Jump to content

Substitutes for Leadership Theory: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Zehramike (talk | contribs)
Substitutes for Leadership Theory
 
Zehramike (talk | contribs)
No edit summary
Line 1: Line 1:
== Substitutes for Leadership Theory ==

{{AFC submission|t||ts=20111020180706|u=Zehramike|ns=5}} <!--- Important, do not remove this line before article has been created. --->
{{AFC submission|t||ts=20111020180706|u=Zehramike|ns=5}} <!--- Important, do not remove this line before article has been created. --->

==Introduction==


'''''Substitutes for leadership theory''''' is a prominent [[leadership]] theory first developed by Steven Kerr and John M. Jermier in 1978. The theory states that different situational factors can enhance, neutralize, or substitute for leader behaviors (Avolio, Walumbwa, & Weber, 2009; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001). It has received criticism for shortcomings due to perceived methodological issues (Dionne, Yammarino, Atwater, & James, 2002; Podsakoff & Mackenzie, 1995). Empirical research has produced mixed results as to its ability to predict subordinate outcomes.
'''''Substitutes for leadership theory''''' is a prominent [[leadership]] theory first developed by Steven Kerr and John M. Jermier in 1978. The theory states that different situational factors can enhance, neutralize, or substitute for leader behaviors (Avolio, Walumbwa, & Weber, 2009; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001). It has received criticism for shortcomings due to perceived methodological issues (Dionne, Yammarino, Atwater, & James, 2002; Podsakoff & Mackenzie, 1995). Empirical research has produced mixed results as to its ability to predict subordinate outcomes.
Line 13: Line 9:
In the mid 1970s, the ''contingency model'' and path-goal theory dominated research (Schriesheim, 1997). The contingency model stated that various leadership styles would be more or less effective depending on the situation (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001; Fiedler, 1965; Hunt, 1967). Path-goal theory proposed that subordinates would be satisfied with their leader if they perceived that their leader’s behavior would bring them future satisfaction. Subordinates would be motivated by their leader if they perceived that completing work tasks would bring them satisfaction, and if the leader provided proper coaching, support, rewards, and guidance (House, 1996). Frustrations with not being able to find significant results with any one theory resulted in the development of reexaminations and new approaches, including questioning which situations necessitated a leader figure and which did not (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).
In the mid 1970s, the ''contingency model'' and path-goal theory dominated research (Schriesheim, 1997). The contingency model stated that various leadership styles would be more or less effective depending on the situation (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001; Fiedler, 1965; Hunt, 1967). Path-goal theory proposed that subordinates would be satisfied with their leader if they perceived that their leader’s behavior would bring them future satisfaction. Subordinates would be motivated by their leader if they perceived that completing work tasks would bring them satisfaction, and if the leader provided proper coaching, support, rewards, and guidance (House, 1996). Frustrations with not being able to find significant results with any one theory resulted in the development of reexaminations and new approaches, including questioning which situations necessitated a leader figure and which did not (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).


Steven Kerr was one of the researchers publicly expressing his frustrations (e.g., Kerr & Harlan, 1973) with current leadership theories. In the 1970s, Kerr was at Ohio State University actively involved in leadership research. Kerr was studying existing proposed subordinate constructs such as organizational independence (Schriesheim, 1997) and proposed leadership constructs such as [[consideration and initiating structure]] (put forth by the [[Leadership Studies|Ohio State Leadership Studies]]; Kerr, Schriesheim, Murphy, & Stogdill, 1974).
Steven Kerr was one of the researchers publicly expressing his frustrations (e.g., Kerr & Harlan, 1973) with current leadership theories. In the 1970s, Kerr was at Ohio State University actively involved in leadership research. Kerr was studying existing proposed subordinate constructs such as ''organizational independence'' (Schriesheim, 1997) and proposed leadership constructs such as [[consideration and initiating structure]] (put forth by the [[Leadership Studies|Ohio State Leadership Studies]]; Kerr, Schriesheim, Murphy, & Stogdill, 1974).


