Jump to content

Digit ratio

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 24.7.59.158 (talk) at 06:00, 20 September 2012. The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Template:Distinguish2

Hand with index finger being shorter than the ring finger, resulting in a small 2D:4D ratio, pointing to a high exposure to testosterone in the uterus.

The digit ratio is the ratio of the lengths of different digits or fingers typically measured from the bottom crease where the finger joins the hand to the tip of the finger.[citation needed] It has been suggested by some scientists that the ratio of two digits in particular, the 2nd (index finger) and 4th (ring finger), is affected by exposure to androgens e.g. testosterone while in the uterus and that this 2D:4D ratio can be considered a crude measure for prenatal androgen exposure, with lower 2D:4D ratios pointing to higher androgen exposure. The 2D:4D ratio is calculated by dividing the length of the index finger of the right hand by the length of the ring finger. A longer index finger will result in a ratio higher than 1, while a longer ring finger will result in a ratio of less than 1.

The 2D:4D digit ratio is sexually dimorphic: while the second digit is typically shorter in both females and males, the difference between the lengths of the two digits is greater in males than females.

A number of studies have shown a correlation between the 2D:4D digit ratio and various physical and behavioral traits.

History of digit ratio research

That a greater proportion of men have shorter index fingers than ring fingers than do women was noted in the scientific literature several times through the late 1800s,[1][2] with the statistically significant sex difference in a sample of 201 men and 109 women established by 1930,[3] after which time the sex difference appears to have been largely forgotten or ignored. In 1983 Dr Glenn Wilson of King's College, London published a study examining the correlation between assertiveness in women and their digit ratio.[4] This was the first study to examine the correlation between digit ratio and a psychological trait within members of the same sex.[5] Wilson proposed that skeletal structure and personality were simultaneously affected by sex hormone levels in utero.[4] In 1998, John T. Manning and colleagues reported the sex difference in digit ratios was present in two-year-old children[6] and further developed the idea that the index was a marker of prenatal sex hormones. Since then research on the topic has burgeoned around the world.

A 2009 study in Biology Letters argues: "Sexual differences in 2D:4D are mainly caused by the shift along the common allometric line with non-zero intercept, which means 2D:4D necessarily decreases with increasing finger length, and the fact that men have longer fingers than women,"[7] which may be the basis for the sex difference in digit ratios and/or any putative hormonal influence on the ratios.

A 2011 paper by Zhengui Zheng and Martin J. Cohn reports that "the 2D:4D ratio in mice is controlled by the balance of androgen to estrogen signaling during a narrow window of digit development."[8] The formation of the digits, in utero, is thought to occur at 14 weeks, and the bone-to-bone ratio is consistent from this point into an individual’s adulthood.[9] During this period if the fetus is exposed to androgens, the exact level of which is thought to be sexually dimorphic, the growth rate of the 4th digit is increased, as can be seen by analyzing the 2D:4D ratio of opposite sex dizygotic twins, where the female twin is exposed to excess androgens from her brother in utero, and thus has a significantly lower 2D:4D ratio.[10]

Importantly, there has been no correlation between the sex hormone levels of an adult and the individual’s 2D:4D,[11] which implies that it is strictly the exposure in utero that causes this phenomenon.

A major problem with the research on this topic comes from the contradiction in the literature as to whether the testosterone level in adults can be predicted by the 2D:4D ratio,[12] but male sexual traits that are stereotypically attributed to testosterone levels have been found in correlation with the 2D:4D. So there should either be a correlation with one or the other but not both.

Digit ratio distribution

From a study of 136 males and 137 females:[13]

  • Males: mean 0.947, standard deviation 0.029.
  • Females: mean 0.965, standard deviation 0.026.

Assuming a normal distribution, the 10th and 90th percentiles for males are 0.910 and 0.984. For females: 0.932 and 0.998.

