Talk:Color vision: Difference between revisions
m Archiving 1 discussion(s) to Talk:Color vision/Archive 1) (bot |
|||
(25 intermediate revisions by 15 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Talk header}} |
{{Talk header}} |
||
{{WikiProject banner shell|class=C|vital=yes|1= |
|||
{{Vital article|level=4|topic=Biology|class=B}} |
|||
⚫ | |||
{{WikiProjectBannerShell|1= |
|||
{{WikiProject |
{{WikiProject Biology |importance=High}} |
||
{{WikiProject |
{{WikiProject Neuroscience |importance=High}} |
||
{{WikiProject |
{{WikiProject Psychology |importance=High}} |
||
⚫ | |||
}} |
}} |
||
{{American English}} |
|||
{{User:MiszaBot/config |
{{User:MiszaBot/config |
||
|archiveheader = {{aan}} |
|archiveheader = {{aan}} |
||
Line 16: | Line 16: | ||
|archive = Talk:Color vision/Archive %(counter)d |
|archive = Talk:Color vision/Archive %(counter)d |
||
}} |
}} |
||
{{Broken anchors|links= |
|||
* <nowiki>[[blue#Optics|blue]]</nowiki> The anchor (#Optics) is no longer available because it was [[Special:Diff/1115943285|deleted by a user]] before. <!-- {"title":"Optics","appear":{"revid":602700069,"parentid":601676319,"timestamp":"2014-04-04T08:57:44Z","replaced_anchors":{"Shades and variations of blue":"Shades and variations","Blue in the ancient world":"In the ancient world","Blue in the Byzantine Empire and the Islamic World":"In the Byzantine Empire and the Islamic World","Blue during the Middle Ages":"During the Middle Ages","Blue in the European Renaissance":"In the European Renaissance","Blue and the Impressionist painters":"The Impressionist painters","Blue in the 20th and 21st century":"In the 20th and 21st century","Blue in science and industry":"In science and industry","Blue pigments and dyes":"Pigments and dyes","Scientific natural standards for blue":"Scientific natural standards","Blue and atmospheric perspective":"Atmospheric perspective","Blue in world culture":"In world culture","Blue as a national and international colour":"As a national and international colour","Associations and sayings about blue":"Associations and sayings"},"removed_section_titles":["Shades and variations of blue","History of blue","Blue in the ancient world","Blue in the Byzantine Empire and the Islamic World","Blue during the Middle Ages","Blue in the European Renaissance","Blue and white porcelain","The blue uniform","Blue and the Impressionist painters","Blue in the 20th and 21st century","Blue in science and industry","Blue pigments and dyes","The optics of blue","Scientific natural standards for blue","Blue and atmospheric perspective","Blue eyes","Blue in nature","Blue in world culture","Blue as a national and international colour","Associations and sayings about blue"],"added_section_titles":["Shades and variations","History","In the ancient world","In the Byzantine Empire and the Islamic World","During the Middle Ages","In the European Renaissance","White porcelain","Uniform","The Impressionist painters","In the 20th and 21st century","In science and industry","Pigments and dyes","Optics","Scientific natural standards","Atmospheric perspective","Eyes","In nature","In world culture","As a national and international colour","Associations and sayings"]},"disappear":{"revid":1115943285,"parentid":1115943249,"timestamp":"2022-10-14T00:40:11Z","removed_section_titles":["Types","Etymology and linguistic differences","Science and nature","Optics","Pigments and dyes","Reflex Blue","Scientific natural standards","Colouring of sky and sea","Atmospheric perspective","Astronomy","Blue eyes","Lasers","CITEREF2003","CITEREFMatson1985","In the Byzantine Empire and the Islamic World","During the Middle Ages","In the European Renaissance","Blue and white porcelain","War of the blues – indigo versus woad","CITEREFFoucaud1846","Blue uniform","CITEREF2009","CITEREFOkidegbe2011","Search for the perfect blue","Impressionist painters","Blue suit","In the 20th and 21st century","In world culture","As a national and international colour","CITEREF2009","CITEREFBalfour-Paul1997","CITEREFBrooks2001","Gender","Music","Transportation","Associations and sayings","CITEREFKoerner2003","Antiquity","Association football","North American leagues","CITEREF2008","CITEREF2011","Cricket","Other","Notes and citations","Bibliography","CITEREFPastoureau2010"],"added_section_titles":["CITEREFDefonseka2019","Etymology and linguistics","CITEREF2012","Optics and colour theory","CITEREF2015","CITEREFSandra Espinet","As a structural colour","Colourants","Artificial blues","Dyes for textiles and food","CITEREFNewsomeCulverVan Breemen2014","Pigments for painting and