Personality development: Difference between revisions
Added genetics information |
Added genetics information |
||
Line 38: | Line 38: | ||
<big>Genetics can have an impact on ones development of personality. Genes are passed on from one generation to the next and contain characteristics of one's being. Personality can be influenced through many genes acting together. Behavioral genetics refers to the results of adoption studies and twin studies.</big> |
<big>Genetics can have an impact on ones development of personality. Genes are passed on from one generation to the next and contain characteristics of one's being. Personality can be influenced through many genes acting together. Behavioral genetics refers to the results of adoption studies and twin studies.</big> |
||
# <big>Adoption Studies: Genetics are not very highly correlated with adoptive families and their personalities. Studies have been preformed comparing adoptive siblings in a family to those who were biological siblings. The findings were that there was a 0.05 correlation between biological siblings and the personalities of those in the family.</big> |
# <big>Adoption Studies: Genetics are not very highly correlated with adoptive families and their personalities. Studies have been preformed comparing adoptive siblings in a family to those who were biological siblings. The findings were that there was a 0.05 correlation between biological siblings and the personalities of those in the family. With adoptive siblings there was a 0.04 correlation between their personalities and the others in the family. This shows that there is no supporting evidence that for genetic differences in personality in relation to a common environment. Similar correlations were associated with parents and their adoptive children compared to their biological children. <ref>{{Cite web |title=ILLiad - Logon |url=https://illiad.mst.edu/illiad/illiad.dll?Action=10&Form=72&Value=316721 |access-date=2023-04-22 |website=illiad.mst.edu}}</ref></big> |
||
Twin and adoption studies have demonstrated that the [[heritability]] of personality traits ranges from .3-.6, with a mean of .5, indicating that 50% of variation in observable personality traits is attributable to genetic influences.<ref name="Harkness, A. R. 19973">{{cite journal|vauthors=Harkness AR, Lilienfeld SO|year=1997|title=Individual differences science for treatment planning: Personality traits|journal=Psychological Assessment|volume=9|issue=4|pages=349–360|doi=10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.349|s2cid=197657764 }}</ref> In contrast, family and adoption studies have demonstrated a low heritability factor of .22.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Vukasović|first1=Tena|last2=Bratko|first2=Denis|date=2015|title=Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|language=en|volume=141|issue=4|pages=769–785|doi=10.1037/bul0000017|issn=1939-1455|pmid=25961374}}</ref> A study conducted on German women using an IAT (implicit association test), shows a connection between the function of specific neurotransmitters and the predisposition to have certain personality traits like anxiety or extraversion.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Osinsky|first1=Roman|last2=Alexander|first2=Nina|last3=Schmitz|first3=Anja|last4=Kuepper|first4=Yvonne|last5=Müller|first5=Eva|last6=Beer|first6=Laura|last7=Koeppe|first7=Lena|last8=Hennig|first8=Jürgen|date=January 2010|title=Genetic Influences on Implicit Measures of Personality|journal=Journal of Individual Differences|language=en|volume=31|issue=3|pages=115–123|doi=10.1027/1614-0001/a000007|issn=1614-0001}}</ref> With the effects of genetic similarity removed, children from the same family often appear no more alike than randomly selected strangers;<ref>Turkheimer, E. & Waldron, M. (2000). Non shared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review.</ref> yet, identical twins raised apart are nearly as similar in personality as identical twins raised together. These findings suggest is that shared family environment has virtually no effect on personality development, and that similarity between relatives is almost entirely due to shared genetics.<ref name="Harkness, A. R. 19973"/> |
Twin and adoption studies have demonstrated that the [[heritability]] of personality traits ranges from .3-.6, with a mean of .5, indicating that 50% of variation in observable personality traits is attributable to genetic influences.<ref name="Harkness, A. R. 19973">{{cite journal|vauthors=Harkness AR, Lilienfeld SO|year=1997|title=Individual differences science for treatment planning: Personality traits|journal=Psychological Assessment|volume=9|issue=4|pages=349–360|doi=10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.349|s2cid=197657764 }}</ref> In contrast, family and adoption studies have demonstrated a low heritability factor of .22.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Vukasović|first1=Tena|last2=Bratko|first2=Denis|date=2015|title=Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|language=en|volume=141|issue=4|pages=769–785|doi=10.1037/bul0000017|issn=1939-1455|pmid=25961374}}</ref> A study conducted on German women using an IAT (implicit association test), shows a connection between the function of specific neurotransmitters and the predisposition to have certain personality traits like anxiety or extraversion.