Evidence of androgen effect on digit ratio

  • Women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), which results in elevated androgen levels before birth, have lower, more masculinized 2D:4D on average.[14][15][16] Other possible physiological effects include an enlarged clitoris and shallow vagina.[17]
  • Males with CAH have more masculine (smaller) digit ratios than control males,[14][15] which also suggests that prenatal androgens affect digit ratios, since amniocentesis samples show that prenatal levels of testosterone are in the high normal range in males with CAH, while levels of the weaker androgen androstenedione are several fold higher than in control males.[18][19][20] These measures indicate that males with CAH are exposed to greater prenatal concentrations of total androgens than are control males.
  • Digit ratio in men correlates with genetic variation in the androgen receptor gene.[21] Men with genes that produce androgen receptors that are more sensitive to testosterone have lower, more masculine, digit ratios.
  • XY individuals with androgen insensitivity syndrome (AIS) due to a dysfunctional gene for the androgen receptor present as women and have feminine digit ratios on average, as would be predicted if androgenic hormones affect digit ratios. This finding also demonstrates that the sex difference in digit ratios is unrelated to the Y chromosome per se.[22]
  • The sex difference in 2D:4D is present before birth in humans,[23][24] which rules out any social influences that might affect digit growth differentially in the two sexes. Because all somatic sex differences in mammals to date have been found to be due to either androgenic masculinization or effects of the sex chromosomes, and as the AIS finding rules out a role for sex chromosomes in the sex difference in digit ratios, the prenatal sexual dimorphism also indicates that androgens act before birth to affect digit ratios.
  • The ratio of testosterone to estradiol measured in 33 amniocentesis samples correlates with the child's subsequent 2D:4D ratio.[25]
  • In pheasants, the ratio of the 2nd to 4th digit of the foot has been shown to be influenced by manipulations of testosterone in the egg.[26]

There is evidence that this reflects fetal exposure to the hormones testosterone[27] and estrogen.[citation needed]

Several studies present evidence that digit ratios are heritable.[28][29]

The level of estrogen in the amniotic fluid is not correlated with higher 2D:4D, and when examined researchers found no difference in estrogen levels between males and females.[30]

Explanation of the digit ratio effect

It is not clear why digit ratio ought to be influenced by prenatal hormones. There is evidence of other similar traits, e.g. otoacoustic emissions and arm-to-trunk length ratio, which show similar effects. Hox genes responsible for both digit and penis development[31] have been implicated in this pleiotropy. Direct effects of sex hormones on bone growth might be responsible, either by regulation of Hox genes in digit development or independently of such genes. Likewise, it is unclear why digit ratio on the right hand should be more responsive than that on the left hand, as is indicated by the greater sex difference on the right than the left.[32]

Geographic and ethnic variation in 2D:4D

Manning and colleagues have shown that 2D:4D ratios vary greatly between different ethnic groups.[33][34] This variation is far larger than the differences between sexes; in Manning's words, "There’s more difference between a Pole and a Finn than a man and a woman."[35]

Correlation between digit ratio and traits

Some authors suggest that digit ratio correlates with health, behavior, and even sexuality in later life. Below is a non-exhaustive list of some traits that have been either demonstrated or suggested to correlate with high digit ratio .

Physiology and disease

Psychological disorders

Physical and competitive behavior

  • Reduced performance in sports [53]
  • Reduced financial trading ability[54]
  • Right handedness Skills[55]

Cognition and personality

Management

Sensory Perception

  • Smell perception[67]
  • Color perception[68]
  • Tactile perception[69]

Sexual orientation

  • Bem sex role score in women;[70] erotic role preferences in gay men.[71]
  • Lesbians vs. straight women;[70][72][73][74][75][76][77][78][79][80][81] butch vs. femme lesbians.[73][82]
  • Gay vs straight men,[74] but most studies find very little difference in digit ratio between gay and straight men. One study found that Gay men, with several older brothers tend to have different digit arrangements.[83][81] Some studies correlate male homosexuality and 2D:4D positively,[84] others negatively.[85]

Transsexualism

  • A recent study in Germany has found a correlation between digit ratio and male to female transsexualism. Male to female transsexuals (trans women) were found to have a higher digit ratio than control males, but one that was comparable to control females.[87]

