Repressed memory: Difference between revisions
No edit summary |
No edit summary |
||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{totallydisputed}} |
{{totallydisputed}} |
||
'''Repressed memory''' is a highly controversial theory regarding a significant [[memory]], usually of a [[Psychological trauma|traumatic]] nature, that has become unavailable for recall. The term is used to describe memories that have been [[Dissociation (psychology)|dissociated]] from awareness as well as those that have been [[psychological repression|repressed]] without dissociation. The existence of repressed memories is not accepted by mainstream psychology. A recent amicus curiae brief to the California Supreme Court drafted by psychologist/attorney R. C. Barden and signed by nearly 100 international experts in the field of human memory emphasized there is "no credible scientific support" for the controversial notions of repressed and recovered memories. The experts summarized the opinion of the relevant scientific community by stating "Decades of research and scientific debate have clarified over and over again, that the notion of traumatic events being somehow “repressed” and later accurately recovered is one of the most pernicious bits of folklore ever to infect psychology and psychiatry. " See, Barden, R. C. Amicus Brief in Taus v. Loftus, Supreme Court of California, Feb. 21, 2006. A small band of clinicians continue to argue for the credibility of "repressed memories" but most all of the proponents of this theory suffered license revocations, licensing disciplinary actions, or malpractice lawsuits. |
|||
'''Repressed memory''' is a significant [[memory]], usually of a [[Psychological trauma|traumatic]] nature, that has become unavailable for recall. The term is used to describe memories that have been [[Dissociation (psychology)|dissociated]] from awareness as well as those that have been [[psychological repression|repressed]] without dissociation. The existence of repressed memories is not totally accepted by mainstream psychology, although some studies have concluded that amnesia can occur in victims of trauma.<ref name="widom1"> {{cite journal | last = Widom | first = Cathy Spatz | coauthors = Shepard, Robin L. | year = 1996 | month = December | title = Accuracy of adult recollections of childhood victimization : Part 1. Childhood physical abuse | journal = Psychological Assessment | volume = 8 | issue = 4 | pages = 412–21 | publisher = American Psychological Association | location = Washington, DC, US | issn = 1040-3590 | id = EJ542113 | url = http://www.eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/custom/portlets/recordDetails/detailmini.jsp?_nfpb=true&_&ERICExtSearch_SearchValue_0=EJ542113&ERICExtSearch_SearchType_0=no&accno=EJ542113 | accessdate = 2007-12-18 | doi = 10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.412}} </ref><ref ="widom2"> {{cite journal | last = Widom | first = Cathy Spatz | coauthors = Morris, Suzanne | year = 1997 | month = March | title = Accuracy of Adult Recollections of Childhood Victimization: Part 2. Childhood Sexual Abuse. | journal = Psychological Assessment | volume = 9 | issue = 1 | pages = 34–46 | publisher = American Psychological Association | location = Washington, DC, US | issn = 1040-3590 | id = EJ545434 | url = http://www.eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/Home.portal?_nfpb=true&ERICExtSearch_SearchValue_0=EJ545434&searchtype=keyword&ERICExtSearch_SearchType_0=no&_pageLabel=RecordDetails&objectId=0900019b801126a6&accno=EJ545434&_nfls=false | accessdate = 2007-12-18 | doi = 10.1037/1040-3590.9.1.34}}</ref><ref name="sheflin"> <!