Jump to content

Shock collar: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m v2.05b - Bot T20 CW#61 - Fix errors for CW project (Reference before punctuation)
m Added word "A" before a sentence. (Style edit)
Line 2: Line 2:
{{More citations needed|date=May 2010}}
{{More citations needed|date=May 2010}}


[[File:Shock collar.jpg|thumb|Typical shock collar.]]
[[File:Shock collar.jpg|thumb|A typical shock collar.]]
[[File:Diensthund.jpg|thumb|Shock collar used on a riot police dog in 2004 in [[Würzburg]]. Since then, Germany has banned the use of shock collars, even by police <ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.animallaw.info/statute/germany-cruelty-german-animal-welfare-act |title=Germany - Cruelty - German Animal Welfare Act &#124; Animal Legal & Historical Center}}</ref>.]]
[[File:Diensthund.jpg|thumb|Shock collar used on a riot police dog in 2004 in [[Würzburg]]. Since then, Germany has banned the use of shock collars, even by police <ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.animallaw.info/statute/germany-cruelty-german-animal-welfare-act |title=Germany - Cruelty - German Animal Welfare Act &#124; Animal Legal & Historical Center}}</ref>.]]
A '''shock collar''' or '''remote training collar''', also known as an '''e-collar''', '''Ecollar''', or '''electronic collar''') is a type of [[Dog training|training]] [[dog collar|collar]] that utilizes [[electrical shocks]] to induce [[pain]] to the neck of a dog<ref> https://www.magonlinelibrary.com/doi/abs/10.12968/vetn.2016.7.1.56 Vets call for ban on electric ‘shock collars’. Sean Wensley. The Veterinary Nurse 7 (1), 56-56, 2016 https://doi.org/10.12968/vetn.2016.7.1.56</ref> or other parts of its body, depending on the desired outcome. These collars incorporate a radio-controlled electronic device and are worn around the dog’s neck. Many European countries view [[shock collars]] as [[cruelty to animals|animal cruelty]] and have banned their use.<ref>{{cite news |url=https://www.thekennelclub.org.uk/about-us/campaigns/electric-shock-collars/ |work=Kennel Club UK |title=Electric Shock Collars}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.animallaw.info/statute/germany-cruelty-german-animal-welfare-act |title=Germany - Cruelty - German Animal Welfare Act &#124; Animal Legal & Historical Center}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |url=https://www.ramstein.af.mil/About/Fact-Sheets/Display/Article/303687/dog-keeping-laws-for-germany/|title = Dog Keeping laws for Germany}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|date=4 April 2019 |url=https://www.rtlnieuws.nl/nieuws/nederland/artikel/4665206/elektronische-halsband-honden-hondenband-verboden|title=Elektronische halsband voor honden vanaf volgend jaar verboden}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |url=https://nltimes.nl/2019/04/04/netherlands-ban-shock-collars-dogs-limit-breeding-designer-cats |title=Netherlands to ban shock collars for dogs, limit breeding of 'designer cats'}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=Achtung vor verbotenen Halsbändern für Hunde |url=https://www.heute.at/s/achtung-diese-halsbander-fur-hunde-sind-verboten-aber-dennoch-auf-amazon-erhaltlich-57018162 |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=Heute.at |language=de}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=Fynsk dyrlæge forarget over salg af stødhalsbånd: Hundene ved ikke, hvad der rammer dem |url=https://www.tv2fyn.dk/fyn/fynsk-dyrlaege-forarget-over-salg-af-stodhalsband-hundene-ved-ikke-hvad-der-rammer-dem |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=TV2 Fyn |language=da}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |title=Animal Welfare Decree |url=https://www.animallaw.info/sites/default/files/Finish%20Animal%20welfare%20decree.pdf |publisher=Ministry of Agriculture and Forestry}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=FINLEX ® - Ajantasainen lainsäädäntö: Eläinsuojelulaki 247/1996 |url=https://finlex.fi/fi/laki/ajantasa/1996/19960247}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |date=2019-01-17 |title=Notkun rafmagnsólarinnar tilkynnt sem ill meðferð til MAST |url=https://www.mannlif.is/frettir/innlent/notkun-rafmagnsolarinnar-tilkynnt-sem-ill-medferd-til-mast/ |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=Mannlíf.is |language=is}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=Rafmagnsólar hunda bannaðar - Vísir |url=https://www.visir.is/g/2011227029d |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=visir.is |date=15 July 2011 |language=is}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |last=Lindin |first=Ina-Kristin |date=2017-01-27 |title=Dømt for bruk av strømhalsbånd |url=https://www.nrk.no/osloogviken/domt-for-bruk-av-stromhalsband-1.13343560 |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=NRK |language=nb-NO}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |last=Gernes |first=Jonatan |date=2021-05-20 |title=Elhalsband gav plågsamma skador på hunden |url=https://www.hallandsposten.se/1.47781288 |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=hallandsposten.se |language=sv}}</ref> The mechanism behind shock collars involve inflicting varying levels and duration of pain, which generates [[fear]]<ref>On the generality of the laws of learning. Martin E Seligman. Psychological review 77 (5), 406, 1970.</ref><ref>A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Orval H Mowrer. Psychological review 46 (6), 553, 1939.</ref><ref>Theory and experiment relating psychoanalytic displacement to stimulus-response generalization. Neal E Miller. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 43 (2), 155, 1948.</ref><ref>Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events
A '''shock collar''' or '''remote training collar''', also known as an '''e-collar''', '''Ecollar''', or '''electronic collar''') is a type of [[Dog training|training]] [[dog collar|collar]] that utilizes [[electrical shocks]] to induce [[pain]] to the neck of a dog<ref> https://www.magonlinelibrary.com/doi/abs/10.12968/vetn.2016.7.1.56 Vets call for ban on electric ‘shock collars’. Sean Wensley. The Veterinary Nurse 7 (1), 56-56, 2016 https://doi.org/10.12968/vetn.2016.7.1.56</ref> or other parts of its body, depending on the desired outcome. These collars incorporate a radio-controlled electronic device and are worn around the dog’s neck. Many European countries view [[shock collars]] as [[cruelty to animals|animal cruelty]] and have banned their use.<ref>{{cite news |url=https://www.thekennelclub.org.uk/about-us/campaigns/electric-shock-collars/ |work=Kennel Club UK |title=Electric Shock Collars}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.animallaw.info/statute/germany-cruelty-german-animal-welfare-act |title=Germany - Cruelty - German Animal Welfare Act &#124; Animal Legal & Historical Center}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |url=https://www.ramstein.af.mil/About/Fact-Sheets/Display/Article/303687/dog-keeping-laws-for-germany/|title = Dog Keeping laws for Germany}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|date=4 April 2019 |url=https://www.rtlnieuws.nl/nieuws/nederland/artikel/4665206/elektronische-halsband-honden-hondenband-verboden|title=Elektronische halsband voor honden vanaf volgend jaar verboden}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |url=https://nltimes.nl/2019/04/04/netherlands-ban-shock-collars-dogs-limit-breeding-designer-cats |title=Netherlands to ban shock collars for dogs, limit breeding of 'designer cats'}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=Achtung vor verbotenen Halsbändern für Hunde |url=https://www.heute.at/s/achtung-diese-halsbander-fur-hunde-sind-verboten-aber-dennoch-auf-amazon-erhaltlich-57018162 |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=Heute.at |language=de}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=Fynsk dyrlæge forarget over salg af stødhalsbånd: Hundene ved ikke, hvad der rammer dem |url=https://www.tv2fyn.dk/fyn/fynsk-dyrlaege-forarget-over-salg-af-stodhalsband-hundene-ved-ikke-hvad-der-rammer-dem |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=TV2 Fyn |language=da}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |title=Animal Welfare Decree |url=https://www.animallaw.info/sites/default/files/Finish%20Animal%20welfare%20decree.pdf |publisher=Ministry of Agriculture and Forestry}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=FINLEX ® - Ajantasainen lainsäädäntö: Eläinsuojelulaki 247/1996 |url=https://finlex.fi/fi/laki/ajantasa/1996/19960247}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |date=2019-01-17 |title=Notkun rafmagnsólarinnar tilkynnt sem ill meðferð til MAST |url=https://www.mannlif.is/frettir/innlent/notkun-rafmagnsolarinnar-tilkynnt-sem-ill-medferd-til-mast/ |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=Mannlíf.is |language=is}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |title=Rafmagnsólar hunda bannaðar - Vísir |url=https://www.visir.is/g/2011227029d |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=visir.is |date=15 July 2011 |language=is}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |last=Lindin |first=Ina-Kristin |date=2017-01-27 |title=Dømt for bruk av strømhalsbånd |url=https://www.nrk.no/osloogviken/domt-for-bruk-av-stromhalsband-1.13343560 |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=NRK |language=nb-NO}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |last=Gernes |first=Jonatan |date=2021-05-20 |title=Elhalsband gav plågsamma skador på hunden |url=https://www.hallandsposten.se/1.47781288 |access-date=2022-04-21 |website=hallandsposten.se |language=sv}}</ref> The mechanism behind shock collars involve inflicting varying levels and duration of pain, which generates [[fear]]<ref>On the generality of the laws of learning. Martin E Seligman. Psychological review 77 (5), 406, 1970.</ref><ref>A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Orval H Mowrer. Psychological review 46 (6), 553, 1939.</ref><ref>Theory and experiment relating psychoanalytic displacement to stimulus-response generalization. Neal E Miller. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 43 (2), 155, 1948.</ref><ref>Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events

