Jump to content

User:Malex7980/Color psychology: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Malex7980 (talk | contribs)
Peer Review Summary Statement
Malex7980 (talk | contribs)
Age[edit]: created wikilinks
Line 4: Line 4:


=== Age[edit] ===
=== Age[edit] ===
Children's toys for younger age groups are often marketed based on color, but as the age group increases, color becomes less gender stereotyped. In general, many toys become gender neutral and hence adopt gender-neutral colors. In the United States it is common to associate baby girls with pink and baby boys with blue. This difference in young children is a learned difference rather than an inborn one. Research has looked at the preference of young children, ages 7 months to 5 years, for small objects in assorted colors. The results showed that by the age of 2–2.5 years socially constructed gendered colors affects children's color preference, where girls prefer pink and boys avoid pink, but show no preference for other colors.
Children's toys for younger age groups are often marketed based on color, but as the age group increases, color becomes less [[Gender role|gender stereotyped]]. In general, many toys become gender neutral and hence adopt gender-neutral colors. In the United States it is common to associate baby girls with pink and baby boys with blue. This difference in young children is a learned difference rather than an inborn one. Research has looked at the preference of young children, ages 7 months to 5 years, for small objects in assorted colors. The results showed that by the age of 2–2.5 years socially constructed gendered colors affects children's color preference, where girls prefer pink and boys avoid pink, but show no preference for other colors.


Contrary to the adult fondness for blue, in children yellow is the most favored color, owing to its associations with happiness. However, children like colors they find to be pleasant and comforting and their preferences do not change much, while adult color preference is usually easily influenced. Slightly older children who have developed a sense of favorite color often tend to pick items that are in that color.
Contrary to the adult fondness for blue, in children yellow is the most favored color, owing to its associations with [[happiness]]. However, children like colors they find to be pleasant and comforting and their preferences do not change much, while adult color preference is usually easily influenced. Slightly older children who have developed a sense of favorite color often tend to pick items that are in that color.


Children's perceptions for color can be seen in a preference for a certain color of a food. The taste perceptions of all colors are not all the same with children and adults. "Each primary color has its own specific taste and the taste of secondary colors is a common taste of their constituent primary colors. Primary colors such as red and yellow, would indicate a similar taste when comparing pink and orange in a red/pink and yellow/orange pairing. <ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |last=Fateminia |first=Mehran |last2=Ghotbabadi |first2=Talayeh Dehghani |last3=Azad |first3=Kamran Mohammadi |date=2020-08 |title=Perceptions of the taste of colors in children and adults |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/col.22503 |journal=Color Research & Application |language=en |volume=45 |issue=4 |pages=743–753 |doi=10.1002/col.22503 |issn=0361-2317}}</ref> It is perceived that infants prefer the pastel pinks and blues that are considered traditionally pleasant for a babies room. Busy patterns and strong colors in an infant's room is thought to contribute to hyperactivity, lack of sleep, and restlessness.<ref name=":1" /> Color priming and infant could be an example of acquired distinctiveness, which is one form of learning that suggests one source of information can predict the outcome, and any future associations with a color could be altered by the nature of this relation.<ref name=":2" /> Still, in other research, using pastels or neutral colors can affect the infant's perception to recognize color and give a better insight into their world of color psychology.
Children's perceptions for color can be seen in a preference for a certain color of a food. The [[Taste perception|taste perceptions]] of all colors are not all the same with children and adults. [[Primary colors of light|Primary colors]] have its own specific taste and the taste of secondary colors is a common to taste of the alike primary colors. Primary colors such as red and yellow, would indicate a similar taste when comparing pink and orange in a red/pink and yellow/orange pairing. <ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |last=Fateminia |first=Mehran |last2=Ghotbabadi |first2=Talayeh Dehghani |last3=Azad |first3=Kamran Mohammadi |date=2020-08 |title=Perceptions of the taste of colors in children and adults |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/col.22503 |journal=Color Research & Application |language=en |volume=45 |issue=4 |pages=743–753 |doi=10.1002/col.22503 |issn=0361-2317}}</ref> It is perceived that infants prefer the pastel pinks and blues that are considered traditionally pleasant for a babies room. Busy patterns and strong colors in an infant's room is thought to contribute to hyperactivity, lack of sleep, and restlessness.<ref name=":1" /> Color priming and infant could be an example of acquired distinctiveness, which is one form of learning that suggests one source of information can predict the outcome, and any future associations with a color could be altered by the nature of this relation.<ref name=":2" /> Still, in other research, using pastels or neutral colors can affect the infant's perception to recognize color and give a better insight into their world of color psychology.


