Social defeat: Difference between revisions
Viralmemesis (talk | contribs) removed idiomatic expressions - clarified |
Viralmemesis (talk | contribs) idioms, terminology, clarity, focus - article needs a rewrite |
||
Line 8: | Line 8: | ||
Social psychology approaches to human [[aggression]] have developed a multitude of perspectives, based on observations of human phenomena like bullying, mobbing, physical and verbal abuse, relational and indirect aggression, etc. Despite the richness of theories developed, the body of knowledge generated have not satisfied scientific requirements of [[testability]] and [[falsifiability|verifiability]]. |
Social psychology approaches to human [[aggression]] have developed a multitude of perspectives, based on observations of human phenomena like bullying, mobbing, physical and verbal abuse, relational and indirect aggression, etc. Despite the richness of theories developed, the body of knowledge generated have not satisfied scientific requirements of [[testability]] and [[falsifiability|verifiability]]. |
||
Animal studies of within-species aggression developed in 2 main branches: A) approaches based on laboratory experiments, on controlled conditions, allowing the measurement of behavioral, [[endocrine]] and [[neurological]] variables, but with the shortcoming of applying unnatural stressors (such as foot-shocks and restraint stress) in unnatural conditions (laboratory cages rarely approximate native habitats); B) approaches based on observations of animals in naturalistic settings, which avoided artificial environments and unnatural stresses, but usually not allowing the measurement of physiological effects or the manipulation of relevant variables. |
|||
In real life situations, animals (as well as humans) have to cope with [[Stress (biological)|stresses]] generated within their own species, during their interactions with conspecifics, especially due to recurrent struggles over the control of limited resources, mates and [[Social hierarchy|social positions]] (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Rohde, 2001; Allen & Badcock, 2003). |
In real life situations, animals (as well as humans) have to cope with [[Stress (biological)|stresses]] generated within their own species, during their interactions with conspecifics, especially due to recurrent struggles over the control of limited resources, mates and [[Social hierarchy|social positions]] (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Rohde, 2001; Allen & Badcock, 2003). |
Revision as of 03:07, 3 April 2012
Part of a series on |
Psychology |
---|
Social defeat refers to losing a confrontation among conspecific animals, or any kind of hostile dispute among humans, in either a dyadic or in a group-individual context, potentially generating very significant practical and psychological consequences in terms of control over resources, access to mates and social positions.
Background
Research on social stress has accumulated a useful body of knowledge, providing perspective on the effects of detrimental social and environmental interaction on the brain. Research and experimentation suffer from many methodological difficulties: usually a lack of ecological validity (similarity with natural conditions and stressors) or are not amenable to scientific investigation (difficult to test and verify).
Social psychology approaches to human aggression have developed a multitude of perspectives, based on observations of human phenomena like bullying, mobbing, physical and verbal abuse, relational and indirect aggression, etc. Despite the richness of theories developed, the body of knowledge generated have not satisfied scientific requirements of testability and verifiability.
Animal studies of within-species aggression developed in 2 main branches: A) approaches based on laboratory experiments, on controlled conditions, allowing the measurement of behavioral, endocrine and neurological variables, but with the shortcoming of applying unnatural stressors (such as foot-shocks and restraint stress) in unnatural conditions (laboratory cages rarely approximate native habitats); B) approaches based on observations of animals in naturalistic settings, which avoided artificial environments and unnatural stresses, but usually not allowing the measurement of physiological effects or the manipulation of relevant variables.
In real life situations, animals (as well as humans) have to cope with stresses generated within their own species, during their interactions with conspecifics, especially due to recurrent struggles over the control of limited resources, mates and social positions (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Rohde, 2001; Allen & Badcock, 2003).
Social defeat is a source of chronic stress in animals and humans, capable of causing significant changes in behaviour, brain functioning, physiology, neurotransmitter and hormone levels, and health (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Rohde, 2001; Allen & Badcock, 2003).
History
The social defeat approach was originated from animal experiments, using the "resident-intruder" paradigm, in which an animal was placed in the cage of another animal or group of animals of the same species, in a manner that allowed a non-lethal conflict.