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Osinsky|first1=Roman|last2=Alexander|first2=Nina|last3=Schmitz|first3=Anja|last4=Kuepper|first4=Yvonne|last5=Müller|first5=Eva|last6=Beer|first6=Laura|last7=Koeppe|first7=Lena|last8=Hennig|first8=Jürgen|date=January 2010|title=Genetic Influences on Implicit Measures of Personality|journal=Journal of Individual Differences|language=en|volume=31|issue=3|pages=115–123|doi=10.1027/1614-0001/a000007|issn=1614-0001}}</ref> With the effects of genetic similarity removed, children from the same family often appear no more alike than randomly selected strangers;<ref>Turkheimer, E. & Waldron, M. (2000). Non shared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review.</ref> yet, identical twins raised apart are nearly as similar in personality as identical twins raised together. These findings suggest is that shared family environment has virtually no effect on personality development, and that similarity between relatives is almost entirely due to shared genetics.<ref name="Harkness, A. R. 19973"/> |
Revision as of 18:28, 9 May 2023
Personality development encompasses the dynamic construction and deconstruction of integrative characteristics that distinguish an individual in terms of interpersonal behavioral traits.[1] Personality development is ever-changing and subject to contextual factors and life-altering experiences. Personality development is also dimensional in description and subjective in nature.[2] That is, personality development can be seen as a continuum varying in degrees of intensity and change. It is subjective in nature because its conceptualization is rooted in social norms of expected behavior, self-expression, and personal growth.[3][4] The dominant viewpoint in personality psychology indicates that personality emerges early and continues to develop across one's lifespan.[5] Adult personality traits are believed to have a basis in infant temperament, meaning that individual differences in disposition and behavior appear early in life, potentially before language of conscious self-representation develop.[6] The Five Factor Model of personality maps onto the dimensions of childhood temperament.[7] This suggests that individual differences in levels of the corresponding personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) are present from young ages.[8]
Theories
The development of personality is supported and attempted to be explained by theories of personality.
Psychoanalytic Theory
The Psychoanalytic Theory of personality was developed by Sigmund Freud. This theory consists of three main ideas that make up personality, the id, the ego, and the superego. The three traits control their own sections of the psyche. Personality is developed by the three traits that make up the Psychoanalytic theory conflicting.
Trait Theory
The Trait Theory of personality is one of the main theories in the study of personality. According to this theory, traits make up personality. Traits can be described as patterns of behavior, thought, or emotion. Some commonly accepted trait theories are the Big Five personality traits and the HEXACO model of personality structure. Generally, strong correlations are seen in the levels of any given personality trait in an individual when they are retested several years later. Traits tend to become more stable after young adulthood, and changes in these traits often follow some noticeable trends with age. For example, the trait Honesty-Humility is typically seen to decrease during teenage years, then steadily rise as the individual ages. The trait conscientiousness is generally seen to increase with age, however, the level of the facet perfectionism stays fairly consistent. [9]
Social Cognitive Theory
The social cognitive theory of personality views personality development in terms of reciprocal interactionism, that is, a perspective that considers the relationship of person-society as an interactive system that defines and molds personal development. Personal interaction with other individuals, society, and nature create experiences in which self-identification is organized in relation to social environment.[10] In other words, personality traits are a function of complex cognitive strategies used to effectively maneuver through social situations. Furthermore, according to the social-cognitive perspective, cognitive processes are central to an individual's unique expression of personality traits and affective processes. Through cognitive mechanism and social competencies, individuals interpret contextual situations to derive beliefs that guide their thoughts and behaviors, thus developing an enduring pattern of personality traits.[11][10]
Evolutionary Theory
The evolutionary theory of personality development is primarily based on the evolutionary process of natural selection.[12] From the evolutionary perspective, evolution resulted in variations of the human mind. Natural selection refined these variations based on their beneficence to humans. Due to human complexity, many opposing personality traits proved to be beneficial in a variety of ways.[13] Primitive humans were collectivists due to tribe culture. The personalities of individuals within a tribe were very similar. The division of labor resulted in differentiation in personality traits in order to achieve a higher efficiency. Differentiation in personality traits increased functionality, therefore becoming adaptive through natural selection. Humans continued to develop personality and individuality through evolution.[14]
