Jump to content

User:Heather-L/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is the current revision of this page, as edited by Citation bot (talk | contribs) at 23:08, 5 June 2022 (Alter: issue, year, url, journal, volume, pages, title, author2. URLs might have been anonymized. Add: newspaper, pages, volume, year, journal, jstor, doi-access, url, doi, issue, pmc, s2cid, pmid, authors 1-1. Removed proxy/dead URL that duplicated identifier. Removed access-date with no URL. Removed parameters. Formatted dashes. Some additions/deletions were parameter name changes. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by AManWithNoPlan | #UCB_Command...). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this version.

(diff) ← Previous revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)

This is a user sandbox.

Possible articles to update for Advanced Social Psychology class:

Implicit-association test - This article can use some expansion and updating in the sections about the debate over the test's validity and reliability.  To me, the article appears to present one side of the argument, without providing the counter-argument(s).  A recent article from APS also summarizes what has been found with the test over its 20 years of use, and will be useful in expanding the article.  Finally, Laurel, my partner, has researched this test before, giving her in-depth knowledge of it that will be useful.

Angry Black Woman - As my partner, Laurel, discussed on her page, this article lacks explanations of its associations to prejudice and discrimination, as well as empirical research. I agree that it is a valid stereotype upon which bias and prejudice is based, and neutral, academic improvements to the article could settle the debate in the talk pages about its validity as a wikipedia article.

List of cognitive biases#Social biases - This page presents a list of social biases - many that are sources of prejudice and discrimination - that are very simply described. One option is to expand these descriptions, to provide examples to better explain them, and/or to summarize the scientific evidence supporting each bias. All but one of the biases, sociability bias of language, has its own page on wikipedia. The other option is to create the page for this bias.

Heterosexism#As discrimination - The section in this article on the effects of heterosexism could be improved by adding a section on the social/psychological effects of this discrimination, including research that illustrates related psychological theory, research on its psychological and social ramifications, and on possible ways to reduce negative consequences.

We could create a page, that does not exist and is not referenced anywhere, as far as I can tell, for the topic of Internalized Devaluation/Stigma, which, based on a cursory reading, is a theory that encompasses the concepts of internalized homophobia/racism/sexism, etc. There is a SAGE pub with a chapter on the topic (Dovidio, J. (2010). The SAGE handbook of prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination. London: SAGE.) and there are several relevant wiki pages that it can be linked to.

Prejudice - This article may benefit from a review of the section entitled "Contemporary Theories and Empirical Findings" to ensure the contents are contemporary, a review of citations, and some research added throughout the section entitled "Controversies and Prominent Topics." Also, the "Religious Discrimination" section focuses on the question of whether religious people are prejudiced, rather than on prejudice against people based on their religion/non-religion, so that should be corrected.

Implicit-association test (possible edits)

[edit]

The implicit-association test (IAT) is a measure within social psychology designed to detect the strength of a person's automatic association between mental representations of objects (concepts) in memory. The IAT was introduced in the scientific literature in 1998 by Anthony Greenwald, Debbie McGhee, and Jordan Schwartz.[1] The IAT is now widely used in social psychology research and, to some extent, in clinical, cognitive, and developmental psychology research. The IAT is the subject of much controversy regarding precisely what it measures, and the lack of reproducibility of many of its results.

Notes: Is this definiton/description correct? Specifically, is the test designed to detect associations between objects? Doesn't it test associations between people and concepts? The sentence does have concepts in parentheses, but I'd say this description could be clarified. I think anyone who doesn't know anything about the IAT would probably be confused by this.

Are the last two statements current? There are no sources, so I can't guess how recent they are. IAT use could be ubiquitous throughout psychology at this point, or dismissed. Also, it's not a subject of "much controversy," only professional debate and discourse, which is common for psychometrics particularly, and psychology and science, in general.

History

[edit]

Implicit cognition and measurement (Note - this heading seems unnecessary, since there is only one paragraph under it)

[edit]

In 1995, social psychology researchers Anthony Greenwald and Mahzarin Banaji asserted that the idea of implicit and explicit memory can apply to social constructs as well.[2] If memories that are not accessible to awareness can influence our actions, associations can also influence our attitudes and behavior. Thus, measures that tap into individual differences in associations of concepts should be developed. This would allow researchers to understand attitudes that cannot be measured through explicit self-report methods due to lack of awareness or social-desirability bias.[3] The first IAT article was published three years later in 1998.[4]

Notes: It has been 20 years since the IAT has been introduced. Surely there is something to add to this section. I have one article that summarizes findings from the test, but need something about its context. Time for some research!

Application and use

[edit]

...The IAT may allow researchers to get around the difficult problem of social-desirability bias and for that reason it has been used extensively to assess people's attitudes towards commonly stigmatized groups.[5]

Notes: Are there any other applications to add?

Notes for the article overall: Should address the test's cross-cultural generalizability. Paragraphs/sections could benefit from some reorganization/re-titling. Writing is awkward in some places.

Theoretical interpretation

[edit]

...The interpretation that the IAT provides a "window" to unconscious mental contents has been challenged by Hahn and colleagues, whose results indicated that people are highly accurate in predicting their own IAT scores for a variety of social groups.[6]

Notes: This is more appropriate in the section covering criticism and controversy, and should be expanded.

Balanced-identity theory and design

[edit]

Notes: Maybe you, Laurel, can make more sense of this than I can. I can't tell if this is the latest evolution of Greenwald's theory behind the IAT, or if it explains why the IAT commonly replaces self-report tests that are subject to social-desirability bias and/or lack of self-awareness (as discussed in the Limitations paragraph). I need to learn more to decide what I think about this content. Either way, the thesis of the section is lost on me!

Limitations

[edit]

The IAT has been widely used as a measure for the balanced identity design because data obtained with this method revealed that predicted consistency patterns from Heider's theory were strongly apparent in the data for implicit measures by IAT but not in those for parallel explicit measures by self-report. The general explanation for why explicit measures by self-report did not reflect the predicted consistency patterns from Heider's theory was that self-report measures can go astray when respondents are either unwilling or unable to report accurately, and these problems could be more than enough to obscure the operation of consistency processes. There are, however, still limitations to the theory. For example, the balanced identity IAT measures only give group results rather than individual results, so it has its limitations when an analysis requires for individual pinpoint data to analyze, for instance, how balanced one's identity is relative to others'. It is hopeful, however, that researchers working with the Implicit Association Test (IAT) are trying hard to overcome challenges such as the one described above.[7]

Criticism and controversy

[edit]

Notes: I think this section should be renamed "Limitations," and opened with an explanation of how every method or measure in psychology has shortfalls and ideal/intended uses, and that criticism of a method or measure does not constitute rejection, nor indecision, nor controversy, but an evolving understanding of its appropriate use, developed through the scientific method. I see nothing controversial in this section at all.