glass","Inks","CITEREFPlackeFischerColditzKunkel2016","Inorganic compounds","In nature","Sky and sea","CITEREFGlenn S. Smith2005","CITEREFAnne Marie Helmenstine","Minerals","Plants and fungi","CITEREFHarmonWeisgraberWeiss1980","CITEREFNuno Mateas, Victor de Freitas2008","Animals","CITEREFUmbers2013","CITEREFVane-Wright1979","CITEREFSimonisSerge2012","CITEREF1995","CITEREF2015","CITEREFPotyrailoBonamHartleyStarkey2015","CITEREF2004","CITEREFAriel RodríguezNicholas I. MundyRoberto IbáñezHeike Pröhl2020","CITEREFMakoto GodaRyozo Fujii1998","Eyes","CITEREF1998","CITEREFWeise2008","CITEREFDouglas Belkin2006","CITEREFSarkarMukherjeeBeraDas2016","CITEREFDIKSHIT2012","CITEREFMoorey1999","In the Middle Ages","Renaissance through 18th century","19th-20th Century","In society and culture","Uniforms","CITEREFWalter H. Bradford","CITEREFBattaglio2016","Works cited","Further reading","CITEREFBowersoxChamberlin1995","CITEREFRiley1995","CITEREFTravis2020","CITEREFLours2020","CITEREFJosserandMeeussenMajid2021","CITEREFMacdonald2018","External links"]}} --> |
|||
}} |
|||
== "Color vision table" -- Types of cones or types of color receptors? == |
|||
The individual articles for monochromacy, dichromacy, etc. describe ''x''-chromacy as having ''x'' types of color receptors or ''x'' independent channels for conveying color information. The "Color vision table" at the end of [[Color_vision#In_other_animal_species|Section 2.5 In other animal species]] erroneously equates this with number of cones. |
|||
== Sentence needs rewriting == |
|||
The existence of non-cone color receptors is well-described in the animal kingdom. For instance, many frogs and salamanders have dual-rod retinae wherein two different rod types are sensitive to different spectra (for a review of this, see e.g. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fevo.2019.00321/full this review], section "Amphibian Opsins and Photoreceptors"). This allows for color vision at the absolute visual threshold, i.e. scotopic conditions, or very low-light conditions. Animals have been shown to utilise this system under ethologically relevant conditions, as demonstrated by [https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/full/10.1098/rstb.2016.0066 these experiments]. |
|||
In the subsection titled '''Theories''', this sentence appears: |
|||
Species with two distinct color-sensitive rod types and two cone types are indeed tetrachromatic, yet they only have two types of cone cells, unlike what the table in this article would lead readers to believe. Additionally, the number of colors perceived is likely closer to 40,000 than 100 million due to significant overlap in the spectral sensitivities of their rods and cones. |
|||
"''This phenomenon of complementary colors demonstrates cyan, rather than green, to be the complement of red and magenta, rather than red, to be the complement of green, as well as demonstrating, as a consequence, that the reddish-green color proposed to be impossible by opponent process theory is, in fact, the colour yellow.''" |
|||
To resolve these issues, I would recommend amending the table to entirely remove the column titled "State". Tetrachromacy does not mean four cone types and does not imply the ability to see 100 million colors, as we have seen from many studies of different species of amphibians. |
|||
I hope someone knowledgeable on this subject can rewrite this sentence in comprehensible English. Ideally this will become more than one sentence, since it appears that too many ideas are crammed into one sentence, making it very hard to understand.[[Special:Contributions/50.205.142.50|50.205.142.50]] ([[User talk:50.205.142.50|talk]]) 15:01, 25 June 2020 (UTC) |
|||
<!-- [[User:DoNotArchiveUntil]] 13:10, 21 September 2030 (UTC) -->{{User:ClueBot III/DoNotArchiveUntil|1916226659}} |
|||
⚫ | |||
:After nine months with no further discussion, I have removed the Color Vision Table. |
|||
== Perception of near ultraviolet as purple instead of blue == |
|||
:[[User:NeuroJasper|NeuroJasper]] ([[User talk:NeuroJasper|talk]]) 14:48, 21 April 2021 (UTC) |
|||
Neither article suggests that any of the animals are tetrachromats. Indeed, the second article explicitly states that the presence of photoreceptors with different spectral sensitivities that are functional at the same light levels is a requirement for colour vision. It then states that some some frogs may have dichromatic colour vision based only on their retinal rods. It does not state that they are tetrachromats, because they are not. |
|||
