Jump to content

Henry L. Roediger III: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Added {{multiple issues}} with parameters notability, peacock and primary sources tag to article (TW)
Daronsen (talk | contribs)
m False Memories: added references for imagination inflation and social contagion
Line 28: Line 28:
Naturally, Roediger’s approach to explaining this phenomenon was through retrieval processes. He thought that perhaps recalling list items would increase the availability of the lure word to a level where it became so available that it was mistaken for a presented word. However, retrieval processes alone were not enough to explain the findings - in a number of studies Roediger and his colleagues showed that a warning about developing a false memory had no effect if it was presented before retrieval, but could reduce false memories if presented before the encoding phase, suggesting an important role being played by the encoding process. Further work led Roediger and his research team to acknowledge both encoding and retrieval processes in explaining this phenomenon.
Naturally, Roediger’s approach to explaining this phenomenon was through retrieval processes. He thought that perhaps recalling list items would increase the availability of the lure word to a level where it became so available that it was mistaken for a presented word. However, retrieval processes alone were not enough to explain the findings - in a number of studies Roediger and his colleagues showed that a warning about developing a false memory had no effect if it was presented before retrieval, but could reduce false memories if presented before the encoding phase, suggesting an important role being played by the encoding process. Further work led Roediger and his research team to acknowledge both encoding and retrieval processes in explaining this phenomenon.


Although a wealth of research was conducted on the DRM paradigm, Roediger’s interest in false memory went further still. His research in to other false memory procedures helped to give rise to the concept of imagination inflation – the idea that imagining an event can make later believe that it really happened. Also, research into the social environment around creating memories helped to shed light on how other people’s memories can become part of our own.
Although a wealth of research was conducted on the DRM paradigm, Roediger’s interest in false memory went further still. His research in to other false memory procedures helped further the field of research on imagination inflation – the idea that imagining an event can make later believe that it really happened <ref> Goff, L.M., & Roediger, H.L. (1998). Imagination inflation for active events: Repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections.Memory and Cognition, 26, 20-33. </ref>. Also, research into the social environment around creating memories helped to shed light on how other people’s memories can become part of our own, a process Roediger and colleagues called 'Social Contagion' <ref> Roediger, H.L., Meade, M.L., & Bergman, E.T. (2001). Social contagion of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8(2), 365-371. </ref>.


==Cognitive Psychology and Education==
==Cognitive Psychology and Education==

Revision as of 03:35, 4 June 2012

Henry L. "Roddy" Roediger III (Born July 24, 1947) is an internationally renowned scholar in the area of human learning and memory. Born in Roanoke, Virginia and raised in Danville, Virginia, Reodiger received his undergraduate education from Washington and Lee University, graduating magna cum laude with a Bachelor of Arts in 1969. He went on to study at Yale University, receiving his PhD in 1973 with his dissertation “Inhibition in recall from cueing with recall targets”. After receiving his doctorate he joined the faculty at Purdue University, where he stayed for fifteen years (except for two appointments as a visiting assistant professor at the University of Toronto: 1976-1978, and 1981-1982). In 1988 he was appointed as the Lynette S. Autrey Professor of Psychology at Rice University, and in 1996 he moved to Washington University where he was the Chair of Psychology until 2004. Since 1998 he has been the James S. McDonnell Distinguished University Professor of Psychology at Washington University. [1]

Throughout his career, Roediger has become known for his focus on memory accessibility and retrieval - the ways in which we access and recall memories that we have stored. From this standpoint he has been incredibly successful in developing theories, exploring phenomena, and pioneering research techniques. He has supervised over 25 students in postgraduate research, and 9 postdoctoral fellows [2]. Also, he has published over 175 articles and has an h-index of over 40 [3]. Alongside his academic work, Roediger is an experienced journal editor, and has been involved in the administration of a number of scientific societies, most notably as the 2003-2004 president of the Association for Psychological Science [4]

The Consequences of Retrieval

Roediger was one of the first scholars to see the value of studying how humans retrieve memories. As Roediger started his career the cognitive revolution was in full swing. Human memory researchers had been predominately focusing on memory storage, and were only beginning to look at memory encoding[5]. However, inspired by his graduate advisor at Yale, Robert G. Crowder, Roediger began to see the importance of a retrieval-based approach to memory research [6]. Since his doctoral dissertation, much of Roediger’s research through the 1970s focused on retrieval based inhibition – the idea that retrieving an item reduces the subsequent accessibility of other stored items. This phenomenon is more commonly experienced when we try to remember a list of items and find that we keep thinking of the ones we have already recalled, rather than the ones we still need to remember [7]. Roediger was able to show, under certain conditions, that recall cues can inhibit recall, which seemed inconsistent with previously widely accepted research findings showing that cues aid recall [8]. Close to a decade of research helped to define the situations in which cues can aid recall and the situations in which cues can inhibit recall. In 1978 Roediger concluded that this dissociation occurs because, although some cues can facilitate recall, other cues provide irrelevant information, which hinders recall. Most importantly, Roediger showed the world that the accessibility of one memory biases the process of searching for another memory [9].