Revision as of 02:54, 20 June 2023

A typical shock collar.
Shock collar used on a riot police dog in 2004 in Würzburg. Since then, Germany has banned the use of shock collars, even by police [1].

A shock collar or remote training collar, also known as an e-collar, Ecollar, or electronic collar) is a type of training collar that utilizes electrical shocks to induce pain to the neck of a dog[2] or other parts of its body, depending on the desired outcome. These collars incorporate a radio-controlled electronic device and are worn around the dog’s neck. Many European countries view shock collars as animal cruelty and have banned their use.[3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11][12][13][14][15] The mechanism behind shock collars involve inflicting varying levels and duration of pain, which generates fear[16][17][18][19] and serves as a deterrent for undesirable behaviors.[20] Some models of shock collar models offer additional features such as a tone or vibrational setting that can be used as an alternative or in combination with the shock. Certain advanced collars include Internet mapping capabilities and GPS functionality to track the dog’s location or notify the owner about its whereabouts.

In the late 1960s, shock collars were initially developed for training hunting dogs, but they were originally designed with only one high level of power. Many modern versions are capable of delivering varying levels of shock. In areas where shock collars are legal, they are generally accessible, although Petco took the lead as the first major U.S. retailer to cease their sale.[21][22] Shock collars have been used in a range of applications, including behavioral modification, obedience training, and pet containment, as well as military, police and service training. Although similar systems exist for other animals, shock collars designed for domestic dogs are the most commonly used.

Types of shock collars

Pet containment system/underground fence with shock collar

Where legal, the most common use of shock collars is pet containment systems that are used to keep a dog inside the perimeter of the residence without the construction of a physical barrier. This use of shock collars is increasingly popular in areas where local laws or homeowners' associations prohibit the construction of a physical fence. Available systems include: in-ground installation to preserve the aesthetics of the yard; above ground installation to reinforce an existing barrier that was not sufficient in containing the dog; and wireless systems to allow for indoor use. Most pet containment systems work by installing a wire around the perimeter of the yard. The wire carries no current (as opposed to electric fences, which carry a current at high voltage that may be lethal in the event of unauthorized or defective installation or equipment) but forms a closed loop with a circuit box that transmits a radio signal to the receiver collar on the dog.[23] As the dog approaches the perimeter the collar shocks the dog.[24]

Bark control shock collars

Bark control shock collars are used to curb excessive or nuisance barking by delivering a shock at the moment the dog begins barking. Bark collars can be activated by microphone or vibration, and some of the most advanced collars use both sound and vibration to eliminate the possibility of extraneous noises activating a response.

Remote shock collars

Remote shock collars can be activated by a handheld device to give the dog an electric shock which causes pain.[25] Unlike automatic shock collars, remote shock collars do require a person to actively trigger every shock, but Dr. Stanley Milgram in the Milgram experiment demonstrated that most people are willing to inflict electric shocks if convinced this will help the learning process.[26][27][28]

Better quality remote trainers have a large variety of levels and functions, can give varying duration of pain,[29] and have a beep or vibration option useful for getting the dog's attention.

Remote shock collars use operant conditioning either as a form of positive punishment, where the correction is applied at the moment an undesired behavior occurs to reduce the frequency of that behavior—or as a form of negative reinforcement, where a continuous stimulation is applied until the moment a desired behavior occurs, to increase the frequency of that behavior.[30]

Frame of reference

Electrical shock is the pain,[31] injury,[32] physiological reaction, or sensation caused by electric current passing through the body. It occurs upon contact of a body part with any source of electricity that causes a sufficient current through the skin, muscles, or hair.

Pain is a difficult outcome to measure because its nature is both multifaceted and subjective,[33][34] (although researchers have found some success in objectivity by measuring blood cortisol levels[35]). As a result, researchers disagree on how much pain a shock collar causes.

Dr Diane Frank, in the Australian Veterinary Journal, takes the position that “Electric shock hurts and the same shock will be perceived differently by different dogs. Regardless, if the dog perceives pain, he or she experiences a stress response that actively interferes with learning positive, more favourable, substitute behaviour. If the shock and pain are profound, it is possible to induce almost immediate long-term potentiation (LTP), or the molecular changes associated with hippocampal memory, which will lead to a strong aversion or phobia.”.[36]

In contrast, commenting in his 2005 textbook on training and behaviour, Steven R. Lindsay defends the use of shock collars. While admitting that shock collars do cause fear, he also believed of the public perception of the term "shock" at the time and its application in the description of training aids that, "At low levels, the term shock is hardly fitting to describe the effects produced by electronic training collars, since there is virtually no effect beyond a pulsing tingling or tickling sensation on the surface of the skin ... the word shock is loaded with biased connotations, images of convulsive spasms and burns, and implications associated with extreme physical pain, emotional trauma, physiological collapse, and laboratory abuses ... the stimulus or signal generated by most modern devices is highly controlled and presented to produce a specific set of behavioral and motivational responses to it."[37]

In 2000, before Germany made shock collars illegal, in an article for the German trade magazine "Office for veterinary service and food control", Dr. Dieter Klein compared the effects of shock collars with other electrical stimulation products; "Modern devices ... are in a range in which normally no organic damage is being inflicted. The electric properties and performances of the modern low current remote stimulation devices ... are comparable to the electric stimulation devices used in human medicine. Organic damage, as a direct impact of the applied current, can be excluded.” [38]

Technical considerations

The intensity of pain caused by electric current can vary significantly due to small changes in amperage. Furthermore, this pain can be further amplified by adjusting the pulse rate and duration. Other factors such as voltage, current, waveform, and frequency of the waveform are not particularly relevant when it comes to assessing the level of pain. While these factors can be used to calculate the amount of energy applied in Joules, they do not indicate the actual intensity of the stimulus or how it will be perceived by the recipient.