Lifespan Theory
Classic theories of personality include Freud's tripartite theory and post-Freudian theory (developmental stage theories and type theories) and indicate that most personality development occurs in childhood, stabilizing by the end of adolescence. Current lifespan perspectives that integrate theory and empirical findings dominate the research literature. The lifespan perspectives of personality are based on the plasticity principle, the principle that personality traits are open systems that can be influenced by the environment at any age.[5] Large-scale longitudinal studies have demonstrated that the most active period of personality development appears to be between the ages of 20–40.[5] Although personality grows increasingly consistent with age and typically plateaus near age 50, personality never reached a period of total stability.[15][16]
Humanistic Theory
Humanistic psychology emphasizes individual choices as voluntary actions that ultimately determine personal development. Individual personalities traits, although essential to the integrated self, are only parts that make up the whole of observable human experiences. Thus, personality development is articulated in terms of purposeful action geared towards experiencing mastery of free choice. Rather than compartmentalized elements of personality traits such as feelings, thoughts, or behavior, Humanistic psychology integrates these elements as functions of being in a greater encompassing system such as societies, cultures, or interpersonal relationships. Consequently, personality development is subjected to shifts in personal meaning and individual goals of achieving an ideal self.[17]
Influencing factors
Personality traits demonstrate moderate levels of continuity, smaller but still significant normative or mean-level changes, and individual differences in change, often late into the life course.[18] This pattern is influenced by genetic, environmental, transactional, and stochastic factors.[19]
Genetics
Genetics can have an impact on ones development of personality. Genes are passed on from one generation to the next and contain characteristics of one's being. Personality can be influenced through many genes acting together. Behavioral genetics refers to the results of adoption studies and twin studies.
- Adoption Studies: Genetics are not very highly correlated with adoptive families and their personalities. Studies have been preformed comparing adoptive siblings in a family to those who were biological siblings. The findings were that there was a 0.05 correlation between biological siblings and the personalities of those in the family. With adoptive siblings there was a 0.04 correlation between their personalities and the others in the family. This shows that there is no supporting evidence that for genetic differences in personality in relation to a common environment. Similar correlations were associated with parents and their adoptive children compared to their biological children. [20]
Twin and adoption studies have demonstrated that the heritability of personality traits ranges from .3-.6, with a mean of .5, indicating that 50% of variation in observable personality traits is attributable to genetic influences.[21] In contrast, family and adoption studies have demonstrated a low heritability factor of .22.[22] A study conducted on German women using an IAT (implicit association test), shows a connection between the function of specific neurotransmitters and the predisposition to have certain personality traits like anxiety or extraversion.[23] With the effects of genetic similarity removed, children from the same family often appear no more alike than randomly selected strangers;[24] yet, identical twins raised apart are nearly as similar in personality as identical twins raised together. These findings suggest is that shared family environment has virtually no effect on personality development, and that similarity between relatives is almost entirely due to shared genetics.[21]
Environmental
The weakness of shared environmental effects in shaping personality surprised many psychologists, spurring research into non-shared environmental effects, the environmental influences that distinguish siblings from one another.[25] The non-shared environment may include differential treatment by parents, individually-distinct reactions to the shared family environment, peer influences, experiences outside the family, and test error in measurement. In adults, the non-shared environment may also include the unique roles and environments experienced after leaving the family of origin.[26] Further effects of environment in adulthood are demonstrated by research suggesting that different work, marital, and family experiences are associated with personality change;[27] these effects are supported by research involving the impact of major positive and negative life events on personality.[28][29]
Family and Childhood Experiences:
Family and childhood experiences can have a significant impact on the development of an individual's personality. Here are some ways in which family and childhood experiences can affect personality development:
- Attachment style: Attachment refers to the emotional bond that an infant develops with their primary caregiver. The quality of this attachment can influence an individual's personality development. For example, individuals who develop a secure attachment style may be more likely to have positive relationships with others, while those who develop an insecure attachment style may be more likely to struggle with relationships.[30]
- Parenting style: Parenting style refers to the way in which parents interact with their children. Different parenting styles can have different effects on an individual's personality development. For example, authoritarian parents, who are highly controlling and demanding, may lead to individuals who are less independent and less self-confident, while authoritative parents, who are warm and supportive but also set clear expectations and limits, may lead to individuals who are more self-confident and have better social skills.[31]
- Family dynamics: Family dynamics, such as the level of conflict, cohesion, and communication within a family, can also affect personality development. For example, individuals who grow up in families with high levels of conflict may be more likely to experience anxiety and depression, while those who grow up in families with supportive and nurturing relationships may be more resilient and better able to cope with stress.[32]
- Trauma and adversity: Childhood experiences of trauma, such as abuse, neglect, or exposure to violence, can have significant long-term effects on personality development. Individuals who experience trauma may be more likely to experience mental health issues such as anxiety and depression, and may also struggle with relationships and trust.[33]
- Cultural and socio-economic background: Cultural and socio-economic background can also influence personality development. For example, individuals from collectivistic cultures, which emphasize the importance of group harmony and interdependence, may have different personality traits than those from individualistic cultures, which emphasize independence and self-achievement. Similarly, individuals from low-income backgrounds may be more likely to experience stress and adversity, which can affect their personality development.[34]
Overall, family and childhood experiences play a critical role in personality development. Understanding the impact of these experiences is essential for promoting healthy development and providing support to individuals who may have experienced trauma or adversity.[35]
Peer Relationships:
Peer relationships can have a significant impact on the development of an individual's personality. Peer relationships refer to the interactions and social connections that an individual has with their peers, such as friends, classmates, and acquaintances. Here are some ways in which peer relationships can affect personality development:
- Socialization: Peer relationships provide opportunities for socialization, which is the process of learning and internalizing social norms, values, and expectations. Through interactions with peers, individuals learn how to behave in social situations, develop communication and negotiation skills, and learn to regulate their emotions and behaviors in ways that are acceptable to others.[36]
- Identity formation: Peer relationships can also influence the development of an individual's identity. Adolescence is a time when individuals are trying to define who they are and what they stand for. Through interactions with peers, individuals can explore different aspects of themselves and develop a sense of identity.[37]
- Risk-taking behavior: Peer relationships can also influence risk-taking behavior. Adolescents who have peers who engage in risky behaviors, such as drug use or delinquency, may be more likely to engage in these behaviors themselves. On the other hand, adolescents who have peers who engage in prosocial behaviors, such as volunteering or academic achievement, are more likely to engage in these behaviors themselves.[38]
Gene-environment interactions
A culmination of research suggests that the development of personality occurs in relation to one's genetics, one's environment, and the interaction between one's genetics and environment.[39][40] Van Gestel and Van Broeckhoven (2003) write, “Almost by definition, complex traits originate from interplay between (multiple) genetic factors and environment.”[41] The corresponsive principle of personality development states that “life experiences may accentuate and reinforce the personality characteristics that were partially responsible for the particular environmental elicitations in the first place”.[42] This principle illustrates how gene-environment interactions maintain and reinforce personality throughout the lifespan. Three main types of gene-environment interactions are active (the process by which individuals with certain genotypes select and create environments that facilitate the expression of those genotypes), passive (the process by which genetic parents provide both the genes and the early environmental influences that contribute to the development of a characteristic in their children), and reactive (the process by which non-family individuals respond to the behavior produced by a genotype in characteristic ways).[21][28]
An example of the way environment can moderate the expression of a gene is the finding by Heath, Eaves, and Martin (1998) that marriage was a protective factor against depression in identical twins, such that the heritability of depression was as low as 29% in a married twin and as high as 51% in an unmarried twin.[43]