Transfer Appropriate Processing

The 1980s saw an increase in research on implicit memory – memories that we have without being aware of them. The norm among researchers in this area was to test implicit memory using some task that required the subject to unintentionally remember previously learned information, such as completing a word fragment (E_E_ _ A _ T to ELEPHANT) or an anagram (PNLEHETA to ELEPHANT), as compared with testing for memories that we are aware of (explicit memory) using direct instructions to remember. These researchers found that the intentionally learned information was better remembered in an intentional remembering test, and unintentionally learned information was better remembered in an unintentional remembering test [10].

Roediger, however, approached this phenomenon from a more retrieval-based standpoint. Rather than looking at intentionality of learning, he looked to the conditions in which the information was to be recalled. He saw that unintentional learning seemed to be driven by bottom-up processes (using small details from the stimulus to build meaning) and that intentional learning seemed to be driven by top-down processes (using pre-existing concepts to make sense of a stimulus). He predicted that information learned in a bottom-up manner (e.g. reading a word) would be better recalled in a bottom-up test (e.g. completing a word fragment), and information learned in a top-down manner (e.g. generating a mental image) would be better remembered in a top-down test (e.g. recalling a list of words) [11]. Roediger hypothesised that the more that the processes used in retrieval matched those used in encoding, the better memory performance would be, and called this framework ‘transfer-appropriate processing’. In a number of experiments Roediger and his colleagues showed that, rather than the intentionality when remembering, it was in fact the overlap between the conditions in which learning and remembering occurred that aided memory. [12]

Neurophysiological studies have provided further evidence suggesting that transfer appropriate processes play a crucial role in memory. Studies using electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging have shown that the overlap in brain activity between encoding and retrieval facilitates memory performance [13] [14].

The theory of transfer appropriate processing has since been adapted by numerous scientists to further study a number of different problems. Scientists have used transfer appropriate processing to better understand how humans that speak more than two languages might organise their different lexica, which has important implications for those trying to learn a new language as well as for potentially better understanding language disorders [15]. Transfer appropriate processing also had a notable impact on the field of marketing, by providing a more comprehensive understanding of consumer memory. A focus on retrieval as the goal of advertising, and a better understanding of how interference can impact marketing communications, left a lasting impression on advertising practice [16].

Transfer appropriate processing theory has also been shown to be particularly valuable in exploring the organisation of memory [17], the workings of prospective memory – remembering to carry out previously planned actions [18], and in exploring how people learn to read fluently [19].

False Memories

Roediger is perhaps most widely known for his research in the area of false memory, looking at why and how people develop memories of events that never happened to them. Throughout the 1990s, he and his colleagues took the methodology from a relatively unknown study by James Deese from 1959 and worked to develop it into one of the most widely used tools in human memory research; the DRM Paradigm. In a typical DRM experiment, a subject listens to a list of related words, for example; Thread, Pin, Eye, Sewing, Sharp, Point, Prick, Thimble, Haystack, Torn, Hurt, Injection, Syringe, Cloth, Knitting, and is then tested on their memory for this list. Typically, subjects will recall or recognise an associated, but unpresented lure word (Needle) [20]. Roediger and McDermott asked people whether they actually remembered hearing this unpresented word, or if they merely felt like they had heard it. Interestingly, participants often reported actually remembering hearing the word, illustrating memory for an event that never occurred.

Naturally, Roediger’s approach to explaining this phenomenon was through retrieval processes. He thought that perhaps recalling list items would increase the availability of the lure word to a level where it became so available that it was mistaken for a presented word. However, retrieval processes alone were not enough to explain the findings - in a number of studies Roediger and his colleagues showed that a warning about developing a false memory had no effect if it was presented before retrieval, but could reduce false memories if presented before the encoding phase, suggesting an important role being played by the encoding process. Further work led Roediger and his research team to acknowledge both encoding and retrieval processes in explaining this phenomenon.

Although a wealth of research was conducted on the DRM paradigm, Roediger’s interest in false memory went further still. His research in to other false memory procedures helped further the field of research on imagination inflation – the idea that imagining an event can make later believe that it really happened [21]. Also, research into the social environment around creating memories helped to shed light on how other people’s memories can become part of our own, a process Roediger and colleagues called 'Social Contagion' [22].