In 2004, Dr. Dieter Klein conducted research and estimated that commercial shock collars, which were later banned in Germany, operated at a minimum setting of 30 milliamps and a maximum setting of 80 milliamps.[39] Another commonly-cited study, conducted by Christiansen et. al., utilized shock collars with a higher intensity, reaching up to 400 milliamps.[40]

To put these numbers in perspective, it can be helpful to look at other shock devices that have been used to produce pain on human beings for inducing compliance. The shock-handcuffs [41]and stun belts that are used on human prisoners in the U.S. and South Africa (illegal in the U.K., the Netherlands, Scandinavian countries, and Greece)[42][43] are approximately one-tenth of the amperage (3-4 milliamps).[44] A cattle prod (which is painful enough to be used illegally for torture of human beings[45]) is not more than 10 milliamps.[46]

To provide further context for the 30-80 milliamps range of shock collars, it is worth considering various comparisons to differ ways pain has been labeled at varying amperages. According to medical sources, an "electrical sensation" is typically associated with 0.2-2 milliamps.[47][48] A study indicated that an animal's tolerance threshold for a prolonged duration was around 0.5 milliamps,[49] while in another study, 1-2+ milliamps was described as a “painful shock”.[50][51] Everyday static electric shocks typically range around 5 milliamps but are very brief in duration.[52] In humans, a sustained current of 10 milliamps is considered the "let go threshold," leading to strong arm contractions and rendering the individual unable to voluntarily control their muscles or release an electrified object.[53]

OSHA gives examples of the effects of shocks in milliamps using words such as “pain” and “extreme pain”, and their chart gives some perspective. “Below 1 milliamp: Generally not perceptible. 5 milliamps: Slight shock felt. Not painful but disturbing. Average individual can let go. Strong involuntary reactions can lead to other injuries. 6-25 milliamps (women): Painful shocks. Loss of muscle control. 9-30 milliamps (men): The freezing current or "let go" range. If extensor muscles are excited by shock, the person may be thrown away from the power source. Individuals cannot let go. Strong involuntary reactions can lead to other injuries. 50-150 milliamps: Extreme pain, respiratory arrest, severe muscle reactions. Death is possible.”[54]

Depending on design, shock collars can be set so that the shock delivered is only mildly uncomfortable [according to whom?]. Variable settings of this kind are essential, so that the shock collar can be adjusted to provide the level of pain that changes the dog’s behavior, as situations change.

Shock collars are sometimes referred to as delivering a "static shock"; however, static electricity is direct current and carries little energy (order of millijoules). Shock collars make use of alternating current. It is therefore inappropriate to refer to shock collars as delivering a static shock.

Consistent pain delivery requires good contact between the collar electrodes and the dog's shock skin.[55] (The shock collar must be fitted according to the manufacturer's instructions.) Local humidity and individual variation in coat density, skin thickness and surface conductivity, also affect the delivery of the pain.[citation needed]

Individual variations in temperament, pain sensitivity and susceptibility to startle of dogs, means that settings must be carefully adjusted to produce pain[56] that is perceived by the dog as only just aversive enough to stop the dog engaging in the unwanted behavior. Normally salient stimuli, such as noises, commands and even shocks, may have no effect on a dog that is highly aroused and focused on an activity such as hunting.[citation needed]

Note that a shock collar’s individual shocks are short duration (6-8 milliseconds[57]) to cause pain but not physical injury, but the pain intensity can be increased by using the same milliamps for each shock but then delivering more shocks per second: "Many e−collars appear to shift intensity levels by altering the pulse duration or repetition rate while keeping the output current and voltage relatively constant, depending on the electrode−skin load."[23]

Potential to cause harm

In 1980 (revised 1987), the US Center for Veterinary Medicine (CVM), a branch of the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA), concurred in regulatory action against a manufacturer of a bark collar, stating "Complaints received, which were later corroborated by our own testing, included severe burns in the collar area and possible personality adjustment injuries to the dogs. The shocking mechanism was found to be activated not only by barking but by vehicle horns, slamming doors or any other loud noise. CVM concurred in regulatory action against the device since it was deemed to be dangerous to the health of the animal."[58] The standing policy of the US FDA is that "Dog collars which are activated by the noise of barking to produce an electric shock are considered as hazardous to the health of the animal."[58]

In countries where shock collars are legal, no regulations exist specifying the performance characteristics or reliability of these devices, so there is considerable variation in shock level, pulse duration, and repetition rate between manufacturers, and perhaps even between batches of collars from a single manufacturer. The lack of regulation or standards, and the fact that some of the safety features of shock collars are patented by specific manufacturers,[59] means that the safety and operational characteristics of individual products cannot be verified.

Training effectiveness

An example of a shock collar

The effectiveness of e-collars is highly debated, with each side having support from activists, scholars, and trainers.[citation needed] When it comes to effectiveness, no studies have shown that e-collars are more effective than positive reinforcement training.[60][61]

Deterring predation in the wild

The Wildlife Society article addresses the use of shock collars as a way to prevent sheep from being preyed upon by wild coyotes. According to (Phillips, 1999)[62] they tested these collars on coyotes for a four-month period and found that the collars stopped thirteen attacks on sheep herds. This also is said to deter future attacks by the tested coyotes. Collars have also been used on wolves for similar reasons. This document is the assessment of the shock collar on wolves’ long-term behavior. The article talks about trying to alter wolves’ behavior over an extended period of time using the collar. The consensus was while it did have an effect while in use and temporally after it was removed, the study concluded that longer exposure would be needed to have any substantial evidence (Hawley, 2008). As far as non-lethal alternatives these two sources both concluded that shock collars are the most effective deterrence to predators. Both groups continued their research and the Wildlife Society has developed a new and improved version that eliminates the risk of neck injury when used on animals that previous versions caused. They have increased battery life and the durability of the unit. They devised a unit that is worn like a back pack for the animal. Previous versions caused excessive rubbing and soreness as well as being irritating for the animal to the point were they would try to take the harness off.

Scientific studies

Christiansen et al. study (2001a)

Christiansen et al., looked at behavioural differences between three breeds of dogs when confronted by domestic sheep (138 dogs; Elkhounds, hare hunting dogs and English setters).[63] Two testing procedures were used and shock collars were used to deter attacks on sheep. The first, a path test, involved observing the dogs' reactions to a set of novel stimuli (rag pulled across the track, bundle of cans thrown down, tethered sheep at 5m) as it was walked. The second test involved monitoring the dog's reaction to a free-roaming sheep flock in a field. In this study they identified several factors that predicted a high hunting motivation and attack severity. These were lack of previous opportunity to chase sheep, low fearfulness towards gunshots and unfamiliar people and general interest in sheep when encountering them. Younger dogs (<3 years of age) showed more pronounced initial hunting motivation and more frequent attacks. Elkhounds showed more hunting behaviour, more attacks and were more frequently given electric shocks during the tests. A shock collar was used to deter attacks on the sheep during the experiments. Shocks (3000 V, 0.4 A, duration 1 second) were delivered when dogs came within a distance of 1–2 m of the sheep, and were repeated until the dogs left the area. The objective was to suppress an attack, but not to damage the hunting ability of the dogs. Despite frequently initiated chases and attacks, few shocks were delivered. This was because few dogs approached closer than 1–2 m, and the intention was to deter proximity to sheep rather than to associate hunting behaviour with an aversive shock, which would impair future hunting behaviour in other contexts.

