Jump to content

Grammatical relation

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Ken H (talk | contribs) at 08:59, 5 August 2005. The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

This page is in development.

If grammar is seen as studying the relations and architectures of language,
such relations and structures would then have supports and anchors, forces and motions,
property equivalencies, and roles galore just as the character of a building does.
If one treats grammar similarly to the descriptive disciplines of mathematics,
logic and programming, then a possibly uncountable number of functions are present
since language recklessly allows seemingly anything.

Certainly then we can see many many routine grammatical functions ubiquitous in daily speech.

If a utilitarian POV is held, then everything in our buildings (our language) is there for a reason.
Our inventions and edifices all conect with various kinds of performance. Even if that performance is static,
as weight or shape or identity bearing, even passive objects are busy receiving or load-sharing.


Necessarily every word (each a part-of-speech) has both a function and probably functions associated with it.
Some languages are very free in the positions that words/part-of-speech can occupy within grammatical or 'correct' sentences.
Their syntax may be loose but through one method or another, relationships are nonetheless reliably fixed
such that users mostly agree on what a particular sentence is about.

Languages that are less free are said to have a more fixed syntax, but again, one way or another, grammatically correct sentences
express a fairly clear meaning as a result of their word ordering or syntactic functions. Among the tasks linguists pursue is the discovery of these functions within general theories of language, or for the understanding of a particular language.

A language such as English has a fair number of constraints. For example:

  • generally a head that is a single word follows its modifier(s)
  • prefixes modify the following head; affixes modify the preceding head; infixes modify the surrounding head
  • a preposition usually precedes its object and relates to its case function
  • subjects (when present) usually precede their predicates (when present).
  • adverbs generally precede verbs or adjectives; adjectives generally precede nouns; etc.

The above shows a general left→right pattern, which, not co-incidentally, is also how English is read.
This also fits with the general English sentence pattern of subject→verb→object.
This precedence relation also allies with an over-arching functional dominance, that can be expressed both informally
(descriptively or philosophically) or formally (logically, mathematically or algorithmically).

Some informal definitions

Some formal definitions

References

Academic references

  • Minsky, Rizzi, Chomsky LGB, PSGs, Construction Grammar, etc.