Digit ratio and development

There is some evidence that 2D:4D ratio may also be indicative for human development and growth. Ronalds et al. (2002) showed that men who had an above average placental weight and a shorter neonatal crown-heel length had higher 2D:4D ratios in adult life.[citation needed] Moreover, studies about 2D:4D correlations with face shape suggest that testosterone exposure early in life may set some constraints for subsequent development. Prenatal sex steroid ratios (in terms of 2D:4D) and actual chromosomal sex dimorphism were found to operate differently on human faces, but affect male and female face shape by similar patterns.[88] However, exposure to very high levels of testosterone and/or estrogen in the womb may have negative effects as well. Fink et al. (2004) found that men with low (indicating high testosterone) and women with high (indicating high estrogen) 2D:4D ratios express greater levels of facial symmetry.[89]

Digit ratio and palaeolithic hand stencils

2D:4D is being used alongside other methods to help sex Palaeolithic hand stencils found in European and Indonesian caves.[90][91][92]

Digit ratio research in animals

  • Dennis McFadden and collaborators have demonstrated sexual dimorphism in hind limb digit ratio in a number of great apes, including gorillas and chimpanzees.[74]
  • Emma Nelson and Susanne Shultz are currently investigating how 2D:4D relates to primate mating strategies and the evolution of human sociality.[93]
  • Sexual dimorphism in hind limb 2D:4D has been demonstrated in mice by two studies by both John Manning and Marc Breedlove's research groups. There is some evidence to suggest that this effect is not seen in all mouse strains.[citation needed]
  • Nancy Burley's research group has demonstrated sexual dimorphism in zebra finches, and found a correlation between digit ratio in females and the strength of their preference for sexually selected traits in males.[citation needed]
  • Front limb D2:D3 has shown to be influenced by prenatal alcohol exposure in female rats.[citation needed]
  • Alžbeta Talarovičová and collaborators found in rats that elevated testosterone during the prenatal period can influence 4D length, the 2D:4D ratio, and open field motor activity.[94]
  • Peter L. Hurd, Theodore Garland, Jr., and their students have examined hindlimb 2D:4D in lines of mice selectively bred for high voluntary wheel-running behavior (see experimental evolution). These high-runner mice exhibit increased 2D:4D. This apparent "feminization" is opposite to the relation seen between 2D:4D and physical fitness in human beings, and is difficult to reconcile with the idea that 2D:4D is a clear proxy for prenatal androgen exposure in mice. The authors suggest that 2D:4D may more accurately reflect effect of glucocorticoids or other factors that regulate any of various genes.[95]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Ecker A (1875). "Einige Bemerkungen über einen Schwankenden Charakter in den Hand des Menschen[Some remarks about a varying character in the hand of humans]". Archiv fur Anthropologie. 8: 68–74.
  2. ^ Baker F (1888). "Anthropological notes on the human hand". The American Anthropologist. 1: 51–75. doi:10.1525/aa.1888.1.1.02a00040.
  3. ^ George R (1930). "Human finger types". Anatomical Record. 46 (2): 199–204. doi:10.1002/ar.1090460210.
  4. ^ a b c Wilson, Glenn D. (1983). "Finger-length as an index of assertiveness in women". Personality and Individual Differences. 4 (1): 111–2. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(83)90061-2.
  5. ^ Wilson, G. (2010). "Fingers to feminism: The rise of 2D:4D". Quarterly Review. 4: 25–32.
  6. ^ Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998). "The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen". Hum Reprod. 13 (11): 3000–3004. doi:10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000. PMID 9853845.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ Kratochvíl L, Flegr J (2009). "Differences in the 2nd to 4th digit length ratio in humans reflect shifts along the common allometric line". Biology Letters. 5 (5): 643–6. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0346. PMC 2781964. PMID 19553247. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  8. ^ Zhengui Z., Cohn M. J. (2011). "Developmental basis of sexually dimorphic digit ratios". Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America. 108 (39): 16289–16294. doi:10.1073/pnas.1108312108.