Christiansen et al. study (2001b)

The dogs used in the first study were re-tested using the same procedures, to assess long-term impact of the training on their reaction to sheep.[64] Again, in the free-running tests the dogs were fitted with a shock collar, which was used to deter approaches to within 1–2 m of the sheep. Dogs that had previously been shocked in year 1 showed a significant increased in latency to approach a person during the path test (p < 0.001), even though this was not a condition under which shocks had been delivered. Owners reported behavioral differences between year 1 and 2 in 24 of the dogs. 18 of the 24 dogs had shown no interest in sheep during that period, even though they had been interested in them during the first year tests. However, only one of those dogs had received shocks, so the change in behaviour could not be attributed to the use of the shock collar. When comparing owners’ reports for the two years, the dogs showed a weaker inclination for chasing sheep and other prey than previously (p < 0:001), but this variable was not affected by shock experience. Dogs that had shown interest in sheep in year 1 showed a persistent interest in year 2. No dogs chased or attacked sheep as their first response, while half of them did so the first year. During the entire test period, the proportion of dogs attacking sheep was reduced to almost one fourth. The number of shocks administered per dog was reduced by the second year, and only one of the dogs that received shocks the first year needed shocks also the second year. The observations that both receivers and non-receivers of shocks the first year showed a reduction in the probability of chasing sheep, but the receivers showing a larger reduction, show that shock treatment provides an additional learning response. No adverse effects on the dogs were observed with this training procedure, but in their discussion the authors commented "In order to ensure no negative effects, we recommend that the electronic dog collar may be used for such purposes only if it is used by skilled trainers with special competence on dog behaviour, learning mechanisms, and of this particular device."

Salgirli dissertation (2008)

The aim of Salgirli's study was "...to investigate whether any stress is caused by the use of specific conditioned signal, quitting signal, and/or pinch collars as alternatives to electric training collars, and if they do so, whether the stress produced in the process is comparable to the one with electric training collars.".[65] The study population were a group of 42 adult police dogs. The quitting signal was a conditioned frustration equivalent to negative punishment. It was conditioned by associating failure to obtain an anticipated food reward with a specific vocal signal. In the test, dogs were walked past a "provocateur" who attempted to taunt the dog into a reaction. If the dog reacted, it was punished, and if it failed to react on subsequent provocations then the punishment was deemed to have had a learning effect. The study is therefore a comparison of negative and positive punishment methods, and not a comparison of punishment with positive reinforcement. Learning effect was measured by assessing the number of dogs that learned to quit a behaviour after application of the punishing stimulus. There was no statistical difference in learning effect between the pinch and shock collar, but the quitting signal produced a significantly poorer learning effect compared to shock or pinch collars (p < 0.01 in both cases). "Although the pinch collar caused more behavioral reactions, in the form of distress, than the electronic training collar, the electronic training collar elicits more vocal reactions in dogs than the pinch collars"; the explanation for increased vocalisation in the shock collar group was that this was due to a startle response rather than pain reactions.

Salivary cortisol was monitored to measure the stress levels of the dogs, but this data was not presented in the dissertation; behavioral observation was the sole measure of stress. The study concluded that the electronic training collar induces less distress and shows stronger “learning effect” in dogs in comparison to the pinch collar. Commenting on the quitting signal, the author stated "It should particularly be mentioned, that the quitting signal training was implied only on adult dogs within the frame of this study. Therefore, the results should not be interpreted as that the quitting signal can not be a suitable method in police dog training. As previously stated training of the quitting signal requires a hard and a structured procedure. Thus, if the training, namely the conditioning, begins in puppyhood, the quitting signal can also be an effective method in police dog training". Comparing the effects of the three punishment methods; "These results can probably be explained by that electronic training collar complies completely with the punishment criteria, which were defined by TORTORA (1982), in case of proof of the proficient and experienced user. On the other hand, when applying the pinch collar, these criteria can not be met even though perfect timing is applied since reactions of the dog and effectiveness of the method depends on several different factors such as the willingness, strength and motivation of the handler, as well as his/her proficiency. In addition to that, the visibility of the administrator and, thus, of the punishment is another important factor influencing the efficiency of the pinch collar because the dog directly links the punishment with its owner. Therefore, this method does not satisfy the ‘‘punishment criteria’’ at all. The quitting signal on the other hand requires criteria, such as good timing and structured training procedure, on account of complete conditioning to achieve effective results. Even if these criteria are met, the personality trait of the dog is another factor, which influences the efficiency of the signal."[65]

Schalke et al. study (2007)

Schalke et al. conducted a 7-month study to investigate the effect of shock collars on stress parameters, in a series of different training situations.[66] Heart rate and saliva cortisol were used to determine the stress levels in three groups of dogs. Group A received the electric shock when they touched the "prey" (a rabbit dummy attached to a motion device), Group H ("here" command) received the electric shock when they did not obey a previously trained recall command during hunting, and Group R (random) received random shocks that were unpredictable and out of context. Group A did not show a significant rise in cortisol levels; the other two groups (R & H) did show a significant rise, with group R showing the highest level of cortisol. Salivary cortisol was measured, as this procedure is less likely to cause stress related rise in cortisol.

From this the researchers concluded that the dogs who could clearly associate the shock with their action (i.e. touching the prey) and as a result were able to predict and control whether they received a shock, did not show considerable or persistent stress. The evidence of increased stress in the other groups was felt to support earlier findings that poor timing and/or inappropriate use of a shock collar puts the dog at high risk of severe and ongoing stress. They conclude that "The results of this study suggest that poor timing in the application of high level electric pulses, such as those used in this study, means there is a high risk that dogs will show severe and persistent stress symptoms. We recommend that the use of these devices should be restricted with proof of theoretical and practical qualification required and then the use of these devices should only be allowed in strictly specified situations."

Schilder & van der Borg study (2004)

Schilder and van der Borg conducted a study to compare the behavior of police service dogs that had previously been trained using a shock collar (Group S) with those that had not (Group C).[67] In the training test no shocks were applied, but the animal's behavior was observed during training tasks. The intention was to investigate whether shock collar based training might have a long-term effect on stress-related behavior even in the absence of shock, and whether this related to specific features of the training context. Behaviors recorded included recognised indicators of stress (panting, lip-licking, yawning, paw lifting and body posture) as well as yelping, squealing, snapping and avoidance. During free walks on the training grounds, groups S dogs showed significantly more stress related behaviors and a lower body posture than group C dogs. During training, the same differences were found. The difference between the groups was more significant when training took place on the familiar training ground, indicating a contextual effect. The presence of the trainer was considered to be part of this context. The authors concluded "We concluded that shocks received during training are not only unpleasant but also painful and frightening."

