbrink |first2=Bernd |last3=Judd |first3=Charles, M. |last4=Park |first4=Bernadette |title=The Police Officer's Dilemma: Using Ethnicity to Disambiguate Potentially Threatening Individuals |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=2002 |volume=83 |issue=6 |pages=1314–1329 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314|pmid=12500813 }}</ref><ref name="bpayne">{{cite journal |last1=Payne |first1=B. Keith |title=Prejudice and Perception: The Role of Automatic and Controlled Processes in Misperceiving a Weapon |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=August 2001 |volume=81 |issue=2 |pages=181–192 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181 |pmid= 11519925|url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11519925/}}</ref><ref name="jcorrell2">{{cite journal |last1=Correll |first1=Joshua |last2=Park |first2=Bernadette |last3=Judd |first3=Charles, M. |last4=Wittenbrink |first4=Bernd |title=The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot |journal=European Journal of Social Psychology |date=3 July 2007 |volume=37 |issue=6 |pages=1102–1117 |doi=10.1002/ejsp.450 |url=http://wittenbrink.org/research/pdf/cpjw07.pdf}}</ref>
A computer game simulation experiment in 2010 that tested college students, community members, and police officers found that racial bias was moderated by "prototypicality", i.e. they were more likely to shoot if a person had stronger features generally associated with black people (broader nose, large lips, etc.) regardless of the suspects actual racial category/skin colour, to the extent that white suspects with strong "black features" suffered more negative bias than black suspects with strong "white features". Furthermore they found greater racial bias among college students and community members than police officers (who are trained to overcome racial bias), and on average could not find racial bias among the police officers tested (although there was prototypicality bias).<ref name="jcorrell3">{{cite journal |last1=Ma |first1=Debbie, S. |last2=Correll |first2=Joshua |title=Target prototypicality moderates racial bias in the decision to shoot |journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology |date=March 2011 |volume=47 |issue=2 |pages=391–396 |doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2010.11.002 |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0022103110002519 |access-date=12 November 2010}}</ref>


== Factors that influence decision to shoot ==
== Factors that influence decision to shoot ==
Line 35: Line 51:
* Implicit racial bias, prejudice, and stereotypes<ref name=":5" />
* Implicit racial bias, prejudice, and stereotypes<ref name=":5" />
* Status armed/unarmed<ref name=":2" />
* Status armed/unarmed<ref name=":2" />
* Whether or not the police officer was threatened or attacked<ref name="fryer1"/>
* Gender (men are more likely to be shot)<ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.propublica.org/article/deadly-force-in-black-and-white|title=Deadly Force, in Black and White|last=Sagara|first=Ryan Gabrielson, Ryann Grochowski Jones, Eric|date=2014-10-10|work=ProPublica|access-date=2017-07-30|language=en}}</ref>
* Gender (men are more likely to be shot)<ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.propublica.org/article/deadly-force-in-black-and-white|title=Deadly Force, in Black and White|last=Sagara|first=Ryan Gabrielson, Ryann Grochowski Jones, Eric|date=2014-10-10|work=ProPublica|access-date=2017-07-30|language=en}}</ref>
* County<ref name=":2" />
* County<ref name=":2" />
* Income and status of the neighborhood<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Smith|first=Douglas A.|date=1986|title=The Neighborhood Context of Police Behavior|journal=Crime and Justice|volume=8|pages=313–341|doi=10.1086/449126}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Terrill|first=William|last2=Reisig|first2=Michael D.|date=2016-08-18|title=Neighborhood Context and Police Use of Force|journal=Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency|language=en|volume=40|issue=3|pages=291–321|doi=10.1177/0022427803253800|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/a780f37f5eaaf9d72c71907d6ea29f17f8774520}}</ref>
* Income and status of the neighbourhood<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Smith|first=Douglas A.|date=1986|title=The Neighborhood Context of Police Behavior|journal=Crime and Justice|volume=8|pages=313–341|doi=10.1086/449126|s2cid=144977273}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Terrill|first1=William|last2=Reisig|first2=Michael D.|date=2016-08-18|title=Neighborhood Context and Police Use of Force|journal=Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency|language=en|volume=40|issue=3|pages=291–321|doi=10.1177/0022427803253800|s2cid=43353487}}</ref>
* Previous experiences of the police officers with the ethnic group<ref name=":0" />
* Previous experiences of the police officers with the ethnic group<ref name=":0" />
* Previous experiences of the victim with the police<ref name=":0" />
* Previous experiences of the victim with the police<ref name=":0" />
* Emotional facial expression of the civilian<ref name=":4" />
* Emotional facial expression of the civilian<ref name=":4" />
* Fatigue of the police officer<ref name=":3">{{Cite journal|last=Ma|first=Debbie S.|last2=Correll|first2=Joshua|last3=Wittenbrink|first3=Bernd|last4=Bar-Anan|first4=Yoav|last5=Sriram|first5=N.|last6=Nosek|first6=Brian A.|date=2013-11-01|title=When Fatigue Turns Deadly: The Association Between Fatigue and Racial Bias in the Decision to Shoot|journal=Basic and Applied Social Psychology|volume=35|issue=6|pages=515–524|doi=10.1080/01973533.2013.840630|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/c177fdb9b81f8e604c395b849b447395ca001b0f}}</ref>
* Fatigue of the police officer<ref name=":3">{{Cite journal|last1=Ma|first1=Debbie S.|last2=Correll|first2=Joshua|last3=Wittenbrink|first3=Bernd|last4=Bar-Anan|first4=Yoav|last5=Sriram|first5=N.|last6=Nosek|first6=Brian A.|date=2013-11-01|title=When Fatigue Turns Deadly: The Association Between Fatigue and Racial Bias in the Decision to Shoot|journal=Basic and Applied Social Psychology|volume=35|issue=6|pages=515–524|doi=10.1080/01973533.2013.840630|s2cid=29835235}}</ref>


