Karl Lashley: Difference between revisions
mNo edit summary Tags: Visual edit Mobile edit Mobile web edit |
Updated short description Tags: Mobile edit Mobile app edit Android app edit App description change |
||
(27 intermediate revisions by 21 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Short description|American psychologist (1890-1958)}} |
|||
{{Infobox scientist |
{{Infobox scientist |
||
|name = Karl Spencer Lashley |
|name = Karl Spencer Lashley |
||
|image = Photo of Karl Lashley.jpg |
|image = Photo of Karl Lashley.jpg |
||
|birth_date = June 7, 1890 |
|birth_date = June 7, 1890 |
||
|birth_place = [[Davis, West Virginia]] |
|birth_place = [[Davis, West Virginia]], US |
||
|death_date = {{Death date and age|1958|8|7|1890|6|7}} |
|death_date = {{Death date and age|1958|8|7|1890|6|7}} |
||
|death_place = [[Poitiers]], France |
|death_place = [[Poitiers]], France |
||
|residence = |
|||
|citizenship = |
|||
|nationality = [[United States]] |
|||
|ethnicity = |
|||
|field = [[Psychology]] |
|field = [[Psychology]] |
||
|work_institutions = [[University of Minnesota]], [[University of Chicago]], [[Harvard University]] |
|work_institutions = [[University of Minnesota]], [[University of Chicago]], [[Harvard University]] |
||
|alma_mater = [[Johns Hopkins University]] |
|alma_mater = [[Johns Hopkins University]] |
||
|doctoral_advisor = |
|doctoral_advisor = [[Herbert S. Jennings]] |
||
|doctoral_students = |
|doctoral_students = |
||
|known_for = [[Learning]] and [[memory]] |
|known_for = [[Learning]] and [[memory]] |
||
Line 19: | Line 16: | ||
|influenced = |
|influenced = |
||
|prizes = {{Plainlist| |
|prizes = {{Plainlist| |
||
* [[Foreign Member of the Royal Society]] (1951)<ref name=frs>{{Cite journal | last1 = Bartlett | first1 = F. C. | |
* [[Foreign Member of the Royal Society]] (1951)<ref name=frs>{{Cite journal | last1 = Bartlett | first1 = F. C. | author-link1 =Frederic Bartlett | year = 1960 | title = Karl Spencer Lashley 1890-1958 | journal = [[Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society]] | volume = 5 | pages = 107–118| doi = 10.1098/rsbm.1960.0010 | s2cid = 73205669 }}</ref> |
||
* [[Daniel Giraud Elliot Medal]] (1943)}} |
* [[Daniel Giraud Elliot Medal]] (1943)}} |
||
|religion = |
|religion = |
||
Line 25: | Line 22: | ||
}} |
}} |
||
'''Karl Spencer Lashley''' (June 7, 1890 – August 7, 1958) was |
'''Karl Spencer Lashley''' (June 7, 1890 – August 7, 1958) was an American psychologist and [[behaviorist]] remembered for his contributions to the study of learning and memory. A ''[[Review of General Psychology]]'' survey, published in 2002, ranked Lashley as the 61st most cited psychologist of the 20th century.<ref name=frs/><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Haggbloom |first1=Steven J. |title=The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century |journal=Review of General Psychology |volume=6 |issue=2 |year=2002 |pages=139–52 |doi=10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139 |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug02/eminent.aspx |last2=Warnick |first2=Renee |last3=Warnick |first3=Jason E. |last4=Jones |first4=Vinessa K. |last5=Yarbrough |first5=Gary L. |last6=Russell |first6=Tenea M. |last7=Borecky |first7=Chris M. |last8=McGahhey |first8=Reagan |last9=Powell |first9=John L. III |last10=Beavers |first10=Jamie |last11=Monte |first11=Emmanuelle |citeseerx=10.1.1.586.1913 |s2cid=145668721 }}</ref> |
||
==Early life and education== |
==Early life and education== |
||
Lashley was born on June 7, 1890 in the town of [[Davis, West Virginia]]. He was the only child of Charles and Maggie Lashley. He grew up in a middle-class family with a reasonably comfortable life. Lashley's father held various local political positions. His mother was a stay-at-home parent, and had a vast collection of books in the home. She brought in women from the community, whom she would teach various subjects. This is no doubt what gave Lashley his love of learning. Lashley has always held his family in high regard. He has said that his father was a kind man.{{citation needed|date=February 2018}} |
Lashley was born on June 7, 1890, in the town of [[Davis, West Virginia]]. He was the only child of Charles and Maggie Lashley. He grew up in a middle-class family with a reasonably comfortable life. Lashley's father held various local political positions. His mother was a stay-at-home parent, and had a vast collection of books in the home. She brought in women from the community, whom she would teach various subjects. This is no doubt what gave Lashley his love of learning. Lashley has always held his family in high regard. He has said that his father was a kind man.{{citation needed|date=February 2018}} |