Subordinates that are organizational independents do not feel tied to one company and are internally motivated instead of motivated by the characteristics of the organization, such as leader behavior (House & Kerr, 1973; Schriesheim, 1997). Leaders high on initiating structure clarify their own role and their subordinates’ roles in obtaining a goal (Kerr et al., 1974). Kerr and colleagues (1974) noticed many studies had found significant [[Moderation (statistics) |moderators]] that played a part in the relationship between initiating structure leader behavior and subordinate outcomes. For example, when a task was ambiguous, the relationship between leader initiating structure and subordinate satisfaction was stronger than if the task was clear. These findings were making it apparent that there were variables that affected the relationship between leader behavior and subordinate outcomes, making the relationship stronger or weaker.
Subordinates that are organizational independents do not feel tied to one company and are internally motivated instead of motivated by the characteristics of the organization, such as leader behavior (House & Kerr, 1973; Schriesheim, 1997). Leaders high on initiating structure clarify their own role and their subordinates’ roles in obtaining a goal (Kerr et al., 1974). Kerr and colleagues (1974) noticed many studies had found significant [[Moderation (statistics) |moderators]] that played a part in the relationship between initiating structure leader behavior and subordinate outcomes. For example, when a task was ambiguous, the relationship between leader initiating structure and subordinate satisfaction was stronger than if the task was clear. These findings were making it apparent that there were variables that affected the relationship between leader behavior and subordinate outcomes, making the relationship stronger or weaker.
Line 27: Line 23:
===Substitutes for Leadership Theory===
===Substitutes for Leadership Theory===


Substitutes for leadership theory States that different situational factors can enhance, neutralize, or substitute for leader behaviors (Avolio et al., 2009; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).
Substitutes for leadership theory states that different situational factors can enhance, neutralize, or substitute for leader behaviors (Avolio et al., 2009; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).


===Substitutes===
===Substitutes===
Line 51: Line 47:
*Leader having the ability to reward subordinates (Howell et al., 1986)
*Leader having the ability to reward subordinates (Howell et al., 1986)
==Scales Used to Measure==
==Scales Used to Measure==
===Kerr & Jermier===
===Kerr and Jermier===
The original scale to measure the effects of various substitutes was developed by Kerr and Jermier (1978). They assessed leadership substitutes via a questionnaire that contained thirteen subscales with a total of 55 items. The items were on a [[Likert scale]] ranging from 1 (almost always untrue or completely untrue), to 5 (almost always true or almost completely true).
The original scale to measure the effects of various substitutes was developed by Kerr and Jermier (1978). They assessed leadership substitutes via a questionnaire that contained thirteen subscales with a total of 55 items. The items were on a [[Likert scale]] ranging from 1 (almost always untrue or completely untrue), to 5 (almost always true or almost completely true).


Line 73: Line 69:


===Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams===
===Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams===
Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams (1993) noted that studies testing the substitutes for leadership model had not been fully supportive of the theory, and believed that one reason for this may be that the quality of the scale developed by Kerr & Jermier (1978) to measure the substitutes constructs may be to blame. In response to this, they designed their own 74-item measure of substitutes for leadership.
Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams (1993) noted that studies testing the substitutes for leadership model had not been fully supportive of the theory, and believed that one reason for this may be that the quality of the scale developed by Kerr and Jermier (1978) to measure the substitutes constructs may be to blame. In response to this, they designed their own 74-item measure of substitutes for leadership.
To test their scale, they administered it to 372 business students. Their analyses of the [[psychometrics|psychometric]] properties of the revised measure revealed their scale to be superior to Kerr and Jermier’s scale, as evidenced by better dimensionality and reliability of the revised scale.
To test their scale, they administered it to 372 business students. Their analyses of the [[psychometrics|psychometric]] properties of the revised measure revealed their scale to be superior to Kerr and Jermier’s scale, as evidenced by better dimensionality and reliability of the revised scale.


