Vocabulary development: Difference between revisions
disambig, fix harv ref errors, ref formatting |
m Remove ellipsis and add citation. |
||
Line 3: | Line 3: | ||
'''Vocabulary development''' is a process by which people acquire words. Babbling shifts towards meaningful speech as infants grow and produce their first words around the age of one year. In early word learning, infants build their vocabulary slowly. By the age of 18 months, infants can typically produce about 50 words and begin to make word combinations. |
'''Vocabulary development''' is a process by which people acquire words. Babbling shifts towards meaningful speech as infants grow and produce their first words around the age of one year. In early word learning, infants build their vocabulary slowly. By the age of 18 months, infants can typically produce about 50 words and begin to make word combinations. |
||
In order to build their vocabularies, infants must learn about the [[Meaning (linguistics)|meanings]] that words carry. The mapping problem asks how infants correctly learn to attach words to [[referent]]s. Constraints theories, [[Domain-general learning|domain-general]] views, social-[[Pragmatics|pragmatic]] accounts, and an [[Emergentism|emergentist]] coalition model have been proposed to account for the mapping problem |
In order to build their vocabularies, infants must learn about the [[Meaning (linguistics)|meanings]] that words carry. The mapping problem asks how infants correctly learn to attach words to [[referent]]s. Constraints theories, [[Domain-general learning|domain-general]] views, social-[[Pragmatics|pragmatic]] accounts, and an [[Emergentism|emergentist]] coalition model have been proposed{{sfn|Hirsh-Pasek|Golinkoff|Hollich|2000|p=145}} to account for the mapping problem. |
||
From an early age, infants use language to [[Communication|communicate]]. [[Caregiver]]s and other family members use language to teach children how to act in society. In their interactions with peers, children have the opportunity to learn about unique conversational roles. Through pragmatic directions, adults often offer children cues for understanding the meaning of words. |
From an early age, infants use language to [[Communication|communicate]]. [[Caregiver]]s and other family members use language to teach children how to act in society. In their interactions with peers, children have the opportunity to learn about unique conversational roles. Through pragmatic directions, adults often offer children cues for understanding the meaning of words. |
Revision as of 20:21, 16 April 2019
Vocabulary development is a process by which people acquire words. Babbling shifts towards meaningful speech as infants grow and produce their first words around the age of one year. In early word learning, infants build their vocabulary slowly. By the age of 18 months, infants can typically produce about 50 words and begin to make word combinations.
In order to build their vocabularies, infants must learn about the meanings that words carry. The mapping problem asks how infants correctly learn to attach words to referents. Constraints theories, domain-general views, social-pragmatic accounts, and an emergentist coalition model have been proposed[1] to account for the mapping problem.
From an early age, infants use language to communicate. Caregivers and other family members use language to teach children how to act in society. In their interactions with peers, children have the opportunity to learn about unique conversational roles. Through pragmatic directions, adults often offer children cues for understanding the meaning of words.
Throughout their school years, children continue to build their vocabulary. In particular, children begin to learn abstract words. Beginning around age 3–5, word learning takes place both in conversation and through reading. Word learning often involves physical context, builds on prior knowledge, takes place in social context, and includes semantic support. The phonological loop and serial order short-term memory may both play an important role in vocabulary development.
Early word learning
Infants begin to understand words such as "Mommy", "Daddy", "hands" and "feet" when they are approximately 6 months old.[2][3] Initially, these words refer to their own mother or father or hands or feet. Infants begin to produce their first words when they are approximately one year old.[4][5] Infants' first words are normally used in reference to things that are of importance to them, such as objects, body parts, people, and relevant actions. Also, the first words that infants produce are mostly single-syllabic or repeated single syllables, such as "no" and "dada".[5] By 12 to 18 months of age, children's vocabularies often contain words such as "kitty", "bottle", "doll", "car" and "eye". Children's understanding of names for objects and people usually precedes their understanding of words that describe actions and relationships. "One" and "two" are the first number words that children learn between the ages of one and two.[6] Infants must be able to hear and play with sounds in their environment, and to break up various phonetic units to discover words and their related meanings.