Lindsay says of this study, "Schilder and Van der Borg (2004) have published a report of disturbing findings regarding the short-term and long- term effects of shock used in the context of working dogs that is destined to become a source of significant controversy.... The absence of reduced drive or behavioral suppression with respect to critical activities associated with shock (e.g., bite work) makes one skeptical about the lasting adverse effects the authors claim to document. Although they offer no substantive evidence of trauma or harm to dogs, they provide loads of speculation, anecdotes, insinuations of gender and educational inadequacies, and derogatory comments regarding the motivation and competence of IPO trainers in its place."[68]

Steiss et al. study (2007)

Steiss, et al., conducted a four-week study of adult shelter dogs’ physiological and behavioral responses to bark control collars. Plasma cortisol was used as the stress measure. Dogs were randomly assigned to either a shock collar, a spray collar, or a dummy collar (control group). Dogs that were known to bark at an unfamiliar dog were used for the study. Test conditions involved presentation of an unfamiliar dog. Dogs wore activated collars for period of 30 minutes per day for three days in two consecutive weeks. The amount of barking was significantly reduced starting on the second day with both the spray and shock collars. There was no significant difference in effect between the two collar types. The treatment group dogs showed a mild yet statistically significant increase in blood cortisol level (an indicator of stress) only on the first day of wearing the collars (as compared to the Control Group.)[69] At the conclusion of the study, Dr. Steiss and her team concluded that "In the present study, with dogs wearing bark control collars intermittently over a 2-week period, the collars effectively deterred barking without statistically significant elevations in plasma cortisol, compared to controls, at any of the time points measured."

Tortora Study (1983)

Tortora applied a method called "safety training" to treat aggression in 36 cases exhibiting a form of "instrumental aggression", selected after screening a population of 476 cases. "Instrumental aggression" was defined as describing aggressive acts that "do not have a clear evolutionary significance, are not directly related to emotional arousal, do not have specific releasing stimuli, are not directly modulated by hormones, and do not have an identifiable focus in the brain". Tortora states that in the context of the article "instrumental aggression" was specifically defined as "aggressive responses that have "a specifiable learning history, show a growth function over time and are modulated by their consequences. These dogs had few operant alternatives to gain reinforcement by compliance and were channeled down a path that allowed their innate aggressiveness to come under the control of the negatively reinforcing contingencies in the environment". The dogs initially behaved as though they "expected" aversive events and that the only way to prevent these events was through aggression. The dogs were therefore a highly selected subset that had not learned strategies for coping with threat.

Each dog was trained to respond to a set of 15 commands taken from the AKC standard for CDX obedience. The commands were selected to provide control over the dog, and included "heel", "stand" "go", "come", "hold", "drop" and "sit". These behaviors were termed "safety behaviors". Training was divided into 9 stages, each of which was composed of 5–20 twice daily training sessions. Dogs could only progress to the next stage after passing a test. On average, dogs took 10–15 sessions to complete each stage. After training on basic commands, the dogs were trained to perform the behaviors they had already learned in order to avoid progressively increasing electric shock. After that, they were conditioned to perform a safety behavior to avoid a "safety tone" that let them anticipate the shock. In later stages of training, dogs were exposed to provocation by a distractor dog, and were punished using full intensity shock if they failed to perform a safety behavior or showed aggression. After training was complete, and the dogs were choosing to perform the safety behaviors instead of aggression, owners were taught to use the shock collar, and the training was transferred into everyday situations. The training resulted in a long-lasting and complete suppression of aggressive behaviour in the dogs. Dogs were followed up three years after the end of training, and the reduction in aggression was maintained.[70]

Criticism

Even in countries where shock collars are legal, their use is controversial.[71][72]

The HSUS (Humane Society of the United States) provides the following comment on the use of aversive collars (choke chains, pinch collars and shock collars): "Some trainers use aversive collars to train "difficult" dogs with correction or punishment. These collars rely on physical discomfort or even pain to teach the dog what not to do. They suppress the unwanted behavior but don't teach him what the proper one is. At best, they are unpleasant for your dog, and at worst, they may cause your dog to act aggressively and even bite you. Positive training methods should always be your first choice." They go on to comment on shock collars specifically: "The least humane and most controversial use of the shock collar is as a training device. The trainer can administer a shock to a dog at a distance through a remote control. There is a greater chance for abuse (delivery of shocks as punishment) or misuse (poor timing of shocks). Your dog also may associate the painful shock with people or other experiences, leading to fearful or aggressive behavior".[73]

PETA (People for the Ethical Treatment of Animals) opposes the use of shock collars, stating "Dogs wearing shock collars can suffer from physical pain and injury (ranging from burns to cardiac fibrillation) and psychological stress, including severe anxiety and displaced aggression. Individual animals vary in their temperaments and pain thresholds; a shock that seems mild to one dog may be severe to another. The anxiety and confusion caused by repeated shocks can lead to changes in the heart and respiration rate or gastrointestinal disorders. Electronic collars can also malfunction, either administering nonstop shocks or delivering no shocks at all".[74]

CABTSG (The Companion Animal Behaviour Therapy Study Group), an affiliate group of the BSAVA (British Small Animal Veterinary Association), now renamed the British Veterinary Behaviour Association, no longer has a policy statement against shock collars because England will outlaw shock collars on February 1, 2024.[75]

On the advice of the RSPCA (Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals) and other welfare groups, the ACPO (Association of Chief Police Officers) banned the use of shock collars for police dog training by all UK police forces. The current ACPO Police Dogs Manual of Guidance states "Equipment that is not approved for use in the training of police dogs includes remote training collars designed to give an electric shock and Pinch Collars".[76]

The RSPCA no longer publishes a policy statement on how much they discourage the use of shock collars because England has banned their use, but has stated on Facebook that they “welcome” the ban.[77]

The UK Kennel Club has won a ten-year campaign[78] to achieve a ban on the sale and use of shock collars. Their campaign had stated that "The Kennel Club is against the use of any negative training methods or devices. The Kennel Club believes that there are many positive training tools and methods that can produce dogs that are trained just as quickly and reliably, with absolutely no fear, pain, or potential damage to the relationship between dog and handler." "The Kennel Club in calling upon the Government and Scottish Parliament to introduce an outright ban on this barbaric method of training dogs.".[79]

The two British members of the World Union of German Shepherd Clubs (WUSV) helped the Kennel Club win a complete ban on shock collars. They passed a motion to exclude this equipment from any of its training branches during official club training times.[80]

The NCAE (Norwegian Council on Animal Ethics) no longer has a position statement against shock collars because Norway has made shock collars illegal.[81]

The APDT (Association of Professional Dog Trainers)‘s has strengthened their policy on shock collars and their newest position statement says, "APDT takes the stance that there are no training or behavior cases which justify the use of intentional aversive punishment-based interventions in any form of training ranging from general obedience and tricks to dealing with severe behavior problems. This is in agreement with the American Veterinary Society for Animal Behavior 8 and available literature. Trainers who use aversive tools such as choke collars, prong collars, shock collars (including “stim-collars” and “e-collars”), bonkers, shaker-cans, citronella spray, water spray, leash-pop/leash-corrections (with any type of collar/harness), yelling, or any other technique designed to cause fear, pain, or startle in the dog are not practicing LIMA as described and used within APDT.”[82]

The AVSAB (American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior) has strengthened their position statement on all aversive methods, including shock collars, which now states “The application of aversive methods – which, by definition, rely on application of force, pain, or emotional or physical discomfort – should not be used in canine training or for the treatment of behavioral disorders.[83]”.