||
Lashley's mother was a strong advocate of schooling, and she encouraged Lashley intellectually from an early age. Lashley was a very active boy, both physically and mentally. He was able to read by the age of four. His favorite thing to do as a child was to wander through the woods and collect animals, like butterflies and mice. He spent most of his childhood alone. Lashley did not have many friends. The reasons for his lack of friendships is unclear. Lashley graduated high school at age 14. |
Lashley's mother was a strong advocate of schooling, and she encouraged Lashley intellectually from an early age. Lashley was a very active boy, both physically and mentally. He was able to read by the age of four. His favorite thing to do as a child was to wander through the woods and collect animals, like butterflies and mice. He spent most of his childhood alone. Lashley did not have many friends. The reasons for his lack of friendships is unclear. Lashley graduated high school at age 14. |
||
Line 34: | Line 31: | ||
Lashley enrolled at [[West Virginia University]], where he had originally intended to become an English major. He took a course in [[zoology]], however, and switched his major to zoology due to his interactions with the professor John Black Johnston. Lashley wrote, "Within a few weeks in his class I knew that I had found my life's work".<ref name=Beach>{{cite book|last=Beach|first=Frank A.|title=Karl Spencer Lashley 1890-1958|year=1961|publisher=National Academy of Sciences|location=Washington, D.C.|page=9|url=http://www.nap.edu/html/biomems/klashley.pdf}}</ref> |
Lashley enrolled at [[West Virginia University]], where he had originally intended to become an English major. He took a course in [[zoology]], however, and switched his major to zoology due to his interactions with the professor John Black Johnston. Lashley wrote, "Within a few weeks in his class I knew that I had found my life's work".<ref name=Beach>{{cite book|last=Beach|first=Frank A.|title=Karl Spencer Lashley 1890-1958|year=1961|publisher=National Academy of Sciences|location=Washington, D.C.|page=9|url=http://www.nap.edu/html/biomems/klashley.pdf}}</ref> |
||
After obtaining his [[Bachelor of Arts]] at West Virginia University, Lashley was awarded a teaching fellowship at the [[University of Pittsburgh]], where he taught [[biology]] along with biological laboratories. While there he also carried out [[research]] which he used for his master's thesis.<ref name="beach"> |
After obtaining his [[Bachelor of Arts]] at West Virginia University, Lashley was awarded a teaching fellowship at the [[University of Pittsburgh]], where he taught [[biology]] along with biological laboratories. While there he also carried out [[research]] which he used for his master's thesis.<ref name="beach">http://www.nasonline.org/publications/biographical-memoirs/memoir-pdfs/lashley-karl.pdf {{Bare URL PDF|date=March 2022}}</ref> Once Lashley completed his master's degree, he studied at [[Johns Hopkins University]], where he received his [[PhD]] in [[genetics]] in June 1911. He became a [[professor]] at [[University of Minnesota]], [[University of Chicago]], and [[Harvard University]]. |
||
At Hopkins, Lashley minored in psychology under [[John B. Watson]], whom he continued to work closely with him after receiving his PhD. It was during this time that Lashley worked with Franz and was introduced to his training/ablation method. Watson had a great deal of influence on Lashley. Together the two conducted field experiments and studied the effects of different drugs on maze learning of rats.<ref name="beach"/> Watson helped Lashley to focus on specific problems in learning and experimental investigation, followed by locating the area of the [[cerebrum]] involved in learning and discrimination. |
At Hopkins, Lashley minored in psychology under [[John B. Watson]], whom he continued to work closely with him after receiving his PhD. It was during this time that Lashley worked with Shepherd Ivory Franz and was introduced to his training/ablation method. Watson had a great deal of influence on Lashley. Together the two conducted field experiments and studied the effects of different drugs on maze learning of rats.<ref name="beach"/> Watson helped Lashley to focus on specific problems in learning and experimental investigation, followed by locating the area of the [[cerebrum]] involved in learning and discrimination. |
