Repressed memory
This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these messages)
|
Repressed memory is a condition where a memory has been unconsciously blocked by an individual due to the high level of stress or trauma contained in that memory.[citation needed] Even though the individual cannot recall the memory, it may still be affecting them consciously.[1]
The existence of repressed memories is a controversial topic in psychology; some studies have concluded that it can occur in victims of trauma, while others dispute it. According to some psychologists, repressed memories can be recovered through therapy. Other psychologists argue that this is in fact rather a process through which false memories are created by blending actual memories and outside influences. Furthermore, some psychologists believe that repressed memories are considered a cultural symptom because there is no written proof of its existence before the 1800s.[2]
According to the American Psychological Association, it is not currently possible to distinguish a true repressed memory from a false one without corroborating evidence.[1] The term repressed memory is derived from the term dissociative amnesia, which is defined in the DSM-IV as “an inability to recall important personal information, usually of a traumatic or stressful nature, that is too extensive to be explained by ordinary forgetfulness”.[3]
Amnesia is referred to any instance in which memories stored in the long-term memory are completely or partially forgotten, usually due to brain injury.[3] According to proponents of the existence of repressed memories, they may sometimes be recovered years or decades after the event, most often spontaneously, triggered by a particular smell, taste, or other identifier related to the lost memory, or via suggestion during psychotherapy.[4]
History
There is no documented writing about repressed memories or sometimes referred to as, dissociative amnesia, before the 1800s'.[5] The concept of repressed memory originated with Sigmund Freud in his 1896 essay Zur Ätiologie der Hysterie ("On the etiology of hysteria").[6] One of the studies published in his essay, involved a young woman by the name of, Anna O. Among her many ailments, she suffered from stiff paralysis on the right side of her body. Freud stated her symptoms to be attached to psychological traumas. The painful memories had separated from her consciousness and has brought harm to her body. Freud used hypnosis to treat Anna O. She is reported to have gained slight mobility in her right side.[7] Freud's repressed memory theory joined his philosophy of psychoanalysis. Repressed memory has remained a heavily debated topic inside of Freud's psychoanalysis philosophy.
Research
Some research indicates that memories of child sexual abuse and other traumatic incidents may be forgotten.[8][9] Evidence of the spontaneous recovery of traumatic memories has been shown,[10][11][12] and recovered memories of traumatic childhood abuse have been corroborated.[9][13][14]
Van der Kolk and Fisler's research shows that traumatic memories are retrieved, at least at first, in the form of mental imprints that are dissociated. These imprints are of the affective and sensory elements of the traumatic experience. Clients have reported the slow emergence of a personal narrative that can be considered explicit (conscious) memory. The level of emotional significance of a memory correlates directly with the memory's veracity. Studies of subjective reports of memory show that memories of highly significant events are unusually accurate and stable over time.[15] The imprints of traumatic experiences appear to be qualitatively different from those of nontraumatic events. Traumatic memories may be coded differently than ordinary event memories, possibly because of alterations in attentional focusing or the fact that extreme emotional arousal interferes with the memory functions of the hippocampus.[15]
Although research on repressed memory is limited, a few studies have suggested that memories of trauma that are forgotten and later recalled have a similar accuracy rate as trauma memories that had not been forgotten.[16]
One prominent proponent of the theory of repressed memory, and the usage of repressed memory in legal actions brought against alleged abusers, is Lenore Terr, a controversial California psychiatrist who championed the notion that repressed memories can be suddenly resurrected by victims of abuse (such as childhood physical or sexual abuse) through exposure to visual or auditory stimuli. The U.S. Ninth Circuit Court of Appeals challenged Terr's theory, when applied to legal actions and remedies, as unreliable and inconsistent, and ultimately denied the admissibility of repressed memory as evidence in a judicial proceeding (Franklin v. Duncan, 1995).[17][18][19]
There has also been significant questioning of the reality of repressed memories. There is considerable evidence that rather than being pushed out of consciousness, the difficulty with traumatic memories for most people are their intrusiveness and inability to forget.[20] One case that is held up as definitive proof of the reality of repressed memories, recorded by David Corwin[14] has been criticized by Elizabeth Loftus and Melvin Guyer for ignoring the context of the original complaint and falsely presenting the sexual abuse as unequivocal and true when in reality there was no definitive proof.[21]
Psychologists Elizabeth Loftus and Katherine Ketcham are authors of the seminal work on the fallacy of repressed memory, The Myth of Repressed Memory (St. Martin's Press, 1994).
