Jump to content

Imitative learning: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m added wikilinks and removed "underlinked" tag
m Distinguished from observational learning in lead section
Line 1: Line 1:
'''Imitative learning''' is a type of [[social learning theory|social learning]] whereby new [[Behavior|behaviors]] are acquired via [[imitation]].<ref name=Ganos>{{cite journal |vauthors=Ganos C, Ogrzal T, Schnitzler A, Münchau A |title=The pathophysiology of echopraxia/echolalia: relevance to Gilles de la Tourette syndrome |journal=Mov. Disord. |volume=27 |issue=10 |pages=1222–9 |date=September 2012 |pmid=22807284 |doi=10.1002/mds.25103}}</ref> Imitation aids in [[communication]], social interaction, and the ability to modulate one's [[Emotion|emotions]] to account for the emotions of others, and is "essential for healthy [[Sensory-motor coupling|sensorimotor]] development and social functioning".<ref name=Ganos/> The ability to match one's actions to those observed in others occurs in humans and animals;<ref name=Ganos/> imitative learning plays an important role in humans in cultural development.<ref name=Heyes>{{cite journal| author= Heyes C |title= Grist and mills: on the cultural origins of cultural learning |journal= Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci |date= Aug 5, 2012 |volume= 367 |issue= 1599 |pages= 2181–91 |pmid= 22734061 |pmc= 3385685 | doi= 10.1098/rstb.2012.0120}}</ref>
'''Imitative learning''' is a type of [[social learning theory|social learning]] whereby new [[Behavior|behaviors]] are acquired via [[imitation]].<ref name=Ganos>{{cite journal |vauthors=Ganos C, Ogrzal T, Schnitzler A, Münchau A |title=The pathophysiology of echopraxia/echolalia: relevance to Gilles de la Tourette syndrome |journal=Mov. Disord. |volume=27 |issue=10 |pages=1222–9 |date=September 2012 |pmid=22807284 |doi=10.1002/mds.25103}}</ref> Imitation aids in [[communication]], social interaction, and the ability to modulate one's [[Emotion|emotions]] to account for the emotions of others, and is "essential for healthy [[Sensory-motor coupling|sensorimotor]] development and social functioning".<ref name=Ganos/> The ability to match one's actions to those observed in others occurs in humans and animals;<ref name=Ganos/> imitative learning plays an important role in humans in cultural development.<ref name=Heyes>{{cite journal| author= Heyes C |title= Grist and mills: on the cultural origins of cultural learning |journal= Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci |date= Aug 5, 2012 |volume= 367 |issue= 1599 |pages= 2181–91 |pmid= 22734061 |pmc= 3385685 | doi= 10.1098/rstb.2012.0120}}</ref> Imitative learning is different from [[observational learning]] in that it requires a duplication of the behaviour exhibited by the model, whereas observational learning can occur when the learner observes an unwanted behaviour and its subsequent consequences and as a result learns to avoid that behaviour.


== Imitative learning in animals ==
== Imitative learning in animals ==

Revision as of 20:15, 26 March 2019

Imitative learning is a type of social learning whereby new behaviors are acquired via imitation.[1] Imitation aids in communication, social interaction, and the ability to modulate one's emotions to account for the emotions of others, and is "essential for healthy sensorimotor development and social functioning".[1] The ability to match one's actions to those observed in others occurs in humans and animals;[1] imitative learning plays an important role in humans in cultural development.[2] Imitative learning is different from observational learning in that it requires a duplication of the behaviour exhibited by the model, whereas observational learning can occur when the learner observes an unwanted behaviour and its subsequent consequences and as a result learns to avoid that behaviour.

Imitative learning in animals

On the most basic level, research performed by A.L. Saggerson, David N. George, and R.C. Honey showed that pigeons were able to learn a basic process that would lead to the delivery of a reward by watching a demonstrator pigeon.[3] A demonstrator pigeon was trained to peck a panel in response to one stimulus (e.g. a red light) and hop on the panel in response to a second stimulus (e.g. a green light). After proficiency in this task was established in the demonstrator pigeon, other learner pigeons were placed in a video-monitored observation chamber. After every second observed trial, these learner pigeons were then individually placed in the demonstrator pigeon's box and presented the same test. The learner pigeons displayed competent performance on the task, and thus it was concluded that the learner pigeons had formed a response-outcome association while observing. However, the researchers noted that an alternative interpretation of these results could be that the learner pigeons had instead acquired outcome-response associations that guided their behavior and that further testing was needed to establish if this was a valid alternative.

References

  1. ^ a b c Ganos C, Ogrzal T, Schnitzler A, Münchau A (September 2012). "The pathophysiology of echopraxia/echolalia: relevance to Gilles de la Tourette syndrome". Mov. Disord. 27 (10): 1222–9. doi:10.1002/mds.25103. PMID 22807284.
  2. ^ Heyes C (Aug 5, 2012). "Grist and mills: on the cultural origins of cultural learning". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 367 (1599): 2181–91. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0120. PMC 3385685. PMID 22734061.
  3. ^ Saggerson, George, & Honey. (2005). Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes, 31 (3), 289 –300.