||
==Career== |
==Career== |
||
Lashley's career began with research concerning [[brain]] mechanisms and how they were related to sense receptors. He also conducted work on instinct as well as color vision. He studied many animals and primates, which had been an interest since his freshman year at college. |
Lashley's career began with research concerning [[brain]] mechanisms and how they were related to sense receptors. He also conducted work on instinct as well as color vision. He studied many animals and primates, which had been an interest since his freshman year at college. |
||
Lashley worked at the [[University of Minnesota]] |
Lashley worked at the [[University of Minnesota]] from 1917 to 1926 and then at the Institute for Juvenile Research in Chicago before becoming a professor at the [[University of Chicago]]. After this he went to [[Harvard]], but was dissatisfied and from there became the director of the Yerkes Laboratory of Primate Biology in Orange Park, Florida. |
||
Lashley's most influential research centered around the cortical basis of learning and discrimination. He researched this by looking at the measurement of behavior before and after specific, carefully quantified, induced brain damage in rats. Lashley trained rats to perform specific tasks (seeking a food reward), then lesioned specific areas of the rats' cortex, either before or after the animals received the training. The cortical lesions had specific effects on acquisition and retention of knowledge, but the location of the removed cortex had no effect on the rats' performance in the maze. This led Lashley to conclude that memories are not localized, but that they are widely distributed across the cortex. Today we know that distribution of [[engram (neuropsychology)|engrams]] does in fact exist, but that the distribution is not equal across all cortical areas, as Lashley assumed.<ref>{{cite journal|title=Sheena A. Josselyn. ''Continuing the search for the engram: examining the mechanism of fear memories''|pmc=2895151| pmid=20569648|volume=35|issue=4|date=July 2010|journal=J Psychiatry Neurosci|pages=221–8|doi=10.1503/jpn.100015|last1=Josselyn|first1=Sheena}}</ref><ref name="history">{{Cite web |url=http://grants.hhp.coe.uh.edu/clayne/HistoryofMC/HistoryMC/Lashley.htm |title= |
Lashley's most influential research centered around the cortical basis of learning and discrimination. He researched this by looking at the measurement of behavior before and after specific, carefully quantified, induced brain damage in rats. Lashley trained rats to perform specific tasks (seeking a food reward), then lesioned specific areas of the rats' cortex, either before or after the animals received the training. The cortical lesions had specific effects on acquisition and retention of knowledge, but the location of the removed cortex had no effect on the rats' performance in the maze. This led Lashley to conclude that memories are not localized, but that they are widely distributed across the cortex. Today we know that distribution of [[engram (neuropsychology)|engrams]] does in fact exist, but that the distribution is not equal across all cortical areas, as Lashley assumed.<ref>{{cite journal|title=Sheena A. Josselyn. ''Continuing the search for the engram: examining the mechanism of fear memories''|pmc=2895151| pmid=20569648|volume=35|issue=4|date=July 2010|journal=J Psychiatry Neurosci|pages=221–8|doi=10.1503/jpn.100015|last1=Josselyn|first1=Sheena}}</ref><ref name="history">{{Cite web |url=http://grants.hhp.coe.uh.edu/clayne/HistoryofMC/HistoryMC/Lashley.htm |title=Karl Lashley |access-date=2013-11-28 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190511192122/http://grants.hhp.coe.uh.edu/clayne/HistoryofMC/HistoryMC/Lashley.htm |archive-date=2019-05-11 }}</ref> His study of V1 (primary visual cortex) led him to believe that it was a site of learning and memory storage (i.e. an engram) in the brain. He reached this erroneous conclusion due to imperfect lesioning methods. |
||