For years Loftus was criticized and marginalized by her colleagues in psychology and psychiatry for her criticism and skepticism of repressed memory. In recent years her research is regarded as the most responsible approach to the controversy of repressed memory.[22]
Hypothesis
It has been speculated that repression may be one method used by individuals to cope with traumatic memories, by pushing them out of awareness (perhaps as an adaptation via psychogenic amnesia) to allow a child to maintain attachment to a person on whom they are dependent for survival.[23] Researchers have proposed that repression can operate on a social level as well.[24]
Authenticity
Memories can be accurate, but they are not always accurate. For example, eyewitness testimony even of relatively recent dramatic events is notoriously unreliable.[25] Misremembering results from confusion between memories for perceived and imagined events, which may result from overlap between particular features of the stored information comprising memories for perceived and imagined events. Memories of events are always a mix of factual traces of sensory information overlaid with emotions, mingled with interpretation and "filled in" with imaginings. Thus there is always skepticism about how valid a memory is as evidence of factual detail. Some believe that accurate memories of traumatic events are often repressed, but remain in the subconscious mind, from where they can be recovered by appropriate therapy. Others believe that truly traumatic events are never forgotten in this way, although often people may not disclose their memories to others. This is a difficult area to study, and unambiguous conclusions are hard to draw, hence there continue to be very divergent opinions. In one study where victims of documented child abuse were reinterviewed many years later as adults, a high proportion of the women denied any memory of the abuse.[26]
Those who doubt the existence of "traumatic amnesia" note that various manipulations can be used to implant false memories (sometimes called "pseudomemories"). These can be quite compelling for those who develop them, and can include details that make them seem credible to others.[27] A classic experiment in memory research, conducted by Elizabeth Loftus, became widely known as "Lost in the Mall"; in this, subjects were given a booklet containing three accounts of real childhood events written by family members and a fourth account of a wholly fictitious event of being lost in a shopping mall. A quarter of the subjects reported remembering the fictitious event, and elaborated on it with extensive circumstantial detail.[28] This experiment inspired many others, and in one of these, Porter et al. could convince about half of his subjects that they had survived a vicious animal attack in childhood.[29]
Such experimental studies have been criticized[30] in particular about whether the findings are really relevant to trauma memories and psychotherapeutic situations.[31] Nevertheless, these studies prompted public and professional concern about recovered memory therapy for past sexual abuse. When memories are 'recovered' after long periods of amnesia, particularly when extraordinary means were used to secure the recovery of memory, it is now widely (but not universally) accepted that the memories are quite likely to be false, i.e. of incidents that had not occurred.[32] It is thus recognised by professional organizations that a risk of implanting false memories is associated with some types of therapy. The American Psychiatric Association advises that "...most leaders in the field agree that although it is a rare occurrence, a memory of early childhood abuse that has been forgotten can be remembered later. However, these leaders also agree that it is possible to construct convincing pseudomemories for events that never occurred. The mechanism(s) by which both of these phenomena happen are not well understood and, at this point it is impossible, without other corroborative evidence, to distinguish a true memory from a false one."[33][34]
Obviously, not all therapists agree that false memories are a major risk with psychotherapy and they argue that this idea overstates the data and is untested. [35][36][37][38] Several studies have reported high percentages of the corroboration of recovered memories,[39][40] and some authors have claimed that the false memory movement has tended to conceal or omit evidence of (the) corroboration" of recovered memories.[41] Herman in her theory of recovery from chronic Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder writes that one of the major recovery stages is the remembering and mourning of the repressed material of traumatic events.[42]