Ecological valence theory has been cited as a plausible reason for differences in color preferences between adults and infants. Because adults have more associations between colors and objects or places from life experiences, their preferences change as they get older.
Ecological valence theory has been cited as a plausible reason for differences in color preferences between adults and infants. Because adults have more associations between colors and objects or places from life experiences, their preferences change as they get older.

Revision as of 14:22, 2 July 2024

Article Draft

Age[edit]

Children's toys for younger age groups are often marketed based on color, but as the age group increases, color becomes less gender stereotyped. In general, many toys become gender neutral and hence adopt gender-neutral colors. In the United States it is common to associate baby girls with pink and baby boys with blue. This difference in young children is a learned difference rather than an inborn one. Research has looked at the preference of young children, ages 7 months to 5 years, for small objects in assorted colors. The results showed that by the age of 2–2.5 years socially constructed gendered colors affects children's color preference, where girls prefer pink and boys avoid pink, but show no preference for other colors.

Contrary to the adult fondness for blue, in children yellow is the most favored color, owing to its associations with happiness. However, children like colors they find to be pleasant and comforting and their preferences do not change much, while adult color preference is usually easily influenced. Slightly older children who have developed a sense of favorite color often tend to pick items that are in that color.

Children's perceptions for color can be seen in a preference for a certain color of a food. The taste perceptions of all colors are not all the same with children and adults. Primary colors have its own specific taste and the taste of secondary colors is a common to taste of the alike primary colors. Primary colors such as red and yellow, would indicate a similar taste when comparing pink and orange in a red/pink and yellow/orange pairing. [1] It is perceived that infants prefer the pastel pinks and blues that are considered traditionally pleasant for a babies room. Busy patterns and strong colors in an infant's room is thought to contribute to hyperactivity, lack of sleep, and restlessness.[2] Color priming and infant could be an example of acquired distinctiveness, which is one form of learning that suggests one source of information can predict the outcome, and any future associations with a color could be altered by the nature of this relation.[3] Still, in other research, using pastels or neutral colors can affect the infant's perception to recognize color and give a better insight into their world of color psychology.

Ecological valence theory has been cited as a plausible reason for differences in color preferences between adults and infants. Because adults have more associations between colors and objects or places from life experiences, their preferences change as they get older.

Lead

Article body

  1. ^ Fateminia, Mehran; Ghotbabadi, Talayeh Dehghani; Azad, Kamran Mohammadi (2020-08). "Perceptions of the taste of colors in children and adults". Color Research & Application. 45 (4): 743–753. doi:10.1002/col.22503. ISSN 0361-2317. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  2. ^ a b Ajilian Abbasi, Maryam; Talaei, Ali; Talaei, Ardeshir; Rezaei, Ali (2014-10-01). "The use of appropriate colors in the design of children's rooms: A Short Review". International Journal of Pediatrics. 2 (4.1): 305–312. doi:10.22038/ijp.2014.3204. ISSN 2345-5047.
  3. ^ a b Wilcox, Teresa; Hirshkowitz, Amy; Hawkins, Laura; Boas, David A. (2014-01-15). "The Effect of Color Priming on Infant Brain and Behavior". NeuroImage. 85 (0 1): 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.045. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.045. ISSN 1053-8119. PMC 3871198. PMID 24007805.
  4. ^ "Color Matters and Child Development | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2024-06-05.

[1]

[2]

  1. ^ Shutts, Kristin; Kinzler, Katherine D.; McKee, Caitlin B.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. (2009-04-30). "Social Information Guides Infants' Selection of Foods". Journal of Cognition and Development. 10 (1–2): 1–17. doi:10.1080/15248370902966636. ISSN 1524-8372. PMC 2756712. PMID 19809590.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  2. ^ Cite error: The named reference :0 was invoked but never defined (see the help page).

PEER REVIEW SUMMARY STATEMENT

I read the peer reviews from members of my LC Milgram Group. I have made the following changes as suggest by:

Megan- I looked up and read the story of the Sad Beige Mom (I've never heard it before) and I used the lead sentence as a new sentence for a paragraph within the topic that I am editing.

Raniya- I did start to add more to my article than when you reviewed it and I fixed grammatical errors as suggested, Also I did not add a section about color psychology in adults as this is already a section in the bigger piece of the article already published on Wikipedia.

Caroline- I chose not to delve into other countries and the infant's perception of colors because I am already finding and working on a lot of information for just the US, and I did not want to overwhelm myself. I did, however, find a few more academic resources as you suggested.

Akeiah- Thank you for your confidence in me and I did seek more articles about infants and color perception.