By the 1950s, two separate principles had grown out of Lashley's research: [[Mass Action Principle (neuroscience)|mass action]] and [[equipotentiality]]. "Mass action" refers to the idea that the rate, efficacy and accuracy of learning depend on the amount of cortex available. If cortical tissue is destroyed following the learning of a complex task, deterioration of performance on the task is determined more by the amount of tissue destroyed than by its location.<ref name="karl">{{Cite web |url=http://www.isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k3007&panel=icb.pagecontent44003%3Ar%241%3Fname%3Dhistoricprofs.html&pageid=icb.page19708&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent44003&view=view.do&viewParam_name=lashley.html |title= |
By the 1950s, two separate principles had grown out of Lashley's research: [[Mass Action Principle (neuroscience)|mass action]] and [[equipotentiality]]. "Mass action" refers to the idea that the rate, efficacy and accuracy of learning depend on the amount of cortex available. If cortical tissue is destroyed following the learning of a complex task, deterioration of performance on the task is determined more by the amount of tissue destroyed than by its location.<ref name="karl">{{Cite web |url=http://www.isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k3007&panel=icb.pagecontent44003%3Ar%241%3Fname%3Dhistoricprofs.html&pageid=icb.page19708&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent44003&view=view.do&viewParam_name=lashley.html |title=About the Department § the Department of Psychology |access-date=2013-11-28 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120407080222/http://www.isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k3007&panel=icb.pagecontent44003%3Ar%241%3Fname%3Dhistoricprofs.html&pageid=icb.page19708&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent44003&view=view.do&viewParam_name=lashley.html |archive-date=2012-04-07 }}</ref> "Equipotentiality" refers to the idea that one part of the cortex can take over the function of another part; within a functional area of the brain, any tissue within that area can perform its associated function.<ref name="equip">{{cite book | doi=10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_729-2 | chapter=Equipotentiality | title=Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology | date=2017 | last1=Kleiner | first1=Jennifer Sue | page=1 | isbn=978-3-319-56782-2 }}</ref> Therefore, to destroy a function, all the tissue within a functional area must be destroyed. If the area is not destroyed then the cortex can take over another part. These two principles grew out of Lashley's research on the cortical basis of learning and discrimination. |
||
==Later life== |
==Later life== |
||
In February 1954, while doing his teaching at Harvard, Lashley unexpectedly collapsed and was hospitalized. He was diagnosed with [[hemolytic anemia]] and put on a [[cortisone]] treatment. This eventually began to soften his vertebrae, and as a result a [[splenectomy]] was performed. |
In February 1954, while doing his teaching at Harvard, Lashley unexpectedly collapsed and was hospitalized. He was diagnosed with [[hemolytic anemia]] and put on a [[cortisone]] treatment. This eventually began to soften his vertebrae, and as a result a [[splenectomy]] was performed. Lashley was on the road to a full recovery until his trip to France with his wife Clair, where he once again unexpectedly collapsed, but this time to his death on August 7, 1958.<ref name="beach"/> |
||
==Honors== |
==Honors== |
||
Lashley was elected to many scientific and philosophical societies, including the [[American Psychological Association]] (Council member 1926–1928; President, 1929), [[Eastern Psychological Association]] (President, 1937), Society of Experimental Psychologists, British Psychological Association (Honorary Fellow), American Society of Zoologists, American Society of Naturalists (President, 1947), British Institute for the Study of Animal Behavior (Honorary Member), American Society of Human Genetics, American Physiological Society, Harvey Society (Honorary Member), [[National Academy of Sciences]] (elected in 1930).<ref name="beach"/> In 1938, |