Other than that, I think we can add some balance and updates to all the discussions regarding validity, but that will take quite a bit of research first.

  1. ^ Greenwald, Anthony G.; McGhee, Debbie E.; Schwartz, Jordan L.K. (1998), "Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (6): 1464–1480, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464, PMID 9654756
  2. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R. (1995). "Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 4–27. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4. PMID 7878162.
  3. ^ Nosek, B. A.; Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R. (2005). "Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method variables and construct validity". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 31 (2): 166–180. doi:10.1177/0146167204271418. PMID 15619590. S2CID 280593.
  4. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; McGhee, D. E.; Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). "Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (6): 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464. PMID 9654756.
  5. ^ Devine, P.G. (2001), "Implicit Prejudice and Stereotyping: How Automatic Are They?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (5): 757–759, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.757
  6. ^ Hahn, A.; Judd, C.M.; Hirsh, H.K.; Blair, I.V. (2014). "Awareness of implicit attitudes". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 143 (3): 1369–1392. doi:10.1037/a0035028. PMC 4038711. PMID 24294868.
  7. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R.; Rudman, L. A.; Farnham, S. D.; Nosek, B. A.; Mellott, D. S. (2002). "A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept". Psychological Review. 109 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.109.1.3. PMID 11863040.

New References

[edit]

[1] The Observer summary of 20 years of IAT

[2] Establishes IAT's use for measuring implicit bias related to disabiities

[3] Study supporting use of IAT for predicting suicide

[4] Study showing IAT use in predicting suicidal ideation

[5]Combined with the other two articles, proof of growing evidence for IAT use in predicting sucidal ideation and behavior - an important application

[6]Textbook with entire chapter on IAT, including discussion of theory, IAT scoring, reliability, (in-depth) validity, a possible explanation for differences in explicit versus implicit bias measurements, and effects of context on both implicit and explicit bias measures

Edited Article - Reviewer, Start Here

[edit]

The implicit-association test (IAT) is a measure within social psychology designed to detect the strength of a person's automatic association between mental representations of objects (concepts) in memory. The IAT was introduced in the scientific literature in 1998 by Anthony Greenwald, Debbie McGhee, and Jordan Schwartz.[7] The IAT is now widely used in social psychology research and, to some extent, in clinical, cognitive, and developmental psychology research. The IAT is the subject of much controversy regarding precisely what it measures, and the lack of reproducibility of many of its results.

History

[edit]

Implicit cognition and measurement

[edit]

In 1995, social psychology researchers Anthony Greenwald and Mahzarin Banaji asserted that the idea of implicit and explicit memory can apply to social constructs as well.[8] If memories that are not accessible to awareness can influence our actions, associations can also influence our attitudes and behavior. Thus, measures that tap into individual differences in associations of concepts should be developed. This would allow researchers to understand attitudes that cannot be measured through explicit self-report methods due to lack of awareness or social-desirability bias.[9] The first IAT article was published three years later in 1998.[10]

Since its original publish date, the seminal IAT article has been cited over 4,000 times[11], making it one of the most influential psychological developments over the past couple of decades.[6] Furthermore, several variations in IAT procedure have been introduced to address test limitations[6], while numerous applications of the IAT were also developed, including versions investigating bias against obesity, suicide risk, romantic attachment, attitudes regarding sexuality, and political preferences, among others.[12] Finally, as is characteristic of any psychological instrumentation, discussion and debate of the IAT's reliability and validity has continued since its introduction, particularly because these factors vary between different variations of the test.[6]

Application and use

[edit]

A computer-based measure, the IAT requires that users rapidly categorize two target concepts with an attribute (e.g. the concepts "male" and "female" with the attribute "logical"), such that easier pairings (faster responses) are interpreted as more strongly associated in memory than more difficult pairings (slower responses).[7]

The IAT is thought to measure implicit attitudes: "introspectively unidentified (or inaccurately identified) traces of past experience that mediate favorable or unfavorable feeling, thought, or action toward social objects."[13] In research, the IAT has been used to develop theories to understand implicit cognition (i.e. cognitive processes of which a person has no conscious awareness). These processes may include memory, perception, attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Because the IAT requires that users make a series of rapid judgments, researchers believe that IAT scores may also reflect attitudes which people are unwilling to reveal publicly.[7] The IAT may allow researchers to get around the difficult problem of social-desirability bias and for that reason it has been used extensively to assess people's attitudes towards commonly stigmatized groups.[14]

Procedure

[edit]

Task 1 (practice):

Black White

Aaliyah

Task 2 (practice):

Pleasant Unpleasant

Suffering

Press E to classify as Pleasant
or I to classify as Unpleasant

Tasks 3 and 4 (data collection):

Black/ White/
Pleasant Unpleasant
Happiness

Press E to classify as Black or Pleasant
or I to classify as White or Unpleasant

Task 5 (practice):

White Black

Eminem

Press E to classify as White
or I to classify as Black

Tasks 6 and 7 (data collection):

White/ Black/
Pleasant Unpleasant
Shanice

Press E to classify as White or Pleasant
or I to classify as Black or Unpleasant

Example of a typical IAT procedure

A typical IAT procedure involves a series of seven tasks.[15] In the first task, an individual is asked to categorize stimuli into two categories. For example, a person might be presented with a computer screen on which the word "Black" appears in the top left-hand corner and the word "White" appears in the top right-hand corner. In the middle of the screen a word, such as a first name, that is typically associated with either the categories of "Black" or "White". For each word that appears in the middle of the screen, the person is asked to sort the word into the appropriate category by pressing the appropriate left-hand or right-hand key. On the second task, the person would complete a similar sorting procedure with an attribute of some kind. For example, the word "Pleasant" might now appear in the top left-hand corner of the screen and the word "Unpleasant" in the top right-hand corner. In the middle of the screen would appear a word that is either pleasant or unpleasant. Once again, the person would be asked to sort each word as being either pleasant or unpleasant by pressing the appropriate key. On the third task, individuals are asked to complete a combined task that includes both the categories and attributes from the first two tasks. In this example, the words "Black/Pleasant" might appear in the top left-hand corner while the words "White/Unpleasant" would appear in the top right-hand corner. Individuals would then see a series of stimuli in the center of the screen consisting of either a name or word. They would be asked to press the left-hand key if the name or word belongs to the "Black/Pleasant" category or the right-hand key if it belongs to the "White/Unpleasant" category. The fourth task is a repeat of the third task but with more repetitions of the names, words, or images.