-- This study showed 38% based upon criminal records http://books.google.com/books?id=qBSuIMgJLNYC&pg=PA147&vq=sheflin&dq=sheflin+brown+1996+amnesia+%22Repressed+memory+or+dissociative%22&lr=&sig=zCjaGFdaY_qCcNmZtJwFsQRd4YY -->{{cite journal | last = Sheflin | first = Alan W | coauthors = Brown, Daniel |date=1996 | title = Repressed Memory or Dissociative Amnesia: What the Science Says. | journal = Journal of Psychiatry & Law | volume = 24 | issue = Summer | pages = 143–88 | issn = 0093-1853 | accessdate = 2007-12-18 }} </ref><ref name="herman119">{{cite book|title=Trauma and recovery: The aftermath of violence from domestic abuse to political terror |first=Judith Lewis |last=Herman |year=1997| publisher=Basic Books |pages= p119-122 |isbn= 0465087302}}</ref> According to the theory's proponents, repressed memories may sometimes be recovered years or decades after the event, most often spontaneously, triggered by a particular smell, taste, or other identifier related to the lost memory, or via suggestion during [[psychotherapy]].<ref name="dissoc-9-4">{{cite journal|title=Memory recovery of childhood sexual abuse |last=Albach |first=Francine |coauthors=Peter Paul Moormann, |
|||
Bob Bermond |journal=Dissociation |volume=Vol. 9, No. 4, p. 261-273 |url=http://hdl.handle.net/1794/1774 |date=Dec-1996 |accessdate=2008-01-03 |issn=0896-2863}}</ref> It is not currently possible to distinguish a true repressed memory from a false one without corroborating evidence.<ref>[http://www.apa.org/topics/memories.html Questions and Answers about Memories of Childhood Abuse], American Psychological Association.</ref> |
|||
Most commentators agree that the most significant recovered memory lawsuit was the Illinois case of Burgus v. Braun resulting in a world record 10.6 Million dollar settlement. This case is often noted as the end of the repressed memory movement in the U.S. Reported on page one, column one of the NY Times the Burgus case galvanized opposition to recovered memory practices. As quoted in the New York Times page one article on the Burgus settlement, The next thing I think there will be is legislation to require informed consent from psychiatric patients for such [recovered memory] 'treatments', said Dr. R. Christopher Barden, a psychologist and lawyer [for the plaintiff Burgus]... I think insurance companies will stop reimbursing people for mental health treatments that are not proven safe and effective. This is the death knell for recovered memory therapy. See, Belluck, P. Recovered Memory Therapy Leads to a Lawsuit and Big Settlement [$10.6 Million], The New York Times, Page 1, Column 1, Nov. 6, 1997; See also, Belluck, P. She Recovered Memories, Then Millions in Damages, The New York Times, Nov 9, 1997, Sec. 4, Week in Review, page 2, Column 3. |
|||
The legal system has also rejected the notion of "repressed memory". Competently conducted Frye/Daubert/Kumho science-law hearings increasingly reflect the state of scientific knowledge in this area and routinely reject “repressed-recovered memory” testimony. See, e.g., Grove, W. M. & Barden, R.C. (2000) Protecting the Integrity of the Legal System : The Admissibility of Testimony from Mental Health Experts Under Daubert/Kumho Analyses, Psychology, Public Policy and Law, Vol 5, No. 1, 234-242. ; See, also Hammane, et al. v. Humenansky, Ramsey County Minnesota File No. C4-94-203, Order of Judge B. Poritsky, June 30, 1995, Transcript page 83-84. “there is no agreement by experts that there is general acceptance that such [recovered memory] evidence is reliable and trustworthy. That’s the Frye standard. As to the Daubert standard, it is also my ruling that such [recovered memory] evidence is not reliable nor helpful to the jury.”; See similar trial court ruling in Carlson v. Humenansky (Minnesota Trial Ct), Judge B. Poritsky (January, 1996). ; State of New Hampshire v. Hungerford and State of New Hampshire v. Morahan 698 A.2d 1244 (N.H. 1997) "The phenomenon of recovery of repressed memories has not yet reached the point where we may perceive these particular recovered memories as reliable."; State of New Hampshire v. Walters 697 A.2d 916 (N.H. 1997) "[W]e conclude, as we did in Hungerford , that " [t]he indicia of reliability present in the particular memories in [this] case do not rise to such a level that they overcome the divisive state of the scientific debate on the issue."; State of Rhode Island v. Quattrocchi, C.A. No. P92-3759 (R.I. 1999) [on remand from the Rhode Island Supreme Court 681 A.2d 879 (R.I. 