Phonological development
Studies related to vocabulary development show that children's language competence depends upon their ability to hear sounds during infancy.[4][7][8] Infants' perception of speech is distinct. Between six and ten months of age, infants can discriminate sounds used in the languages of the world.[4] By 10 to 12 months, infants can no longer discriminate between speech sounds that are not used in the language(s) to which they are exposed.[4] Among six-month-old infants, seen articulations (i.e. the mouth movements they observe others make while talking) actually enhance their ability to discriminate sounds, and may also contribute to infants' ability to learn phonemic boundaries.[9] Infants' phonological register is completed between the ages of 18 months and 7 years.[4]
Children's phonological development normally proceeds as follows:[4]
6–8 weeks: Cooing appears
16 weeks: Laughter and vocal play appear
6–9 months: Reduplicated (canonical) babbling appears
12 months: First words use a limited sound repertoire
18 months: Phonological processes (deformations of target sounds) become systematic
18 months–7 years: Phonological inventory completion
At each stage mentioned above, children play with sounds and learn methods to help them learn words.[7] There is a relationship between children's prelinguistic phonetic skills and their lexical progress at age two: failure to develop the required phonetic skills in their prelinguistic period results in children's delay in producing words.[10] Environmental influences may affect children's phonological development, such as hearing loss as a result of ear infections.[4] Deaf infants and children with hearing problems due to infections are usually delayed in the beginning of vocal babbling.
Babbling
Babbling is an important aspect of vocabulary development in infants, since it appears to help practice producing speech sounds.[11] Babbling begins between five and seven months of age. At this stage, babies start to play with sounds that are not used to express their emotional or physical states, such as sounds of consonants and vowels.[7] Babies begin to babble in real syllables such as "ba-ba-ba, neh-neh-neh, and dee-dee-dee,"[7] between the ages of seven and eight months; this is known as canonical babbling.[4] Jargon babbling includes strings of such sounds; this type of babbling uses intonation but doesn't convey meaning. The phonemes and syllabic patterns produced by infants begin to be distinctive to particular languages during this period (e.g., increased nasal stops in French and Japanese babies) though most of their sounds are similar.[4][7] There is a shift from babbling to the use of words as the infant grows.[12]
Vocabulary spurt
As children get older their rate of vocabulary growth increases. Children probably understand their first 50 words before they produce them. By the age of eighteen months, children typically attain a vocabulary of 50 words in production, and between two and three times greater in comprehension.[5][7] A switch from an early stage of slow vocabulary growth to a later stage of faster growth is referred to as the vocabulary spurt.[13] Young toddlers acquire one to three words per month. A vocabulary spurt often occurs over time as the number of words learned accelerates. It is believed that most children add about 10 to 20 new words a week.[13] Between the ages of 18 to 24 months, children learn how to combine two words such as no bye-bye and more please.[5] Three-word and four-word combinations appear when most of the child's utterances are two-word productions. In addition, children are able to form conjoined sentences, using and.[5] This suggests that there is a vocabulary spurt between the time that the child's first word appears, and when the child is able to form more than two words, and eventually, sentences. However, there have been arguments as to whether or not there is a spurt in acquisition of words. In one study of 38 children, only five of the children had an inflection point in their rate of word acquisition as opposed to a quadratic growth.[13] This study suggests that most children do not have a vocabulary spurt.
Mapping problem
In word learning, the mapping problem refers to the question of how infants attach the forms of language to the things that they experience in the world.[14] There are infinite objects, concepts, and actions in the world that words could be mapped onto.[14] Many theories have been proposed to account for the way in which the language learner successfully maps words onto the correct objects, concepts, and actions.
While domain-specific accounts of word learning argue for innate constraints that limit infants' hypotheses about word meanings,[15] domain-general perspectives argue that word learning can be accounted for by general cognitive processes, such as learning and memory, which are not specific to language.[16] Yet other theorists have proposed social pragmatic accounts, which stress the role of caregivers in guiding infants through the word learning process.[17] According to some[who?] research, however, children are active participants in their own word learning, although caregivers may still play an important role in this process.[18][19] Recently, an emergentist coalition model has also been proposed to suggest that word learning cannot be fully attributed to a single factor. Instead, a variety of cues, including salient and social cues, may be utilized by infants at different points in their vocabulary development.[1]
Theories of constraints
Theories of word-learning constraints argue for biases or default assumptions that guide the infant through the word learning process. Constraints are outside of the infant's control and are believed to help the infant limit their hypotheses about the meaning of words that they encounter daily.[15][20] Constraints can be considered domain-specific (unique to language).