Praise

In his 2005 textbook on training and behavior, Steven R. Lindsay writes "Instead of instilling social aversion and anxiety ... animal and human research supports the notion that competent shock [collar] training appears to promote positive social attachment, safety, and reward effects that may be provided and amplified via affectionate petting and reassuring praise. The preponderance of scientific evidence suggests that [electrical stimulation] escape/avoidance and pain reduction should promote long-term effects that are incompatible with fear and stress, making the trainer an object of significant extrinsic reward that actually enhances the dog's welfare via an improved capacity for social coping, learning, and adaptation".[68] Steven Lindsay states "If minimizing the intensity, duration, and frequency of aversive stimulation during training is recognized as a significant factor in the definition of humane dog training, then the radio controlled e-collar must be ranked as one of the most humane dog-training tools currently available".[84]

The International Association of Canine Professionals released a statement on the matter:

The International Association of Canine Professionals (IACP) strongly opposes legislation that bans or limits the humane use of any training tool, saying It is our conviction that limiting the humane use of training tools would result in a higher incidence of nuisance and dangerous dog behavior, and more dogs being surrendered to already over-burdened public shelters.... Dog training is a very diverse field with a single common thread: communication. Dogs are trained for many different tasks such as assisting the disabled, police work, herding, hunting, protection, competition and companionship. Professional trainers achieve these training goals by using a wide variety of tools to communicate with the dog, both at close range, and over long distances. Done effectively, this communication increases desirable behaviors and reduces the incidence of problem behaviors in dogs.... Any efforts to ban or limit the use of training tools would hinder this communication, and our ability to train dogs would suffer. Working dogs would no longer be able to achieve highly specialized tasks, and families with pet dogs would have fewer options available to correct behavioral problems.... Training tools, when properly utilized, are safe and humane.

— International Association of Canine Professionals, IACP Position Statement on Training Tools[85]

Regions where shock collars are banned

Shock collars are banned in:

They will be banned in Flanders in Belgium after 2027.[103][104][105] A ban comes into effect in England from 1st February 2024[106] Automatic anti-barking shock collars are banned in Switzerland.[107][108]

In 2001, a British woman was prosecuted after one of her dogs attacked and killed a Shih Tzu whilst on a walk. A control order, rather than a destruction order, was imposed as the magistrates accepted the defense that the dog's aggressive behaviour was attributable to the effects of the shock collar. The defendant claimed her dogs "connected the pain of the electric shock with little dogs because of the first time I used the collar". She first shocked the dogs accidentally while they were walking by a small dog.[109]

In 2002, the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA) in Victoria, Australia lost a defamation lawsuit to a shock collar manufacturer and was ordered to pay AUD100,000 in damages. The RSPCA was found to have falsely claimed that shock collars can cause burns, deliver 3,000 volt shocks to dogs, and that the current from a shock collar had caused a 60 kilogram dog to perform backflips and resulted in brain damage. RSPCA's claims that these collars caused epileptic fits, vomiting, seizures, burning and bleeding was also found to be misleading.[110] The RSPCA's senior inspector had falsified evidence in an attempt to demonstrate that shock collars can cause burns.[111]

In 2010, after the Welsh Assembly voted to ban the use of shock collars in Wales, it was unsuccessfully challenged by Petsafe, a manufacturer of these devices, and the Electronic Collar Manufacturers' Association, who claimed that it breached Article 1 of the First Protocol of the European Convention of Human Rights (concerning the right to property).[112]

In 2011, a Welsh man became the first person convicted of illegal use of a shock collar in Wales,[113] receiving a fine for £2,000.[114]