If animals are allowed to fight on a single occasion only, it is usually regarded as a model of acute stress; if they are allowed to fight on several different occasions, on different days, consecutive or not, it is regarded as a model of chronic stress. After the defeat or in the interval between fights, the subordinate animal may also be exposed to threats from the dominant one, by having to stay in a cage or compartment beside or nearby the dominant, exposed to its visual or olfactory cues.
Later, the social defeat approach was also applied to observations of animal within-species aggression, in the wild, which suggested that the hypotheses generated on artificial laboratory settings can also be applied in observed in natural settings, confirming the predictions of the model.
In humans
It has been proposed that animal models of social conflict may be useful for studying a number of mental disorders, including major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, drug abuse, aggressive psychopathologies, eating disorders and schizophrenia (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Selten & Cantor-Graae, 2005; Rohde, 2001).
The social defeat model has been extended to include observations of human aggression, bullying, relational aggression, chronic subordination and humiliation. The social defeat model attempts to extend animal studies to include human behaviour as well, in contrast to the social psychology study of aggression, in which comparisons are drawn exclusively from experiments involving humans (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Rohde, 2001).
Bullying has interesting parallels with animal models of social defeat, the bully being equivalent to the dominant animal and the victim the subordinate one. At stake are possessions of material objects, money, etc, social position in the group, represented by in-group prestige, and the consequent lack of access to mates, including for socio-sexual behaviors like copulation. Human victims typically experience symptoms like low self-esteem (due to low regard by the group), feelings of depression (due to unworthiness of efforts), social withdrawal (reduced investments in the social environment), anxiety (due to a threatening environment), and they can also be shown to experience a plethora of physiological effects, e.g. increased corticosterone levels, and also a shift towards sympathetic balance in the autonomic nervous system (Bjorkqvist, 2001).
Research about human aggression, usually conducted by psychologists or social psychologists, resembles to a great extent the research about social defeat and animal aggression, usually conducted by biologists or physiological psychologists. However, there is the problem of the use of different terminologies for similar concepts, which hinders communication between the two bodies of knowledge (Bjorkqvist, 2001).
Similarly, research on depression has employed similar constructs, such as learned helplessness, although that theory is focused on the perceived inability to escape any sort of negative stimuli rather than on social factors.
Behavioral and Physiological effects
Social defeat is a very potent stressor and can lead to a variety of behavioral effects, like social withdrawal (reduced interactions with conspecifics), lethargy (reduced locomotor activity), reduced exploratory behavior (of both open field and novel objects), anhedonia (reduced reward-related behaviors), decreased socio-sexual behaviors (including decreased attempts to mate and copulate after defeat), various motivational deficits, decreased levels of testosterone (due to a decline in the functionality of the Laydig cells of the testes), increased tendencies to stereotyped behaviours and self-administration of drugs and alcohol (Rygula et alli, 2005; Huhman, 2006).
Research also implicates that the referred behavioral effects are moderated by neuroendocrine phenomena involving serotonin, dopamine, epinephrine, norepinephrine, and in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, locus ceruleus and limbic systems (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Rygula et alli, 2005; Selten & Cantor-Graae, 2005; Marinia et alli, 2006; Huhman, 2006).
Both animal and human studies suggest that the social environment has a strong influence on the consequences of stresses. This finding seems to be especially true in the case of social stresses, like social defeat (Bjorkqvist, 2001; Rygula et alli, 2005; de Jong et alli, 2005).
In animal studies, animals housed collectively showed reduced symptoms after defeat, in comparison with those housed alone; and animals that live in more stable groups (with stable hierarchies, less intra-group aggression) exhibit reduced effects after a defeat, in comparison with those housed in a more unstable group (de Jong et alli, 2005).
In human studies, individuals with greater support seem to be protected against excessive neuroendocrine activation, thereby reducing the adverse effects of stresses in general, and especially stresses of social origin.
This apparent confusion, in which social defeat generates behavioral and neuroendocrine effects, both of which depending on social contextual variables, raises the question of how to interpret this data. A useful concept is the concept of “causal chain”, in which recurrent evolutionary events, in this case intra-specific competition, generates selective pressures that last for thousands of generations, influencing a whole species. This way physiological phenomena may evolve, in this case the referred neuro-endocrine phenomena, to facilitate adaptive patterns of action by individuals, in this case the referred behavioral effects. According to this framework, selective pressures generated by intra-specific competition can be considered as the ultimate cause, the neuroendocrine phenomena can be considered to be the proximate causes (sometimes also called mechanisms or moderators) and the observed behavioral alterations are considered the effects (the end events in the causal chain)(Gilbert et alli, 2002; Allen & Badcock, 2003; Rygula et alli, 2005).