Critics[who?] argue that theories of constraints focus on how children learn nouns, but ignore other aspects of their word learning.[21] Although constraints are useful in explaining how children limit possible meanings when learning novel words, the same constraints would eventually need to be overridden because they are not utilized in adult language.[22] For instance, adult speakers often use several terms, each term meaning something slightly different, when referring to one entity, such as a family pet. This practice would violate the mutual exclusivity constraint.[22]
Below, the most prominent constraints in the literature are detailed:
- Reference is the notion that a word symbolizes or stands in for an object, action, or event.[23] Words consistently stand for their referents, even if referents are not physically present in context.[23]
- Mutual Exclusivity is the assumption that each object in the world can only be referred to by a single label.[15][24]
- Shape has been considered to be one of the most critical properties for identifying members of an object category.[25] Infants assume that objects that have the same shape also share a name.[26] Shape plays an important role in both appropriate and inappropriate extensions.[25]
- The Whole Object Assumption is the belief that labels refer to whole objects instead of parts or properties of those objects.[15][27] Children are believed to hold this assumption because they typically label whole objects first, and parts of properties of objects later in development.[27]
- The Taxonomic Assumption reflects the belief that speakers use words to refer to categories that are internally consistent.[28] Labels to pick out coherent categories of objects, rather than those objects and the things that are related to them.[15][28] For example, children assume that the word "dog" refers to the category of "dogs", not to "dogs with bones", or "dogs chasing cats".[28]
Domain-general views
Domain-general views of vocabulary development argue that children do not need principles or constraints in order to successfully develop word-world mappings.[16] Instead, word learning can be accounted for through general learning mechanisms such as salience, association, and frequency.[16] Children are thought to notice the objects, actions, or events that are most salient in context, and then to associate them with the words that are most frequently used in their presence.[16] Additionally, research on word learning suggests that fast mapping, the rapid learning that children display after a single exposure to new information, is not specific to word learning. Children can also successfully fast map when exposed to a novel fact, remembering both words and facts after a time delay.[21]
Domain-general views have been criticized for not fully explaining how children manage to avoid mapping errors when there are numerous possible referents to which objects, actions, or events might point.[29] For instance, if biases are not present from birth, why do infants assume that labels refer to whole objects, instead of salient parts of these objects?[29] However, domain-general perspectives do not dismiss the notion of biases. Rather, they suggest biases develop through learning strategies instead of existing as built-in constraints. For instance, the whole object bias could be explained as a strategy that humans use to reason about the world; perhaps we are prone to thinking about our environment in terms of whole objects, and this strategy is not specific to the language domain.[21] Additionally, children may be exposed to cues associated with categorization by shape early in the word learning process, which would draw their attention to shape when presented with novel objects and labels.[30] Ordinary learning could, then, lead to a shape bias.[30]
Social pragmatic theories
Social pragmatic theories, also in contrast to the constraints view, focus on the social context in which the infant is embedded.[17] According to this approach, environmental input removes the ambiguity of the word learning situation.[17] Cues such as the caregiver's gaze, body language, gesture, and smile help infants to understand the meanings of words.[17] Social pragmatic theories stress the role of the caregiver in talking about objects, actions, or events that the infant is already focused-in upon.[17]
Joint attention is an important mechanism through which children learn to map words-to-world, and vice versa.[31] Adults commonly make an attempt to establish joint attention with a child before they convey something to the child. Joint attention is often accompanied by physical co-presence, since children are often focused on what is in their immediate environment.[31] As well, conversational co-presence is likely to occur; the caregiver and child typically talk together about whatever is taking place at their locus of joint attention.[31] Social pragmatic perspectives often present children as covariation detectors, who simply associate the words that they hear with whatever they are attending to in the world at the same time.[32] The co-variation detection model of joint attention seems problematic when we consider that many caregiver utterances do not refer to things that occupy the immediate attentional focus of infants. For instance, caregivers among the Kaluli, a group of indigenous peoples living in New Guinea, rarely provide labels in the context of their referents.[32] While the covariation detection model emphasizes the caregiver's role in the meaning-making process, some theorists[who?] argue that infants also play an important role in their own word learning, actively avoiding mapping errors.[19] When infants are in situations where their own attentional focus differs from that of a speaker, they seek out information about the speaker's focus, and then use that information to establish correct word-referent mappings.[18][32] Joint attention can be created through infant agency, in an attempt to gather information about a speaker's intent.[32]
From early on, children also assume that language is designed for communication. Infants treat communication as a cooperative process.[33] Specifically, infants observe the principles of conventionality and contrast. According to conventionality, infants believe that for a particular meaning that they wish to convey, there is a term that everyone in the community would expect to be used.[33][34] According to contrast, infants act according to the notion that differences in form mark differences in meaning.[33][34] Children's attention to conventionality and contrast is demonstrated in their language use, even before the age of 2 years; they direct their early words towards adult targets, repair mispronunciations quickly if possible, ask for words to relate to the world around them, and maintain contrast in their own word use.[33]
Emergentist coalition model