The fifth task is a repeat of the first task with the exception that the position of the two target words would be reversed. For example, "Black" would now appear in the top right-hand corner of the screen and "White" in the top left-hand corner. The sixth task would be a repeat of the third, except that the objects and subjects of study would be in opposite pairings from previous trials. In this case, "Black/Unpleasant" would now appear in the top right-hand corner and "White/Pleasant" would now appear in the top left-hand corner. The seventh task is a repeat of the sixth task but with more repetitions of the names, words, or images. If the categories under study (e.g. Black or White) are associated with the presented attributes (e.g. Pleasant/Unpleasant) to differing degrees, the pairing reflecting the stronger association (or the "compatible" pairing) should be easier for the participant.[7] In the Black/White-Pleasant/Unpleasant example, a participant will be able to categorize more quickly when White and Pleasant are paired together than when Black and Pleasant are paired if he or she has more positive associations with White people than with Black people (and vice versa if Black and Pleasant are categorized more quickly).

Variations of the IAT include the Go/No-go Association Test (GNAT),[16] the Brief-IAT[17] and the Single-Category IAT.[18] An idiographic approach using the IAT and the SC-IAT for measuring implicit anxiety showed that personalized stimulus selection did not affect the outcome, reliabilities and correlations to outside criteria.[19]

Types

[edit]

Valence

[edit]

Valence IATs measure associations between concepts and positive or negative valence. They are generally interpreted as a preference for one category over another. For example, the Race IAT shows that more than 70% of individuals have an implicit preference for Whites over Blacks.[20][21] On the other hand, only half of Black individuals prefer Blacks over Whites (cf. the earlier "doll experiment" developed by psychologists Kenneth and Mamie Clark during the early civil rights era). Similarly, the Age IAT generally shows that most individuals have an implicit preference for young over old, regardless of the age of the person taking the IAT. Some other valence IATs include the Weight IAT, the Sexuality IAT, the Arab-Muslim IAT, and the Skin-tone IAT.

Stereotype

[edit]

Stereotype IATs measure associations between concepts that often reflect the strength to which a person holds a particular societal stereotype. For example, the Gender-Science IAT reveals that most people associate women more strongly with liberal arts and men more strongly with science.[21] Similarly, the Gender-Career IAT indicates that most people associate women more strongly with family and men more strongly with careers. The Asian IAT shows that many people more strongly associate Asian Americans with foreign landmarks and European Americans more strongly with American landmarks. Some other stereotype IATs include the Weapons IAT and the Native IAT.

Self-esteem

[edit]

The self-esteem IAT measures implicit self-esteem by pairing "self" and "other" words with words of positive and negative valence.[22] Those who find it easier to pair "self" with positive words than negative words are purported to have higher implicit self-esteem. Generally, measures of implicit self-esteem, including the IAT, are not strongly related to one another and are not strongly related to explicit measures of self-esteem.[23]

Brief

[edit]

The Brief IAT (BIAT) uses a similar procedure to the standard IAT but requires fewer classifications.[17] It involves approximately four to six tasks rather than seven, only uses combined tasks (corresponding most closely to tasks 3, 4, 6, and 7 on the standard IAT), and has fewer repetitions. Additionally, it requires specification of a focal concept in each task as well as a single attribute, instead of two. For example, although, White, Black, Pleasant, and Unpleasant stimuli all appear, participants would press one key when White and Pleasant words appear and another key when "anything else" appears. Subsequently, participants would press one key when Black and Pleasant words appear and another key when "anything else" appears.

Child

[edit]

The Child IAT (Ch-IAT)[24] allows for children as young as four years of age to take the IAT. Rather than words and pictures, the Ch-IAT uses sound and pictures. For example, positive and negative valence are indicated with smiling and frowning faces. Positive and negative words to be classified are voiced out loud to children.

Studies using the Ch-IAT have revealed that six-year-old White children, ten-year-old White children, and White adults have comparable implicit attitudes on the Race IAT.[24]

Theoretical interpretation

[edit]

According to Greenwald, the IAT provides a "window" into a level of mental operation that operates in unthinking (unconscious, automatic, implicit, impulsive, intuitive, etc.) fashion because associations operating without active thought (automatically) can help performance in one of the IAT's two "combined" tasks, while interfering with the other. Respondents to the IAT experience a higher (conscious, controlled, explicit, reflective, analytic, rational, etc.) level of mental operation, when they try to overcome the effects of the automatic associations. The IAT succeeds as a measure because the higher level fails to completely overcome the lower level.[25]

The interpretation that the IAT provides a "window" to unconscious mental contents has been challenged by Hahn and colleagues, whose results indicated that people are highly accurate in predicting their own IAT scores for a variety of social groups.[26]

De Houwer theorizes that the IAT is a measure of a response compatibility effect, in which participants first learn to associate positive and negative words and concepts with pressing specific keys on the keyboard. Later in the test, when participants are instructed to sort words and concepts that are both negative and positive with the same keyboard key, De Houwer argues that much of the latency and incorrect responses that result from this change are due to the increased cognitive complexity of the task, and not necessarily a reflection of implicit bias.[6][27]

Brendl, Markman, and Messner have proposed a random walk model process to explain responses in the critical portions of the IAT. They theorize that test respondents base their responses on a process of mental evidence-gathering that continues until the evidence for one option or the other (right or left key) reaches a threshold, at which time a decision is made, and action is taken. This requires consideration of both the concept and the attribute, which can be congruent or incongruent - all factors that affect decision speed. All evidence during the compatible block of the test is congruent, allowing for fast decision-making. However, incongruent concept and attribute in the incompatible task leads to longer processing time. Increased task difficulty also increases evidence threshold criterion, further decreasing decision speed.[6]