Line 130: Line 126:
{{reflist}}
{{reflist}}
<!--- After listing your sources please cite them using inline citations and place them after the information they cite. Please see http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:REFB for instructions on how to add citations. --->
<!--- After listing your sources please cite them using inline citations and place them after the information they cite. Please see http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:REFB for instructions on how to add citations. --->
Avolio, B. J., Walumbwa, F. O., & Weber, T. J. (2009). Leadership: Current theories, research, and future directions. ''Annual Review of Psychology'', ''60,'' 421-449.
*
::doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163621
*

*
Bass, B. M. (1990). ''Bass & Stogdill's handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and managerial applications'' (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Free Press.
*

Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2002). Leadership in organizations. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil,
::& C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), ''Handbook of industrial, work and organizational psychology, Volume 2: Organizational
::psychology'' (166-187). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Dionne, S. D., Yammarino, F. J., Atwater, L. E., & James, L. R. (2002). Neutralizing substitutes for leadership
::theory: Leadership effects and common-source bias. ''Journal of Applied Psychology'', ''87,'' 454-464. doi:
::10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.454

Dionne, S. D., Yammarino, F. J., Howell, J. P., & Villa, J. (2005). Theoretical letters: Substitutes for leadership,
::or not. ''The Leadership Quarterly'', ''16,'' 169-193. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.09.012

Fiedler, E. (1965). A contingency model of leadership effectiveness. In H. Proshansky, & B. Seidenberg (Eds.), ''Basic
::studies in social psychology'' (538-551). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Gulowsen, J. (1972). A measure of work group autonomy. In L. E. Davis, & J. C. Taylor (Eds.), ''Design of jobs''
::(374-390). Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.

Howell, J. P., & Dorfman, P. W. (1981). Substitutes for leadership: Test of a construct. ''Academy of Management
::Journal'', ''24,'' 714-728. doi: 10.2307/256171

Howell, J. P., Dorfman, P. W., & Kerr, S. (1986). Moderator variables in leadership research. ''The Academy of
::Management Review'', ''11,'' 88-102. doi: 10.2307/258333

House, R. J. (1971). A path-goal theory of leader effectiveness. ''Administrative Science Quarterly'', ''16,'' 321-338.

House, R. J. (1996). Path-goal theory of leadership: Lessons, legacy and a reformulated theory. ''The Leadership
::Quarterly'', ''7,'' 323-352. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(96)90024-7

House, R. J., & Kerr, S. (1973). Organizational independence, leader behavior, and managerial practices: A
::replicated study. ''Journal of Applied Psychology'', ''58,'' 173-180. doi: 10.1037/h0035664

Hunt, J. G. (1967). Fiedler’s leadership contingency model: An empirical test in three organizations. ''Organizational
::Behavior and Human Performance'', ''2,'' 290-308. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(67)90023-2

Keller, R. T. (2006). Transformational leadership, initiating structure, and substitutes for leadership: A
::longitudinal study of research and development project team performance. ''Journal of Applied Psychology'', ''91,''
::202-210. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.1.202

Kerr, S. (1973). Ability- and willingness-to-leave as moderators of relationships between task and leader variables
::and satisfaction. ''Journal of Business Research'', ''1,'' 115-128. doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(73)80002-5

Kerr, S., & Harlan, A. (1973). Predicting the effects of leadership training and experience from the contingency
::model: Some remaining problems. ''Journal of Applied Psychology'', ''57,'' 114-117. doi: 10.1037/h0037122

Kerr, S., & Jermier, J. M. (1978). Substitutes for leadership: Their meaning and measurement. ''Organizational
::Behavior and Human Performance'', ''22,'' 375-403. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(78)90023-5

Kerr, S., Schriesheim, C. A., Murphy, C. J., & Stogdill, R. M. (1974). Toward a contingency theory of leadership
::based upon the consideration and initiating structure literature. ''Organizational Behavior and Human Performance',
::''12,'' 62-82. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(74)90037-3

Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P. (1992). The potential for "groupthink" in autonomous work groups. In R. Glaser (Ed.),
::'Classic readings in self-managing teamwork: 20 of the most important articles'' (401-411). King of Prussia, PA:
::Organization Design and Development.

Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1995). An examination of substitutes for leadership within a levels-
::of-analysis framework. ''The Leadership Quarterly'', ''6,'' 289-228. doi: 10.1016/1048-9843(95)90011-X

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Bommer, W. H. (1996). Meta-analysis of the relationships between Kerr and
::Jermier's substitutes for leadership and employee job attitudes, role perceptions, and performance. ''Journal of
::Applied Psychology'', ''81,'' 380-399. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.81.4.380

Podsakoff, P. M., Niehoff, B. P., MacKenzie, S. B., & Williams, M. L. (1993). Do substitutes for leadership really
::substitute for leadership? An empirical examination of Kerr and Jermier's situational leadership model.
::''Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes'', ''54,'' 1-44. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1993.1001

Schriesheim, C. A. (1997). Substitutes-for-leadership theory: Development and basic concepts. ''The Leadership
::Quarterly'', ''8,'' 103-108. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(97)90009-6

::Thoreson, C. E., & Mahoney, M. J. (1974). ''Behavioral self-control.'' New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Villa, J. R., Howell, J. P., Dorfman, P. W., Daniel, D. L. (2003). Problems with detecting moderators in leadership
::research using moderated multiple regression. ''The Leadership Quarterly'', ''14,'' 3-23. doi:
::10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00184-4

Wall, T. D., Kemp, N. J., Jackson, P. R., & Clegg, C. W. (1986). Outcomes of autonomous workgroups: A long-term
::field experiment. ''Academy of Management Journal'', ''29,'' 280-304. doi: 10.2307/256189

Yukl, G. (1998). ''Leadership in organizations'' (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Revision as of 18:50, 20 October 2011

Substitutes for leadership theory is a prominent leadership theory first developed by Steven Kerr and John M. Jermier in 1978. The theory states that different situational factors can enhance, neutralize, or substitute for leader behaviors (Avolio, Walumbwa, & Weber, 2009; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001). It has received criticism for shortcomings due to perceived methodological issues (Dionne, Yammarino, Atwater, & James, 2002; Podsakoff & Mackenzie, 1995). Empirical research has produced mixed results as to its ability to predict subordinate outcomes.

Origins

Over the years, researchers have developed many leadership theories. Prior to the 1970s, trait leadership theory and path-goal theory were the two theories that dominated research (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001). Proponents of trait leadership theory held that the ability to lead is a characteristic some people innately have and others do not. Effort was put forth to uncover which characteristics and abilities leaders had that separated them from non-leaders. Leadership style was another angle researchers took. Proponents of this approach did not believe the ability to lead was innate, rather it was a set of behaviors anyone could learn (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

In the mid 1970s, the contingency model and path-goal theory dominated research (Schriesheim, 1997). The contingency model stated that various leadership styles would be more or less effective depending on the situation (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001; Fiedler, 1965; Hunt, 1967). Path-goal theory proposed that subordinates would be satisfied with their leader if they perceived that their leader’s behavior would bring them future satisfaction. Subordinates would be motivated by their leader if they perceived that completing work tasks would bring them satisfaction, and if the leader provided proper coaching, support, rewards, and guidance (House, 1996). Frustrations with not being able to find significant results with any one theory resulted in the development of reexaminations and new approaches, including questioning which situations necessitated a leader figure and which did not (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

Steven Kerr was one of the researchers publicly expressing his frustrations (e.g., Kerr & Harlan, 1973) with current leadership theories. In the 1970s, Kerr was at Ohio State University actively involved in leadership research. Kerr was studying existing proposed subordinate constructs such as organizational independence (Schriesheim, 1997) and proposed leadership constructs such as consideration and initiating structure (put forth by the Ohio State Leadership Studies; Kerr, Schriesheim, Murphy, & Stogdill, 1974).