==== Implicit racial bias ====
==== Implicit racial bias ====
An implicit [[Bias|racial bias]] refers to unintentional judgments a person makes of a group (e.g. good/bad) of a certain ethnicity. So a person who shows implicit racial bias might not be aware of it. Police officers have been found to show a racial bias against black people in the decision to shoot.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Correll|first=Joshua|last2=Park|first2=Bernadette|last3=Judd|first3=Charles M.|last4=Wittenbrink|first4=Bernd|last5=Sadler|first5=Melody S.|last6=Keesee|first6=Tracie|title=Across the thin blue line: Police officers and racial bias in the decision to shoot|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|language=en|volume=92|issue=6|pages=1006–1023|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1006|pmid=17547485|year=2007|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/0ff0f0442048d1a372a5c7e51b5aa69fc3953a32}}</ref>
An implicit [[Bias|racial bias]] refers to unintentional judgments a person makes of a group (e.g. good/bad) of a certain ethnicity. Therefore, a person who shows implicit racial bias might not be aware of it. Police officers have been found to show a racial bias against black people in the decision to shoot,<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Correll|first1=Joshua|last2=Park|first2=Bernadette|last3=Judd|first3=Charles M.|last4=Wittenbrink|first4=Bernd|last5=Sadler|first5=Melody S.|last6=Keesee|first6=Tracie|title=Across the thin blue line: Police officers and racial bias in the decision to shoot|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|language=en|volume=92|issue=6|pages=1006–1023|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1006|pmid=17547485|year=2007|s2cid=17992608}}</ref> and implicit biases are directly correlated with shooting behavior.<ref name=":6" />


In one of the studies, researchers investigated how stereotypes affected police officers' decisions to shoot. They used a video game and exposed their participants to pictures of either Whites or Non-Whites who were armed or unarmed. During this video game, the participants were asked to choose between "shoot" or "don't shoot" as quickly as possible. The results were that the participants shot armed black people faster than armed white people and chose "don't shoot" faster for unarmed white than unarmed black persons.
In one of the studies, researchers investigated how stereotypes affected police officers' decisions to shoot. They used a video game and exposed their participants to pictures of either Whites or Non-Whites who were armed or unarmed. During this video game, the participants were asked to choose between "shoot" or "don't shoot" as quickly as possible. The results were that the participants shot armed black people faster than armed white people and chose "don't shoot" faster for unarmed white than unarmed black persons.


The authors explained those findings by the activation of stereotype thinking which lead white people to associate black people with danger. Because of this stereotype, the participants expected blacks to carry a gun and therefore were quicker to make the "shooting" decision.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal | doi=10.1002/ejsp.450 |title = The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot|journal = European Journal of Social Psychology|volume = 37|issue = 6|pages = 1102–1117|year = 2007|last1 = Correll|first1 = Joshua|last2 = Park|first2 = Bernadette|last3 = Judd|first3 = Charles M.|last4 = Wittenbrink|first4 = Bernd}}</ref> Other studies have found similar results.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Correll|first=Joshua|last2=Wittenbrink|first2=Bernd|last3=Crawford|first3=Matthew T.|last4=Sadler|first4=Melody S.|title=Stereotypic vision: How stereotypes disambiguate visual stimuli|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|language=en|volume=108|issue=2|pages=219–233|doi=10.1037/pspa0000015|pmid=25603373|year=2015|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Payne|first=B. Keith|title=Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|language=en|volume=81|issue=2|pages=181–192|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181|year=2001|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/5786f2dcefa2ed42ae6e642add36279809b53e28}}</ref> The time pressure to make a decision in decisions to shoot might magnify the effects of racial bias.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Payne|first=B. Keith|date=2006-12-01|title=Weapon Bias: Split-Second Decisions and Unintended Stereotyping|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|language=en|volume=15|issue=6|pages=287–291|doi=10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00454.x|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/9f6bb45d369afdf99ef315844d800b736047ddca}}</ref>
The authors explained those findings by the activation of stereotype thinking which lead white people to associate black people with danger. Because of this stereotype, the participants expected blacks to carry a gun and therefore were quicker to make the "shooting" decision.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal | doi=10.1002/ejsp.450 |title = The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot|journal = European Journal of Social Psychology|volume = 37|issue = 6|pages = 1102–1117|year = 2007|last1 = Correll|first1 = Joshua|last2 = Park|first2 = Bernadette|last3 = Judd|first3 = Charles M.|last4 = Wittenbrink|first4 = Bernd}}</ref> Other studies have found similar results.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Correll|first1=Joshua|last2=Wittenbrink|first2=Bernd|last3=Crawford|first3=Matthew T.|last4=Sadler|first4=Melody S.|title=Stereotypic vision: How stereotypes disambiguate visual stimuli|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|language=en|volume=108|issue=2|pages=219–233|doi=10.1037/pspa0000015|pmid=25603373|year=2015|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Payne|first=B. Keith|title=Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|language=en|volume=81|issue=2|pages=181–192|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181|year=2001|pmid=11519925|s2cid=15865607}}</ref> The time pressure to make a decision in decisions to shoot might magnify the effects of racial bias.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Payne|first=B. Keith|date=2006-12-01|title=Weapon Bias: Split-Second Decisions and Unintended Stereotyping|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|language=en|volume=15|issue=6|pages=287–291|doi=10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00454.x|s2cid=16595511|doi-access=free}}</ref> However, more recent research indicates that despite scoring moderately high on an Implicit Association Test, police officers are less likely to shoot black people in a video simulation<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=James|first1=Lois|last2=James|first2=Stephen M.|last3=Vila|first3=Bryan J.|date=2016-01-14|title=The Reverse Racism Effect|journal=Criminology & Public Policy|volume=15|issue=2|pages=457–479|doi=10.1111/1745-9133.12187|issn=1538-6473}}</ref>