An alternative or complementary theory from Mierke and Klauer holds that the cognitive control processes required to switch back and forth between categorizing based on concept versus based on attribute leads to reduced speeds in the critical blocks of the test. In other words, it is much less mentally demanding to sort concepts in the compatible block when only one aspect of the concept must be focused upon. Comparatively, sorting concepts in the incompatible block, which requires focusing on both concept and attribute not only takes longer to process because of the increased complexity, but also because the previous concept may have required a different cognitive effort.[6]

Fiinally, Rothermund and Wentura propose a figure-ground model of explanation for the IAT. In essence, this theory suggests that IAT respondents simplify their task by relying on salience. For example, negative is salient for most people, so if a respondent is to press the right key for negative words, the individual will plan to press the right key for all negative words (figure), and the left key for any other (non-negative) words (ground). This leads to fast decision-making in the compatible task and two of the critical tasks, but not for the third critical task, in which two salient categories require different keys pushed.[6]

There is empirical support for all these explanations of the IAT’s effects, but this is not necessarily evidence against the IAT’s overall validity, as these theories are not mutually exclusive.  Furthermore, regardless of the fundamental cognitive processes of the IAT, studies show that multiple implementations of the test validly measure their targeted constructs, and the psychometric value of each implementation varies as a function of its individual characteristics (e.g., construct measured, participant characteristics, testing environment).[6][28]

Balanced-identity theory and design

[edit]

Heider's balance theory

[edit]

In 1958, Fritz Heider proposed the balance theory, which stated that a system of liking and disliking relationships is balanced if the product of the valence of all relationships within the system is positive. In the theory, there are concepts and associations. Concepts are persons, groups, or attributes; and among attribute concepts, there are positive and negative valences. Associations are relations between pairs of concepts, and the strength of association is the potential for one concept to activate another, either by external stimuli or by excitation through their associations with other, already active, concepts. The theory followed the assumption of associative social knowledge: an important portion of social knowledge could be represented as a network of variable-strength associations among person concepts (including self and groups) and attributes (including valence).[29]

Balance–congruity principle

[edit]

When two unlinked or weakly linked nodes are linked to the same third node, the association between these two should strengthen. This is the principle of balance–congruity. The nodes in the principle of balance–congruity are equivalent to the concepts in Heider's balance theory, and the three involved nodes/concepts make up a system. Since every relationship within the system here is positively associated, this, according to a derivation of Heider's theory, also represents a balanced system where the product of the direction of all associations within the system is positive.[29]

Balanced-identity research design

[edit]

In 2002, Greenwald and his colleagues introduced the balanced-identity design as a method to test correlational predictions of Heider's balance theory. The balanced identity design incorporated Heider's theory, the balance–congruity principle, and the assumption of centrality of self. The assumption of centrality of self is that in an associative knowledge structure, the self's centrality can be represented by its being associated with many other concepts that are themselves highly connected in the structure. The concepts in a typical balanced identity design are the self, a social group/object, and either a valence attribute or nonvalence attribute. There are thus five important associations possible in a typical balanced identity design that connect these three categories of concepts. An attitude is the association of a social group/object with a valence attribute; a stereotype is the association of a social group with one or more nonvalence attribute(s); self-esteem is the association of the self with a valence attribute; a self-concept is the association of the self with one or more nonvalence attribute(s); and the last important association is between the self and a social group/object, which is called an identity. However, in a typical balanced identity design, only three of the five possible associations come into play, and they are usually either identity, self-concept, and stereotype or identity, self-esteem, and attitude. Researchers using a balanced identity design are the ones to determine the set of concepts they want to investigate, and each one of the associations within the system that the researchers created will then be tested and analyzed statistically with both implicit and explicit measures.[29]

Typical results of balanced-identity research design with implicit measures

[edit]

A typical result of a balanced identity design usually shows that a group's identity is balanced, at least with implicit measures. According to a derivation of Heider's balance theory, since there are three concepts in a typical balanced identity design, the identity is balanced either when all three relations are positive or when one positive and two negative relations are present in the triad system. The triad system of "me—male—being good at math," and its typical result acquired from the IAT can be used as an example here. For male subjects, the three associations within this triad are usually all positive. For female subjects, the "me—male" association is usually negative, the "male—being good at math" association is usually positive, and the "me—being good at math" association is usually negative. As it's shown, for both the male and female subjects, their group identities are balanced.[29]

Comparison to findings with explicit reports

[edit]

Self-reporting is also usually used in a balanced identity design. Although self-reports don’t necessarily reflect the predicted consistency patterns from Heider's theory, they are often used to compare with the results from the IAT. Any discrepancies between self-reports and IAT results on the same association in a balanced identity design can be an indication of an experience of conflict. The triad system of "me—male—being good at math" is a good example. For female subjects, whereas the IAT typically shows a stronger positive association of "male" and "being good at math," the explicit self-reporting usually shows a weaker positive association or even a weaker negative association of "male" and "being good at math." Also, whereas the IAT typically shows a stronger negative association of "me" and "being good at math" for the same female subjects, self-reporting usually shows a weaker negative or even a weaker positive association of "me" and "being good at math." In this case, the female group is believed to be experiencing a conflict. The theorized explanation for a group experiencing such a conflict is that it reflects an implicit measure of the individual's acceptance of the stereotype, along with the explicit effort to reject it.[29]

Limitations

[edit]

The IAT has been widely used as a measure for the balanced identity design because data obtained with this method revealed that predicted consistency patterns from Heider's theory were strongly apparent in the data for implicit measures by IAT but not in those for parallel explicit measures by self-report. The general explanation for why explicit measures by self-report did not reflect the predicted consistency patterns from Heider's theory was that self-report measures can go astray when respondents are either unwilling or unable to report accurately, and these problems could be more than enough to obscure the operation of consistency processes. There are, however, still limitations to the theory. For example, the balanced identity IAT measures only give group results rather than individual results, so it has its limitations when an analysis requires individual pinpoint data to analyze, for instance, how balanced one's identity is relative to others'.[29]

Criticism and controversy

[edit]