  9. ^ Garn S. M., Burdi A. R., Babler W. J., Stinson S. (1975). "Early prenatal attainment of adult metacarpal-phalangeal rankings and proportions". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 43 (3): 327–332. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330430305.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  10. ^ van Anders SM, Vernon PA, Wilbur CJ (2006). "Finger-length ratios show evidence of prenatal hormone-transfer between opposite-sex twins". Hormones and Behavior. 49 (3): 315–9. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.08.003. PMID 16143332.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. ^ Hönekopp Johannes, Bartholdt Luise, Beier Lothar, Liebert Andreas. "Second to fourth digit length ratio (2D:4D) and adult sex hormone levels: New data and a meta-analytic review". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 32 (4): 313–321. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.007.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  12. ^ Hönekopp Johannes, Bartholdt Luise, Beier Lothar, Liebert Andreas. "Second to fourth digit length ratio (2D:4D) and adult sex hormone levels: New data and a meta-analytic review". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 32 (4): 313–321. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.007.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  13. ^ a b Bailey AA, Hurd PL (2005). "Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women". Biological Psychology. 68 (3): 215–22. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.05.001. PMID 15620791. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |laydate= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysource= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  14. ^ a b Brown WM, Hines M, Fane BA, Breedlove SM (2002). "Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia" (PDF). Hormones and Behavior. 42 (4): 380–6. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1830. PMID 12488105. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^ a b Okten A, Kalyoncu M, Yariş N (2002). "The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency". Early Human Development. 70 (1–2): 47–54. doi:10.1016/S0378-3782(02)00073-7. PMID 12441204. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ Ciumas C, Lindén Hirschberg A, Savic I. (2009). "High fetal testosterone and sexually dimorphic cerebral networks in females". Cereb Cortex. 19 (5): 1164–72. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn160. PMID 18854582.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. ^ Richard D. McAnulty, M. Michele Burnette (2006) Sex and sexuality, Volume 1, Greenwood Publishing Group, p.165
  18. ^ Pang S, Levine LS, Cederqvist LL, Fuentes M, Riccardi VM,Holcombe JH, Nitowsky HM, Sachs G, Anderson CE, Duchon MA,Owens R, Merkatz I, New MI (1980). "Amniotic fluid concentrations of delta5 and delta4 steroids in fetuses with congenital adrenal hyperplasia". J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 51 (2): 223–229. doi:10.1210/jcem-51-2-223. PMID 6447160.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. ^ Dorr, H. G., and Sippell, W. G. (1993). "Prenatal dexamethasone treatment in pregnancies at risk for congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency: Effect on midgestational amniotic fluid steroid levels". J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 76 (1): 117–120. doi:10.1210/jc.76.1.117. PMID 8421074.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. ^ LWudy, S. A., Dorr, H. G., Solleder, C., Djalali, M., and Homoki, J. (1999). "Profiling steroid hormones in amniotic fluid of midpregnancy by routine stable isotope dilution/gas chromatography­ mass spectrometry: Reference values and concentrations in fetuses at risk for 21-hydroxylase deficiency". J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 84 (8): 2724–2728. doi:10.1210/jc.84.8.2724. PMID 10443667. {{cite journal}}: soft hyphen character in |title= at position 115 (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  21. ^ Manning, John T.; Bundred, Peter E.; Newton, Darren J.; Flanagan, Brian F. (2003). "The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene". Evolution and Human Behavior. 24 (6): 399–405. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00052-7.
  22. ^ Berenbaum SA, Bryk KK, Nowak N, Quigley CA, Moffat S (2009). "Fingers as a Marker of Prenatal Androgen Exposure". Endocrinology. 150 (11): 5119–24. doi:10.1210/en.2009-0774. PMC 2775980. PMID 19819951. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ Malas MA, Dogan S, Evcil EH, Desdicioglu K. (2006). "Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D)". Early Hum Dev. 82 (7): 469–475. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.12.002. PMID 16473482.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Galis F, Ten Broek CM, Van Dongen S, Wijnaendts LC (2009). "Sexual Dimorphism in the Prenatal Digit Ratio (2D:4D)". Arch Sex Behav. 38 (1): 57–62. doi:10.1007/s10508-009-9485-7. PMC 2811245. PMID 19301112.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT (2004). "2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol". Early Human Development. 77 (1–2): 23–8. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.12.002. PMID 15113628. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  26. ^ a b Romano M, Leoni B, Saino N (2006). "Examination marks of male university students positively correlate with finger length ratios (2D:4D)". Biological Psychology. 