Stability of personality
Over the course of an individual's lifespan, the stability of their personality has been shown in a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies to be variable, although this variability levels out in adulthood. The beginning of one's personality stability is most evident at the age of 25 years.[44] Behavioral genetics can account for the variability experienced across the lifespan. This is highly evident in the transitions between childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. From childhood to mid-adolescence, the rate of individual differences in personality increases, primarily due to environmental influences. However, genetic influences play a larger role than environmental influences in adulthood, resulting in fewer individual differences in personality between individuals who share similar genetics.[45] The stability of personality across one's lifespan is further evidenced by a longitudinal study conducted on individuals across the span of fifty years from adolescence through adulthood. The results of this longitudinal study suggested that the personality was malleable, although variations in the level of malleability stabilized in adulthood.[46]
A study published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology examined personality developing in college students based on the Big Five personality trait domains and facets within those domains. The results suggested that the rank-order stabilities of facets were high, with values greater than .50 (indicating a strong correlation); the results for trait domains were similar to individual facets.[47] High rank-order stability is further evidenced by another study that integrated personality structure, process, and development. This study included previous research that indicated high-order rank stability; it also included research that indicated variation in this stability across periods of the lifespan, such as adolescence and adulthood.[48] The stability and variation of personality is explained by a complex interaction between one's genetics and one's environment.[45]
References
- ^ Wrzus, Cornelia (2021), "Processes of personality development: An update of the TESSERA framework", The Handbook of Personality Dynamics and Processes, Elsevier, pp. 101–123, doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-813995-0.00005-4, ISBN 9780128139950, S2CID 234237414, retrieved 2022-01-21
- ^ Hang, Yuzhan; Soto, Christopher J.; Lee, Billy; Mõttus, René (2021-04-02). "Social Expectations and Abilities to Meet Them as Possible Mechanisms of Youth Personality Development". dx.doi.org. doi:10.31234/osf.io/8yp6x. S2CID 236660320. Retrieved 2022-01-21.
- ^ BILOHUR, V. (2019). Formation and Development of Personality Established by Change and Growth. Humanities Bulletin of Zaporizhzhe State Engineering Academy, 76, 77–89.
- ^ Caspi A, Roberts BW (2001). "Personality development across the life course: The argument for change and continuity". Psychological Inquiry. 12 (2): 49–66. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1202_01. S2CID 144947217.
- ^ a b c Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2010). The development of personality traits in adulthood. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervi (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 375-398). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- ^ Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). "Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122–135. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID 10653510.
- ^ Putnam SP, Ellis LK, Rothbart MK (2001). Eliasz A, Angleitner A (eds.). "The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence". Advances in Research on Temperament. Germany: Pabst Science: 165–182.
- ^ Deal JE, Halverson CF, Havill V, Martin R (2005). "Temperament factors as longitudinal predictors of young adult personality". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 51 (3): 315–334. doi:10.1353/mpq.2005.0015. S2CID 143953664.
- ^ Ashton, Michael (2018). Individual Differences and Personality. 125 London Wall, London EC2Y 5AS, United Kingdom: Elsevier. pp. 91–106. ISBN 978-0-12-809845-5.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location (link) - ^ a b Cervone, Daniel; Shadel, William G.; Jencius, Simon (February 2001). "Social-Cognitive Theory of Personality Assessment". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 5 (1): 33–51. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_3. ISSN 1088-8683. S2CID 16223943.
- ^ "The Social Cognitive Theory". sphweb.bumc.bu.edu. Retrieved 2020-04-30.
- ^ Hettema PJ, Deary IJ (11 November 2013). Foundations of Personality. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 7. ISBN 978-94-011-1660-2.
- ^ Penke L, Denissen JJ, Miller GF (August 2007). "The Evolution of Personality Variation in Humans and Other Animals" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 21 (5): 549–587. doi:10.1002/per.629. ISSN 0890-2070.
- ^ Bell, M. G. (2010, January 1). Consciousness: The Evolution Of The Self And Personal Individuality. Retrieved November 1, 2014, from www.agenthuman.com/product/evolution_self_personal_individuality.html#selfevol
- ^ Roberts BW, Mroczek D (February 2008). "Personality Trait Change in Adulthood". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (1): 31–35. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. PMC 2743415. PMID 19756219.