Lashley was elected to many scientific and philosophical societies, including the [[American Psychological Association]] (Council member 1926–1928; President, 1929), [[Eastern Psychological Association]] (President, 1937), Society of Experimental Psychologists, British Psychological Association (Honorary Fellow), American Society of Zoologists, American Society of Naturalists (President, 1947), British Institute for the Study of Animal Behavior (Honorary Member), American Society of Human Genetics, American Physiological Society, Harvey Society (Honorary Member), [[National Academy of Sciences]] (elected in 1930).<ref name="beach"/> In 1938, Lashley was elected a Member of the [[American Philosophical Society]], the oldest learned society in the United States, dating to 1743. Since 1957, the Society has awarded the annual Karl Spencer Lashley Award in recognition of work on the integrative neuroscience of behavior.<ref>"Award Ceremony: Karl Spencer Lashley Award, 2008." American Philosophical Society. {{cite web |url=http://amphilsoc.org/prizes/April2009 |title=Award Ceremony | American Philosophical Society |access-date=2010-06-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100801053117/http://www.amphilsoc.org/prizes/April2009 |archive-date=2010-08-01 }}</ref> In 1943, Lashley was awarded the [[Daniel Giraud Elliot Medal]] from the [[United States National Academy of Sciences|National Academy of Sciences]].<ref name=Elliot>{{cite web|title=Daniel Giraud Elliot Medal |url=http://www.nasonline.org/site/PageServer?pagename=AWARDS_elliot |publisher=National Academy of Sciences |access-date=15 February 2011 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120801121352/http://nas.nasonline.org/site/PageServer?pagename=AWARDS_elliot |archive-date=August 1, 2012 }}</ref> |
||
Lashley was awarded honorary Doctor of Science degrees from the [[University of Pittsburgh]] (1936), the [[University of Chicago]] (1941), [[Western Reserve University]] (1951), the [[University of Pennsylvania]]; in 1953, [[Johns Hopkins University]] presented him with an honorary Doctor of Laws degree.<ref name="beach"/> |
Lashley was awarded honorary Doctor of Science degrees from the [[University of Pittsburgh]] (1936), the [[University of Chicago]] (1941), [[Western Reserve University]] (1951), the [[University of Pennsylvania]]; in 1953, [[Johns Hopkins University]] presented him with an honorary Doctor of Laws degree.<ref name="beach"/> |
||
==Criticisms== |
==Criticisms== |
||
Lashley had a reputation as an objective scientist, but Nadine Weidman has tried to expose him as |
Lashley had a reputation as an objective scientist, but Nadine Weidman has tried to expose him as racist and a genetic determinist. However, Donald Dewsbury and others have disputed the claim that he was a genetic determinist, citing research of Lashley's in which he found evidence of both genetic and environmental influences on organisms. Dewsbury does admit, however, that Lashley was quite racist. He cites a line from a letter that Lashley wrote to a German colleague which reads: "Too bad that the beautiful tropical countries are all populated by negros. Heil Hitler and Apartheit!"<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Dewsbury | first1 = D. A. | title = Constructing representations of Karl Spencer Lashley | doi = 10.1002/jhbs.10060 | journal = Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences | volume = 38 | issue = 3 | pages = 225–245 | year = 2002 | pmid = 12115784}}</ref> This line alone would leave little debate on this matter, but he cites others as well. |
||
==Notable publications== |
==Notable publications== |
||
Line 106: | Line 103: | ||
|last=Sapetskiĭ |
|last=Sapetskiĭ |
||
|first=A O |
|first=A O |
||
|publication-date= |
|||
|year=1999 |
|year=1999 |
||
|title=[A physiologist's dialog with a psychologist] |
|title=[A physiologist's dialog with a psychologist] |
||
|volume=49 |
|volume=49 |
||
|issue=6 |
|issue=6 |
||
|periodical=Zhurnal Vyssheĭ Nervnoĭ Deiatelnosti |
|periodical=Zhurnal Vyssheĭ Nervnoĭ Deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova |
||
|pages=909–18 |
|pages=909–18 |
||
}} |
}} |
||
Line 152: | Line 148: | ||
|pages=1410–2 |
|pages=1410–2 |
||
|doi = 10.1126/science.129.3360.1410 |
|doi = 10.1126/science.129.3360.1410 |
||
|bibcode=1959Sci...129.1410C |
|||
}} |
}} |
||
*{{Citation |
*{{Citation |
||
Line 163: | Line 160: | ||
|issue=11 |
|issue=11 |
||
|periodical=Neurology |
|periodical=Neurology |
||
| |
|page=870 |
||
|doi=10.1212/wnl.8.11.870 |
|doi=10.1212/wnl.8.11.870 |
||
}} |
}} |
||
Line 191: | Line 188: | ||
[[Category:1890 births]] |
[[Category:1890 births]] |
||
[[Category:1958 deaths]] |
[[Category:1958 deaths]] |
||
[[Category:American psychologists]] |
|||
[[Category:Neuropsychologists]] |
[[Category:Neuropsychologists]] |
||
[[Category: |
[[Category:American cognitive neuroscientists]] |
||
[[Category:Memory researchers]] |
[[Category:Memory researchers]] |
||
[[Category:Animal testing]] |
[[Category:Animal testing]] |
||
[[Category:Johns Hopkins University alumni]] |
[[Category:Johns Hopkins University alumni]] |