71 (2): 175–82. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.03.006. PMID 15978716. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  27. ^ McIntyre MH (2006). "The use of digit ratios as markers for perinatal androgen action". Reproductive Biology and Endocrinology. 4: 10. doi:10.1186/1477-7827-4-10. PMC 1409789. PMID 16504142.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  28. ^ Paul SN, Kato BS, Hunkin JL, Vivekanandan S, Spector TD (2006). "The Big Finger: the second to fourth digit ratio is a predictor of sporting ability in women". British Journal of Sports Medicine. 40 (12): 981–3. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2006.027193. PMC 2577466. PMID 17008344. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  29. ^ Gobrogge, K.L., S.M.Breedlove & K.L.Klump (2008). "Genetic and environmental influences on 2d:4d finger length ratios: a study of monozygotic and dizygotic male and female twins". Archives Sexual Behavior. 37 (1): 112–118. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9272-2. PMID 18074216.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  30. ^ Lutchmaya S., Baron-Cohen S., Raggatt P., Knickmeyer R., Manning J. T. (2004). "2nd To 4th Digit Ratios, Fetal Testosterone and Estradiol". Early human development. 77 (1–2): 23–8. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.12.002. PMID 15113628.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  31. ^ Dickman S. (1997). "HOX gene links limb, genital defects". Science. 275 (5306): 1568–9. doi:10.1126/science.275.5306.1568. PMID 9072822. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  32. ^ Honekopp J, Watson S (2010). "Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand". American Journal of Human Biology. online (5): 619–30. doi:10.1002/ajhb.21054. PMID 20737609.
  33. ^ Manning JT; Barley L; Walton J; et al. (2000). "The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success. evidence for sexually antagonistic genes?". Evolution and Human Behavior. 21 (3): 163–183. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00029-5. PMID 10828555. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |author-separator= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  34. ^ Manning JT, Stewart A, Bundred PE, Trivers RL (2004). "Sex and ethnic differences in 2nd to 4th digit ratio of children". Early Human Development. 80 (2): 161–8. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.06.004. PMID 15500996. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  35. ^ Terrance, J. (30 March 2000). "Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation". Nature. 404 (6777): 455–456. doi:10.1038/35006555. PMID 10761903. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help), available on-line at "Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation". University of Nebraska-Lincoln. (quoted from New Scientist)
  36. ^ Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998). "The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen". Human Reproduction. 13 (11): 3000–4. doi:10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000. PMID 9853845. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  37. ^ Manning JT, Bundred PE (2001). "The ratio of second to fourth digit length and age at first myocardial infarction in men: a link with testosterone?". British Journal of Cardiology. 8 (12): 720–3. ISSN 0969-6113.
  38. ^ Fink B, Manning JT, Neave N (2006). "The 2nd-4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and neck circumference: implications for risk factors in coronary heart disease". International Journal of Obesity. 30 (4): 711–4. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803154. PMID 16261185. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  39. ^ Walsh, Fergus (1 December 2010). "Index finger length prostate cancer clue". BBC News. Retrieved 1 December 2010.
  40. ^ Ronalds G, Phillips DIW, Godfrey KM, Manning JT. The ratio of second to fourth digit lengths: a marker of impaired fetal growth? Early Hum. Dev. 2002;68:21–6.
  41. ^ Manning JT, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Sanders G (2001). "The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism". Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 43 (3): 160–4. doi:10.1017/S0012162201000317. PMID 11263685. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  42. ^ Bailey, A. & Hurd, P. (2005). "Depression in men is associated with more feminine finger length ratios". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (4): 829–836. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.017.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  43. ^ Arató M, Frecska E, Beck C, An M, Kiss H (2004). "Digit length pattern in schizophrenia suggests disturbed prenatal hemispheric lateralization". Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 28 (1): 191–4. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.020. PMID 14687873. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  44. ^ McFadden, D., Westhafer, J.G., Pasanen, E.G., Carlson, C.L., and Tucker, D.M. (2005). "Physiological evidence of hypermasculinization in boys with the inattentive subtype of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)". Clinical Neuroscience Research. 5 (5–6): 233–245. doi:10.1016/j.cnr.2005.09.004.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  45. ^ Stevenson JC, Everson PM, Williams DC, Hipskind G, Grimes M, Mahoney ER. (2007). "Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and digit ratios in a college sample". Am J Hum Biol. 19 (1): 41–50. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20571. PMID 17160985.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  46. ^ Martel, M.M, K.L.Gobrogge, S.M.Breedlove & J.T.Nigg (2008). "Masculinized Finger-Length Ratios of Boys, but Not Girls, Are Associated With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Behavioral Neuroscience. 122 (2): 273–281. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.273. PMC 2902868. PMID 18410167.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  47. ^ Martel, M.M. (2009). "Conscientiousness as a mediator of the association between masculinized finger-length ratios and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 50 (7): 790–798. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02065.x. PMID 19298468.
  48. ^ Klump, K. L., Gobrogge, K. L., Perkins, P. S., Thorne, D., Sisk, C. L., Breedlove, S.M. (2006). "Preliminary evidence that gonadal hormones organize and activate disordered eating". Psychol Med. 36 (4): 539–546. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006653. PMID 16336745.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  49. ^ Smith, A. R., Hawkeswood, S. E., Joiner, T. E. (2009). "The measure of a man: Associations between digit ratio and disordered eating in males". Int J Eat Disord. 28 (1): 191–4. doi:10.1002/eat.20736. PMID 19718667.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  50. ^ Blanchard, A. (2010). "An Investigation into the Relationship between Digit Length Ratio and Psychopathy". British Journal of Forensic Practice. 12 (2). {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  51. ^ Kornhuber J, Erhard G, Lenz B, Kraus T, Sperling W, Bayerlein K, Biermann T, Stoessel C (2011). Zhang, Xiang Yang (ed.). "Low Digit Ratio 2D∶4D in Alcohol Dependent Patients". PLoS ONE. 6 (4): e19332. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019332. PMC 3081847. PMID 21547078. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  52. ^ Evardone & Alexander (2009). "Anxiety, Sex-linked Behavior, and Digit Ratios". Arch Sex Behav. 38 (3): 442–55. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9260-6. PMC 2768336. PMID 17943431.
  53. ^ Manning JT, Taylor RP (2001). "Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: implications for sexual selection in humans". Evolution and Human Behavior. 22 (1): 61–69. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00063-5. PMID 11182575. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  54. ^ Coates JM, Gurnell M, Rustichini A (2009). "Second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts success among high-frequency financial traders". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (2): 623–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0810907106. PMC 2626753. PMID 19139402. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |laydate= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysource= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  55. ^ Fink B, Manning JT, Neave N, Tan U (2004). "Second to fourth digit ratio and hand skill in Austrian children". Biological Psychology. 67 (3): 375–84. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.012. PMID 15294393. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  56. ^ Benderlioglu Z, Nelson RJ (2004). "Digit length ratios predict reactive aggression in women, but not in men". Hormones and Behavior. 46 (5): 558–64. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.06.004. PMID 15555497. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  57. ^ Beech, John R.; Mackintosh, Isla C. (2005). "Do differences in sex hormones affect handwriting style? Evidence from digit ratio and sex role identity as determinants of the sex of handwriting". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (2): 459–68. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.024. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  58. ^ Neave N, Laing S, Fink B, Manning JT (2003). "Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 270 (1529): 2167–72. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2502. PMC 1691489. PMID 14561281. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  59. ^ Burriss RP, Little AC, Nelson EC (2007). "2D:4D and sexually dimorphic facial characteristics". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 36 (3): 377–84. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9136-1. PMID 17203400. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  60. ^ Austin, Elizabeth J.; Manning, John T.; McInroy, Katherine; Mathews, Elizabeth (2002). "A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio". Personality and Individual Differences. 33 (7): 1115–24. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00002-8. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  61. ^ Fink et al. 2004[verification needed]