I'm a newbie, but I'd like to request someone knowledgeable edit this page to reflect why we see near ultraviolet as purple/violet and not just really pure blue. I came to this page to learn the answer but instead had to find this elsewhere. The chart showing the sensitivity of the red/green/blue cones suggests that red fades off such that it has no sensitivity to near-UV spectra. Other sites (e.g. https://midimagic.sgc-hosting.com/huvision.htm) show that the red cones have a secondary sensitivity peaking at 420nm and so both red and blue cones are triggered by near-ultraviolet light and we see purple. That makes complete sense, but no way to figure that out from the article as written. Thanks for helping a future searcher and making Wikipedia a better than Quora! |
|||
You created an "issue" with the table by redefining "tetrachromacy" based on an incomplete understanding of how colours are discriminated using simultaneous signals from different receptors stimulated by the same wavelength. |
|||
⚫ | |||
This is underscored by your further assertion that such tetrachromacy would be limited to the perception of only 40000 colours due to overlap in the spectral sensitivities of the rods and cones. |
|||
All that is required is a different response between two types of receptors at any and all wavelengths to which they are sensitive. The range that they respond to can be exactly the same, and indeed, the long and medium cones in our eyes do have a very similar range, and very similar wavelength of maximum sensitivity. It is this that gives us our greatest colour discrimination in the orange to green part of the spectrum. |
|||
In fact, if the rods and cones did work together at any light levels, the spectral overlap would increase the number of distinguishable colours. I believe you are confusing the number of distinguishable colours with total spectral range of vision. Distant spacing of spectral sensitivities increases the spectral range of colour vision, but decreases the ability to distinguish different wavelengths, requiring a larger difference in the wavelengths of two lights in order to perceive them as different colours. This is why humans can perceive a different colour with a change of 1nm in the spectral region covered by all three cones, but require a difference of about 10nm in those parts of the spectrum in which only two cones are sensitive. In the very long wavelengths, from 750nm to 950nm, considered infrared but visible with very bright, very pure sources, such as infrared LEDs, increasing the wavelength is perceived only as a decrease in brightness. The very deep red colour perceived does not vary, and it is impossible to determine if the "colour" has changed or if it has simply been dimmed. A sufficiently bright 900nm light will appear exactly the same as a dimmer 800nm light. |
|||
The table was useful, and it was correct. You are in no position to estimate the number of different colours distinguishable with any number of different receptors, and until you find a published reference to the contrary, the chromacy of vision is determined by the number of different photoreceptors functional at a given level of light. By convention, this is based on cones, functional at photopic light levels. |
|||
I will reinstate the table in a month or so unless you respond with some evidence that your assertions are in fact accepted by visual scientists. <!-- Template:Unsigned --><span class="autosigned" style="font-size:85%;">— Preceding [[Wikipedia:Signatures|unsigned]] comment added by [[User:At least I try|At least I try]] ([[User talk:At least I try#top|talk]] • [[Special:Contributions/At least I try|contribs]]) 17:45, 19 December 2021 (UTC)</span> <!--Autosigned by SineBot--> |
Latest revision as of 13:07, 3 July 2024
This is the talk page for discussing improvements to the Color vision article. This is not a forum for general discussion of the article's subject. |
Article policies
|
Find sources: Google (books · news · scholar · free images · WP refs) · FENS · JSTOR · TWL |
Archives: 1Auto-archiving period: 2 months |
This level-4 vital article is rated C-class on Wikipedia's content assessment scale. It is of interest to the following WikiProjects: | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
This article is written in American English, which has its own spelling conventions (color, defense, traveled) and some terms that are used in it may be different or absent from other varieties of English. According to the relevant style guide, this should not be changed without broad consensus. |
This article links to one or more target anchors that no longer exist.