The emergentist coalition model suggests that children make use of multiple cues to successfully attach a novel label to a novel object.[1] The word learning situation may offer an infant combinations of social, perceptual, cognitive, and linguistic cues. While a range of cues are available from the start of word learning, it may be the case that not all cues are utilized by the infant when they begin the word learning process.[1] While younger children may only be able to detect a limited number of cues, older, more experienced word learners may be able to make use of a range of cues. For instance, young children seem to focus primarily on perceptual salience, but older children attend to the gaze of caregivers and use the focus of caregivers to direct their word mapping.[1] Therefore, this model argues that principles or cues may be present from the onset of word learning, but the use of a wide range of cues develops over time.[35]
Supporters of the emergentist coalition model argue that, as a hybrid, this model moves towards a more holistic explanation of word learning that is not captured by models with a singular focus. For instance, constraints theories typically argue that constraints/principles are available to children from the onset of word learning, but do not explain how children develop into expert speakers who are not limited by constraints.[36] Additionally, some argue[who?] that domain-general perspectives do not fully address the question of how children sort through numerous potential referents in order to correctly sort out meaning.[36] Lastly, social pragmatic theories claim that social encounters guide word learning. Although these theories describe how children become more advanced word learners, they seem to tell us little about children's capacities at the start of word learning.[36] According to its proponents, the emergentist coalion model incorporates constraints/principles, but argues for the development and change in these principles over time, while simultaneously taking into consideration social aspects of word learning alongside other cues, such as salience.[37]
Pragmatic development
Both linguistic and socio-cultural factors affect the rate at which vocabulary develops.[38] Children must learn to use their words appropriately and strategically in social situations.[39] They have flexible and powerful social-cognitive skills that allow them to understand the communicative intentions of others in a wide variety of interactive situations. Children learn new words in communicative situations.[40] Children rely on pragmatic skills to build more extensive vocabularies.[41] Some aspects of pragmatic behaviour can predict later literacy and mathematical achievement, as children who are pragmatically skilled often function better in school. These children are also generally better liked.[42]
Children use words differently for objects, spatial relations and actions. Children ages one to three often rely on general purpose deictic words such as "here", "that" or "look" accompanied by a gesture, which is most often pointing, to pick out specific objects.[41] Children also stretch already known or partly known words to cover other objects that appear similar to the original. This can result in word overextension or misuses of words. Word overextension is governed by the perceptual similarities children notice among the different referents. Misuses of words indirectly provide ways of finding out which meanings children have attached to particular words.[41] When children come into contact with spatial relations, they talk about the location of one object with respect to another. They name the object located and use a deictic term, such as here or "there" for location, or they name both the object located and its location. They can also use a general purpose locative marker, which is a preposition, postposition or suffix depending on the language that is linked in some way to the word for location.[41] Children's earliest words for actions usually encode both the action and its result. Children use a small number of general purpose verbs, such as "do" and "make" for a large variety of actions because their resources are limited. Children acquiring a second language seem to use the same production strategies for talking about actions. Sometimes children use a highly specific verb instead of a general purpose verb. In both cases children stretch their resources to communicate what they want to say.[41]
Infants use words to communicate early in life and their communication skills develop as they grow older. Communication skills aid in word learning. Infants learn to take turns while communicating with adults. While preschoolers lack precise timing and rely on obvious speaker cues, older children are more precise in their timing and take fewer long pauses.[43] Children get better at initiating and sustaining coherent conversations as they age. Toddlers and preschoolers use strategies such as repeating and recasting their partners' utterances to keep the conversation going. Older children add new relevant information to conversations. Connectives such as then, so, and because are more frequently used as children get older.[44] When giving and responding to feedback, preschoolers are inconsistent, but around the age of six, children can mark corrections with phrases and head nods to indicate their continued attention. As children continue to age they provide more constructive interpretations back to listeners, which helps prompt conversations.[45]
Pragmatic influences
Caregivers use language to help children become competent members of society and culture. From birth, infants receive pragmatic information. They learn structure of conversations from early interactions with caregivers. Actions and speech are organized in games, such as peekaboo to provide children with information about words and phrases. Caregivers find many ways to help infants interact and respond. As children advance and participate more actively in interactions, caregivers adapt their interactions accordingly.[46] Caregivers also prompt children to produce correct pragmatic behaviours. They provide input about what children are expected to say, how to speak, when they should speak, and how they can stay on topic. Caregivers may model the appropriate behaviour, using verbal reinforcement, posing a hypothetical situation, addressing children's comments, or evaluating another person.[47]
Family members contribute to pragmatic development in different ways. Fathers often act as secondary caregivers, and may know the child less intimately. Older siblings may lack the capacity to acknowledge the child's needs. As a result, both fathers and siblings may pressure children to communicate more clearly. They often challenge children to improve their communication skills, therefore preparing them to communicate with strangers about unfamiliar topics. Fathers have more breakdowns when communicating with infants, and spend less time focused on the same objects or actions as infants. Siblings are more directive and less responsive to infants, which motivates infants to participate in conversations with their older siblings.[48] There are limitations to studies that focus on the influences of fathers and siblings, as most research is descriptive and correlational. In reality, there are many variations of family configurations, and context influences parent behaviour more than parent gender does.[49] The majority of research in this field is conducted with mother/child pairs.