The IAT has engendered some controversy in both the scientific literature[30] and in the public sphere (e.g. in the Wall Street Journal).[31][32] For example, it has been interpreted as assessing familiarity,[33] perceptual salience asymmetries,[34] or mere cultural knowledge irrespective of personal endorsement of that knowledge.[35] A more recent critique argued that there is a lack of empirical research justifying the diagnostic statements that are given to the lay public.[36] For instance, feedback may report that someone has a [slight/moderate/strong] automatic preference for [European Americans/African Americans]. Proponents of the IAT have responded to these charges,[37] but the debate continues. According to The New York Times, "there isn't even that much consistency in the same person's scores if the test is taken again".[38] In addition, researchers have recently claimed that results of the IAT might be biased by the participant's lacking cognitive capability to adjust to switching categories, thus biasing results in favor of the first category pairing (e.g. pairing "Asian" with positive stimuli first, instead of pairing "Asian" with negative stimuli first).[39]

According to Jesse Singal, some of these issues have been settled in the research literature, but others continue to inspire debate among researchers and lay people alike.[31]

Validity research

[edit]

Since its introduction into the scientific literature in 1998, a great deal of research has been conducted in order to examine the psychometric properties of the IAT as well as to address other criticisms on validity and reliability.[37]

Construct validity

[edit]

The IAT is purported to measure relative strength of associations. However, some researchers have asserted that the IAT may instead be measuring constructs such as salience of attributes[34] or cultural knowledge.[35]

Predictive validity
[edit]

A 2009 meta-analysis[28] has concluded that the IAT has predictive validity independent of the predictive validity of explicit measures. However, a follow-up meta-analysis[40] questioned some of these results, finding that implicit measures were only weakly predictive of behaviors and no better than explicit measures. Some research has found that the IAT tends to be a better predictor of behavior in socially sensitive contexts (e.g. discrimination and suicidal behaviour)[41] than traditional "explicit" self-report methods,[42] whereas explicit measures tend to be better predictors of behavior in less socially sensitive contexts (e.g. political preferences). Specifically, the IAT has been shown to predict voting behavior (e.g. ultimate candidate choice of undecided voters),[43] mental health (e.g. a self-injury IAT differentiated between adolescents who injured themselves and those who did not),[44] medical outcomes (e.g. medical recommendations by physicians),[45] employment outcomes (e.g. interviewing Muslim-Arab versus Swedish job applicants),[46] education outcomes (e.g. gender-science stereotypes predict gender disparities in nations' science and math test scores),[47] and environmentalism (e.g., membership of a pro-environmental organisation)[48].

In applied settings, the IAT has been used in marketing and industrial psychology. For example, in determining the predictors of risk-taking behaviour of pilots in general aviation, attitudes towards risky flight behaviour as measured through an IAT have shown to be a more accurate forecast of risky flight behaviour than traditional explicit attitude or personality scales.[49] The IAT has also been used in clinical psychology research to test the hypothesis that implicit associations may be a causal factor in the development of anxiety disorders.[50]

Salience asymmetry
[edit]

Researchers have argued that the IAT may measure salience of concepts rather than associations. Whereas IAT proponents claim that faster response times when pairing concepts indicate stronger associations, critics claim that faster response times indicate that concepts are similar in salience (and slower response times indicate that concepts differ in salience).[34] There is some support for this claim. For example, in an old-young IAT, old faces would be more salient than young faces. As a result, researchers created an old-young IAT that involved pairing young and old faces with neutral words (non-salient attribute) and non-words (salient attribute). Response times were faster when old faces (salient) were paired with non-words (salient) than when old faces (salient) were paired with neutral words (non-salient), supporting the assertion that faster response time can be facilitated by matching salience.

Although proponents of the IAT acknowledge that it may be influenced by salience asymmetry, they argue that this does not preclude interpreting the IAT as a measure of associations.[51]

Culture versus person
[edit]

Another criticism of the IAT is that it may measure associations that are picked up from cultural knowledge rather than associations actually residing within a person.[35] The counter-argument is that such associations may indeed arise from the culture, but they can nonetheless influence behavior.[52]

To address the possibility that the IAT picks up on cultural knowledge rather than beliefs that are present in a person, some critics of the standard IAT created the personalized IAT.[53] The primary difference between a standard valence IAT and the personalized IAT is that rather than using pleasant and unpleasant words as category labels, it uses "I like" and "I don't like" as category labels. Additionally, the Personalized IAT does not provide error feedback for an incorrect response as in the standard IAT. This form of the IAT is more strongly related to explicit self-report measures of bias.

Proponents of the standard IAT argue that the Personalized IAT increases the likelihood that those taking it will evaluate the concept rather than classify it.[54] This would increase its relationship with explicit measures without necessarily removing the effect of cultural knowledge. In fact, some researchers have examined the relationship between perceptions of general American attitudes and Personalized IAT scores and have concluded that the relationship between the IAT and cultural knowledge is not decreased by personalizing it. However, it is important to note that there was no relationship between cultural knowledge and standard IAT scores either.[citation needed]

Internal validity

[edit]
Ability to fake result
[edit]

The IAT has also demonstrated a reasonable amount of resistance to social-desirability bias. Individuals asked to fake their responses on the IAT have demonstrated difficulty in doing so in some studies. For example, participants who were asked to present a positive impression of themselves were able to do so on a self-report measure of anxiety but not an IAT measuring anxiety.[55] Nonetheless, faking is possible,[56] and recent research indicates that the most effective method of faking the IAT is to intentionally slow down responses for pairings that should be relatively easy. Most subjects, however, do not discover this strategy on their own, so faking is relatively rare. An algorithm developed to estimate IAT faking can identify those who are faking with approximately 75% accuracy.[57]

There is a recent study showing that participants can even speed up their responses during the relatively difficult response pairings in an autobiographical implicit association test that aims to test the veracity of autobiographical statement. Specifically, participants who were instructed to speed up their responses to fake the test were able to do so. The effect was larger when participants were trained in speeding up. Most importantly, guilty participants who speed up their responses during the difficult response pairing successfully beat the test to obtain an innocent results. In other words, participants can reverse their test outcome without being detected. Clearly, this poses new challenges to the IATs.[58]

Susceptibility to conscious control
[edit]

Distinct from faking (the deliberate obscuring of a true association), some studies have shown that heightening awareness about the nature of the test can change the outcome, potentially by activating different fluencies and associations. For example, in one study, a simple reminder from the experimenter ("Please be careful not to stereotype on the next section of the task") was sufficient to significantly reduce the expression of biased associations on a race IAT.[59] Notably, there was not a significant decrease in overall reaction time in this experiment, indicating that this "control" may also be implicit.