Please help fix the broken anchors. You can remove this template after fixing the problems. | Reporting errors |
"Color vision table" -- Types of cones or types of color receptors?
[edit]The individual articles for monochromacy, dichromacy, etc. describe x-chromacy as having x types of color receptors or x independent channels for conveying color information. The "Color vision table" at the end of Section 2.5 In other animal species erroneously equates this with number of cones.
The existence of non-cone color receptors is well-described in the animal kingdom. For instance, many frogs and salamanders have dual-rod retinae wherein two different rod types are sensitive to different spectra (for a review of this, see e.g. this review, section "Amphibian Opsins and Photoreceptors"). This allows for color vision at the absolute visual threshold, i.e. scotopic conditions, or very low-light conditions. Animals have been shown to utilise this system under ethologically relevant conditions, as demonstrated by these experiments.
Species with two distinct color-sensitive rod types and two cone types are indeed tetrachromatic, yet they only have two types of cone cells, unlike what the table in this article would lead readers to believe. Additionally, the number of colors perceived is likely closer to 40,000 than 100 million due to significant overlap in the spectral sensitivities of their rods and cones.
To resolve these issues, I would recommend amending the table to entirely remove the column titled "State". Tetrachromacy does not mean four cone types and does not imply the ability to see 100 million colors, as we have seen from many studies of different species of amphibians.
NeuroJasper (talk) 14:55, 22 July 2020 (UTC)
- After nine months with no further discussion, I have removed the Color Vision Table.
- NeuroJasper (talk) 14:48, 21 April 2021 (UTC)
Neither article suggests that any of the animals are tetrachromats. Indeed, the second article explicitly states that the presence of photoreceptors with different spectral sensitivities that are functional at the same light levels is a requirement for colour vision. It then states that some some frogs may have dichromatic colour vision based only on their retinal rods. It does not state that they are tetrachromats, because they are not. You created an "issue" with the table by redefining "tetrachromacy" based on an incomplete understanding of how colours are discriminated using simultaneous signals from different receptors stimulated by the same wavelength. This is underscored by your further assertion that such tetrachromacy would be limited to the perception of only 40000 colours due to overlap in the spectral sensitivities of the rods and cones. All that is required is a different response between two types of receptors at any and all wavelengths to which they are sensitive. The range that they respond to can be exactly the same, and indeed, the long and medium cones in our eyes do have a very similar range, and very similar wavelength of maximum sensitivity. It is this that gives us our greatest colour discrimination in the orange to green part of the spectrum. In fact, if the rods and cones did work together at any light levels, the spectral overlap would increase the number of distinguishable colours. I believe you are confusing the number of distinguishable colours with total spectral range of vision. Distant spacing of spectral sensitivities increases the spectral range of colour vision, but decreases the ability to distinguish different wavelengths, requiring a larger difference in the wavelengths of two lights in order to perceive them as different colours. This is why humans can perceive a different colour with a change of 1nm in the spectral region covered by all three cones, but require a difference of about 10nm in those parts of the spectrum in which only two cones are sensitive. In the very long wavelengths, from 750nm to 950nm, considered infrared but visible with very bright, very pure sources, such as infrared LEDs, increasing the wavelength is perceived only as a decrease in brightness. The very deep red colour perceived does not vary, and it is impossible to determine if the "colour" has changed or if it has simply been dimmed. A sufficiently bright 900nm light will appear exactly the same as a dimmer 800nm light. The table was useful, and it was correct. You are in no position to estimate the number of different colours distinguishable with any number of different receptors, and until you find a published reference to the contrary, the chromacy of vision is determined by the number of different photoreceptors functional at a given level of light. By convention, this is based on cones, functional at photopic light levels. I will reinstate the table in a month or so unless you respond with some evidence that your assertions are in fact accepted by visual scientists. — Preceding unsigned comment added by At least I try (talk • contribs) 17:45, 19 December 2021 (UTC)
- C-Class level-4 vital articles
- Wikipedia level-4 vital articles in Biology and health sciences
- C-Class vital articles in Biology and health sciences
- C-Class color articles
- Top-importance color articles
- All WikiProject Color pages
- C-Class Biology articles
- High-importance Biology articles
- WikiProject Biology articles
- C-Class neuroscience articles
- High-importance neuroscience articles
- C-Class psychology articles
- High-importance psychology articles
- WikiProject Psychology articles
- Wikipedia articles that use American English