Peers help expose children to multi-party conversations. This allows children to hear a greater variety of speech, and to observe different conversational roles. Peers may be uncooperative conversation partners, which pressures the children to communicate more effectively. Speaking to peers is different from speaking to adults, but children may still correct their peers. Peer interaction provides children with a different experience filled with special humour, disagreements and conversational topics.[42]
Culture and context in infants’ linguistic environment shape their vocabulary development. English learners have been found to map novel labels to objects more reliably than to actions compared to Mandarin learners. This early noun bias in English learners is caused by the culturally reinforced tendency for English speaking caregivers to engage in a significant amount of ostensive labelling as well as noun-friendly activities such as picture book reading.[50] Adult speech provides children with grammatical input. Both Mandarin and Cantonese languages have a category of grammatical function word called a noun classifier, which is also common across many genetically unrelated East Asian languages. In Cantonese, classifiers are obligatory and specific in more situations than in Mandarin. This accounts for the research found on Mandarin-speaking children outperforming Cantonese-speaking children in relation to the size of their vocabulary.[38]
Pragmatic directions
Pragmatic directions provide children with additional information about the speaker's intended meaning. Children's learning of new word meanings is guided by the pragmatic directions that adults offer, such as explicit links to word meanings.[51] Adults present young children with information about how words are related to each other through connections, such as "is a part of", "is a kind of", "belongs to", or "is used for". These pragmatic directions provide children with essential information about language, allowing them to make inferences about possible meanings for unfamiliar words.[52] This is also called inclusion. When children are provided with two words related by inclusion, they hold on to that information. When children hear an adult say an incorrect word, and then repair their mistake by stating the correct word, children take into account the repair when assigning meanings to the two words.[51]
In school-age children
Vocabulary development during the school years builds upon what the child already knows, and the child uses this knowledge to broaden his or her vocabulary. Once children have gained a level of vocabulary knowledge, new words are learned through explanations using familiar, or "old" words. This is done either explicitly, when a new word is defined using old words, or implicitly, when the word is set in the context of old words so that the meaning of the new word is constrained.[53] When children reach school-age, context and implicit learning are the most common ways in which their vocabularies continue to develop.[54] By this time, children learn new vocabulary mostly through conversation and reading.[55] Throughout schooling and adulthood, conversation and reading are the main methods in which vocabulary develops. This growth tends to slow once a person finishes schooling, as they have already acquired the vocabulary used in everyday conversation and reading material and generally are not engaging in activities that require additional vocabulary development.[53][56]
During the first few years of life, children are mastering concrete words such as "car", "bottle", "dog", "cat". By age 3, children are likely able to learn these concrete words without the need for a visual reference, so word learning tends to accelerate around this age.[57] Once children reach school-age, they learn abstract words (e.g. "love", "freedom", "success").[58] This broadens the vocabulary available for children to learn, which helps to account for the increase in word learning evident at school age.[59] By age 5, children tend to have an expressive vocabulary of 2,100–2,200 words. By age 6, they have approximately 2,600 words of expressive vocabulary and 20,000–24,000 words of receptive vocabulary.[60] Some claim that children experience a sudden acceleration in word learning, upwards of 20 words per day,[56] but it tends to be much more gradual than this. From age 6 to 8, the average child in school is learning 6–7 words per day, and from age 8 to 10, approximately 12 words per day.[21]
Means
Exposure to conversations and engaging in conversation with others help school-age children develop vocabulary. Fast mapping is the process of learning a new concept upon a single exposure and is used in word learning not only by infants and toddlers, but by preschool children and adults as well.[21] This principle is very useful for word learning in conversational settings, as words tend not to be explained explicitly in conversation, but may be referred to frequently throughout the span of a conversation.