==== Location and context ====
==== Location and context ====
The American police officers live and work in a society full of prejudices against minorities. In addition, police officers usually deal with high crime rates in minority neighborhoods. These experiences reinforce their existing prejudices by ignoring that most people with a non-white ethnic background don't become criminals. This leads to discrimination against minorities. Other factors that lead to discrimination by the police are institutionalized language barriers between police and some ethnic groups, experiences with disrespectful or hostile residents in certain minority neighborhoods and low punishment for police officers who misbehave towards minorities.
In the USA, police officers live and work in a society where prejudices against minorities are common. In addition, police officers usually deal with higher crime rates in minority neighborhoods. These experiences tend to reinforce existing prejudices, leading to discrimination against minorities. Other factors that lead to discrimination by police are language barriers between police and some ethnic groups, experiences with disrespectful or hostile residents in certain minority neighborhoods, and low punishment for police officers who misbehave towards minorities.


Due to these prejudices, minorities may develop hostile attitudes towards police and be more likely to behave disrespectfully or to insult officers. This may lead to a self-reinforcing cycle, where the biases of either group reinforce those of the other.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal | doi=10.1016/0047-2352(96)00015-3 |title = Racial discrimination in the criminal justice system: Findings and problems in the literature|journal = Journal of Criminal Justice|volume = 24|issue = 4|pages = 309–322|year = 1996|last1 = Weitzer|first1 = Ronald}}</ref>
Those prejudices by the police are of course being noticed by the groups of minorities. So in return, the groups believe that the officers have prejudices against them. This might lead to the findings that Non-Whites are more likely to behave disrespectfully or to insult officers.


===== Examples that showcase prejudices among police officers include:<ref name=":0" /> =====
To sum up, there may exist a mutual influence between the prejudices that police officers have against minorities and the prejudices that minorities have against officers.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal | doi=10.1016/0047-2352(96)00015-3 |title = Racial discrimination in the criminal justice system: Findings and problems in the literature|journal = Journal of Criminal Justice|volume = 24|issue = 4|pages = 309–322|year = 1996|last1 = Weitzer|first1 = Ronald}}</ref>

===== Examples that showcase prejudices among police officers<ref name=":0" /> =====
*Disruptive actions by the police like stopping and questioning citizens on the street are more common in minority neighborhoods
*Disruptive actions by the police like stopping and questioning citizens on the street are more common in minority neighborhoods
*More cases of Non-Whites are being dropped than of Whites. This is because the reasons for arresting them were too weak and unjustified in the first place.
*More cases of non-whites are dropped than those of whites. This may be because the reasons for arresting them were too weak or unjustified in the first place.
*American police officers shoot more easily Non-Whites than Whites


== Ways to reduce shooting bias ==
== Ways to reduce shooting bias ==
Possibly the biggest change could be made by [[police accountability|holding police officers accountable]] for their actions.<ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/posteverything/wp/2014/12/06/i-was-a-st-louis-cop-my-peers-were-racist-and-violent-and-theres-only-one-fix/|title=Being a cop showed me just how racist and violent the police are. There's only one fix.|newspaper=Washington Post|access-date=2017-07-30|last=Hudson |first=Redditt|date=2014-12-06}}</ref> Internal investigations have been criticized for bias in favor of officers, leading to a lack of punishment. The work culture within some police departments has been criticized as racist, in light of many recent scandals.<ref>{{Cite news|url=http://www.nydailynews.com/news/national/michigan-police-suspend-behind-racist-footage-black-man-article-1.1527387|title=Michigan police suspend cop behind 'racist' footage of black man|work=NY Daily News|access-date=2017-07-30|language=en}}</ref><ref>{{Cite news|url=http://www.nydailynews.com/news/crime/ohio-cops-suspended-racist-texts-leaked-nonprofit-article-1.2033713|title=Ohio cops suspended for exchanging racist text messages|work=NY Daily News|access-date=2017-07-30|language=en}}</ref><ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.bet.com/article/l526i4/two-black-cops-write-statements-about-racism-in-lapd|title=Two Black Cops Write Statements About Racism in LAPD|work=BET.com|access-date=2017-07-30}}</ref>
Knowing the factors that influence police officers decisions to shoot there are a few possible solutions for reducing shooting bias. The main factor is implicit racial bias, which in turn is exuberated by certain factors that could be addressed.

Knowing the factors that influence police officers decisions to shoot there are a few possible solutions for reducing shooting bias. The main factor is implicit racial bias, which in turn becomes augmented or intensified by certain factors that could be addressed.