Familiarity
[edit]

A common criticism of the IAT is that it may be difficult to associate positive attributes with less familiar concepts.[33] For example, if a person has had less contact with members of a particular ethnic group, he or she may have a more difficult time associating members of that ethnic group with positive words simply because of this lack of familiarity. There is some evidence against the familiarity based on studies that have ensured equal familiarity with the African American and White names as well as the faces appearing on the Race IAT.[60]

Order
[edit]

As the IAT relies on a comparison of response times in different tasks pairing concepts and attributes, researchers and others taking the IAT have speculated that the pairing on the first combined task may affect performance on the next combined task. For example, a participant who begins a gender stereotype IAT by pairing female names with family words may subsequently find the task of pairing female names with career words more difficult. Research has indeed shown a small effect of order. As a result, it is recommended to increase the number of classifications required in the fifth IAT task.[9] This gives participants more practice before doing the second pairing, thus reducing the order effect. When studying groups of people, this effect could be countered by giving pairings first to different participants (e.g. half of participants pair female names and family words first, the other half pair female names with career words first).

Cognitive fluency and age
[edit]

The IAT is influenced by individual differences in average IAT response times such that those with slower overall response times tend to have more extreme IAT scores.[61] Older subjects also tend to have more extreme IAT scores, and this may be related to cognitive fluency, or slower overall response times.

An improved scoring algorithm for the IAT, which reduces the effect of cognitive fluency on the IAT, has been introduced.[62] A summary of the scoring algorithm can be found on Greenwald's webpage.[63]

Experience
[edit]

Repeated administrations of the IAT tend to decrease the magnitude of the effect for a particular person. This issue is somewhat ameliorated with the improved scoring algorithm.[62] An additional safeguard to control for IAT experience is to include a different type of IAT as a comparison. This allows researchers to evaluate the degree of magnitude decrease when administering subsequent IATs.

Reliability
[edit]

The IAT demonstrates inconsistent internal consistency and its test-retest reliability stands at 0.60, a relatively weak level.[9] However, IAT scores do seem to vary between multiple administrations, indicating that it may measure a combination of trait (stable characteristics of people) and state (subject to variation based on situation-specific circumstances) characteristics. One example of the latter case is that scores on the Race IAT are known to be less biased against African Americans when those taking it imagine positive Black exemplars beforehand (e.g. Martin Luther King).[64] Similarly, the Race IAT scores for an individual may indicate bias, but that bias is diminished on another IAT administered after associating with a mixed-race group.[65] In fact, Race IAT scores can be changed even more easily; administering the IAT in different languages yields significantly different scores for bilingual individuals. For example, studies conducted with Moroccan participants fluent in both French and Arabic showed that participants are biased when completing an IAT in their native language; however, that bias is diminished when completing an IAT in another language.[66] Similar results were found in the United States when administering an English and Spanish IAT on bilingual Hispanic Americans.[66] Another state characteristic that may well influence IAT scores is the time of day a person completes the task, with findings that holding a preference for one's own racial group is lowest in the morning, but increases over the course of the day and into the evening;[67] however, this may be more to do with who completes the task at each time of day than a function of circadian rhythms.[68]

[edit]

After establishing the IAT in the scientific literature, Greenwald, along with Mahzarin Banaji (Professor of Psychology at Harvard University) and Brian Nosek (Associate Professor of Psychology at the University of Virginia), co-founded Project Implicit,[69] a virtual laboratory and educational outreach organization that facilitates research on implicit cognition.

The IAT has been profiled in major media outlets (e.g. in the Washington Post)[70] and in the popular book Blink, where it was suggested that one could score better on the implicit racism test by visualizing respected black leaders such as Nelson Mandela. The IAT was also discussed in a 2006 episode of The Oprah Winfrey Show.[71]

In the episode "Racist Dawg" on King of the Hill, Hank and Peggy take an IAT, colloquially referred to as the "racist test" to see if they prefer the company of white or black people.