Reading is considered to be a key element of vocabulary development in school-age children.[53][60][61][62] Before children are able to read on their own, children can learn from others reading to them. Learning vocabulary from these experiences includes using context, as well as explicit explanations of words and/or events in the story.[63] This may be done using illustrations in the book to guide explanation and provide a visual reference or comparisons, usually to prior knowledge and past experiences.[64] Interactions between the adult and the child often include the child's repetition of the new word back to the adult.[65] When a child begins to learn to read, their print vocabulary and oral vocabulary tend to be the same, as children use their vocabulary knowledge to match verbal forms of words with written forms. These two forms of vocabulary are usually equal up until grade 3. Because written language is much more diverse than spoken language, print vocabulary begins to expand beyond oral vocabulary.[66] By age 10, children's vocabulary development through reading moves away from learning concrete words to learning abstract words.[67]
Generally, both conversation and reading involve at least one of the four principles of context that are used in word learning and vocabulary development: physical context, prior knowledge, social context and semantic support.[68]
Physical context
Physical context involves the presence of an object or action that is also the topic of conversation. With the use of physical context, the child is exposed to both the words and a visual reference of the word. This is frequently used with infants and toddlers, but can be very beneficial for school-age children, especially when learning rare or infrequently used words.[62] Physical context may include props such as in toy play. When engaging in play with an adult, a child's vocabulary is developed through discussion of the toys, such as naming the object (e.g. "dinosaur") or labeling it with the use of a rare word (e.g., stegosaurus).[68] These sorts of interactions expose the child to words they may not otherwise encounter in day-to-day conversation.
Prior knowledge
Past experiences or general knowledge is often called upon in conversation, so it is a useful context for children to learn words. Recalling past experiences allows the child to call upon their own visual, tactical, oral, and/or auditory references.[68] For example, if a child once went to a zoo and saw an elephant, but did not know the word elephant, an adult could later help the child recall this event, describing the size and color of the animal, how big its ears were, its trunk, and the sound it made, then using the word elephant to refer to the animal. Calling upon prior knowledge is used not only in conversation, but often in book reading as well to help explain what is happening in a story by relating it back to the child's own experiences.[69]
Social context
Social context involves pointing out social norms and violations of these norms.[70] This form of context is most commonly found in conversation, as opposed to reading or other word learning environments. A child's understanding of social norms can help them to infer the meaning of words that occur in conversation. In an English-speaking tradition, "please" and "thank you" are taught to children at a very early age, so they are very familiar to the child by school-age. For example, if a group of people is eating a meal with the child present and one person says, "give me the bread" and another responds with, "that was rude. What do you say?", and the person responds with "please", the child may not know the meaning of "rude", but can infer its meaning through social context and understanding the necessity of saying "please".[70]
Semantic support
Semantic support is the most obvious method of vocabulary development in school-age children. It involves giving direct verbal information of the meaning of a word.[61][71] By the time children are in school, they are active participants in conversation, so they are very capable and willing to ask questions when they do not understand a word or concept. For example, a child might see a zebra for the first time and ask, what is that? and the parent might respond, that is a zebra. It is like a horse with stripes and it is wild so you cannot ride it.[71]
Memory
Memory plays an important role in vocabulary development, however the exact role that it plays is disputed in the literature. Specifically, short-term memory and how its capacities work with vocabulary development is questioned by many researchers[who?].
The phonology of words has proven to be beneficial to vocabulary development when children begin school. Once children have developed a vocabulary, they utilize the sounds that they already know to learn new words.[72] The phonological loop encodes, maintains and manipulates speech-based information that a person encounters. This information is then stored in the phonological memory, a part of short term memory. Research shows that children's capacities in the area of phonological memory are linked to vocabulary knowledge when children first begin school at age 4–5 years old. As memory capabilities tend to increase with age (between age 4 and adolescence), so does an individual's ability to learn more complex vocabulary.[72]
Serial-order short-term memory may be critical to the development of vocabulary.[73] As lexical knowledge increases, phonological representations have to become more precise to determine the differences between similar sound words (i.e. "calm", "come"). In this theory, the specific order or sequence of phonological events is used to learn new words, rather than phonology as a whole.[73]
See also
References
- ^ a b c d e Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 145.
- ^ Tincoff & Jusczyk 1999.
- ^ Tincoff & Jusczyk 2012.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i Hoff 2006.
- ^ a b c d e Hulit & Howard 2002.
- ^ Barner, Zapf & Lui 2012.
- ^ a b c d e f Pinker 1994.
- ^ Waxman & Booth 2000.
- ^ Teinonen et al. 2008.
- ^ Keren-Portnoy, Majorano & Vihman 2009.
- ^ Fagan 2009.
- ^ Vihman 1993.
- ^ a b c Ganger & Brent 2004.
- ^ a b Bloom 2000, p. 22.
- ^ a b c d e Clark 2009, p. 284.
- ^ a b c d Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 143.
- ^ a b c d e Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 141.
- ^ a b Baldwin 1995.
- ^ a b Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 142.
- ^ Bloom 2000, p. 20.
- ^ a b c d e Bloom & Markson 1998.
- ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 53.
- ^ a b Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 139.
- ^ Merriman, Bowman & MacWhinney 1989, p. 3.
- ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 45.
- ^ Smith 2000, p. 52.
- ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 50.
- ^ a b c Clark 1993, p. 52.
- ^ a b Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 144.