1999)] "The State has not met its burden of establishing that repressed recollection is reliable and admissible as scientific evidence."; State of New Hampshire vs. Bourgelais, Docket No. 02-S-2834, Judge T. Nadeau, April 4, 2005. “the State’s motion [to use repressed memory evidence at trial] is denied… the court determines, based on the law and the evidence, that the reliability of memory retrieval has not been sufficiently established…” ; Rivers v. Father Flanagan’s Boys Town, Doc 1024, Case No. 743, Nebraska State Court Judge S. Dougherty, November 25, 2005. “... the Court finds and concludes that Plaintiff has not met his burden of establishing that repressed and recovered memory is reliable and admissible as scientific evidence or that it is properly applied in this case. The Plaintiff’s evidence lacks the scientific reliability and proper application necessary... the Court finds and concludes that the Defendants’ Motion [banning all testimony regarding repressed and recovered memories] shall be sustained.” ; Given the state of the evidence many litigants are simply withdrawing Daubert motions, See, e.g., Duffy v. Father Flanagan’s Boys Town, Case No. 8:03CV31, United States District Court for the District of Nebraska, Memorandum and Order of January 26, 2006 by L. Smith Camp, U.S. District Judge. “[Plaintiff] ... filed a motion of withdrawal of expert testimony on the issue of repressed memory.... [thus] judgment will be granted to [Defendant].”; and many other cases. |
|||
== History == |
== History == |
Revision as of 13:27, 25 June 2008
Template:Totallydisputed Repressed memory is a highly controversial theory regarding a significant memory, usually of a traumatic nature, that has become unavailable for recall. The term is used to describe memories that have been dissociated from awareness as well as those that have been repressed without dissociation. The existence of repressed memories is not accepted by mainstream psychology. A recent amicus curiae brief to the California Supreme Court drafted by psychologist/attorney R. C. Barden and signed by nearly 100 international experts in the field of human memory emphasized there is "no credible scientific support" for the controversial notions of repressed and recovered memories. The experts summarized the opinion of the relevant scientific community by stating "Decades of research and scientific debate have clarified over and over again, that the notion of traumatic events being somehow “repressed” and later accurately recovered is one of the most pernicious bits of folklore ever to infect psychology and psychiatry. " See, Barden, R. C. Amicus Brief in Taus v. Loftus, Supreme Court of California, Feb. 21, 2006. A small band of clinicians continue to argue for the credibility of "repressed memories" but most all of the proponents of this theory suffered license revocations, licensing disciplinary actions, or malpractice lawsuits.
Most commentators agree that the most significant recovered memory lawsuit was the Illinois case of Burgus v. Braun resulting in a world record 10.6 Million dollar settlement. This case is often noted as the end of the repressed memory movement in the U.S. Reported on page one, column one of the NY Times the Burgus case galvanized opposition to recovered memory practices. As quoted in the New York Times page one article on the Burgus settlement, The next thing I think there will be is legislation to require informed consent from psychiatric patients for such [recovered memory] 'treatments', said Dr. R. Christopher Barden, a psychologist and lawyer [for the plaintiff Burgus]... I think insurance companies will stop reimbursing people for mental health treatments that are not proven safe and effective. This is the death knell for recovered memory therapy. See, Belluck, P. Recovered Memory Therapy Leads to a Lawsuit and Big Settlement [$10.6 Million], The New York Times, Page 1, Column 1, Nov. 6, 1997; See also, Belluck, P. She Recovered Memories, Then Millions in Damages, The New York Times, Nov 9, 1997, Sec. 4, Week in Review, page 2, Column 3.