References

  1. ^ "Germany - Cruelty - German Animal Welfare Act | Animal Legal & Historical Center".
  2. ^ https://www.magonlinelibrary.com/doi/abs/10.12968/vetn.2016.7.1.56 Vets call for ban on electric ‘shock collars’. Sean Wensley. The Veterinary Nurse 7 (1), 56-56, 2016 https://doi.org/10.12968/vetn.2016.7.1.56
  3. ^ "Electric Shock Collars". Kennel Club UK.
  4. ^ "Germany - Cruelty - German Animal Welfare Act | Animal Legal & Historical Center".
  5. ^ "Dog Keeping laws for Germany".
  6. ^ "Elektronische halsband voor honden vanaf volgend jaar verboden". 4 April 2019.
  7. ^ "Netherlands to ban shock collars for dogs, limit breeding of 'designer cats'".
  8. ^ "Achtung vor verbotenen Halsbändern für Hunde". Heute.at (in German). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  9. ^ "Fynsk dyrlæge forarget over salg af stødhalsbånd: Hundene ved ikke, hvad der rammer dem". TV2 Fyn (in Danish). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  10. ^ "Animal Welfare Decree" (PDF). Ministry of Agriculture and Forestry.
  11. ^ "FINLEX ® - Ajantasainen lainsäädäntö: Eläinsuojelulaki 247/1996".
  12. ^ "Notkun rafmagnsólarinnar tilkynnt sem ill meðferð til MAST". Mannlíf.is (in Icelandic). 2019-01-17. Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  13. ^ "Rafmagnsólar hunda bannaðar - Vísir". visir.is (in Icelandic). 15 July 2011. Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  14. ^ Lindin, Ina-Kristin (2017-01-27). "Dømt for bruk av strømhalsbånd". NRK (in Norwegian Bokmål). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  15. ^ Gernes, Jonatan (2021-05-20). "Elhalsband gav plågsamma skador på hunden". hallandsposten.se (in Swedish). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  16. ^ On the generality of the laws of learning. Martin E Seligman. Psychological review 77 (5), 406, 1970.
  17. ^ A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Orval H Mowrer. Psychological review 46 (6), 553, 1939.
  18. ^ Theory and experiment relating psychoanalytic displacement to stimulus-response generalization. Neal E Miller. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 43 (2), 155, 1948.
  19. ^ Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events Joseph E Dunsmoor, Vishnu P Murty, Lila Davachi, Elizabeth A Phelps Nature 520 (7547), 345-348, 2015.
  20. ^ Guilherme Fernandes, Joana; Olsson, I. Anna S.; Vieira de Castro, Ana Catarina (2017-11-01). "Do aversive-based training methods actually compromise dog welfare?: A literature review". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 196: 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2017.07.001. ISSN 0168-1591.
  21. ^ "Petco will stop selling electronic 'shock' collars". CNN. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  22. ^ "Petco ends sale of electronic shock collars, calls on competitors to also 'Stop the Shock' with online petition". USA TODAY. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  23. ^ a b Lindsay (2005), p. 573.
  24. ^ Blackwell, Emily J., et Al. “The use of electronic collars for training domestic dogs: estimated prevalence, reasons and risk factors for use, and owner perceived success as compared to other training methods.” BMC Veterinary Research 8 (2012):1-11.
  25. ^ Polska, Richard H. “Electronic shock collars: are they worth the risks?.” Journal of American Animal Hospital Association 30.5 (1994): 463-468
  26. ^ Some conditions of obedience and disobedience to authority. Stanley Milgram. Human relations 18 (1), 57-76, 1965.
  27. ^ Milgram's obedience to authority experiments: Origins and early evolution. Nestar John Charles Russell British Journal of Social Psychology 50 (1), 140-162, 2011.
  28. ^ Behind the shock machine: The untold story of the notorious Milgram psychology experiments. Gina Perry. New Press, The, 2013.
  29. ^ Clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations. E Schalke, J Stichnoth, S Ott, R Jones-Baade. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105 (4), 369-380, 2007.
  30. ^ Fernandez, Joana Guilherme, I. Anna S. Olson, and Anna Catarina Viera de Castro. “Do aversive-based training methods actually compromise dog welfare?: A literature review.” Applied Animal Behaviour Science 196 (2017); 1-12.
  31. ^ https://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&as_sdt=0%2C24&q=do+electrical+shocks++cause+pain&btnG=#d=gs_qabs&t=1684771728001&u=%23p%3Dhm4qim4ekxUJ Physical, physiological, and psychological factors that affect pain reaction to electric shock. Bernard Tursky. Psychophysiology 11 (2), 95-112, 1974
  32. ^ https://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&as_sdt=0%2C24&q=do+electrical+shocks++cause+inuury&btnG=#d=gs_qabs&t=1684771853660&u=%23p%3DklH2oeVdvAgJ Biophysical injury mechanisms in electrical shock trauma. Raphael C Lee, Dajun Zhang, Jurgen Hannig. Annual review of biomedical engineering 2 (1), 477-509, 2000
  33. ^ https://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&as_sdt=0%2C24&q=pain+can%E2%80%99t+be+measured+&btnG=#d=gs_qabs&t=1684785744471&u=%23p%3D-ePatJIJzOgJ Pain outcomes: a brief review of instruments and techniques. Jarred Younger, Rebecca McCue, Sean Mackey. Current pain and headache reports 13, 39-43, 2009.
  34. ^ Frampton, C. L.; Hughes-Webb, P. (2011-08-01). "The Measurement of Pain". Clinical Oncology. Pain in Patients with Cancer. 23 (6): 381–386. doi:10.1016/j.clon.2011.04.008. ISSN 0936-6555.
  35. ^ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4153538/#pone.0102722-Companion1 The Welfare Consequences and Efficacy of Training Pet Dogs with Remote Electronic Training Collars in Comparison to Reward Based Training. Jonathan J. Cooper, Nina Cracknell, Jessica Hardiman, Hannah Wright, and Daniel Mills. September 3, 2013.
  36. ^ https://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&as_sdt=0%2C24&q=definition+shock+collar+pain&btnG=#d=gs_qabs&t=1684771995308&u=%23p%3DjOchC957l3EJ Electronic collars “hurt”. Dr Diane Frank. Australian Veterinary Journal 77 (6), 408-409, 1999
  37. ^ Lindsay (2005), p. 569.
  38. ^ Klein, Dieter R., 2000, How Dangerous are Remote Stimulation Devices for the Training of Dogs?” Amtstierärztlicher Dienst und Lebensmittelkontrolle
  39. ^ Dieter Klein, "How Dangerous are Remote Stimulation Devices for the Training of Dogs?" originally published in Amtstierärzelicher Dienst und Lebensmittelkontrolle, 2000.
  40. ^ Christiansen, Frank O., Morten, Braastadb, Bjorne O., Behavioural changes and aversive conditioning in hunting dogs by the second-year confrontation with domestic sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Volume 72, pp. 131-143.
  41. ^ Friedersdorf, Conor (2015-06-25). "High-Voltage Handcuffs Mean Cops Can Shock Prisoners From Across the Room". The Atlantic. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  42. ^ "USA: Cruelty in control? the stun belt and other electro-shock equipment in law enforcement". Amnesty International. 1999-06-07. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  43. ^ "United States of America: Use of electro-shock stun belts". Amnesty International. 1996-06-11. Retrieved 2023-05-23.
  44. ^ "United States of America: Use of electro-shock stun belts". Amnesty International. 1996-06-11. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  45. ^ Eugene F Roth Jr, Inge Lunde, Gudrun Boysen, Inge Kemp Genefkez. “Torture and its treatment.” American journal of public health 77 (11), 1404-1406, 1987.
  46. ^ 4] "Banned Devices; Proposal To Ban Electrical Stimulation Devices Used To Treat Self-Injurious or Aggressive Behavior". Federal Register. 2016-04-25. Retrieved 2020-08-08.
  47. ^ "CPSC NEISS On-Line Query System". U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  48. ^ The Fire Protection Research Foundation. Multiple cause of death, 1999-2015 results form. Available at: https://wonder.cdc.gov/controller/datarequest/D77. Accessed October 10, 2018. (Government database)
  49. ^ Illich, Paul A., Tamara E. Kong, and James W. Grau. “Impact of shock on pain reactivity: I. Whether hypo- or hyperalgesia is observed on how pain reactivity is tested.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 21.4 (1995): 331.
  50. ^ "CPSC NEISS On-Line Query System". U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  51. ^ The Fire Protection Research Foundation. Multiple cause of death, 1999-2015 results form. Available at: https://wonder.cdc.gov/controller/datarequest/D77. Accessed October 10, 2018. (Government database)
  52. ^ Binns, Corey (17 April 2006). "The Shocking Truth Behind Static Electricity". LiveScience. Retrieved 26 July 2018.
  53. ^ John Cadick et. al. (ed.) Electrical Safety Hamdbook Third Edition, McGraw Hill, 2005 ISBN 978-0-07-145772-9 page 1-4
  54. ^ https://www.osha.gov/sites/default/files/2019-04/Basic_Electricity_Materials.pdf Train-the-Trainer: Basic Electricity Safety This material was produced under a Susan Harwood Training Grant #SH-24896-SH3 from the Occupational Safety and Health Administration, U.S. Department of Labor. Public Domain This article incorporates text from this source, which is in the public domain.
  55. ^ Electronic shock collars: are they worth the risks?. Richard H Polsky. Journal of the American Animal Hospital Association 30 (5), 463-468, 1994.
  56. ^ Schalke, E.; Stichnoth, J.; Ott, S.; Jones-Baade, R. (2007-07-01). "Clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. Veterinary Behavioural Medicine. 105 (4): 369–380. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.11.002. ISSN 0168-1591.
  57. ^ US6459378B2, Gerig, Duane A., "Animal shock collar with low impedance transformer", issued 2002-10-01 
  58. ^ a b FDA (20 February 2020). "FDA Compliance enforcement". Food and Drug Administration.
  59. ^ Innotek. "Innotek patent".
  60. ^ Masson, Sylvia; de la Vega, Silvia; Gazzano, Angelo; Mariti, Chiara; Pereira, Gonçalo Da Graça; Halsberghe, Christine; Muser Leyvraz, Anneli; McPeake, Kevin; Schoening, Barbara (2018-05-01). "Electronic training devices: Discussion on the pros and cons of their use in dogs as a basis for the position statement of the European Society of Veterinary Clinical Ethology". Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 25: 71–75. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2018.02.006. ISSN 1558-7878. S2CID 148769178.
  61. ^ Berry, Alistair; Lincoln, University of. "Electronic collars less effective than reward-based training, research finds". phys.org. Retrieved 2023-04-21.
  62. ^ Andelt, William F.; Phillips, Robert L.; Gruver, Kenneth S.; Guthrie, Jerry W. (1999). "Coyote Predation on Domestic Sheep Deterred with Electronic Dog-Training Collar". Wildlife Society Bulletin. 27 (1): 12–18. JSTOR 3783933.
  63. ^ Christiansen, Frank O., Bakkenb, Morten, Braastadb, Bjarne O., 2001, Behavioural differences between three breed groups of hunting dogs confronted with domestic sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Volume 72(2), pp. 115-129.
  64. ^ Christiansen, Frank O., Bakkenb, Morten, Braastadb, Bjarne O., 2001, Behavioural changes and aversive conditioning in hunting dogs by the second-year confrontation with domestic sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Volume 72, pp. 131–143.
  65. ^ a b Salgirli, Yasemin (2008). "Comparison of stress and learning effects of three different training methods" (Document). {{cite document}}: Cite document requires |publisher= (help); Unknown parameter |url= ignored (help)
  66. ^ Schalke, E., Stichnoth, J., Ott, S., Jones-Baade, R., 2007, Clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105, pp. 369-380.
  67. ^ Schilder, M.B.H., van der Borg, J.A.M, 2004, Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short and long term behavioral effects. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85, pp. 319-334.
  68. ^ a b Lindsay (2005), pp. 611–612.
  69. ^ Steiss, J.E., Schaffer, C., Ahmad, H.A., and Voith, V.L., 2007, Evaluation of Plasma Cortisol Levels and Behavior in Dogs Wearing Bark Control Collars. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106, pp. 96-106.
  70. ^ Tortora, Daniel F. Safety training: The elimination of avoidance-motivated aggression in dogs. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1984-03181-001 https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.112.2.176
  71. ^ "Ban on electric shock collars for pets". BBC News. 2018-08-27. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  72. ^ "'No more effective than training': Brussels to ban controversial dog collars". www.brusselstimes.com. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  73. ^ HSUS. "HSUS statement".
  74. ^ PETA (10 August 2010). "Electric Fences and Shock Collars".
  75. ^ "England to ban 'cruel' dog training device after 10-year campaign". The Independent. 2023-04-28. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  76. ^ "ACPO Police Dogs Manual of Guidance" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-07-02.
  77. ^ https://m.facebook.com/RSPCA/photos/a.76251508046/10157732465378047/?type=3
  78. ^ "'Cruel' electric shock collars banned in England | Kennel Club". www.thekennelclub.org.uk. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  79. ^ Kennel Club. "Kennel Club Campaign".
  80. ^ WUSV. "WUSV Ban". Archived from the original on 2012-03-25.
  81. ^ "Shock collars for dogs slammed by vets, RSPCA, as hundreds sign petition to ban - ABC News". amp.abc.net.au. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  82. ^ "Official APDT Position Statements | APDT | LIMA | Statement". APDT. Retrieved 2023-06-02.
  83. ^ https://avsab.org/wp-content/uploads/2021/08/AVSAB-Humane-Dog-Training-Position-Statement-2021.pdf AVSAB Humane Dog Training Position Statement
  84. ^ Lindsay (2005), p. 586.
  85. ^ IACP. "IACP Position Paper on Training Tools" (PDF).
  86. ^ "Electric Shock Collars". Kennel Club UK.
  87. ^ "Germany - Cruelty - German Animal Welfare Act | Animal Legal & Historical Center".
  88. ^ "Dog Keeping laws for Germany".
  89. ^ "Elektronische halsband voor honden vanaf volgend jaar verboden". 4 April 2019.
  90. ^ "Netherlands to ban shock collars for dogs, limit breeding of 'designer cats'".
  91. ^ "Achtung vor verbotenen Halsbändern für Hunde". Heute.at (in German). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  92. ^ "Fynsk dyrlæge forarget over salg af stødhalsbånd: Hundene ved ikke, hvad der rammer dem". TV2 Fyn (in Danish). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  93. ^ "Animal Welfare Decree" (PDF). Ministry of Agriculture and Forestry.
  94. ^ "FINLEX ® - Ajantasainen lainsäädäntö: Eläinsuojelulaki 247/1996".
  95. ^ "Notkun rafmagnsólarinnar tilkynnt sem ill meðferð til MAST". Mannlíf.is (in Icelandic). 2019-01-17. Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  96. ^ "Rafmagnsólar hunda bannaðar - Vísir". visir.is (in Icelandic). 15 July 2011. Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  97. ^ Lindin, Ina-Kristin (2017-01-27). "Dømt for bruk av strømhalsbånd". NRK (in Norwegian Bokmål). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  98. ^ Gernes, Jonatan (2021-05-20). "Elhalsband gav plågsamma skador på hunden". hallandsposten.se (in Swedish). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  99. ^ "Is the use of electronic dog collars legal?". RSPCA Australia.
  100. ^ a b Arana, Lucía (2016-10-14). "El alto precio de los collares de castigo". ElDiario.es (in Spanish). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  101. ^ "Los collares eléctricos, prohibidos para adiestrar perros de caza en Castilla-La Mancha". Revista Jara y Sedal (in Spanish). 2020-09-24. Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  102. ^ Irati (2018-09-25). "¿Está permitido el uso de collares eléctricos para perros?". Jaime Valladolid (in Spanish). Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  103. ^ "Flanders to ban shock collars for pets". 11 August 2021.
  104. ^ "Uitdoofscenario voor stroomhalsbanden, verbod vanaf 2027".
  105. ^ "Stroomhalsbanden voor honden verboden vanaf 2027". 10 August 2021.
  106. ^ "England Moves to Ban Electric Shock Collars for Dogs | Psychology Today United Kingdom". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2023-05-02.
  107. ^ "Great News For Dogs As Switzerland Passes Law To Ban Cruel Anti-Barking Devices". Holidog Times EN. 2018-02-13. Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  108. ^ "Ordonnances du domaine vétérinaire: modifications". www.admin.ch. Retrieved 2022-04-21.
  109. ^ "Dogfight nets RSPCA chief $30,000". The Age. 6 July 2002. Retrieved 27 July 2018.
  110. ^ "Orion Pet Products Pty Ltd v Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (Vic)". Federal Court of Australia. Federal Court of Australia. Retrieved 27 July 2018.
  111. ^ Catriona Murdoch (23 November 2010). "Electric shock pet collar ban did not breach human rights". UK Human Rights Blog.
  112. ^ "Shock collar found on pet dog roaming on Vale beach". BBC News. April 18, 2011.
  113. ^ "Ogmore illegal shock collar dog owner gets £2,000 fine". BBC News. July 18, 2011.