- ^ a b Smith 2000, p. 55.
- ^ a b c Clark 2009, p. 285.
- ^ a b c d Sabbagh & Baldwin 2005.
- ^ a b c d Clark 2009, p. 286.
- ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 64.
- ^ Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 146.
- ^ a b c Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 159.
- ^ Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 160.
- ^ a b Tardif et al. 2009.
- ^ Bryant 2009, p. 339.
- ^ Tomasello 2000.
- ^ a b c d e Clark 1978.
- ^ a b Bryant 2009, pp. 352–353.
- ^ Bryant 2009, p. 342.
- ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 342–343.
- ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 343–345.
- ^ Bryant 2009, p. 348.
- ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 348–349.
- ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 350–351.
- ^ Bryant 2009, p. 351.
- ^ Chan et al. 2011.
- ^ a b Clark & Grossman 1998.
- ^ Clark & Andrew 2002.
- ^ a b c Baker, Simmons & Kameenui 1995.
- ^ Newton, Padak & Rasinski 2008, pp. 7–8.
- ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, pp. 93–110.
- ^ a b Anglin & Miller 2000.
- ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 103.
- ^ Nippold 2004, pp. 1–8.
- ^ McKeown & Curtis 1987, p. 7.
- ^ a b Lorraine 2008.
- ^ a b Newton, Padak & Rasinski 2008.
- ^ a b Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 97.
- ^ Nagy, Herman & Anderson 1985.
- ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, pp. 101–103.
- ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 101.
- ^ Kamil & Hiebert 2005.
- ^ McKeown & Curtis 1987, p. 8.
- ^ a b c Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 105.
- ^ Newton, Padak & Rasinski 2008, pp. xvii.
- ^ a b Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 106.
- ^ a b Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, pp. 107.
- ^ a b Gathercole et al. 1992.
- ^ a b Leclercq & Majerus 2010.
Bibliography
- Anglin, Jeremy M.; Miller, George A. (2000). Vocabulary Development: A Morphological Analysis. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 131–132, 136. ISBN 978-0-631-22443-3.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Baker, S. K.; Simmons, D. C.; Kameenui, E. J. (1995). Vocabulary acquisition: Synthesis of the research. Technical Report No. 13. Eugene, OR.: National Center to Improve the Tools of Educators.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Baldwin, D. (1995). "Understanding the link between joint attention and language". In Moore, C.; Dunham, P. J. (eds.). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. pp. 131–158.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Barner, D.; Zapf, J.; Lui, T. (2012). "Is two a plural marker in early child language?". Developmental Psychology. 48 (1): 10–17. doi:10.1037/a0025283. PMID 21928879.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Bloom, L. (2000). "The intentionality model of word learning: How to learn a word, any word". In Golinkoff, R. M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Bloom, L.; Smith, L. B.; Woodward, A. L.; Akhtar, N.; Hollich, G. (eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 19–50.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Bloom, P.; Markson, L. (1998). "Capacities underlying word learning". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2 (2): 67–73. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01121-8. PMID 21227068.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Bryant, J. B. (2009). "Pragmatic development". In Bavin, E. L. (ed.). The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 339–357.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Chan, C.Y.; Tardif, T.; Chen, J.; Meng, X.; Zhu, Liqi; Meng, Xiangzhi (2011). "English- and Chinese-learning infants map novel labels to objects and actions differently". Developmental Psychology. 47 (5): 1459–1471. doi:10.1037/a0024049. PMID 21744954.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Clark, E. V. (1978). "Strategies for communicating". Child Development. 49 (4): 953–959. doi:10.2307/1128734. JSTOR 1128734.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 32–66. ISBN 978-0-521-48464-0.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Clark, E. V. (2009). "Lexical meaning". In Bavin, E. L. (ed.). The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 283–300.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Clark, E. V.; Andrew, D.-W., W. (2002). "Pragmatic directions about language use: Offers of words and relations". Language in Society. 31 (2): 181–212. doi:10.1017/s0047404501020152.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Clark, E. V.; Grossman, J. B. (1998). "Pragmatics directions and children's word learning". Journal of Child Language. 25 (1): 1–18. doi:10.1017/S0305000997003309. PMID 9604566.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Fagan, M. (June 2009). "Mean length of utterance before words and grammar: Longitudinal trends and developmental implications of infant vocalizations". Journal of Child Language. 36 (3): 495–527. doi:10.1017/S0305000908009070. PMID 18922207.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Ganger, J.; Brent, M. R. (2004). "Reexamining the vocabulary spurt". Developmental Psychology. 40 (4): 621–632. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.621. PMID 15238048.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Gathercole, S. E.; Willis, C. S.; Emslie, H.; Baddeley, A. D. (1992). "Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study". Developmental Psychology. 28 (5): 887–898. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.887.