Jump to content

Electroreception and electrogenesis: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
AnomieBOT (talk | contribs)
m Dating maintenance tags: {{Clarification}}
Jajava (talk | contribs)
Added cockroach electrolocation example. Reword waggle dance sentence for clarity.
Line 1: Line 1:
'''Electroreception''' is the biological ability to perceive natural [[electrical]] stimuli. It has been observed almost exclusively in aquatic or amphibious animals, since water is a much better [[Conductor (material)|conductor]] than air, the currently known exceptions being echidnas and bees. Electroreception is used in electrolocation (detecting objects) and for [[animal communication|electrocommunication]].
'''Electroreception''' is the biological ability to perceive natural [[electrical]] stimuli. It has been observed almost exclusively in aquatic or amphibious animals, since water is a much better [[Conductor (material)|conductor]] than air, the currently known exceptions being echidnas, cockroaches and bees. Electroreception is used in [[electrolocation]] (detecting objects) and for [[animal communication|electrocommunication]].


== Overview ==
== Overview ==
Line 5: Line 5:


==Electrolocation==
==Electrolocation==
Electroreceptive animals use this sense to locate objects around them. This is important in [[ecological niche]]s where the animal cannot depend on vision: for example in caves, in murky water and at night. Many fish use electric fields to detect buried prey. Some shark embryos and pups "freeze" when they detect the characteristic electric signal of their predators.<ref name=Coplin2004>{{cite journal|last=Coplin|first=Shaun P.|coauthors=Darryl Whitehead|title=The functional roles of passive electroreception in non-electric shes|journal=Animal Biology|year=2004|volume=54|issue=1|pages=1–25|doi=10.1163/157075604323010024}}</ref>
Electroreceptive animals use this sense to locate objects around them. This is important in [[ecological niche]]s where the animal cannot depend on vision: for example in caves, in murky water and at night. Many fish use electric fields to detect buried prey. Some shark embryos and pups "freeze" when they detect the characteristic electric signal of their predators.<ref name=Coplin2004>{{cite journal|last=Coplin|first=Shaun P.|coauthors=Darryl Whitehead|title=The functional roles of passive electroreception in non-electric shes|journal=Animal Biology|year=2004|volume=54|issue=1|pages=1–25|doi=10.1163/157075604323010024}}</ref> It has been proposed that sharks can use their acute electric sense to detect the earth's magnetic field by detecting the weak electric currents induced by their swimming or by the flow of ocean currents. The walking behaviour of cockroaches can be affected by the presence of a static electric field: they like to avoid the electric field.<ref name=":0">1. Jackson, C.W., E. Hunt, S. Sharkh, and P.L. Newland. 2011. Static electric fields modify the locomotory behaviour of cockroaches. The Journal of experimental biology. 214: 2020–6.</ref> Cabbage loopers are also known to avoid electric fields.<ref name=":0" />


==<nowiki/>==
===Active electrolocation===
===Active electrolocation===
[[File:Active electro.png|thumb|right|Active electrolocation. Conductive objects concentrate the field and resistive objects spread the field.]]
[[File:Active electro.png|thumb|right|Active electrolocation. Conductive objects concentrate the field and resistive objects spread the field.]]
Line 21: Line 22:
Fish use passive electroreception to supplement or replace their other senses when detecting prey and predators. In sharks, sensing an electric dipole alone is sufficient to cause them to try to eat it.
Fish use passive electroreception to supplement or replace their other senses when detecting prey and predators. In sharks, sensing an electric dipole alone is sufficient to cause them to try to eat it.


Electrocommunication
It has been proposed that sharks can use their acute electric sense to detect the earth's magnetic field by detecting the weak electric currents induced by their swimming or by the flow of ocean currents.