Diversity in police departments might not reduce shooting bias,<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Smith|first=Brad W.|date=2003-05-01|title=The Impact of Police Officer Diversity on Police-Caused Homicides|journal=Policy Studies Journal|language=en|volume=31|issue=2|pages=147–162|doi=10.1111/1541-0072.t01-1-00009}}</ref> but reducing fatigue might lower the impact of racial bias on the decision to shoot.<ref name=":3" /> Changing the training of police officers so as to not showcase black armed targets more often than white ones could help reduce racial bias.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Sim|first=Jessica J.|last2=Correll|first2=Joshua|last3=Sadler|first3=Melody S.|date=2013-03-01|title=Understanding Police and Expert Performance: When Training Attenuates (vs. Exacerbates) Stereotypic Bias in the Decision to Shoot|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin|language=en|volume=39|issue=3|pages=291–304|doi=10.1177/0146167212473157|pmid=23401478}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Plant|first=E. Ashby|last2=Peruche|first2=B. Michelle|last3=Butz|first3=David A.|title=Eliminating automatic racial bias: Making race non-diagnostic for responses to criminal suspects|journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology|volume=41|issue=2|pages=141–156|doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2004.07.004|year=2005}}</ref>
Diversity in police departments might not reduce shooting bias,<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Smith|first=Brad W.|date=2003-05-01|title=The Impact of Police Officer Diversity on Police-Caused Homicides|journal=Policy Studies Journal|language=en|volume=31|issue=2|pages=147–162|doi=10.1111/1541-0072.t01-1-00009|doi-access=free}}</ref> but reducing fatigue might lower the impact of racial bias on the decision to shoot.<ref name=":3" /> Changing the training of police officers so as to not showcase black armed targets more often than white ones could help reduce racial bias.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Sim|first1=Jessica J.|last2=Correll|first2=Joshua|last3=Sadler|first3=Melody S.|date=2013-03-01|title=Understanding Police and Expert Performance: When Training Attenuates (vs. Exacerbates) Stereotypic Bias in the Decision to Shoot|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin|language=en|volume=39|issue=3|pages=291–304|doi=10.1177/0146167212473157|pmid=23401478|s2cid=34947053}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Plant|first1=E. Ashby|last2=Peruche|first2=B. Michelle|last3=Butz|first3=David A.|title=Eliminating automatic racial bias: Making race non-diagnostic for responses to criminal suspects|journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology|volume=41|issue=2|pages=141–156|doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2004.07.004|year=2005}}</ref>


Training police officers in making decisions under stress as well as assigning officers to certain locations so they can become familiar with its residents could reduce the numbers of civilians killed, says Richmond's police chief, Chris Magnus.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.vox.com/2014/9/18/6111457/example-good-police-relations-cops-killing-outreach-culture-training|title=How to not shoot civilians: 9 community policing tips from a chief who got it right|last=Lind|first=Dara|date=2014-09-18|website=Vox|access-date=2017-07-30}}</ref>
Training police officers in making decisions under stress as well as assigning officers to certain locations so they can become familiar with its residents could reduce the numbers of civilians killed, says Richmond's police chief, Chris Magnus.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.vox.com/2014/9/18/6111457/example-good-police-relations-cops-killing-outreach-culture-training|title=How to not shoot civilians: 9 community policing tips from a chief who got it right|last=Lind|first=Dara|date=2014-09-18|website=Vox|access-date=2017-07-30}}</ref>


Racial bias can be contagious within a social group or neighborhood.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Weisbuch|first=Max|last2=Pauker|first2=Kristin|date=2011-12-01|title=The Nonverbal Transmission of Intergroup Bias: A Model of Bias Contagion with Implications for Social Policy|journal=Social Issues and Policy Review|language=en|volume=5|issue=1|pages=257–291|doi=10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01032.x|pmc=3756600|pmid=23997812}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Weisbuch|first=Max|last2=Pauker|first2=Kristin|last3=Ambady|first3=Nalini|date=2009-12-18|title=The Subtle Transmission of Race Bias via Televised Nonverbal Behavior|journal=Science|language=en|volume=326|issue=5960|pages=1711–1714|doi=10.1126/science.1178358|pmc=3764987|pmid=20019288|bibcode=2009Sci...326.1711W}}</ref> Training police officers to be aware of this might help in reducing this effect.
Racial bias can be contagious within a social group or neighborhood.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Weisbuch|first1=Max|last2=Pauker|first2=Kristin|last3=Ambady|first3=Nalini|date=2009-12-18|title=The Subtle Transmission of Race Bias via Televised Nonverbal Behavior|journal=Science|language=en|volume=326|issue=5960|pages=1711–1714|doi=10.1126/science.1178358|pmc=3764987|pmid=20019288|bibcode=2009Sci...326.1711W}}</ref> Training police officers to be aware of this might help in reducing this effect.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Weisbuch|first1=Max|last2=Pauker|first2=Kristin|date=2011-12-01|title=The Nonverbal Transmission of Intergroup Bias: A Model of Bias Contagion with Implications for Social Policy|journal=Social Issues and Policy Review|language=en|volume=5|issue=1|pages=257–291|doi=10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01032.x|pmc=3756600|pmid=23997812}}</ref>


==See also==
*[[List of unarmed African Americans killed by law enforcement officers in the United States]]
== References ==
== References ==
<references />
<references />
Line 79: Line 97:
[[Category:Cognitive biases]]
[[Category:Cognitive biases]]
[[Category:Law enforcement in the United States]]
[[Category:Law enforcement in the United States]]
[[Category:Killings by law enforcement officers]]

Latest revision as of 17:16, 13 September 2024

The term shooting bias, also known as "shooter bias", is a proposed form of implicit racial bias which refers to the apparent tendency among the police to shoot black civilians more often than white civilians, even when they are unarmed.[1] In countries where white people aren't the majority, shooting bias may still apply, with different minority groups facing discrimination.