External references

  • Lindsay, Steven R. (2000), Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training, vol. 1, Blackwell, p. 136.
  • Lindsay, Steven R. (2005), Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior Vol 3, Blackwell, pp. 557–633.
  • Polsky, R.H. (2000), "Can aggression be elicited through electronic pet containment systems", Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, vol. 3, no. 4, pp. 345–357, doi:10.1207/S15327604JAWS0304_6, S2CID 144523674.
  • Schalke, E.; Stichnoth, J.; Ott, S.; Jones-Baade, R. (2007), "Clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations", Applied Animal Behaviour Science, vol. 105, no. 4, p. 369, doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.11.002.
  • Schilder, Matthijs B.H.; van der Borg, Joanne A.M. (2004), "Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short and long term behavioural effects", Applied Animal Behaviour Science, vol. 85, no. 3–4, pp. 319–334, doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2003.10.004.
  • Andelt, William F.; Phillips, Robert L.; Gruver, Kenneth S.; Guthrie, Jerry W. (Spring 1999), "Coyote Predation on Domestic Sheep Deterred with Electronic Dog-Training Collar", Wildlife Society Bulletin, 27 (1): 12–18.
  • Hawley, Jason E.; Rossler, Shawn T.; Gehring, Thomas M.; Schultz, Ronald N.; Callahan, Peggy A.; Clark, Raymond; Cade, Jerry; Wydeven, Adrian P. (2013), "Developing a New Shock-Collar Design for Safe and Efficient Use on Wild Wolves", Wildlife Society Bulletin, 37 (2): 416–422, doi:10.1002/wsb.234, JSTOR wildsocibull2011.37.2.416
  • Hawley, Jason E.; Gehring, Thomas M.; Schultz, Ronald N.; Rossler, Shawn T.; Wydeven, Adrian P. (2009). "Assessment of Shock Collars as Nonlethal Management for Wolves in Wisconsin". The Journal of Wildlife Management. 73 (4): 518–525. doi:10.2193/2007-066. JSTOR 40208400. S2CID 85070426.
  • Reynolds, Linda (ed.). Companion Dog Obedience Training With Electronic Collars.[full citation needed]

See also