==Electrocommunication==
Weakly electric fish can also communicate by modulating the electrical [[waveform]] they generate, an ability known as electrocommunication.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Hopkins
Weakly electric fish can also communicate by modulating the electrical [[waveform]] they generate, an ability known as electrocommunication.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Hopkins
| first = CD
| first = CD
Line 61: Line 61:
===Bees===
===Bees===
Bees collect a positive static charge while flying through the air (see [[Atmospheric electricity]]). When a bee visits a flower, the charge deposited on the flower takes a while to leak away into the ground. Bees can detect both the presence and the pattern of electric fields on flowers, and use this information to know if a flower has been recently visited by another bee and is therefore likely to have a reduced concentration of pollen.<ref name=Clarke2013/>
Bees collect a positive static charge while flying through the air (see [[Atmospheric electricity]]). When a bee visits a flower, the charge deposited on the flower takes a while to leak away into the ground. Bees can detect both the presence and the pattern of electric fields on flowers, and use this information to know if a flower has been recently visited by another bee and is therefore likely to have a reduced concentration of pollen.<ref name=Clarke2013/>
The mechanism of electric field reception in animals living in the air like bees is based on mechano- reception, not electroreception. Bees receive the electric field changes via the [[Johnston's organ]]s in their [[Antenna (biology)|antennae]] and possibly other mechano-receptors. They distinguish different temporal patterns and learn them. [[Honeybee]]s appear to use the electric field emanating from the dancing bee during the [[waggle dance]] for distance communication.<ref name=Greggers>{{cite journal|last=Greggers|first=Uwe|coauthors= Koch G, Schmidt V, Dürr A, Floriou-Servou A, Piepenbrock D, Göpfert MC, Menzel R,|title=Reception and learning of electric fields|journal=Proc. Roy.Soc. B|date=2013|year=2013|month=March|doi=10.1098/rspb.2013.0528}}</ref><ref name=greggers1>{{cite web|last=Greggers|first=Uwe|title=ESF in bees|url=http://www.honeybee.neurobiologie.fu-berlin.de/column/ESF.html}}</ref>
The mechanism of electric field reception in animals living in the air like bees is based on mechano- reception, not electroreception. Bees receive the electric field changes via the [[Johnston's organ]]s in their [[Antenna (biology)|antennae]] and possibly other mechano-receptors. They distinguish different temporal patterns and learn them. During the [[waggle dance]], [[Honeybee]]s appear to use the electric field emanating from the dancing bee for distance communication.<ref name=Greggers>{{cite journal|last=Greggers|first=Uwe|coauthors= Koch G, Schmidt V, Dürr A, Floriou-Servou A, Piepenbrock D, Göpfert MC, Menzel R,|title=Reception and learning of electric fields|journal=Proc. Roy.Soc. B|date=2013|year=2013|month=March|doi=10.1098/rspb.2013.0528}}</ref><ref name=greggers1>{{cite web|last=Greggers|first=Uwe|title=ESF in bees|url=http://www.honeybee.neurobiologie.fu-berlin.de/column/ESF.html}}</ref>


==See also==
==See also==

Revision as of 06:55, 11 July 2013

Electroreception is the biological ability to perceive natural electrical stimuli. It has been observed almost exclusively in aquatic or amphibious animals, since water is a much better conductor than air, the currently known exceptions being echidnas, cockroaches and bees. Electroreception is used in electrolocation (detecting objects) and for electrocommunication.

Overview

Until recently, electroreception was known only in vertebrates. Recent research has shown that bees can detect the presence and pattern of a static charge on flowers.[1] Electroreception is found in lampreys, cartilaginous fishes (sharks, rays, chimaeras), lungfishes, bichirs, coelacanths, sturgeons, paddlefishes, catfishes, gymnotiformes, elephantfishes, monotremes, and at least one species of cetacean. The electroreceptor organs in all these groups are derived embryologically from a mechanosensory system. In fishes they are developed from the lateral lines. In most groups electroreception is passive, where it is used predominantly in predation. Two groups of teleost fishes are weakly electric and engage in active electroreception; the Neotropical knifefishes (Gymnotiformes) and the African elephantfishes (Notopteroidei). A rare terrestrial exception is the Western long-beaked echidna which has about 2,000 electroreceptors on its bill, compared to 40,000 for its semi-aquatic monotreme relative, the duck-billed platypus.[2]

Electrolocation

Electroreceptive animals use this sense to locate objects around them. This is important in ecological niches where the animal cannot depend on vision: for example in caves, in murky water and at night. Many fish use electric fields to detect buried prey. Some shark embryos and pups "freeze" when they detect the characteristic electric signal of their predators.[3] It has been proposed that sharks can use their acute electric sense to detect the earth's magnetic field by detecting the weak electric currents induced by their swimming or by the flow of ocean currents. The walking behaviour of cockroaches can be affected by the presence of a static electric field: they like to avoid the electric field.[4] Cabbage loopers are also known to avoid electric fields.[4]

Active electrolocation

Active electrolocation. Conductive objects concentrate the field and resistive objects spread the field.

In active electrolocation,[5] the animal senses its surrounding environment by generating electric fields and detecting distortions in these fields using electroreceptor organs. This electric field is generated by means of a specialised electric organ consisting of modified muscle or nerves. This field may be modulated so that its frequency and wave form are unique to the species and sometimes, the individual (see Jamming avoidance response). Animals that use active electroreception include the weakly electric fish, which either generate small electrical pulses (termed "pulse-type") or produce a quasi-sinusoidal discharge from the electric organ (termed "wave-type").[6] These fish create a potential which is usually smaller than one volt. Weakly electric fish can discriminate between objects with different resistance and capacitance values, which may help in identifying the object. Active electroreception typically has a range of about one body length, though objects with an electrical impedance similar to that of the surrounding water are nearly undetectable.

