Jump to content

Ostrich effect: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Addbot (talk | contribs)
m Bot: Migrating 1 interwiki links, now provided by Wikidata on d:q2598759
Added more examples
Line 2: Line 2:


Galai and Sade (2006) explain differences in returns in the fixed income market by using a psychological explanation, which they name the "ostrich effect," attributing this anomalous behavior to an aversion to receiving information on potential interim losses.<ref name=GalaiSade2006>{{Cite journal |last1=Galai|first1=Dan|last2=Sade|first2=Orly|authorlink2=Orly Sade|journal=[[Journal of Business]]|volume=79|title=The "Ostrich Effect" and the Relationship between the Liquidity and the Yields of Financial Assets|year=2006|issue=5 |postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref> They also provide evidence that the entrance to a leading financial portal in Israel is positively related to the equity market. Later, research by George Loewenstein and Duane Seppi determined that people in [[Scandinavia]] looked up the value of their investments 50% to 80% less often during bad markets.<ref>{{cite news | last =Zweig | first =Jason | title =Should You Fear the Ostrich Effect? | work =[[The Wall Street Journal]] | pages =B1 | language = | publisher = | date =September 13, 2008 | url = | accessdate = }}</ref>
Galai and Sade (2006) explain differences in returns in the fixed income market by using a psychological explanation, which they name the "ostrich effect," attributing this anomalous behavior to an aversion to receiving information on potential interim losses.<ref name=GalaiSade2006>{{Cite journal |last1=Galai|first1=Dan|last2=Sade|first2=Orly|authorlink2=Orly Sade|journal=[[Journal of Business]]|volume=79|title=The "Ostrich Effect" and the Relationship between the Liquidity and the Yields of Financial Assets|year=2006|issue=5 |postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref> They also provide evidence that the entrance to a leading financial portal in Israel is positively related to the equity market. Later, research by George Loewenstein and Duane Seppi determined that people in [[Scandinavia]] looked up the value of their investments 50% to 80% less often during bad markets.<ref>{{cite news | last =Zweig | first =Jason | title =Should You Fear the Ostrich Effect? | work =[[The Wall Street Journal]] | pages =B1 | language = | publisher = | date =September 13, 2008 | url = | accessdate = }}</ref>

The original paper cites that investors prefer financial investments where the risk is unreported over those with a similar risk-return profile and frequently reported risks. Other examples include:
*Averted Gaze: for example, avoiding the gaze of your financial manager after discussing a potential financial loophole.<ref>Bill Kahn, [http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-ostrich-effect/201203/the-averted-gaze-0 "The Ostrich Effect"], ''Psychology Today''</ref>
*Counterfeit problems: a subject avoids addressing a certain problem by instead creating a larger problem as a result of the former. For example, a partner in a firm has had communication issues with the other partner. He feels that he is doing the majority of the work. Instead of addressing the issue the partner asks his team to create a partner buyout plan.<ref>Bill Kahn, [http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-ostrich-effect/201204/counterfeit-problems "Counterfeit Problems"], ''Psychology Today''</ref>


==See also==
==See also==

Revision as of 19:11, 4 April 2013

In behavioral finance, the ostrich effect is the avoidance of apparently risky financial situations by pretending they do not exist. The name comes from the common (but false)[1] legend that ostriches bury their heads in the sand to avoid danger.

Galai and Sade (2006) explain differences in returns in the fixed income market by using a psychological explanation, which they name the "ostrich effect," attributing this anomalous behavior to an aversion to receiving information on potential interim losses.[2] They also provide evidence that the entrance to a leading financial portal in Israel is positively related to the equity market. Later, research by George Loewenstein and Duane Seppi determined that people in Scandinavia looked up the value of their investments 50% to 80% less often during bad markets.[3]

The original paper cites that investors prefer financial investments where the risk is unreported over those with a similar risk-return profile and frequently reported risks. Other examples include:

  • Averted Gaze: for example, avoiding the gaze of your financial manager after discussing a potential financial loophole.[4]
  • Counterfeit problems: a subject avoids addressing a certain problem by instead creating a larger problem as a result of the former. For example, a partner in a firm has had communication issues with the other partner. He feels that he is doing the majority of the work. Instead of addressing the issue the partner asks his team to create a partner buyout plan.[5]

See also

References

  1. ^ Karl Kruszelnicki, Ostrich Head in Sand, ABC Science: In Depth.
  2. ^ Galai, Dan; Sade, Orly (2006). "The "Ostrich Effect" and the Relationship between the Liquidity and the Yields of Financial Assets". Journal of Business. 79 (5).
  3. ^ Zweig, Jason (September 13, 2008). "Should You Fear the Ostrich Effect?". The Wall Street Journal. pp. B1.
  4. ^ Bill Kahn, "The Ostrich Effect", Psychology Today
  5. ^ Bill Kahn, "Counterfeit Problems", Psychology Today