The concept proposes that the probability of being shot by the police depends on ethnicity in addition to the other known factors like location, the income of the neighborhood and whether or not the person is carrying a weapon[2] as well as the emotions shown by the victim.[3] Shooting bias is not limited to one race, as studies have shown that both black and white individuals demonstrated almost equivalent levels of shooting bias.[4]

Key Questions in Literature

[edit]

Benchmark

[edit]

Violent crime is committed by a very small number of individuals. These individuals tend to come from very poor disadvantaged communities. Since minorities are overrepresented in these communities, minorities are also overrepresented in violent crime. Thus, any statistical analysis of police bias must take this into account. Using population, police-citizen interactions, or total arrests as a benchmark, we observe that black citizens appear more likely than white citizens to be fatally shot by police officers. Using violent crime arrests or weapons offense arrests, we observe that black citizens appear less likely to be fatally shot by police officers.[5]

Resisting Arrest

[edit]

Additionally, data on the percent of individuals who resist arrest is also needed to draw conclusions about police bias. That data was available for Texas and California. Researchers were able to analyze this data to determine that there was shooting bias in California but not in Texas.[6]

Number of deaths

[edit]

Problems with police reporting practices

[edit]

Departments can voluntarily include justifiable homicides in the crime statistics of the FBI's Uniform Crime Reports, which means that a lot of departments don't provide data at all. Some cities haven't reported their data in years.[7] This means that the official data doesn't accurately reflect the number of civilians that are shot by the police.[8]

Newspapers like The Guardian and The Washington Post have started compiling databases of fatal police shootings,[9][10] revealing that in 2015 twice as many civilians had been fatally shot as the FBI's data suggested.[11] An FBI working group has started working on a proposal for making the reports more accurate, but they would still rely on voluntary data and therefore wouldn't fix the main reporting issue.[12]

Alternative database by The Guardian

[edit]

The database developed by The Guardian is currently the largest database on fatal shootings available.[13] They gather data through police reports, monitoring of regional news, fact-checked witness statements and other crowdsourced police fatality databases.[14]

Number of deaths

[edit]

In 2016, The Guardian counted 1093 people[9] who were killed by the police in the United States. Out of these, 574 were white and 266 were black. 95 of the white victims were unarmed, while 42 of the black victims were unarmed.

More white than black people are shot. It is important to distinguish to differentiate between the number of deaths of an ethnic group and the likelihood of being shot by police. The likelihood of being shot as a black rather than a white person is higher, whether the victim is armed or not.[2]

Likelihood of being shot with respect to race

[edit]

Observational studies

[edit]

A study carried out at the University of California found "evidence of a significant bias in the killing of unarmed black Americans compared to unarmed white Americans".[2] In this study, the probability of being shot by the police as a black, unarmed person versus as a white, unarmed person was 3.49 times higher. Unarmed Hispanics' likelihood to be shot was 1.67 times higher than for unarmed Whites. Black people have been 28% of those killed by police since 2013 despite being only 13% of the population.[15]

On the other hand a 2016–2018 study by the National Bureau of Economic Research (NBER) of twenty-first century data recorded by 12 police departments across the U.S., which took into account various factors such as type of confrontation, whether the suspect was armed or not, and whether or not they drew a gun, found that while overall "blacks are 21 percent more likely than whites to be involved in an interaction with police in which at least a weapon is drawn" and that in the raw data from New York City's Stop and Frisk program "blacks and Hispanics are more than fifty percent more likely to have an interaction with police which involves any use of force" after "[p]artitioning the data in myriad ways, we find no evidence of racial discrimination in officer-involved shootings."[17] The study did find bias against blacks and Hispanics in non-lethal and less-extreme lethal violence, stating that "as the intensity of force increases (e.g. handcuffing civilians without arrest, drawing or pointing a weapon, or using pepper spray or a baton), the probability that any civilian is subjected to such treatment is small, but the racial difference remains surprisingly constant", and noted that "[u]ntil recently, data on officer-involved shootings were extremely rare and contained little information on the details surrounding an incident".[17]

After the NBER study was published in the peer-reviewed Journal of Political Economy, a comment on it by Steven Durlauf and (Nobel Memorial Prize in Economics recipient) James Heckman of the Harris School of Public Policy Studies at the University of Chicago stated, "[i]n our judgment, this paper does not establish credible evidence on the presence or absence of discrimination against African Americans in police shootings."[18] The NBER study's author, Roland G. Fryer Jr., responded by saying Durlauf and Heckman erroneously claim that his sample is "based on stops". Further, he states that the "vast majority of the data...is gleaned from 911 calls for service in which a civilian requests police presence."[19]