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R. M.; Hollich, G. (2000). "An emergentist coalition model for word learning: Mapping words to objects is a product of the interaction of multiple cues". In Golinkoff, R. M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Bloom, L.; Smith, L. B.; Woodward, A. L.; Akhtar, N.; Hollich, G. (eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 136–164.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Hoff, E. (2006). "Language Experience and Language Milestones During Early Childhood". In McCartney, K.; Phillips, D. (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Early Childhood Development. Blackwell. pp. 233–251. doi:10.1002/9780470757703.ch12.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Hulit, L. M.; Howard, M. R. (2002). Born to talk. Toronto: Allyn and Bacon.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Kamil, M. L.; Hiebert, E. H. (2005). "Teaching and learning vocabulary: Perspectives and persistent issues". In Hiebert, E. H.; Kamil, M. L. (eds.). Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 1–26.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Keren-Portnoy, T.; Majorano, M.; Vihman, M. M. (2009). "From phonetics to phonology: The emergence of first words in Italian". Journal of Child Language. 36 (2): 235–267. doi:10.1017/S0305000908008933. PMID 18789180.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Leclercq, A.; Majerus, S. (2010). "Serial-order short-term memory predicts vocabulary development: Evidence from a longitudinal study". Developmental Psychology. 46 (2): 417–427. doi:10.1037/a0018540. PMID 20210500.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Lorraine, S. (2008). Vocabulary development: Super duper handouts number 149. Greenville, SC: Super Duper Publications.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help)[1] - McKeown, M. G.; Curtis, M. E. (1987). The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0-89859-548-2.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Merriman, W. E.; Bowman, L. L.; MacWhinney, B. (1989). "The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 54 (3–4, Serial No. 220): 1–30. doi:10.2307/1166130. JSTOR 1166130.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Nagy, W. E.; Herman, P. A.; Anderson, R. C. (1985). "Learning words from context". Reading Research Quarterly. 22 (2): 233–253. doi:10.2307/747758. JSTOR 747758.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Newton, E.; Padak, N. D.; Rasinski, T. V. (2008). Evidence-based instruction in reading: A professional development guide to vocabulary. Boston, MA: Pearson Education.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Nippold, M. (2004). "Research on later language development:International perspectives". In Berman, R. A. (ed.). Language Development Across Childhood and Adolescence. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 1–8.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: Morrow and Co. pp. 262–296. ISBN 978-0-688-12141-9.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Sabbagh, M. A.; Baldwin, D. (2005). "Understanding the role of communicative intentions in word learning". In Elain, N.; Hoerl, C.; McCormack, T.; et al. (eds.). Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds: Issues in Philosophy and Psychology. New York: Clarendon/Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199245635.001.0001. ISBN 9780199245635.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Smith, L. B. (2000). "Learning how to learn words: An associative crane". In Golinkoff, R. M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Bloom, L.; Smith, L. B.; Woodward, A. L.; Akhtar, N.; Hollich, G. (eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 51–80.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Tabors, P. O.; Beals, D. E.; Weizman, Z. O. (2001). "'You know what oxygen is?': Learning new words at home". In Dickinson, D. K.; Tabor, P. O. (eds.). Beginning literacy with language. Baltimore, ML: Paul H. Brookes. pp. 93–110.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Tardif, T.; Fletcher, P.; Liang, W.; Kaciroti, N. (2009). "Early vocabulary development in Mandarin (Putonghua) and Cantonese". Journal of Child Language. 36 (5): 1115–1144. doi:10.1017/S0305000908009185. PMID 19435545.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Teinonen, T.; Aslin, R.; Alku, P.; Csibra, G. (2008). "Visual speech contributes to phonetic learning in 6-month-old infants". Cognition. 108 (3): 850–855. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.009. PMID 18590910.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Tincoff, Ruth; Jusczyk, Peter W. (1999). "Some Beginnings of Word Comprehension in 6-Month-Olds". Psychological Science. 10 (2): 172–175. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00127.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Tincoff, Ruth; Jusczyk, Peter W. (2012). "Six-Month-Olds Comprehend Words That Refer to Parts of the Body". Infancy. 17 (4): 432–444. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00084.x.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Tomasello, M. (2000). "The social-pragmatic theory of word learning". Pragmatics: Quarterly Publication of the International Pragmatics Association. 10 (4): 401–413.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Vihman, M. (1993). "Variable paths to early word production". Journal of Phonetics. 21: 61–82.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Waxman, S. R.; Booth, A. E. (2000). "Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts". Cognition. 77 (2): B33–B43. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00103-7. PMID 10986366.
{{cite journal}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help)