||
[[Category:University of Chicago faculty]] |
[[Category:University of Chicago faculty]] |
||
[[Category:Harvard University faculty]] |
[[Category:Harvard University Department of Psychology faculty]] |
||
[[Category:Foreign |
[[Category:Foreign members of the Royal Society]] |
||
[[Category:Presidents of the American Psychological Association]] |
[[Category:Presidents of the American Psychological Association]] |
||
[[Category:People from Tucker County, West Virginia]] |
[[Category:People from Tucker County, West Virginia]] |
||
[[Category:20th-century psychologists]] |
[[Category:20th-century American psychologists]] |
Latest revision as of 12:30, 10 October 2024
Karl Spencer Lashley | |
---|---|
Born | June 7, 1890 |
Died | August 7, 1958 Poitiers, France | (aged 68)
Alma mater | Johns Hopkins University |
Known for | Learning and memory |
Awards | |
Scientific career | |
Fields | Psychology |
Institutions | University of Minnesota, University of Chicago, Harvard University |
Doctoral advisor | Herbert S. Jennings |
Karl Spencer Lashley (June 7, 1890 – August 7, 1958) was an American psychologist and behaviorist remembered for his contributions to the study of learning and memory. A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Lashley as the 61st most cited psychologist of the 20th century.[1][2]
Early life and education
[edit]Lashley was born on June 7, 1890, in the town of Davis, West Virginia. He was the only child of Charles and Maggie Lashley. He grew up in a middle-class family with a reasonably comfortable life. Lashley's father held various local political positions. His mother was a stay-at-home parent, and had a vast collection of books in the home. She brought in women from the community, whom she would teach various subjects. This is no doubt what gave Lashley his love of learning. Lashley has always held his family in high regard. He has said that his father was a kind man.[citation needed]
Lashley's mother was a strong advocate of schooling, and she encouraged Lashley intellectually from an early age. Lashley was a very active boy, both physically and mentally. He was able to read by the age of four. His favorite thing to do as a child was to wander through the woods and collect animals, like butterflies and mice. He spent most of his childhood alone. Lashley did not have many friends. The reasons for his lack of friendships is unclear. Lashley graduated high school at age 14.
Lashley enrolled at West Virginia University, where he had originally intended to become an English major. He took a course in zoology, however, and switched his major to zoology due to his interactions with the professor John Black Johnston. Lashley wrote, "Within a few weeks in his class I knew that I had found my life's work".[3]
After obtaining his Bachelor of Arts at West Virginia University, Lashley was awarded a teaching fellowship at the University of Pittsburgh, where he taught biology along with biological laboratories. While there he also carried out research which he used for his master's thesis.[4] Once Lashley completed his master's degree, he studied at Johns Hopkins University, where he received his PhD in genetics in June 1911. He became a professor at University of Minnesota, University of Chicago, and Harvard University.
At Hopkins, Lashley minored in psychology under John B. Watson, whom he continued to work closely with him after receiving his PhD. It was during this time that Lashley worked with Shepherd Ivory Franz and was introduced to his training/ablation method. Watson had a great deal of influence on Lashley. Together the two conducted field experiments and studied the effects of different drugs on maze learning of rats.[4] Watson helped Lashley to focus on specific problems in learning and experimental investigation, followed by locating the area of the cerebrum involved in learning and discrimination.
Career
[edit]Lashley's career began with research concerning brain mechanisms and how they were related to sense receptors. He also conducted work on instinct as well as color vision. He studied many animals and primates, which had been an interest since his freshman year at college.
Lashley worked at the University of Minnesota from 1917 to 1926 and then at the Institute for Juvenile Research in Chicago before becoming a professor at the University of Chicago. After this he went to Harvard, but was dissatisfied and from there became the director of the Yerkes Laboratory of Primate Biology in Orange Park, Florida.