The legal system has also rejected the notion of "repressed memory". Competently conducted Frye/Daubert/Kumho science-law hearings increasingly reflect the state of scientific knowledge in this area and routinely reject “repressed-recovered memory” testimony. See, e.g., Grove, W. M. & Barden, R.C. (2000) Protecting the Integrity of the Legal System : The Admissibility of Testimony from Mental Health Experts Under Daubert/Kumho Analyses, Psychology, Public Policy and Law, Vol 5, No. 1, 234-242. ; See, also Hammane, et al. v. Humenansky, Ramsey County Minnesota File No. C4-94-203, Order of Judge B. Poritsky, June 30, 1995, Transcript page 83-84. “there is no agreement by experts that there is general acceptance that such [recovered memory] evidence is reliable and trustworthy. That’s the Frye standard. As to the Daubert standard, it is also my ruling that such [recovered memory] evidence is not reliable nor helpful to the jury.”; See similar trial court ruling in Carlson v. Humenansky (Minnesota Trial Ct), Judge B. Poritsky (January, 1996). ; State of New Hampshire v. Hungerford and State of New Hampshire v. Morahan 698 A.2d 1244 (N.H. 1997) "The phenomenon of recovery of repressed memories has not yet reached the point where we may perceive these particular recovered memories as reliable."; State of New Hampshire v. Walters 697 A.2d 916 (N.H. 1997) "[W]e conclude, as we did in Hungerford , that " [t]he indicia of reliability present in the particular memories in [this] case do not rise to such a level that they overcome the divisive state of the scientific debate on the issue."; State of Rhode Island v. Quattrocchi, C.A. No. P92-3759 (R.I. 1999) [on remand from the Rhode Island Supreme Court 681 A.2d 879 (R.I. 1999)] "The State has not met its burden of establishing that repressed recollection is reliable and admissible as scientific evidence."; State of New Hampshire vs. Bourgelais, Docket No. 02-S-2834, Judge T. Nadeau, April 4, 2005. “the State’s motion [to use repressed memory evidence at trial] is denied… the court determines, based on the law and the evidence, that the reliability of memory retrieval has not been sufficiently established…” ; Rivers v. Father Flanagan’s Boys Town, Doc 1024, Case No. 743, Nebraska State Court Judge S. Dougherty, November 25, 2005. “... the Court finds and concludes that Plaintiff has not met his burden of establishing that repressed and recovered memory is reliable and admissible as scientific evidence or that it is properly applied in this case. The Plaintiff’s evidence lacks the scientific reliability and proper application necessary... the Court finds and concludes that the Defendants’ Motion [banning all testimony regarding repressed and recovered memories] shall be sustained.” ; Given the state of the evidence many litigants are simply withdrawing Daubert motions, See, e.g., Duffy v. Father Flanagan’s Boys Town, Case No. 8:03CV31, United States District Court for the District of Nebraska, Memorandum and Order of January 26, 2006 by L. Smith Camp, U.S. District Judge. “[Plaintiff] ... filed a motion of withdrawal of expert testimony on the issue of repressed memory.... [thus] judgment will be granted to [Defendant].”; and many other cases.
History
The concept was originated by Sigmund Freud in his 1896 essay Zur Ätiologie der Hysterie ("On the etiology of hysteria"). Freud abandoned his theory between 1897 and 1905, replacing it during 1920-1923 with his impulse-based concept of Id, Super-ego, and Ego. Friedrich Nietzsche was the first to suggest an active, conscious thought management method in the second essay of his On the Genealogy of Morals as a necessary fundament of efficiency, responsibility, and maturity. Also called motivated forgetting in which a subject blocks out painful or traumatic times in one's life.
Research
Repressed memory syndrome, the clinical entity used to describe repressed memories, is often compared to psychogenic amnesia, and some sources compare the two as equivalent.[1][2] Some research indicates that memories of child sexual abuse and other traumatic incidents can be forgotten.[3][4][5][6] Evidence of the spontaneous recovery of traumatic memories has been shown,[7][8][9] and recovered memories of traumatic childhood abuse have been corroborated.[10][11][12][6][13] Although the science of repressed memory is limited, a few studies have suggested that memories of trauma that are forgotten and later recalled have a similar accuracy rate as trauma memories that had not been forgotten.[1] It has been speculated that repression may be one method used by individuals to cope with traumatic memories, by pushing them out of awareness[5] (perhaps as an adaptation via psychogenic amnesia) to allow a child to maintain attachment to a person on whom they are dependent for survival.[14] Researchers have proposed that repression can operate on a social level as well.[15]