A 2018 study in the journal Social Psychological and Personality Science sought to "argue for more reasonable benchmarks to compare fatal shooting rates across racial groups" than "comparing the group's raw shooting numbers against each group's overall representation in the population". On the premise that "the more [racial] group members are involved in criminal activity, the more exposure they have to situations in which police shootings would be likely to occur" the authors analyzed The Guardian's database through their own measurements of each race's involvement in criminal activity, calculated from the Federal Bureau of Investigation's Summary Reporting System (SRS) and National Incident-Based Reporting System (NIBRS), the National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS), and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention's WONDER database. Once the data were weighted by these measurements, the authors were able to conclude that, for example, although "[o]dds were 3.7 times higher for Blacks relative to Whites to be fatally shot given population proportions" while holding or reaching for a harmless object, this was appropriate given higher rates of average criminal involvement among blacks. They propose, "[i]f officers are more likely to misidentify a harmless object in the hands of a Black citizen due to stereotypes, the cause of officers holding those stereotypes may rest with (the very small percentage of) those who are more likely to engage in criminal activity."[20]

A more recent study was conducted by Michigan State University and the University of Maryland, compiling a list of more than 900 fatal U.S. police shootings in 2015 using crowdsourced databases from The Washington Post and The Guardian.[1] Then, they asked police departments for information about the race of the officers responsible for the shootings. They found black police were more likely to kill black civilians than white civilians. However, the same held true for white and Hispanic officers: Each group of police was more likely to shoot civilians of their own race. Researchers claimed this is true because police tend to be drawn from the communities they work in and are thus more likely to have deadly encounters with civilians of the same race. They conclude that "increasing diversity among officers by itself is unlikely to reduce racial disparity in police shootings".

Experimental studies

[edit]

Several experimental studies by social psychologists, in which college students are tested playing computer game simulations, have uncovered racial bias in their decisions to shoot.[21][22][23][24] A computer game simulation experiment in 2010 that tested college students, community members, and police officers found that racial bias was moderated by "prototypicality", i.e. they were more likely to shoot if a person had stronger features generally associated with black people (broader nose, large lips, etc.) regardless of the suspects actual racial category/skin colour, to the extent that white suspects with strong "black features" suffered more negative bias than black suspects with strong "white features". Furthermore they found greater racial bias among college students and community members than police officers (who are trained to overcome racial bias), and on average could not find racial bias among the police officers tested (although there was prototypicality bias).[25]

Factors that influence decision to shoot

[edit]

List of empirically researched factors

[edit]

Empirical research suggests the following factors to influence the decision to shoot:

  • Implicit racial bias, prejudice, and stereotypes[26]
  • Status armed/unarmed[2]
  • Whether or not the police officer was threatened or attacked[17]
  • Gender (men are more likely to be shot)[27]
  • County[2]
  • Income and status of the neighbourhood[28][29]
  • Previous experiences of the police officers with the ethnic group[30]
  • Previous experiences of the victim with the police[30]
  • Emotional facial expression of the civilian[3]
  • Fatigue of the police officer[31]

Implicit racial bias

[edit]

An implicit racial bias refers to unintentional judgments a person makes of a group (e.g. good/bad) of a certain ethnicity. Therefore, a person who shows implicit racial bias might not be aware of it. Police officers have been found to show a racial bias against black people in the decision to shoot,[32] and implicit biases are directly correlated with shooting behavior.[1]

In one of the studies, researchers investigated how stereotypes affected police officers' decisions to shoot. They used a video game and exposed their participants to pictures of either Whites or Non-Whites who were armed or unarmed. During this video game, the participants were asked to choose between "shoot" or "don't shoot" as quickly as possible. The results were that the participants shot armed black people faster than armed white people and chose "don't shoot" faster for unarmed white than unarmed black persons.

The authors explained those findings by the activation of stereotype thinking which lead white people to associate black people with danger. Because of this stereotype, the participants expected blacks to carry a gun and therefore were quicker to make the "shooting" decision.[26] Other studies have found similar results.[33][34] The time pressure to make a decision in decisions to shoot might magnify the effects of racial bias.[35] However, more recent research indicates that despite scoring moderately high on an Implicit Association Test, police officers are less likely to shoot black people in a video simulation[36]

Location and context

[edit]

In the USA, police officers live and work in a society where prejudices against minorities are common. In addition, police officers usually deal with higher crime rates in minority neighborhoods. These experiences tend to reinforce existing prejudices, leading to discrimination against minorities. Other factors that lead to discrimination by police are language barriers between police and some ethnic groups, experiences with disrespectful or hostile residents in certain minority neighborhoods, and low punishment for police officers who misbehave towards minorities.

Due to these prejudices, minorities may develop hostile attitudes towards police and be more likely to behave disrespectfully or to insult officers. This may lead to a self-reinforcing cycle, where the biases of either group reinforce those of the other.[30]

Examples that showcase prejudices among police officers include:[30]
[edit]
  • Disruptive actions by the police like stopping and questioning citizens on the street are more common in minority neighborhoods
  • More cases of non-whites are dropped than those of whites. This may be because the reasons for arresting them were too weak or unjustified in the first place.

Ways to reduce shooting bias

[edit]

Possibly the biggest change could be made by holding police officers accountable for their actions.[37] Internal investigations have been criticized for bias in favor of officers, leading to a lack of punishment. The work culture within some police departments has been criticized as racist, in light of many recent scandals.[38][39][40]

Knowing the factors that influence police officers decisions to shoot there are a few possible solutions for reducing shooting bias. The main factor is implicit racial bias, which in turn becomes augmented or intensified by certain factors that could be addressed.