Subordinates that are organizational independents do not feel tied to one company and are internally motivated instead of motivated by the characteristics of the organization, such as leader behavior (House & Kerr, 1973; Schriesheim, 1997). Leaders high on initiating structure clarify their own role and their subordinates’ roles in obtaining a goal (Kerr et al., 1974). Kerr and colleagues (1974) noticed many studies had found significant moderators that played a part in the relationship between initiating structure leader behavior and subordinate outcomes. For example, when a task was ambiguous, the relationship between leader initiating structure and subordinate satisfaction was stronger than if the task was clear. These findings were making it apparent that there were variables that affected the relationship between leader behavior and subordinate outcomes, making the relationship stronger or weaker.

Kerr (1973) was the first to coin substitutes for leadership as elements in the work setting that lessened leader effectiveness on subordinate outcomes. Further publications (e.g., Kerr et al., 1974) led to Kerr and Jermier’s 1978 paper, which unveiled substitutes for leadership theory. This paper presented two types of elements in the job environment: substitutes and neutralizers. These elements were proposed to serve as moderators in the relationship between leader behavior and subordinate outcomes.

The theory originally classified substitutes as characteristics of the subordinate, characteristics of the task, and characteristics of the organization. Howell, Dorfman, & Kerr (1986) proposed an alteration to the theory in terms of subordinate classification. They argued that moderators should be grouped based on their effect on the criterion. The original theory had already proposed moderators that act as substitutes and moderators that act as neutralizers. Howell and colleagues added enhancers to these.

Substitutes for leadership theory dominated leadership research until the late 1980s, when transformational leadership became a hot topic (Dionne, Yammarino, Howell, & Villa, 2005).

Definitions

Substitutes for Leadership Theory

Substitutes for leadership theory states that different situational factors can enhance, neutralize, or substitute for leader behaviors (Avolio et al., 2009; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

Substitutes

Substitutes are variables that make leadership unnecessary for subordinates (Schriesheim, 1997) and reduce the extent to which subordinates rely on their leader (Kerr et al.,1974)

Examples of Substitutes

  • Subordinate ability (Kerr & Jermier, 1978)
  • Subordinate’s professional orientation (Kerr & Jermier, 1978)
  • Unambiguous and routine task (when all subordinates are performing menial labor, there is little role leadership can play; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001)
  • Task that provides its own feedback as to how well the task is being done (Kerr & Jermier, 1978)
  • Task that is intrinsically satisfying (Kerr & Jermier, 1978)
  • Cohesive work groups (a tight-knit group of employees have less need for a leader; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001)
  • Organizational formulation (clear job goals that are written down, performance appraisals that are written down; Howell & Dorfman, 1981; Kerr and Jermier, 1987)
  • Self-managed work teams (employees rely on each other, not their leader; Villa, Howell, Dorfman, & Daniel, 2003)

Enhancers

Enhancers are variables that serve to strengthen leader influence on subordinate outcomes (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001; Howell et al., 1986; Schriesheim, 1997).

Examples of Enhancers
  • Having group norms that encourage cooperation with leaders (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001)
  • Subordinates having experience (those more experienced will be able to translate even the most ambiguous instructions into results; Howell et al., 1986)
  • Leader having the ability to reward subordinates (Howell et al., 1986)

Scales Used to Measure

Kerr and Jermier

The original scale to measure the effects of various substitutes was developed by Kerr and Jermier (1978). They assessed leadership substitutes via a questionnaire that contained thirteen subscales with a total of 55 items. The items were on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost always untrue or completely untrue), to 5 (almost always true or almost completely true).

The subscales included were:

  1. Ability, experience, training, and knowledge
  2. Professional orientation
  3. Indifference towards organizational rewards
  4. Unambiguous, routine, and methodically invariant tasks
  5. Task-provided feedback concerning accomplishment
  6. Intrinsically satisfying tasks
  7. Organizational formulization
  8. Organizational inflexibility
  9. Advisory and staff functions
  10. Closely-knit, cohesive, interdependent work groups
  11. Organizational rewards not within the leader’s control
  12. Spatial distance between superior and subordinates
  13. Subordinate need for independence

Kerr and Jermier (1978) tested nine of these subscales in a lab setting and found that they were independent and had adequate internal reliabilities. They claim that the subscales produce easily-interpretable data which describe the extent to which substitutes for leadership are present or absent in a given work situation. They went on to test their subscales in a field setting using police officers, and concluded that the subscales met acceptable standards of reliability, and can be used to assess the validity of the substitutes for leadership construct in future studies.

Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams

Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams (1993) noted that studies testing the substitutes for leadership model had not been fully supportive of the theory, and believed that one reason for this may be that the quality of the scale developed by Kerr and Jermier (1978) to measure the substitutes constructs may be to blame. In response to this, they designed their own 74-item measure of substitutes for leadership. To test their scale, they administered it to 372 business students. Their analyses of the psychometric properties of the revised measure revealed their scale to be superior to Kerr and Jermier’s scale, as evidenced by better dimensionality and reliability of the revised scale.

Consequences

Kerr and Jermier (1978) proposed that substitute variables should render leader behaviors unable to predict subordinate outcomes. Researchers that have tested this characteristic of substitutes have found mixed results (Howell & Dorfman, 1981; Podsakoff & MacKenzie, 1995).

A study involving hospital personnel found that tasks that gave feedback regarding performance were correlated with job satisfaction. Routine repetitive work tasks were correlated with organizational commitment. Intrinsically satisfying work, organizational formulation, and cohesive work groups were substitutes that were significantly correlated with both job satisfaction and organizational commitment. However, they found little evidence that substitutes prevented or replaced a leader’s effect on subordinate job satisfaction or organizational commitment. The only substitute that served as a replacement was organizational formulation, which replaced leader behavior’s predictive power in explaining subordinate job satisfaction and organizational commitment (Howell & Dorfman, 1981).

Another study looked at 1,235 employees working for 265 leaders in a variety of job settings. Among their findings were that intrinsically satisfying tasks were positively related to subordinate satisfaction, indifference to rewards was negatively related to organizational commitment, and organizational formulation was negatively related to subordinate perceptions of role ambiguity. They concluded that substitutes for leadership were useful in predicting subordinate outcomes, but leader behaviors and substitutes should always be studied together, because together they explained about a third of the total variance in subordinate outcomes in their sample (Podsakoff & Mackenzie, 1995). A more recent study collected data from 940 subordinates. Unsatisfied with the fact that prior studies had tested substitutes, enhancers, and neutralizers as moderators, Dionne and colleagues (2002) tested the effect of substitutes as mediators as well as moderators. Testing a variety of different substitutes and outcomes, the researchers found only very weak evidence that substitutes make a difference, and concluded that leader behaviors are the only important variable in predicting employee outcomes.

Meta-analysis Examining the Effectiveness of Substitutes for Leadership

Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Bommer (1996) conducted a meta-analysis in which they took the results of 22 studies that examined substitutes for leadership and how they moderate the effect of leader behaviors on subordinate outcomes. They found evidence supporting the theory— taking into account both leader behaviors and substitutes accounted for more of the variance in subordinate outcomes than taking into account leader behaviors alone.

Applications

Autonomous Work Groups

Bass (1990) suggested that autonomous work groups can substitute for formal leadership. In this scenario, employees are divided into groups that are responsible for managing their own day-to-day work (i.e. collective control over pace, distribution of tasks, organization of breaks, recruitment, and training; Gulowsen, 1972). A quasi-experiment conducted by Wall, Kemp, Jackson, and Clegg (1986) found that implementing autonomous work groups of 8 to 12 shop-floor employees in a manufacturing setting positively affected both the intrinsic and extrinsic job satisfaction of employees, while obviating some supervisory positions.

Self-Management

Self-management is defined by Thoresen & Mahoney (1974) as occurring when an individual behaves in a way he would not normally behave, and there are no external forces dictating that the person maintain that behavior. Self-management requires self-observations (e.g., keeping a log of what one has discussed with others on the phone), specification of goals (e.g., being responsible for setting one’s own schedule and priorities), cueing strategies (e.g, putting a checkout board by the exit to remind an employee to let their secretary know where they are going and when they expect to return) , rehearsal (e.g., recording one’s presentation to clients and making corrections as needed), self-evaluation (e.g., using a chart to keep track of work quality and quantity), and self-reinforcement (accomplished by building intrinsic rewards for the performance of tasks (Bass, 1990; Manz & Sims 1980). These strategies can be understood as being substitutes for leadership. Instead of requiring that a supervisor monitor a subordinate’s progress on a work task, a subordinate can self-manage by utilizing one of the strategies listed above. This makes the supervisor’s guidance unnecessary for the subordinate.