Some authors, for example Randolph Nesse, warn us that patterns of behavior commonly considered inappropriate or even pathological may well have adaptive value. According to this view, several authors propose that behaviors considered abnormal may be in fact part of an adaptive response to stressors in modern or at least in old environments, for example social stressors from chronic subordination or interpersonal conflicts (Gilbert et alli, 2002; Allen & Badcock, 2003). Evolutionary psychology provides several possible explanations for why humans typically respond to social dyanamics in the way that they do, including possible functions of self-esteem in relation to dominance hierarchies.
See also
- Resource holding potential
- Dehumanization
- Hazing
- Social rejection
- Psychological trauma
- Learned helplessness
- Evolutionary psychology
References
- Rohde, P. (2001). "The relevance of hierarchies, territories, defeat for depression in humans- hypotheses and clinical predictions". Journal of Affective Disorders. 65 (3): 221–230. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00219-6. PMID 11511402.
- Laviola, G. (2004). "Social withdrawal, neophobia, and stereotyped behavior in developing rats exposed to neonatal asphyxia". Psychopharmacology. 175 (2): 196–205. doi:10.1007/s00213-004-1800-3. PMID 14985924.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Bartolomuccia, A (2005). "Social factors and individual vulnerability to chronic stress exposure". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 29 (1): 67–81. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.009. PMID 15652256.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Bjorkqvist, K (2001). "Social defeat as a stressor in humans". Physiology & Behavior. 73 (3): 435–442. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00490-5. PMID 11438372.
- Huhman, K.L. (2006). "Social conflict models- Can they inform us about human psychopathology?". Hormones and Behavior. 50 (4): 640–646. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.022. PMID 16870189.
- Marinia, F (2006). "Single exposure to social defeat increases corticotropin-releasing factor and glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in rat hippocampus". Brain Research. 1067 (1): 25–35. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.002. PMID 16360122.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Gilbert, P (2002). "Relationship of anhedonia and anxiety to social rank, defeat and entrapment". Journal of Affective Disorders. 71 (1–3): 141–151. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00392-5. PMID 12167510.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Van Reeth, O (2000). "Physiology of sleep (review) - Interactions between stress and sleep; from basic research to clinical situations". Sleep Medicine Reviews. 4 (2): 201–219. doi:10.1053/smrv.1999.0097.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Buwaldaa, B (2005). "Long-term effects of social stress on brain and behavior: a focus on hippocampal functioning". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 29 (1): 83–97. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.05.005. PMID 15652257.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Rygula, R. (2005). "Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: Impact of chronic social stress". Behavioural Brain Research. 162 (1): 127–134. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2005.03.009. PMID 15922073.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Allen, N. B. (2003). "The Social Risk Hypothesis of Depressed Mood: Evolutionary, Psychosocial, and Neurobiological Perspectives". Psychological Bulletin. 129 (6): 887–913. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.887. PMID 14599287.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthor=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Sloman, L. (2006). "The interactive functioning of anxiety and depression in agonistic encounters and reconciliation". Journal of Affective Disorders. 90 (2–3): 93–99. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.001. PMID 16442166.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Sgoifoa, A. (2005). "Individual differences in cardiovascular response to social challenge". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 29 (1): 59–66. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.07.001. PMID 15652255.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- Selten, J.-P. (2005). "Social defeat: risk factor for schizophrenia?". The British Journal of Psychiatry. 187 (101–102): 101. doi:10.1192/bjp.187.2.101. PMID 16055818.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
- de Jong JG, van der Vegt BJ, Buwalda B, Koolhaas JM (2005). "Social environment determines the long-term effects of social defeat". Physiol. Behav. 84 (1): 87–95. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.10.013. PMID 15642611.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
- Handbook of Stress and the Brain (part 2), T. Steckler, N. H. Kalin, J. M. H. M. Reul, 2005. ISBN: 0-444-51823-1. Pg 113+