Passive electrolocation

In passive electrolocation, the animal senses the weak bioelectric fields generated by other animals and uses it to locate them. These electric fields are generated by all animals due to the activity of their nerves and muscles. A second source of electric fields in fish is the ion pumps associated with osmoregulation at the gill membrane. This field is modulated by the opening and closing of the mouth and gill slits.[3][7] Many fish that prey on electrogenic fish use the discharges of their prey to detect them. This has driven the prey to evolve more complex or higher frequency signals that are harder to detect.[8]

Passive electroreception is carried out solely by ampullary electroreceptors in fish. It is tuned to low frequency signals (less than 1 Hz to tens of Hz).

Fish use passive electroreception to supplement or replace their other senses when detecting prey and predators. In sharks, sensing an electric dipole alone is sufficient to cause them to try to eat it.

Electrocommunication

Weakly electric fish can also communicate by modulating the electrical waveform they generate, an ability known as electrocommunication.[9] They may use this for mate attraction and territorial displays. Some species of catfish use their electric discharges only in agonistic displays.

In one species of Brachyhypopomus (a genus of South American river fish belonging to the family Hypopomidae, commonly known as bluntnose knifefishes), the electric discharge pattern is similar to the low voltage electrolocative discharge of the electric eel. This is hypothesised to be a form of Batesian mimicry of the dangerous eel.[10][clarification needed]

Sensory mechanism

Active electroreception relies upon tuberous electroreceptors which are sensitive to high frequency (20-20,000 Hz) stimuli. These receptors have a loose plug of epithelial cells which capacitively couples the sensory receptor cells to the external environment. Passive electroreception however, relies upon ampullary receptors which are sensitive to low frequency stimuli (below 50 Hz). These receptors have a jelly-filled canal leading from the sensory receptors to the skin surface. Mormyrid electric fish from Africa use tuberous receptors known as Knollenorgans to sense electric communication signals.

Examples

Elasmobranchii

Electroreceptors (Ampullae of Lorenzini) and lateral line canals in the head of a shark.

Sharks and rays (members of the subclass Elasmobranchii) rely heavily on electrolocation in the final stages of their attacks, as can be demonstrated by the robust feeding response elicited by electric fields similar to those of their prey. Sharks are the most electrically sensitive animals known, responding to DC fields as low as 5 nV/cm.

The electric field sensors of sharks are called the ampullae of Lorenzini. They consist of electroreceptor cells connected to the seawater by pores on their snouts and other zones of the head. A problem with the early submarine telegraph cables was the damage caused by sharks who sensed the electric fields produced by these cables. It is possible that sharks may use Earth's magnetic field to navigate the oceans using this sense.

Bony fish

The electric eel, besides its ability to generate high voltage electric shocks, uses lower voltage pulses for navigation and prey detection in its turbid habitat. This ability is shared with other gymnotiformes.

Monotremes

The platypus also uses electroreception.

The monotremes are the only group of land mammals known to have evolved electroreception. While the electroreceptors in fish and amphibians evolved from mechanosensory lateral line organs, those of monotremes are based on cutaneous glands innervated by trigeminal nerves. The electroreceptors of monotremes consist of free nerve endings located in the mucous glands of the snout. Among the monotremes, the duck-billed platypus (Ornithorhynchus anatinus) has the most acute electric sense.[11][12] The platypus has almost 40,000 electroreceptors arranged in a series of stripes along the bill, which probably aids the localisation of prey.[13] The platypus electroreceptive system is highly directional, with the axis of greatest sensitivity pointing outwards and downwards. By making short-latency head movements called "saccades" when swimming, platypuses constantly expose the most sensitive part of their bill to the stimulus to localise prey as accurately as possible. The platypus appears to use electroreception along with pressure sensors to determine the distance to prey from the delay between the arrival of electrical signals and pressure changes in the water.[12]

The electroreceptive capabilities of the two species of echidna (which are terrestrial) are much more simple. Western long-beaked echidnas (Zaglossus bruijnii) possess only 2,000 receptors and short-beaked echidnas (Tachyglossus aculeatus) merely 400 that are concentrated in the tip of the snout.[13] This difference can be attributed to their habitat and feeding methods. Western long-beaked echidnas live in wet tropical forests where they feed on earthworms in damp leaf litter, so their habitat is probably favourable to the reception of electrical signals. Contrary to this is the varied but generally more arid habitat of their short-beaked relative which feeds primarily on termites and ants in nests; the humidity in these nests presumably allows electroreception to be used in hunting for buried prey, particularly after rains.[14] Experiments have shown that echidnas can be trained to respond to weak electric fields in water and moist soil. The electric sense of the echidna is hypothesised to be an evolutionary remnant from a platypus-like ancestor.[12]