The existence of repressed memories has not been completely accepted by mainstream psychology,[16][17][18][19] nor unequivocally proven to exist, and some experts in the field of human memory feel that no credible scientific support exists for the notions of repressed/recovered memories.[20] One research report states that a distinction should be made between spontaneously recovered memories and memories recovered during suggestions in therapy. [21]
van der Kolk and Fisler's research shows that traumatic memories are retrieved, at least at first, in the form of mental imprints that are dissociated. These imprints are of the affective and sensory elements of the traumatic experience. Clients have reported the slow emergence of a personal narrative that can be considered explicit (conscious) memory. The level of emotional significance of a memory correlates directly with the memory's veracity. Studies of subjective reports of memory show that memories of highly significant events are unusually accurate and stable over time. The imprints of traumatic experiences appear to be qualitatively different from those of nontraumatic events. Traumatic memories may be coded differently than ordinary event memories, possibly because of alterations in attentional focusing or the fact that extreme emotional arousal interferes with the memory functions of the hippocampus.[22]
Legal issues
Some criminal cases have been based on a witness' testimony of recovered repressed memories, often of alleged childhood sexual abuse. In some jurisdictions, the statute of limitations for child abuse cases has been extended to accommodate the phenomena of repressed memories as well as other factors. The repressed memory concept came into wider public awareness in the 1980s and 1990s followed by a reduction of public attention after a series of scandals, lawsuits, and license revocations.[23]
In a 1996 ruling, a US District Court allowed repressed memories entered into evidence in court cases.[24] Jennifer Freyd writes that Ross Cheit's case of suddenly remembered sexual abuse is one of the most well-documented cases available for the public to see. Cheit prevailed in two lawsuits, located five additional victims and tape-recorded a confession.[9]
Recovered memory therapy
Recovered memory therapy (RMT) is a term coined by affiliates of the False Memory Syndrome Foundation[25][26][27] referring to what they described as a range of psychotherapy methods based on recalling memories of abuse that had previously been forgotten by the patient.[28]The term is not listed in DSM-IV or used by mainstream formal psychotherapy modality.[25] The hypothesis that therapy can create false memories through suggestion techniques is controversial and has been neither proven nor disproven; some research has shown evidence supporting the hypothesis; and, the evidence is questioned by some researchers.[29] [25][30] Even when patients who decide their recovered memories are false and retract their claims, they can suffer posttraumatic stress disorder due to the trauma of illusory memories.[31] The number of reported retractions is small when compared to the large number of actual child sexual abuse cases.[32] Some have suggested that a child may retract their story of abuse due to guilt, a feeling of obligation to protect their family or may be reacting to the familial stress brought on by their allegations.[33]
See also
References
- ^ a b Scheflin, A (1999-11-01). "Ground lost: The false memory/recovered memory therapy debate". Psychiatric Times. 16 (11).
- ^ Fink, P (2005). "Press Release by the Leadership Council: Recovered Memories: True or False?". The Leadership Council. Retrieved 2008-02-06.
- ^ Williams LM (1994). "Recall of childhood trauma: a prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse". J Consult Clin Psychol. 62 (6): 1167–76. PMID 7860814.
- ^ Cheit, RE (2005-07-01). "The Archive: 101 Corroborated Cases of Recovered Memory". The Recovered Memory Project. Retrieved 2008-02-07.
- ^ a b "Research on the Effect of Trauma on Memory". The Leadership Council. 2005. Retrieved 2008-02-07.
- ^ a b Bowman, E.S. (1996). "Delayed memories of child abuse: Part I: An overview of research findings on forgetting, remembering, and corroborating trauma". Dissociation. 9: 221–231.; lay summary "Corroboration of child abuse memories". About.com. 2006-02-20. Retrieved 2008-01-06.
- ^ Chu, J (1999). "Memories of childhood abuse: dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration". American Journal of Psychiatry. 156 (5): 749–55. PMID 10327909.
{{cite journal}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Duggal, S. (1998). "Recovered memory of childhood sexual trauma: A documented case from a longitudinal study". Journal of Traumatic Stress. 11 (2): 301–321. doi:10.1023/A:1024403220769. Retrieved 2007-12-31.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ a b Freyd, Jennifer J. (1996). Betrayal Trauma - The Logic of Forgetting Childhood Abuse. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-06805-x.