- ^ Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF (January 2000). "The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. PMID 10668348.
- ^ Taylor, Eugene (July 2000). ""What is Man, Psychologist, that Thou Art So Unmindful of him?": Henry A. Murray on the Historical Relation between Classical Personality Theory and Humanistic Psychology". Journal of Humanistic Psychology. 40 (3): 29–42. doi:10.1177/0022167800403003. ISSN 0022-1678. S2CID 143696226.
- ^ Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt T (2001). "The kids are alright: Growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (4): 670–683. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.670. PMID 11642353.
- ^ Fraley RC, Roberts BW (January 2005). "Patterns of continuity: a dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course". Psychological Review. 112 (1): 60–74. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.1.60. PMID 15631588.
- ^ "ILLiad - Logon". illiad.mst.edu. Retrieved 2023-04-22.
- ^ a b c Harkness AR, Lilienfeld SO (1997). "Individual differences science for treatment planning: Personality traits". Psychological Assessment. 9 (4): 349–360. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.349. S2CID 197657764.
- ^ Vukasović, Tena; Bratko, Denis (2015). "Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies". Psychological Bulletin. 141 (4): 769–785. doi:10.1037/bul0000017. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 25961374.
- ^ Osinsky, Roman; Alexander, Nina; Schmitz, Anja; Kuepper, Yvonne; Müller, Eva; Beer, Laura; Koeppe, Lena; Hennig, Jürgen (January 2010). "Genetic Influences on Implicit Measures of Personality". Journal of Individual Differences. 31 (3): 115–123. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000007. ISSN 1614-0001.
- ^ Turkheimer, E. & Waldron, M. (2000). Non shared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review.
- ^ Plomin R, Daniels D (June 2011). "Why are children in the same family so different from one another?". International Journal of Epidemiology. 40 (3): 563–82. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00055941. PMC 3147063. PMID 21807642.
- ^ Plomin R, Asbury K, Dunn J (April 2001). "Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later". Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 46 (3): 225–33. doi:10.1177/070674370104600302. PMID 11320676.
- ^ Roberts BW, Wood D, Smith JL (2005). "Evaluating Five Factor Theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development". Journal of Research in Personality. 39: 166–184. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.08.002.
- ^ a b Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). "Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305.
- ^ Jeronimus BF, Ormel J, Aleman A, Penninx BW, Riese H (November 2013). "Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism". Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID 23410535. S2CID 43717734.
- ^ Fuchshuber, Jürgen; Hiebler-Ragger, Michaela; Kresse, Adelheid; Kapfhammer, Hans-Peter; Unterrainer, Human Friedrich (2019). "The Influence of Attachment Styles and Personality Organization on Emotional Functioning After Childhood Trauma". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 10. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00643/full. ISSN 1664-0640.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Sun, Yanshu; Wilkinson, Jeffrey (12/20/2018). [file:///Users/loganfuchs/Downloads/11226-44998-1-PB%20(1).pdf "Parenting Style, Personality Traits, and Interpersonal Relationships: A Model of Prediction of Internet Addiction"] (PDF). International Journal of Communication: 26.
{{cite journal}}
: Check|url=
value (help); Check date values in:|date=
(help); line feed character in|title=
at position 70 (help) - ^ Schofield TJ, Conger RD, Donnellan MB, Jochem R, Widaman KF, Conger KJ. Parent Personality and Positive Parenting as Predictors of Positive Adolescent Personality Development Over Time. Merrill Palmer Q (Wayne State Univ Press). 2012 Apr;58(2):255-283. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2012.0008. PMID: 22822287; PMCID: PMC3399663.
- ^ Bozzatello, Paola; Rocca, Paola; Baldassarri, Lorenzo; Bosia, Marco; Bellino, Silvio (2021). "The Role of Trauma in Early Onset Borderline Personality Disorder: A Biopsychosocial Perspective". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 12. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.721361/full. ISSN 1664-0640.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Deckers, Thomas; Falk, Arimin; Kosse, Fabian; Schildberg-Hörisch, Hannah (April 2015). "How Does Socio-Economic Status Shape a Child's Personality?" (PDF). Institute for the Study of Labor. 8977: 38.