Criticisms and Methodological Problems with Research

Common-Source Bias

Dionne and colleagues (2002) argued that significant effects of substitutes found in prior studies may be a statistical artifact due to common-source bias, or bias occurring when independent and dependent variables are collected from the same person or group of people. In a study sampling 49 organizations, Dionne and colleagues controlled for the effect of common-source bias and found no moderating or mediating effects of substitutes on the relationship between leader behavior and group effectiveness.

In a study by Podsakoff and Mackenzie (1995), the predictor variables, as well as the job attitude and role perception variables, were both taken from individual employees, while the performance measures were taken from supervisors. They found that their predictors accounted for a higher proportion of variance in job attitudes and role perceptions, than in employee performance. They posit the reason for this was that their predictors shared a common source with the job attitudes and role perception criterion measures, but not with the employee performance criterion measures. They suggested that common-source bias may not be the only reason for this, but that it should be controlled in future research.

Lack of Longitudinal Studies

Keller (2006) conducted a longitudinal study in which he pointed out that the vast majority of studies conducted on substitutes for leadership theory are cross-sectional in nature, making it more difficult to discern a causal relationship between substitutes and their effects on employee performance. Cross-sectional research is focused on finding relationships between variables at a specific point in time, whereas longitudinal studies involve taking multiple measures over a longer period of time. Keller’s longitudinal study of the effect of substitutes on Research & Development teams found that two of the seven substitutes (ability and intrinsic satisfaction) affected team performance over time.

Conceptual Weakness

Yukl (1998) pointed out that it is hard to identify specific substitutes and neutralizers for broad behavior categories, and that an improvement on the theory would be to use more specific behaviors in place of “supportive and instrumental leadership behaviors.” Yukl said that recent studies testing the theory have used specific behaviors (e.g., contingent reward behavior and role clarification), however the development of the theory does not reflect these developments.

The contingency model

The contingency model of leadership was developed by Fred Fiedler in the 1960s after Fiedler spent 12 years collecting data from over 800 groups of employees. This model predicts that the most effective method of leadership will vary depending on the situation. A study which supported the model was one Fiedler did with 48 petty officers and 240 recruiters at a Belgium naval training center (Fiedler, 1965). Fiedler found that a leader’s fit with the group and the task was more important in predicting outcomes than the leader’s characteristics. For example, controlling leaders went best with heterogeneous groups with low position power (Fiedler, 1965; Hunt 1967). There are many different theories within the contingency paradigm, which differ on what situational factors change leadership effectiveness. The most influential theory within this model is path-goal theory (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

Path-goal theory

Proposed by Robert House in 1971, this theory predicts that subordinates will be satisfied with their leader if they feel their leader’s behavior will lead them to satisfaction. Also, subordinates will be motivated when they feel that their satisfaction depends on their performance and their leader acts in a way to help them reach goals (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001, House, 1971). Path-goal theory predicts that when goals and the paths to those goals are clear, subordinates may not need leader guidance (Kerr & Jermier, 1978).

See also

Leadership

Moderator

Industrial and Organizational Psychology

Path–goal theory

[[Contingency Theory|Contingency Model]


References

Avolio, B. J., Walumbwa, F. O., & Weber, T. J. (2009). Leadership: Current theories, research, and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 421-449.

doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163621

Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass & Stogdill's handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and managerial applications (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Free Press.

Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2002). Leadership in organizations. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil,

& C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work and organizational psychology, Volume 2: Organizational
psychology (166-187). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Dionne, S. D., Yammarino, F. J., Atwater, L. E., & James, L. R. (2002). Neutralizing substitutes for leadership

theory: Leadership effects and common-source bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 454-464. doi:
10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.454

Dionne, S. D., Yammarino, F. J., Howell, J. P., & Villa, J. (2005). Theoretical letters: Substitutes for leadership,

or not. The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 169-193. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.09.012

Fiedler, E. (1965). A contingency model of leadership effectiveness. In H. Proshansky, & B. Seidenberg (Eds.), Basic

studies in social psychology (538-551). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Gulowsen, J. (1972). A measure of work group autonomy. In L. E. Davis, & J. C. Taylor (Eds.), Design of jobs

(374-390). Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.

Howell, J. P., & Dorfman, P. W. (1981). Substitutes for leadership: Test of a construct. Academy of Management

Journal, 24, 714-728. doi: 10.2307/256171

Howell, J. P., Dorfman, P. W., & Kerr, S. (1986). Moderator variables in leadership research. The Academy of

Management Review, 11, 88-102. doi: 10.2307/258333

House, R. J. (1971). A path-goal theory of leader effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 16, 321-338.

House, R. J. (1996). Path-goal theory of leadership: Lessons, legacy and a reformulated theory. The Leadership

Quarterly, 7, 323-352. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(96)90024-7

House, R. J., & Kerr, S. (1973). Organizational independence, leader behavior, and managerial practices: A

replicated study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 58, 173-180. doi: 10.1037/h0035664

Hunt, J. G. (1967). Fiedler’s leadership contingency model: An empirical test in three organizations. Organizational

Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 290-308. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(67)90023-2

Keller, R. T. (2006). Transformational leadership, initiating structure, and substitutes for leadership: A

longitudinal study of research and development project team performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91,
202-210. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.1.202

Kerr, S. (1973). Ability- and willingness-to-leave as moderators of relationships between task and leader variables

and satisfaction. Journal of Business Research, 1, 115-128. doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(73)80002-5

Kerr, S., & Harlan, A. (1973). Predicting the effects of leadership training and experience from the contingency

model: Some remaining problems. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 114-117. doi: 10.1037/h0037122

Kerr, S., & Jermier, J. M. (1978). Substitutes for leadership: Their meaning and measurement. Organizational

Behavior and Human Performance, 22, 375-403. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(78)90023-5

Kerr, S., Schriesheim, C. A., Murphy, C. J., & Stogdill, R. M. (1974). Toward a contingency theory of leadership

based upon the consideration and initiating structure literature. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance',
12, 62-82. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(74)90037-3

Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P. (1992). The potential for "groupthink" in autonomous work groups. In R. Glaser (Ed.),

'Classic readings in self-managing teamwork: 20 of the most important articles (401-411). King of Prussia, PA:
Organization Design and Development.

Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1995). An examination of substitutes for leadership within a levels-

of-analysis framework. The Leadership Quarterly, 6, 289-228. doi: 10.1016/1048-9843(95)90011-X

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Bommer, W. H. (1996). Meta-analysis of the relationships between Kerr and

Jermier's substitutes for leadership and employee job attitudes, role perceptions, and performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 81, 380-399. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.81.4.380

Podsakoff, P. M., Niehoff, B. P., MacKenzie, S. B., & Williams, M. L. (1993). Do substitutes for leadership really

substitute for leadership? An empirical examination of Kerr and Jermier's situational leadership model.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 1-44. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1993.1001

Schriesheim, C. A. (1997). Substitutes-for-leadership theory: Development and basic concepts. The Leadership

Quarterly, 8, 103-108. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(97)90009-6
Thoreson, C. E., & Mahoney, M. J. (1974). Behavioral self-control. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Villa, J. R., Howell, J. P., Dorfman, P. W., Daniel, D. L. (2003). Problems with detecting moderators in leadership

research using moderated multiple regression. The Leadership Quarterly, 14, 3-23. doi:
10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00184-4

Wall, T. D., Kemp, N. J., Jackson, P. R., & Clegg, C. W. (1986). Outcomes of autonomous workgroups: A long-term

field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 280-304. doi: 10.2307/256189

Yukl, G. (1998). Leadership in organizations (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.