{{cite book}}
: Check|isbn=
value: invalid character (help) - ^ Cheit, Ross E. (1998). "Consider This, Skeptics of Recovered Memory". ETHICS & BEHAVIOR. 8 (2): 141–160. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0802_4. Retrieved 2007-12-25.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|month=
(help) - ^ Corwin, D. (1997). "Videotaped Discovery of a Reportedly Unrecallable Memory of Child Sexual Abuse: Comparison with a Childhood Interview Videotaped 11 Years Before". Child Maltreatment. 2 (2): 91–112. doi:10.1177/1077559597002002001. Retrieved 2008-01-09.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Leadership Council on Child Abuse & Interpersonal Violence: Summary of Research Examining the Prevalence of Full or Partial Dissociative Amnesia for Traumatic Events
- ^ Template:Cite article
- ^ Freyd, J. (1994). "Betrayal trauma: traumatic amnesia as an adaptive response to childhood abuse" (PDF). Ethics & Behavior. 4 (4): 307–330. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0404_1. Retrieved 2008-01-13.
- ^ Freyd, Jennifer J. (2007). "Archiving Dissociation as a Precaution Against Dissociating Dissociation" (PDF). Journal of Trauma & Dissociation. 8 (3). Retrieved 2007-12-29.
- ^ McNally, R.J. (2004). "The Science and Folklore of Traumatic Amnesia". Clinical Psychology Science and Practice. 11 (1): 29–33. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph056.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|doi_brokendate=
ignored (|doi-broken-date=
suggested) (help) - ^ McNally RJ (2007). "Dispelling confusion about traumatic dissociative amnesia". Mayo Clin. Proc. 82 (9): 1083–90. PMID 17803876.
- ^ McNally RJ (2004). "Is traumatic amnesia nothing but psychiatric folklore?". Cogn Behav Ther. 33 (2): 97–101, discussion 102–4, 109–11. PMID 15279316.
- ^ McNally RJ (2005). "Debunking myths about trauma and memory". Can J Psychiatry. 50 (13): 817–22. PMID 16483114.
- ^ Amicus Curiae brief in Taus v. Loftus (Superme Court of California 2006-02-21).
- ^ Geraerts, E. (2007). "The Reality of Recovered Memories: Corroborating Continuous and Discontinuous Memories of Childhood Sexual Abuse" (PDF). Psychological Science. 18 (7): 564–568. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01940.x.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ van der Kolk BA, Fisler R (1995). "Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: overview and exploratory study". J Trauma Stress. 8 (4): 505–25. PMID 8564271. Retrieved 2008-03-22.
- ^ Robbins Susan P., The Social and Cultural Context of Satanic Ritual Abuse Allegations, published in Institute for Psychological Therapies magazine, vol 10 1998.[1]
- ^ 923 Federal Supplement 286 (D. Mass. 1996); CIVIL ACTION NO.: 92-12139-EFH MEMORANDUM AND ORDER; May 8, 1996
- ^ a b c Whitfield, Charles L. (2001). Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors. Haworth Press. pp. p56. ISBN 0789019019.
{{cite book}}
:|pages=
has extra text (help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Salter, Stephanie (1993-04-07). "Feminist Treason and Intellectual Fascism" (reprint). San Francisco Examiner. Retrieved 2007-12-15.
- ^ Underwager, Ralph (1994). Return of the Furies: An Investigation into Recovered Memory Therapy. Open Court Pub Co. p. 360. ISBN 978-0812692716.
{{cite book}}
: Unknown parameter|Month=
ignored (help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Lief, Harold I (1999). "Patients Versus Therapists: Legal Actions Over Recovered Memory Therapy". Psychiatric Times. XVI (11).
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Hammond, D. Corydon; Brown, Daniel P.; Scheflin, Alan W. (1998). Memory, trauma treatment, and the law. New York: W.W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-70254-5.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Brandon S, Boakes J, Glaser D, Green R (1998). "Recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Implications for clinical practice". Br J Psychiatry. 172: 296–307. PMID 9722329.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Lambert, K (2007-10-01). "Brain Stains". Scientific American. Retrieved 2008-01-25.
{{cite web}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Whitfield M.D., Charles L. (1995). Memory and Abuse - Remembering and Healing the Effects of Trauma. Deerfield Beach, FL: Health Communications, Inc. p. 375. ISBN 1-55874-320-0.
- ^ Summit, R. (1983). "The child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome". Child Abuse & Neglect. 7: 177–193. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(83)90070-4.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|month=
(help)