Dolphins

Dolphins have evolved electroreception in structures different to those of fish, amphibians and monotremes. The hairless vibrissal crypts on the rostrum of the Guiana dolphin (Sotalia guianensis), originally associated with mammalian whiskers, are capable of electroreception as low as 4.8 μV/cm, sufficient to detect small fish. This is comparable to the sensitivity of electroreceptors in the platypus.[15] It should be noted that to-date (June 2013), these cells have been described from only a single dolphin specimen.

Bees

Bees collect a positive static charge while flying through the air (see Atmospheric electricity). When a bee visits a flower, the charge deposited on the flower takes a while to leak away into the ground. Bees can detect both the presence and the pattern of electric fields on flowers, and use this information to know if a flower has been recently visited by another bee and is therefore likely to have a reduced concentration of pollen.[1] The mechanism of electric field reception in animals living in the air like bees is based on mechano- reception, not electroreception. Bees receive the electric field changes via the Johnston's organs in their antennae and possibly other mechano-receptors. They distinguish different temporal patterns and learn them. During the waggle dance, Honeybees appear to use the electric field emanating from the dancing bee for distance communication.[16][17]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi: 10.1126/science.1230883, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi= 10.1126/science.1230883 instead.
  2. ^ "Electroreception in fish, amphibians and monotremes". Map of Life. Retrieved 26 October 2012.
  3. ^ a b Coplin, Shaun P. (2004). "The functional roles of passive electroreception in non-electric shes". Animal Biology. 54 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1163/157075604323010024. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  4. ^ a b 1. Jackson, C.W., E. Hunt, S. Sharkh, and P.L. Newland. 2011. Static electric fields modify the locomotory behaviour of cockroaches. The Journal of experimental biology. 214: 2020–6.
  5. ^ Albert, J. S., and W. G. R. Crampton. 2006. Electroreception and electrogenesis. Pp. 429-470 in P. L. Lutz, ed. The Physiology of Fishes. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL.
  6. ^ Babineau, David (2006-09-15). "Modeling the Electric Field of Weakly Electric Fish". Journal of Experimental Biology. 209 (18): 3636–3651. doi:10.1242/jeb.02403. ISSN 1477-9145 0022-0949, 1477-9145. PMID 16943504. Retrieved 2012-05-17. {{cite journal}}: Check |issn= value (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  7. ^ BODZNICK, DAVID (1992). "SUPPRESSION OF COMMON MODE SIGNALS WITHIN THE ELECTROSENSORY SYSTEM OF THE LITTLE SKATE RAJA ERINACEA". J. Exp. Biol. 171: 107–125. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  8. ^ Stoddard, Philip K. (2002). "The evolutionary origins of electric signal complexity". Journal of Physiology - Paris. 96 (5–6): 485–491. doi:10.1016/S0928-4257(03)00004-4.
  9. ^ Hopkins, CD (1999). "Design features for electric communication". J Exp Biol. 202 (Pt 10): 1217–1228. PMID 10210663. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  10. ^ Stoddard, PK. (1999). "Predation enhances complexity in the evolution of electric fish signals". Nature. 400 (6741): 254–6. doi:10.1038/22301. PMID 10421365. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  11. ^ H, Scheich (1986 January 30-February 5). "Electroreception and electrolocation in platypus". Nature. 319 (6052). Nature Publishing Group: 401–2. doi:10.1038/319401a0. PMID 3945317. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  12. ^ a b c Pettigrew, John D. (1999). "Electroreception in Monotremes" (PDF). The Journal of Experimental Biology (202): 1447–1454. Retrieved 19 September 2006.
  13. ^ a b "Electroreception in fish, amphibians and monotremes". Map Of Life. 2010. Retrieved June 12, 2013.
  14. ^ Proske, U. (1998). "Sensory receptors in monotremes". Phil. T rans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 353 (1372): 1187–1198. doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0275. PMC 1692308. PMID 9720114. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  15. ^ Czech-Damal, Liebschner, Miersch, Klauer, Hanke, Marshall, Dehnhardt & Hanke. 2011. Electroreception in the Guiana dolphin (Sotalia guianensis). Proc Roy Soc B doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1127
  16. ^ Greggers, Uwe (2013). "Reception and learning of electric fields". Proc. Roy.Soc. B. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.0528. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link) CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link)
  17. ^ Greggers, Uwe. "ESF in bees".