Diversity in police departments might not reduce shooting bias,[41] but reducing fatigue might lower the impact of racial bias on the decision to shoot.[31] Changing the training of police officers so as to not showcase black armed targets more often than white ones could help reduce racial bias.[42][43]

Training police officers in making decisions under stress as well as assigning officers to certain locations so they can become familiar with its residents could reduce the numbers of civilians killed, says Richmond's police chief, Chris Magnus.[44]

Racial bias can be contagious within a social group or neighborhood.[45] Training police officers to be aware of this might help in reducing this effect.[46]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Feingold, Jonathan; Lorang, Karen (2012-01-01). "Defusing Implicit Bias". UCLA Law Review Discourse. 59: 212.
  2. ^ a b c d e Ross, Cody T. (2015-11-05). "A Multi-Level Bayesian Analysis of Racial Bias in Police Shootings at the County-Level in the United States, 2011–2014". PLOS ONE. 10 (11): e0141854. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1041854R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0141854. PMC 4634878. PMID 26540108.
  3. ^ a b Kubota, Jennifer T.; Ito, Tiffany A. (2014). "The role of expression and race in weapons identification". Emotion. 14 (6): 1115–1124. doi:10.1037/a0038214. PMC 4235656. PMID 25401289.
  4. ^ Benforado, Adam (2010-10-29). "Quick on the Draw: Implicit Bias and the Second Amendment". SSRN 1701089.
  5. ^ Tregle, Brandon; Nix, Justin; Alpert, Geoffrey P. (2018-12-07). "Disparity does not mean bias: making sense of observed racial disparities in fatal officer-involved shootings with multiple benchmarks". Journal of Crime and Justice. 42 (1): 18–31. doi:10.1080/0735648x.2018.1547269. ISSN 0735-648X. S2CID 150005614.
  6. ^ Shjarback, John A.; Nix, Justin (January 2020). "Considering violence against police by citizen race/ethnicity to contextualize representation in officer-involved shootings". Journal of Criminal Justice. 66: 101653. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2019.101653. ISSN 0047-2352.
  7. ^ Byers, Christine; Moskop, Walker (2014-11-09). "Nobody counts police killings in the U.S." stltoday.com. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  8. ^ Klinger, David A. (2011-12-13). "On the Problems and Promise of Research on Lethal Police Violence". Homicide Studies. 16 (1): 78–96. doi:10.1177/1088767911430861. S2CID 145366158.
  9. ^ a b Swaine, Jon; Laughland, Oliver; Lartey, Jamiles; Davis, Kenan; Harris, Rich; Popovich, Nadja; Powell, Kenton; team, Guardian US interactive. "The Counted: people killed by police in the United States – interactive". the Guardian. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  10. ^ "Police shootings 2017 database". Washington Post. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  11. ^ Kindy, Kimberly; et al. (2015-05-30). "Fatal police shootings in 2015 approaching 400 nationwide". Washington Post. Retrieved 2019-12-08.
  12. ^ Kindy, Kimberly (2015-12-08). "FBI to sharply expand system for tracking fatal police shootings". Washington Post. Retrieved 2019-12-08.
  13. ^ Hehman, Eric; Flake, Jessica K.; Calanchini, Jimmy (2017-07-27). "Disproportionate Use of Lethal Force in Policing Is Associated With Regional Racial Biases of Residents". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 9 (4): 393–401. doi:10.1177/1948550617711229. S2CID 51773001.
  14. ^ "About The Counted: why and how the Guardian is counting US police killings". the Guardian. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  15. ^ https://mappingpoliceviolence.org/ [bare URL]
  16. ^ Fryer, Roland Gerhard (June 2019). "An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force". Journal of Political Economy. 127 (3). University of Chicago: 1210–1261. doi:10.1086/701423. ISSN 0022-3808. OCLC 8118094562. S2CID 158634577.
  17. ^ a b c Fryer, Roland Gerhard (July 2016). An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force (PDF) (Report). NBER Working Papers (Revised January 2018 ed.). National Bureau of Economic Research. doi:10.3386/w22399. OCLC 956328193. S2CID 158634577. JELJ01, K0. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-10-31.[16]
  18. ^ Durlauf, Steven Neil; Heckman, James Joseph (2020-07-21). "An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force: A Comment". Journal of Political Economy. 128 (10). University of Chicago: 3998–4002. doi:10.1086/710976. ISSN 0022-3808. OCLC 8672021465. S2CID 222811199. Archived from the original on 2020-11-08.
  19. ^ Fryer, Roland Gerhard (2020-07-21). "An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force: A Response". Journal of Political Economy. 128 (10). University of Chicago: 4003–4008. doi:10.1086/710977. ISSN 0022-3808. OCLC 8672034484. S2CID 222813143. Archived from the original on 2020-11-08.
  20. ^ Cesario, Joseph; Johnson, David; Terrill, William (July 2019). "Is There Evidence of Racial Disparity in Police Use of Deadly Force? Analyses of Officer-Involved Fatal Shootings in 2015–2016". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 10 (5): 586–595. doi:10.1177/1948550618775108. S2CID 54057840. Archived from the original on November 8, 2020. Retrieved November 8, 2020.
  21. ^ Greenwald, Anthony, G.; Oakes, Mark, A.; Hoffman, Hunter, G. (11 April 2003). "Targets of discrimination: Effects of race on responses to weapons holders". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 39 (4): 399–405. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00020-9.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  22. ^ Correll, Joshua; Wittenbrink, Bernd; Judd, Charles, M.; Park, Bernadette (2002). "The Police Officer's Dilemma: Using Ethnicity to Disambiguate Potentially Threatening Individuals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 83 (6): 1314–1329. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314. PMID 12500813.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ Payne, B. Keith (August 2001). "Prejudice and Perception: The Role of Automatic and Controlled Processes in Misperceiving a Weapon". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (2): 181–192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181. PMID 11519925.