- ^ Daines, Chantel L.; Hansen, Dustin; Novilla, M. Lelinneth B.; Crandall, AliceAnn (2021-04-05). "Effects of positive and negative childhood experiences on adult family health". BMC Public Health. 21 (1): 651. doi:10.1186/s12889-021-10732-w. ISSN 1471-2458. PMC 8022401. PMID 33820532.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ "4.1: Socialization and Culture". Social Sci LibreTexts. 2020-07-20. Retrieved 2023-02-18.
- ^ Topolewska-Siedzik, Ewa; Cieciuch, Jan (2018). "Trajectories of Identity Formation Modes and Their Personality Context in Adolescence". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 47 (4): 775–792. doi:10.1007/s10964-018-0824-7. ISSN 0047-2891. PMC 5852173. PMID 29492870.
- ^ Shan, Xiaoyue; Zölitz, Ulf (2022-05-19). "Peers Affect Personality Development". Rochester, NY.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Baumert, Anna; Schmitt, Manfred; Perugini, Marco; Johnson, Wendy; Blum, Gabriela; Borkenau, Peter; Costantini, Giulio; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Fleeson, William; Grafton, Ben; Jayawickreme, Eranda (2017). "Integrating Personality Structure, Personality Process, and Personality Development" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 31 (5): 503–528. doi:10.1002/per.2115. hdl:20.500.11820/7cbc3cf8-c406-4216-a805-a0b131b143dd. ISSN 1099-0984. S2CID 148637952.
- ^ South SC, Krueger RF (2008). "An interactionist perspective on genetic and environmental contributions to personality". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2 (2): 929–948. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00062.x.
- ^ Van Gestel S, Van Broeckhoven C (October 2003). "Genetics of personality: are we making progress?". Molecular Psychiatry. 8 (10): 840–52. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001367. PMID 14515135.
- ^ Donnellan, M. B. & Robins, R. W. (2009). The development of personality across the lifespan. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 191-204). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
- ^ Heath AC, Eaves LJ, Martin NG (September 1998). "Interaction of marital status and genetic risk for symptoms of depression". Twin Research. 1 (3): 119–22. doi:10.1375/136905298320566249. PMID 10051335.
- ^ Bleidorn, Wiebke; Schwaba, Ted; Zheng, Anqing; Hopwood, Christopher J.; Sosa, Susana S.; Roberts, Brent W.; Briley, D. A. (2022-07-14). "Personality stability and change: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies". Psychological Bulletin. doi:10.1037/bul0000365. ISSN 1939-1455.
- ^ a b Mõttus R, Briley DA, Zheng A, Mann FD, Engelhardt LE, Tackett JL, et al. (September 2019). "Kids becoming less alike: A behavioral genetic analysis of developmental increases in personality variance from childhood to adolescence". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (3): 635–658. doi:10.1037/pspp0000194. PMC 6687565. PMID 30920282.
- ^ Damian, Rodica Ioana; Spengler, Marion; Sutu, Andreea; Roberts, Brent W. (September 2019). "Sixteen going on sixty-six: A longitudinal study of personality stability and change across 50 years". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (3): 674–695. doi:10.1037/pspp0000210. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 30113194. S2CID 52013579.
- ^ Klimstra, Theo A.; Noftle, Erik E.; Luyckx, Koen; Goossens, Luc; Robins, Richard W. (August 2018). "Personality development and adjustment in college: A multifaceted, cross-national view" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 115 (2): 338–361. doi:10.1037/pspp0000205. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 30024246. S2CID 51699611.
- ^ Baumert, Anna; Schmitt, Manfred; Perugini, Marco; Johnson, Wendy; Blum, Gabriela; Borkenau, Peter; Costantini, Giulio; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Fleeson, William; Grafton, Ben; Jayawickreme, Eranda (2017). "Integrating Personality Structure, Personality Process, and Personality Development" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 31 (5): 503–528. doi:10.1002/per.2115. hdl:20.500.11820/7cbc3cf8-c406-4216-a805-a0b131b143dd. ISSN 1099-0984. S2CID 148637952.