Lashley's most influential research centered around the cortical basis of learning and discrimination. He researched this by looking at the measurement of behavior before and after specific, carefully quantified, induced brain damage in rats. Lashley trained rats to perform specific tasks (seeking a food reward), then lesioned specific areas of the rats' cortex, either before or after the animals received the training. The cortical lesions had specific effects on acquisition and retention of knowledge, but the location of the removed cortex had no effect on the rats' performance in the maze. This led Lashley to conclude that memories are not localized, but that they are widely distributed across the cortex. Today we know that distribution of engrams does in fact exist, but that the distribution is not equal across all cortical areas, as Lashley assumed.[5][6] His study of V1 (primary visual cortex) led him to believe that it was a site of learning and memory storage (i.e. an engram) in the brain. He reached this erroneous conclusion due to imperfect lesioning methods.
By the 1950s, two separate principles had grown out of Lashley's research: mass action and equipotentiality. "Mass action" refers to the idea that the rate, efficacy and accuracy of learning depend on the amount of cortex available. If cortical tissue is destroyed following the learning of a complex task, deterioration of performance on the task is determined more by the amount of tissue destroyed than by its location.[7] "Equipotentiality" refers to the idea that one part of the cortex can take over the function of another part; within a functional area of the brain, any tissue within that area can perform its associated function.[8] Therefore, to destroy a function, all the tissue within a functional area must be destroyed. If the area is not destroyed then the cortex can take over another part. These two principles grew out of Lashley's research on the cortical basis of learning and discrimination.
Later life
[edit]In February 1954, while doing his teaching at Harvard, Lashley unexpectedly collapsed and was hospitalized. He was diagnosed with hemolytic anemia and put on a cortisone treatment. This eventually began to soften his vertebrae, and as a result a splenectomy was performed. Lashley was on the road to a full recovery until his trip to France with his wife Clair, where he once again unexpectedly collapsed, but this time to his death on August 7, 1958.[4]
Honors
[edit]Lashley was elected to many scientific and philosophical societies, including the American Psychological Association (Council member 1926–1928; President, 1929), Eastern Psychological Association (President, 1937), Society of Experimental Psychologists, British Psychological Association (Honorary Fellow), American Society of Zoologists, American Society of Naturalists (President, 1947), British Institute for the Study of Animal Behavior (Honorary Member), American Society of Human Genetics, American Physiological Society, Harvey Society (Honorary Member), National Academy of Sciences (elected in 1930).[4] In 1938, Lashley was elected a Member of the American Philosophical Society, the oldest learned society in the United States, dating to 1743. Since 1957, the Society has awarded the annual Karl Spencer Lashley Award in recognition of work on the integrative neuroscience of behavior.[9] In 1943, Lashley was awarded the Daniel Giraud Elliot Medal from the National Academy of Sciences.[10]
Lashley was awarded honorary Doctor of Science degrees from the University of Pittsburgh (1936), the University of Chicago (1941), Western Reserve University (1951), the University of Pennsylvania; in 1953, Johns Hopkins University presented him with an honorary Doctor of Laws degree.[4]
Criticisms
[edit]Lashley had a reputation as an objective scientist, but Nadine Weidman has tried to expose him as racist and a genetic determinist. However, Donald Dewsbury and others have disputed the claim that he was a genetic determinist, citing research of Lashley's in which he found evidence of both genetic and environmental influences on organisms. Dewsbury does admit, however, that Lashley was quite racist. He cites a line from a letter that Lashley wrote to a German colleague which reads: "Too bad that the beautiful tropical countries are all populated by negros. Heil Hitler and Apartheit!"[11] This line alone would leave little debate on this matter, but he cites others as well.
Notable publications
[edit]- 1923 "The behavioristic interpretation of consciousness." Psychological Review
- 1929 "Brain mechanisms and intelligence."
- 1930 "Basic neural mechanisms in behavior." Psychological Review
- 1932 "Studies in the dynamics of behavior." University of Chicago Press.
- 1935 "The mechanism of vision", Part 12: Nervous structures concerned in the acquisition and retention of habits based on reactions to light. Comparative Psychology Monographs 11: 43–79.
- 1943 "Studies of Cerebral Function in Learning", Journal of Comparative Neurology vol. 79.
- 1950 "In search of the engram." Society of Experimental Biology Symposium 4: 454–482.
- 1951 "The problem of serial order in behavior." Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior.
See also
[edit]References
[edit]- ^ a b Bartlett, F. C. (1960). "Karl Spencer Lashley 1890-1958". Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society. 5: 107–118. doi:10.1098/rsbm.1960.0010. S2CID 73205669.