  62. ^ Luxen, Marc F.; Buunk, Bram P. (2005). "Second-to-fourth digit ratio related to Verbal and Numerical Intelligence and the Big Five". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (5): 959–66. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.016. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  63. ^ Brosnan MJ (2008). "Digit ratio as an indicator of numeracy relative to literacy in 7-year-old British schoolchildren". British Journal of Psychology. 99 (Pt 1): 75–85. doi:10.1348/000712607X197406. PMID 17535470. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |laydate= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysource= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  64. ^ Sluming, Vanessa A.; Manning, John T. (2000). "Second to fourth digit ratio in elite musicians Evidence for musical ability as an honest signal of male fitness". Evolution and Human Behavior. 21 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00026-4. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  65. ^ Derval, Diana. The Right Sensory Mix: Targeting Consumer Product Development Scientifically. Springer, 2010, p. 129-130.
  66. ^ Derval, Diana. The Right Sensory Mix: Targeting Consumer Product Development Scientifically. Springer, 2010, p. 129-135.
  67. ^ Derval, Diana. The Right Sensory Mix: Targeting Consumer Product Development Scientifically. Springer, 2010, p. 62-67.
  68. ^ Derval, Diana. The Right Sensory Mix: Targeting Consumer Product Development Scientifically. Springer, 2010, p. 112-122.
  69. ^ Derval, Diana (2011). "Hormonal Quotient and tactile sensitivity: a segmentation model to understand and predict individuals' texture preferences based on prenatal exposure to hormones". Proceedings of Society for Behavioral Neuroendocrinology 15th Annual Meeting, Queretaro, Mexico, p.125.
  70. ^ a b Csathó A, Osváth A, Bicsák E, Karádi K, Manning J, Kállai J (2003). "Sex role identity related to the ratio of second to fourth digit length in women". Biological Psychology. 62 (2): 147–56. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00127-8. PMID 12581689. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  71. ^ McIntyre MH (2003). "Digit ratios, childhood gender role behavior, and erotic role preferences of gay men". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 32 (6): 495–6. doi:10.1023/A:1026054625638. PMID 14627046. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  72. ^ a b Williams TJ; Pepitone ME; Christensen SE; Cooke, Bradley M.; Huberman, Andrew D.; Breedlove, Nicholas J.; Breedlove, Tessa J.; Jordan, Cynthia L.; Breedlove, S. Marc (2000). "Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation" (PDF). Nature. 404 (6777): 455–6. doi:10.1038/35006555. PMID 10761903. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |author-separator= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |display-8authors= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  73. ^ a b Tortorice JL (2002). "Written on the body: butch/femme lesbian gender identity and biological correlates". Rutgers University. OCLC 80234273. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  74. ^ a b c McFadden D, Shubel E (2002). "Relative lengths of fingers and toes in human males and females". Hormones and Behavior. 42 (4): 492–500. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1833. PMID 12488115. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  75. ^ a b Hall LS, Love CT (2003). "Finger-length ratios in female monozygotic twins discordant for sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 32 (1): 23–8. doi:10.1023/A:1021837211630. PMID 12597269. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  76. ^ Rahman Q, Wilson GD (2003). "Sexual orientation and the 2nd to 4th finger length ratio: evidence for organising effects of sex hormones or developmental instability?". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 28 (3): 288–303. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00022-7. PMID 12573297. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  77. ^ Putz, David A.; Gaulin, Steven J. C.; Sporter, Robert J.; McBurney, Donald H. (2004). "Sex hormones and finger length: What does 2D:4D indicate?" (PDF). Evolution and Human Behavior. 25 (3): 182–99. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.03.005. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  78. ^ Rahman Q (2005). "Fluctuating asymmetry, second to fourth finger length ratios and human sexual orientation". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 30 (4): 382–91. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.10.006. PMID 15694118. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  79. ^ Kraemer B, Noll T, Delsignore A, Milos G, Schnyder U, Hepp U (2006). "Finger length ratio (2D:4D) and dimensions of sexual orientation". Neuropsychobiology. 53 (4): 210–4. doi:10.1159/000094730. PMID 16874008.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  80. ^ Wallien MS, Zucker KJ, Steensma TD, Cohen-Kettenis PT (2008). "2D:4D finger-length ratios in children and adults with gender identity disorder". Hormones and Behavior. 54 (3): 450–4. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.002. PMID 18585715. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  81. ^ a b Grimbos T, Dawood K, Burriss RP, Zucker KJ, Puts DA (2010). "Sexual orientation and the second to fourth finger length ratio: a meta-analysis in men and women". Behav Neurosci. 124 (2): 278–287. doi:10.1037/a0018764. PMID 20364887.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  82. ^ Brown WM, Finn CJ, Cooke BM, Breedlove SM (2002). "Differences in finger length ratios between self-identified 'butch' and 'femme' lesbians" (PDF). Archives of Sexual Behavior. 31 (1): 123–7. doi:10.1023/A:1014091420590. PMID 11910785. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  83. ^ http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/695142.stm/. {{cite news}}: Missing or empty |title= (help); Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (|url-status= suggested) (help) [dead link]
  84. ^ Churchchill AJG, Manning JT, Peters M (2007). "The effects of sex, ethnicity, and sexual orientation on self-measured digit ratio (2D:4D)". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 36 (2): 251–260. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9166-8. PMID 17394056.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  85. ^ S.J. Robinson, J.T. Manning (2000). "The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length and male homosexuality". Evolution and Human Behavior. 21 (5): 333–345. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00052-0. PMID 11053694.
  86. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9255-3, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1007/s10508-007-9255-3 instead.
  87. ^ Schneider HJ, Pickel J, Stalla GK (2006). "Typical female 2nd-4th finger length (2D:4D) ratios in male-to-female transsexuals-possible implications for prenatal androgen exposure". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 31 (2): 265–9. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.005. PMID 16140461. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  88. ^ Fink B; Grammer K; Mitteroecker P; et al. (2005). "Second to fourth digit ratio and face shape". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 272 (1576): 1995–2001. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3179. PMC 1559906. PMID 16191608. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |author-separator= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  89. ^ Fink, Bernhard; Manning, John T.; Neave, Nick; Grammer, Karl (2004). "Second to fourth digit ratio and facial asymmetry". Evolution and Human Behavior. 25 (2): 125–32. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00084-9. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  90. ^ Snow, Dean R. (2006). "Sexual dimorphism in Upper Palaeolithic hand stencils". Antiquity. 80 (308): 390–404.
  91. ^ Chazine, Jean-Michel; Noury, Arnaud (2006). "Sexual Determination of Hand Stencils at the Masri II Cave". Inora Newsletter. 44: 21–6.
  92. ^ Nelson, Emma C.; Manning, John T.; Sinclair, Anthony G. M. (2006). "Using the length of the 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) to sex cave art hand stencils: factors to consider" (PDF). Before Farming. 1 (6): 1–7.
  93. ^ Nelson, Emma. "Investigating relationships between the second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D), social and bonding behaviours in non-human anthropoids". Retrieved 29 October 2009.[dead link][self-published source?]
  94. ^ Talarovičová A, Kršková L, Blažeková J (2009). "Testosterone enhancement during pregnancy influences the 2D:4D ratio and open field motor activity of rat siblings in adulthood". Hormones and Behavior. 55 (1): 235–9. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.10.010. PMID 19022257. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  95. ^ Yan RH, Malisch JL, Hannon RM, Hurd PL, Garland T (2008). Svensson, Erik I. (ed.). "Selective Breeding for a Behavioral Trait Changes Digit Ratio". PLoS ONE. 3 (9): e3216. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003216. PMC 2528935. PMID 18797502.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)