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Sleek, Scott (2018). "The bias beneath: Two decades of measuring implicit associations". The Observer, A Publication of the Association of Psychological Science. 31 (2): 11–14 – via https://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/the-bias-beneath-two-decades-of-measuring-implicit-associations. {{cite journal}}: External link in |via= (help)
  2. ^ Wilson, M.; Scior, K. (2013). "Attitudes towards individuals with disabilities as measured by the Implicit Association Test: A literature review". Research in Developmental Disabilities. 35 (2): 294–321. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.11.003. PMID 24316588.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  3. ^ Barnes, S. M.; et al. (2016). "Moving Beyond Self-Report: Implicit Associations about Death/Life Prospectively Predict Suicidal Behavior among Veterans" (PDF). Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior: 1–11 – via Harvard Nock Lab. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |last= (help)
  4. ^ Ellis, T. E.; Rufino, K. A.; Green, K. L. (2015). "Implicit Measure of Life/Death Orientation Predicts Response of Suicidal Ideation to Treatment in Psychiatric Inpatients". Archives of Suicide Research. 20 (1): 59–68. doi:10.1080/13811118.2015.1004483. PMID 25923054. S2CID 24148516 – via Taylor & Francis.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  5. ^ Nock, M. K.; Park, J. M.; Finn, C. T.; Deliberto, T. L.; Dour, H. J.; Banaji; M. R. (2010). "Measuring the Suicidal Mind: Implicit Cognition Predicts Suicidal Behavior". Psychological Science. 21 (4): 511–517. doi:10.1177/0956797610364762. PMC 5258199. PMID 20424092.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  6. ^ a b c d e f g h i Perugini, M.; Costantini, G.; Richetin, J.; & Zogmaister, C. (2015). Behavior-based Assessment in Psychology. Hogrefe Publishing. pp. not numbered - Chapter 2. ISBN 9781613344378.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ a b c d Greenwald, Anthony G.; McGhee, Debbie E.; Schwartz, Jordan L.K. (1998), "Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (6): 1464–1480, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464, PMID 9654756
  8. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R. (1995). "Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 4–27. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4. PMID 7878162.
  9. ^ a b c Nosek, B. A.; Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R. (2005). "Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method variables and construct validity". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 31 (2): 166–180. doi:10.1177/0146167204271418. PMID 15619590. S2CID 280593.
  10. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; McGhee, D. E.; Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). "Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (6): 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464. PMID 9654756.
  11. ^ "Web of Science Journal Citation Reports". Web of Science. 2018. Retrieved 2018. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |access-date= (help)
  12. ^ Sleek, Scott (2018). "The Bias Beneath: Two Decades of Measuring Implicit Associations". Association for Psychological Science-Observer. Retrieved 2018. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |access-date= (help)
  13. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R. (1995). "Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 8. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4. PMID 7878162.
  14. ^ Devine, P.G. (2001), "Implicit Prejudice and Stereotyping: How Automatic Are They?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (5): 757–759, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.757
  15. ^ Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The Implicit Association Test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review (pp. 265–292). In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior. Psychology Press.
  16. ^ Nosek, Brian A.; Banaji, Mahzarin R. (2001). "The Go/No-Go Association Task" (PDF). Social Cognition. 19 (6): 625–664. doi:10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886.
  17. ^ a b Sriram, N.; Greenwald, A. G. (2009). "The Brief Implicit Association Test". Experimental Psychology. 56 (4): 283–294. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.283. PMID 19439401.
  18. ^ Karpinski, Andrew; Steinman, Ross B. (2006). "The single category Implicit Association Test as a measure of implicit social cognition". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 91 (1): 16–32. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.16. PMID 16834477.
  19. ^ Stieger, Stefan; Göritz, Anja S.; Burger, Christoph (2010). "Personalizing the IAT and the SC-IAT: Impact of idiographic stimulus selection in the measurement of implicit anxiety". Personality and Individual Differences. 48 (8): 940–944. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.027.
  20. ^ Sabin, J. A.; Nosek, B. A.; Greenwald, A. G.; Rivara, F. P. (2009). "Physicians' Implicit and Explicit Attitudes About Race by MD Race, Ethnicity, and Gender". Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved. 20 (3): 896–913. doi:10.1353/hpu.0.0185. PMC 3320738. PMID 19648715.
  21. ^ a b Nosek, B. A.; Banaji, M. R.; Greenwald, A. G. (2002). "Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration website". Group Dynamics. 6 (1): 101–115. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101.
  22. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; Farnham, S. D. (2000). "Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 79 (6): 1022–1038. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022. PMID 11138752.
  23. ^ Bosson, J. K.; Swann, W. B. Jr.; Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). "Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited?". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 79 (4): 631–643. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.631. PMID 11045743.
  24. ^ a b Baron, A. S.; Banaji, M. R. (2006). "The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6, 10 & adulthood". Psychological Science. 17 (1): 53–58. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x. PMID 16371144. S2CID 15019361.
  25. ^ Televised Lecture "The Psychology of Blink: Understanding How Our Minds Work Unconsciously - Part 1 of 2" (recorded March 5, 2008)
  26. ^ Hahn, A.; Judd, C.M.; Hirsh, H.K.; Blair, I.V. (2014). "Awareness of implicit attitudes". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 143 (3): 1369–1392. doi:10.1037/a0035028. PMC 4038711. PMID 24294868.
  27. ^ De Houwer, Jan (2001). "A Structural and Process Analysis of the Implicit Association Test". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 37 (6): 443–451. doi:10.1006/jesp.2000.1464 – via Elsevier Science Direct.
  28. ^ a b Greenwald, A. G.; Poehlman, T. A.; Uhlmann, E.; Banaji, M. R. (2009). "Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 97 (1): 17–41. doi:10.1037/a0015575. PMID 19586237.
  29. ^ a b c d e f Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R.; Rudman, L. A.; Farnham, S. D.; Nosek, B. A.; Mellott, D. S. (2002). "A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept". Psychological Review. 109 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.109.1.3. PMID 11863040.
  30. ^ Azar, B (2008). "IAT: Fad or fabulous?". Monitor on Psychology. 39: 44.
  31. ^ a b Singal, Jesse. "Psychology's Favorite Tool for Measuring Racism Isn't Up to the Job". Science of Us. Retrieved 2017-01-13.
  32. ^ Wax, Amy; Tetlock, Philip (December 1, 2005). "We're all racists at heart". Wall Street Journal. Retrieved 2011-06-09.
  33. ^ a b Ottaway, S. A.; Hayden, D. C.; Oakes, M. A. (2001). "Implicit attitudes and racism: Effects of word familiarity and frequency on the implicit association test". Social Cognition. 19 (2): 97–144. doi:10.1521/soco.19.2.97.20706.
  34. ^ a b c Rothermund, K.; Wentura, D. (2004). "Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test(IAT): Dissociating salience from associations". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 133 (2): 139–165. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.139. PMID 15149248.