Cognitive Psychology and Education

Roediger’s most recent interests have involved applying knowledge from cognitive psychology research to the realm of education. Although many teachers feel that using standardized tests can stifle creativity and takes time that could be better spent teaching, Roediger believes that testing enhances learning. Roediger’s early research on testing effects and hypermnesia showed that subjects that learn material and then receive two tests on that material perform better on the final test than subjects that are only tested once, even if no feedback is given on any of the tests. This effect is still seen when the group that is only tested once is given a second opportunity to study the material. Roediger explains this effect in terms of memory retrieval, claiming that testing provides practice at retrieving memories, making the memory itself stronger [23]. One key finding that Roediger and his colleagues have shown is that short answer questions tend to produce a stronger testing effect than multiple choice tests. Further research is being conducted to discover the optimal timing between tests and the best media for conducting tests [24].


References

  1. ^ Roediger, 2010 Curriculum Vitae [1]
  2. ^ Roediger, 2010 Curriculum Vitae [2]
  3. ^ Roediger, H.L. (2006). The h index in science: A new measure of scholarly contribution. The Academic Observer, 19(4) [3]
  4. ^ Nuselovici, J.(2003). Roediger Is 2003-04 APS President. The Academic Observer, 16(9) http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/uncategorized/roediger-is-2003-04-aps-president.html#hide
  5. ^ Nairne, J. S. (2007). Roddy Roediger's Memory. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press http://www1.psych.purdue.edu/~nairne/pdfs/44.pdf
  6. ^ Roediger, H. L., & Stadler, M. A. (2001). Robert G. Crowder and his intellectual heritage. In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 3-16). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press http://psych.wustl.edu/memory/Roddy%20article%20PDF's/BC_Roediger%20&%20Stadler%20(2001).pdf
  7. ^ Nairne, J. S. (2007). Roddy Roediger's Memory. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press http://www1.psych.purdue.edu/~nairne/pdfs/44.pdf
  8. ^ Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381-391. http://www.alicekim.ca/7.ET_Pearlstone.pdf
  9. ^ Nairne, J. S. (2007). Roddy Roediger's Memory. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press http://www1.psych.purdue.edu/~nairne/pdfs/44.pdf
  10. ^ Nairne, J. S. (2007). Roddy Roediger's Memory. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press http://www1.psych.purdue.edu/~nairne/pdfs/44.pdf
  11. ^ Nairne, J. S. (2007). Roddy Roediger's Memory. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press http://www1.psych.purdue.edu/~nairne/pdfs/44.pdf
  12. ^ Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., & Challis, B. H. (1989). Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 3-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum http://psych.wustl.edu/memory/Roddy%20article%20PDF's/BC_Roediger%20et%20al%20(1989b).pdf
  13. ^ Schendan, H.E., & Kutas, M. (2007). Neurophysiological evidence for transfer appropriate processing of memory: Processing versus feature similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(4), 612-619.
  14. ^ Park, H., & Rugg, M.D. (2008). The relationship between study processing and the effects of cue congruency at retrieval: fMRI support for transfer appropriate processing. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 868-875.
  15. ^ Schonpflung, U. (2003). The transfer appropriate processing approach and the trilingual’s organisation of the lexicon. In J. Cenoz, B. Hufeisen, & U. Jessner (Eds.), The Multilingual Lexicon (pp. 27-43). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
  16. ^ Hill, M.E., Radtke, R., & King, M. (1997). A transfer appropriate processing view of consumer memory. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 19, 1-21.
  17. ^ Franks, J.J., Bilbrey, C.W., Lien, K.G., & McNamara, T.P. (2000). Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) and repetition priming. Memory and Cognition, 28(7), 1140-1151.
  18. ^ Meier, B., & Graf, P. (2000). Transfer appropriate processing for prospective memory tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S11-S27.
  19. ^ Martin-Chang, S.L., & Levy, B.A. (2006). Word reading fluency: A transfer appropriate processing account of fluency transfer. Reading and Writing, 19, 517-542. http://www.psych.yorku.ca/gigi/documents/MartinChang_Levy_2006.pdf
  20. ^ Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 803-814. http://memory.wustl.edu/Pubs/1995_Roediger.pdf
  21. ^ Goff, L.M., & Roediger, H.L. (1998). Imagination inflation for active events: Repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections.Memory and Cognition, 26, 20-33.
  22. ^ Roediger, H.L., Meade, M.L., & Bergman, E.T. (2001). Social contagion of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8(2), 365-371.
  23. ^ Kleeman, J. (2011). Advice from cognitive psychologist Roddy Roediger on using retrieval practice to aid learning. http://blog.questionmark.com/advice-from-cognitive-psychologist-roddy-roediger-on-using-retrieval-practice-to-aid-learning
  24. ^ Roediger, H.L., McDaniel, M., & McDermott, K. (2006). Test enhanced learning. Observer, 19 (3) http://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/getArticle.cfm?id=1951

Template:Persondata