- ^ Haggbloom, Steven J.; Warnick, Renee; Warnick, Jason E.; Jones, Vinessa K.; Yarbrough, Gary L.; Russell, Tenea M.; Borecky, Chris M.; McGahhey, Reagan; Powell, John L. III; Beavers, Jamie; Monte, Emmanuelle (2002). "The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century". Review of General Psychology. 6 (2): 139–52. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.586.1913. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139. S2CID 145668721.
- ^ Beach, Frank A. (1961). Karl Spencer Lashley 1890-1958 (PDF). Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Sciences. p. 9.
- ^ a b c d e http://www.nasonline.org/publications/biographical-memoirs/memoir-pdfs/lashley-karl.pdf [bare URL PDF]
- ^ Josselyn, Sheena (July 2010). "Sheena A. Josselyn. Continuing the search for the engram: examining the mechanism of fear memories". J Psychiatry Neurosci. 35 (4): 221–8. doi:10.1503/jpn.100015. PMC 2895151. PMID 20569648.
- ^ "Karl Lashley". Archived from the original on 2019-05-11. Retrieved 2013-11-28.
- ^ "About the Department § the Department of Psychology". Archived from the original on 2012-04-07. Retrieved 2013-11-28.
- ^ Kleiner, Jennifer Sue (2017). "Equipotentiality". Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. p. 1. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_729-2. ISBN 978-3-319-56782-2.
- ^ "Award Ceremony: Karl Spencer Lashley Award, 2008." American Philosophical Society. "Award Ceremony | American Philosophical Society". Archived from the original on 2010-08-01. Retrieved 2010-06-20.
- ^ "Daniel Giraud Elliot Medal". National Academy of Sciences. Archived from the original on August 1, 2012. Retrieved 15 February 2011.
- ^ Dewsbury, D. A. (2002). "Constructing representations of Karl Spencer Lashley". Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences. 38 (3): 225–245. doi:10.1002/jhbs.10060. PMID 12115784.
Further reading
[edit]- Dewsbury, Donald A (2002), "Constructing representations of Karl Spencer Lashley.", Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 225–45, doi:10.1002/jhbs.10060, PMID 12115784
- Dewsbury, D A (2002), "The Chicago Five: a family group of integrative psychobiologists.", History of Psychology, vol. 5, no. 1 (published Feb 2002), pp. 16–37, doi:10.1037/1093-4510.5.1.16, PMID 11894885
- Sapetskiĭ, A O (1999), "[A physiologist's dialog with a psychologist]", Zhurnal Vyssheĭ Nervnoĭ Deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova, vol. 49, no. 6, pp. 909–18, PMID 10693270
- Bruce, D (1986), "Lashley's shift from bacteriology to neuropsychology, 1910–1917, and the influence of Jennings, Watson, and Franz.", Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, vol. 22, no. 1 (published Jan 1986), pp. 27–44, doi:10.1002/1520-6696(198601)22:1<27::AID-JHBS2300220104>3.0.CO;2-Y, PMID 3511136
- Roofe, P G (1970), "Some letters from the Herrick-Lashley correspondence.", Neuropsychologia, vol. 8, no. 1 (published Jan 1970), pp. 3–12, doi:10.1016/0028-3932(70)90021-7, PMID 4941968
- CARMICHAEL, L (1959), "Karl Spencer Lashley, experimental psychologist.", Science, vol. 129, no. 3360 (published May 22, 1959), pp. 1410–2, Bibcode:1959Sci...129.1410C, doi:10.1126/science.129.3360.1410, PMID 13658968
- WALSHE, F M (1958), "Karl S. Lashley.", Neurology, vol. 8, no. 11 (published Nov 1958), p. 870, doi:10.1212/wnl.8.11.870, PMID 13590401
- Weidman, Nadine (2002), "The depoliticization of Karl Lashley: A response to Dewsbury.", Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 247–53, discussion 255–7, doi:10.1002/jhbs.10061, PMID 12115785
External links
[edit]- 1890 births
- 1958 deaths
- Neuropsychologists
- American cognitive neuroscientists
- Memory researchers
- Animal testing
- Johns Hopkins University alumni
- University of Chicago faculty
- Harvard University Department of Psychology faculty
- Foreign members of the Royal Society
- Presidents of the American Psychological Association
- People from Tucker County, West Virginia
- 20th-century American psychologists