  35. ^ a b c Arkes, H. R.; Tetlock, P. E. (2004). ""Attributions of Implicit Prejudice, or "Would Jesse Jackson 'Fail' the Implicit Association Test?". Psychological Inquiry. 15: 257–278. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1504_01. S2CID 9834343.
  36. ^ Blanton, Hart; Jaccard, James (January 2006). "Arbitrary Metrics in Psychology" (PDF). American Psychologist. 61 (1). American Psychological Association: 27–41. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.1.27. PMID 16435974.
  37. ^ a b Greenwald, Anthony. "Dr. Anthony Greenwald / IAT Materials". Retrieved 2008-10-11.
  38. ^ Tierney, John (November 17, 2008). "In Bias Test, Shades of Gray". The New York Times. Retrieved 2009-01-09.
  39. ^ Messner, Claude; Vosgerau, Joachim (2010). "Cognitive Inertia and the Implicit Association Test" (PDF). Journal of Marketing Research. 47 (2). American Marketing Association: 374–386. doi:10.1509/jmkr.47.2.374. S2CID 144453510.
  40. ^ Oswald, F.L.; Mitchell, G.; Blanton, H.; Jaccard, J.; Tetlock, P. E. (2013). "Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: A meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 105 (2): 171–192. doi:10.1037/a0032734. PMID 23773046.
  41. ^ Matthew K. Nock, Jennifer M. Park, Christine T. Finn, Tara L. Deliberto, Halina J. Dour, & Mahzarin R. Banaji. "Measuring the Suicidal Mind: Implicit Cognition Predicts Suicidal Behavior".{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  42. ^ Poehlman, T. Andrew; Uhlmann, Eric Luis; Greenwald, Anthony G.; Banaji, Mahzarin. "Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of Predictive Validity" (PDF). Retrieved 2008-10-11. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  43. ^ Arcuri, L.; Castelli, L.; Galdi, S.; Zogmaister, C.; Amadori, A. (2008). "Predicting the vote: Implicit attitudes as predictors of the future behavior of the decided and undecided voters". Political Psychology. 29 (3): 369–387. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00635.x.
  44. ^ Nock, M. K.; Banaji, M. R. (2007a). "Assessments of self-injurious thoughts using a behavioral test". American Journal of Psychiatry. 164 (5): 820–823. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.164.5.820. PMID 17475742.
  45. ^ Green, A. R.; Carney, D. R.; Pallin, D. J.; Ngo, L. H.; Raymond, K. L.; Iezzoni, L. I.; Banaji, M. R. (2007). "The presence of implicit bias in physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for Black and White patients". Journal of General Internal Medicine. 22 (9): 1231–1238. doi:10.1007/s11606-007-0258-5. PMC 2219763. PMID 17594129.
  46. ^ Rooth, D-O (2010). "Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: Real world evidence". Labour Economics. 17 (3): 523–534. doi:10.1016/j.labeco.2009.04.005.
  47. ^ Nosek, B. A.; Smyth, F. L.; Sriram, N.; Lindner, N. M.; Devos, T.; Ayala, A.; Bar-Anan, Y.; Bergh, R.; Cai, H.; Gonsalkorale, K.; Kesebir, S.; Maliszewski, N.; Neto, F.; Olli, E.; Park, J.; Schnabel, K.; Shiomura, K.; Tulbure, B.; Wiers, R. W.; Somogyi, M.; Akrami, N.; Ekehammar, B.; Vianello, M.; Banaji, M. R.; Greenwald, A. G. (2009). "National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (26): 10593–10597. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809921106. PMC 2705538. PMID 19549876.
  48. ^ Thomas, Gregory (June 2016). "The Development and Validation of an Implicit Measure Based on Biospheric Values". Environment & Behavior. 48 (5): 659–685. doi:10.1177/0013916514553836. S2CID 2485432 – via Sage.
  49. ^ Molesworth, Brett; Chang, Betty (2009). "Predicting pilots' risk-taking behaviour through an Implicit Association Test". Human Factors. 51 (6). Sage: 846–857. doi:10.1177/0018720809357756. PMID 20415159. S2CID 206409604.
  50. ^ Teachman, B. A.; Woody, S. (2004). "Staying tuned to research in implicit cognition: Relevance for clinical practice with anxiety disorders". Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 11 (2): 149–159. doi:10.1016/s1077-7229(04)80026-9.
  51. ^ Greenwald, A. G.; Nosek, B. A.; Banaji, M. R.; Klauer, K. C. (2005). "Validity of the salience asymmetry interpretation of the IAT: Comment on Rothermund and Wentura (2004)". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 134 (3): 420–425. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.420. PMID 16131272.
  52. ^ Banaji, M. R.; Nosek, B. A.; Greenwald, A. G. (2004). "No place for nostalgia in science: A response to Arkes and Tetlock". Psychological Inquiry. 15: 279–289. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1504_02. S2CID 219728859.
  53. ^ Olson, M. A.; Fazio, R. H. (2004). "Reducing the influence of extra-personal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (5): 653–667. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.653. PMID 15161392.
  54. ^ Nosek, B. A.; Hansen, J. J. (2008). "Personalizing the Implicit Association Test increases explicit evaluation of the target concepts". European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 25 (4): 226–236. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.226.
  55. ^ Egloff, B.; Schmukle, S.C. (2002), "Predictive Validity of an Implicit Association Test for Assessing Anxiety", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (6): 1441–1455, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1441, PMID 12500823
  56. ^ Kim, D. Y. (2003). "Voluntary controllability of the Implicit Association Test (IAT)". Social Psychology Quarterly. 66 (1): 83–96. doi:10.2307/3090143. JSTOR 3090143.
  57. ^ http://ilabs.washington.edu/sites/default/files/Cvencek%26al.BASP_.2010.pdf
  58. ^ Hu, X.; Rosenfeld, J. P. Bodenhausen (2012). "Combating automatic autobiographical associations: The effect of instruction and training in strategically concealing information in the autobiographical implicit association test". Psychological Science. 23 (10): 1079–1085. doi:10.1177/0956797612443834. PMID 22894937. S2CID 26283148.
  59. ^ Wallaert, Matthew; Ward, Andrew; Mann, Traci (2010). "Explicit control of implicit responses: Simple directives can alter IAT performance". Social Psychology. 41 (3): 152–157. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000022. PMC 3137766. PMID 21769299.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  60. ^ Dasgupta, N.; McGhee, D. E.; Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M. R. (2000). "Automatic preference for White Americans: Eliminating the familiaritexplanation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 36 (3): 316–328. doi:10.1006/jesp.1999.1418.
  61. ^ McFarland, S. G.; Crouch, Z. (2002). "A cognitive skill confound on the Implicit Association Test". Social Cognition. 20 (6): 483–510. doi:10.1521/soco.20.6.483.22977.
  62. ^ a b Greenwald, A. G; Nosek, B. A.; Banaji, M. R. (2003). "Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 85 (2): 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197. PMID 12916565.
  63. ^ Faculty.washington.edu
  64. ^ Dasgupta, N.; Greenwald, A. G. (2001). "On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (5): 800–814. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.800. PMID 11708558.
  65. ^ Van Bavel, J. J.; Cunningham, W. A. (2009). "Self-categorization with a novel mixed-race group moderates automatic social and racial biases". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 35 (3): 321–335. doi:10.1177/0146167208327743. PMID 19098257. S2CID 15986029.
  66. ^ a b Ogunnaike, O.; Dunham, Y.; Banaji, M. R. (2010). "The language of implicit preferences". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 46 (6): 999–1003. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.006.
  67. ^ Zadra, Jonathan; Proffitt, Dennis (2014). "Implicit Associations Have a Circadian Rhythm". PLOS ONE. 9 (11): e110149. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0110149. PMC 4217730. PMID 25365254.
  68. ^ Schofield, Timothy (2016). "Time-of-day effects in implicit racial in-group preferences are likely selection effects, not circadian rhythms". PeerJ. 4: e1947. doi:10.7717/peerj.1947. PMC 4841218. PMID 27114886.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  69. ^ Projectimplicit.net
  70. ^ Vedantam, Shankar (2005-01-23). "See No Bias". Washington Post. Retrieved 2008-10-10.
  71. ^ http://www.oprah.com/oprahshow/Overcoming-Prejudice/13
[edit]