  24. ^ Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles, M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd (3 July 2007). "The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot" (PDF). European Journal of Social Psychology. 37 (6): 1102–1117. doi:10.1002/ejsp.450.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ Ma, Debbie, S.; Correll, Joshua (March 2011). "Target prototypicality moderates racial bias in the decision to shoot". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 47 (2): 391–396. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.11.002. Retrieved 12 November 2010.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  26. ^ a b Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd (2007). "The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot". European Journal of Social Psychology. 37 (6): 1102–1117. doi:10.1002/ejsp.450.
  27. ^ Sagara, Ryan Gabrielson, Ryann Grochowski Jones, Eric (2014-10-10). "Deadly Force, in Black and White". ProPublica. Retrieved 2017-07-30.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  28. ^ Smith, Douglas A. (1986). "The Neighborhood Context of Police Behavior". Crime and Justice. 8: 313–341. doi:10.1086/449126. S2CID 144977273.
  29. ^ Terrill, William; Reisig, Michael D. (2016-08-18). "Neighborhood Context and Police Use of Force". Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 40 (3): 291–321. doi:10.1177/0022427803253800. S2CID 43353487.
  30. ^ a b c d Weitzer, Ronald (1996). "Racial discrimination in the criminal justice system: Findings and problems in the literature". Journal of Criminal Justice. 24 (4): 309–322. doi:10.1016/0047-2352(96)00015-3.
  31. ^ a b Ma, Debbie S.; Correll, Joshua; Wittenbrink, Bernd; Bar-Anan, Yoav; Sriram, N.; Nosek, Brian A. (2013-11-01). "When Fatigue Turns Deadly: The Association Between Fatigue and Racial Bias in the Decision to Shoot". Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 35 (6): 515–524. doi:10.1080/01973533.2013.840630. S2CID 29835235.
  32. ^ Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd; Sadler, Melody S.; Keesee, Tracie (2007). "Across the thin blue line: Police officers and racial bias in the decision to shoot". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 92 (6): 1006–1023. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1006. PMID 17547485. S2CID 17992608.
  33. ^ Correll, Joshua; Wittenbrink, Bernd; Crawford, Matthew T.; Sadler, Melody S. (2015). "Stereotypic vision: How stereotypes disambiguate visual stimuli". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 108 (2): 219–233. doi:10.1037/pspa0000015. PMID 25603373.
  34. ^ Payne, B. Keith (2001). "Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (2): 181–192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181. PMID 11519925. S2CID 15865607.
  35. ^ Payne, B. Keith (2006-12-01). "Weapon Bias: Split-Second Decisions and Unintended Stereotyping". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (6): 287–291. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00454.x. S2CID 16595511.
  36. ^ James, Lois; James, Stephen M.; Vila, Bryan J. (2016-01-14). "The Reverse Racism Effect". Criminology & Public Policy. 15 (2): 457–479. doi:10.1111/1745-9133.12187. ISSN 1538-6473.
  37. ^ Hudson, Redditt (2014-12-06). "Being a cop showed me just how racist and violent the police are. There's only one fix". Washington Post. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  38. ^ "Michigan police suspend cop behind 'racist' footage of black man". NY Daily News. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  39. ^ "Ohio cops suspended for exchanging racist text messages". NY Daily News. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  40. ^ "Two Black Cops Write Statements About Racism in LAPD". BET.com. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  41. ^ Smith, Brad W. (2003-05-01). "The Impact of Police Officer Diversity on Police-Caused Homicides". Policy Studies Journal. 31 (2): 147–162. doi:10.1111/1541-0072.t01-1-00009.
  42. ^ Sim, Jessica J.; Correll, Joshua; Sadler, Melody S. (2013-03-01). "Understanding Police and Expert Performance: When Training Attenuates (vs. Exacerbates) Stereotypic Bias in the Decision to Shoot". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 39 (3): 291–304. doi:10.1177/0146167212473157. PMID 23401478. S2CID 34947053.
  43. ^ Plant, E. Ashby; Peruche, B. Michelle; Butz, David A. (2005). "Eliminating automatic racial bias: Making race non-diagnostic for responses to criminal suspects". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 41 (2): 141–156. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.07.004.
  44. ^ Lind, Dara (2014-09-18). "How to not shoot civilians: 9 community policing tips from a chief who got it right". Vox. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  45. ^ Weisbuch, Max; Pauker, Kristin; Ambady, Nalini (2009-12-18). "The Subtle Transmission of Race Bias via Televised Nonverbal Behavior". Science. 326 (5960): 1711–1714. Bibcode:2009Sci...326.1711W. doi:10.1126/science.1178358. PMC 3764987. PMID 20019288.
  46. ^ Weisbuch, Max; Pauker, Kristin (2011-12-01). "The Nonverbal Transmission of Intergroup Bias: A Model of Bias Contagion with Implications for Social Policy". Social Issues and Policy Review. 5 (1): 257–291. doi:10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01032.x. PMC 3756600. PMID 23997812.