Both true and false 'memories' can be recovered using memory work techniques, but there is no evidence that reliable discriminations can be made between them.[43] Some believe that memories "recovered" under hypnotism are particularly likely to be false.[44] According to The Council on Scientific Affairs for the American Medical Association, recollections obtained during hypnosis can involve confabulations and pseudomemories and appear to be less reliable than nonhypnotic recall.[45] Brown et al. estimate that 3 to 5% of laboratory subjects are vulnerable to post-event misinformation suggestions. They state that 5 - 8% of the general population is the range of high-hypnotizability. Twenty-five percent of those in this range are vulnerable to suggestion of pseudomemories for peripheral details, which can rise to 80% with a combination of other social influence factors. They conclude that the rates of memory errors run 0 - 5% in adult studies, 3 - 5% in children's studies and that the rates of false allegations of child abuse allegations run 4 - 8% in the general population.[36]
Neurological basis of memory
The neuroscientist Donald Hebb (1904–1985) was the first to distinguish between short-term memory and long-term memory. According to current theories in neuroscience, things that we "notice" are stored in short-term memory for up to a few minutes; this memory depends on 'reverberating' electrical activity in neuronal circuits, and is very easily destroyed by interruption or interference. Memories stored for longer than this are stored in long-term memory. Whether information is stored in long-term memory depends on its 'importance'; for any animal, memories of traumatic events are potentially important for the adaptive value that they have for future avoidance behaviour, and hormones that are released during stress have a role in determining what memories are preserved. In humans, traumatic stress is associated with acute secretion of epinephrine and norepinephrine (adrenaline and noradrenaline) from the adrenal medulla and cortisol from the adrenal cortex. Increases in these facilitate memory, but chronic stress associated with prolonged hypersecretion of cortisol may have the opposite effect. The limbic system is involved in memory storage and retrieval as well as giving emotional significance to sensory inputs. Within the limbic system, the hippocampus is important for explicit memory, and for memory consolidation; it is also sensitive to stress hormones, and has a role in recording the emotions of a stressful event. The amygdala may be particularly important in assigning emotional values to sensory inputs.[46]
Although memory distortion occurs in everyday life, the brain mechanisms involved are not easy to study in the laboratory, but neuroimaging techniques have recently been applied to this subject. In particular, there have recently been studies of false recognition, where individuals incorrectly claim to have encountered a novel object or event, and the results suggest that the hippocampus and several cortical regions may contribute to such false recognition, while the prefrontal cortex may be involved in retrieval monitoring that can limit the rate of false recognition.[47]
Amnesia
Amnesia is partial or complete loss of memory that goes beyond mere forgetting. Often it is temporary and involves only part of a person's experience. Amnesia is often caused by an injury to the brain, for instance after a blow to the head, and sometimes by psychological trauma. Anterograde amnesia is a failure to remember new experiences that occur after damage to the brain; retrograde amnesia is the loss of memories of events that occurred before a trauma or injury. For a memory to become permanent (consolidated), there must be a persistent change in the strength of connections between particular neurons in the brain. Anterograde amnesia can occur because this consolidation process is disrupted; retrograde amnesia can result either from damage to the site of memory storage or from a disruption in the mechanisms by which memories can be retrieved from their stores. Many specific types of amnesia are recognized, including:
- Childhood amnesia is the normal inability to recall memories from the first three years of life. Sigmund Freud was the first to observe this phenomenon and realized that not only do humans not remember anything from birth to three years, but they also have “spotty” recollection of anything occurring from three to seven years of age.[48] There are various theories as to why this occurs: some believe that language development is important for efficient storage of long-term memories; others believe that early memories do not persist because the brain is still developing.
- A fugue state, formally dissociative fugue, is a rare condition precipitated by a stressful episode. It is characterized by episode(s) of traveling away from home and creating a new identity.[49]
The form of amnesia that is linked with recovered memories is dissociative amnesia (formerly known as psychogenic amnesia). This results from a psychological cause, not by direct damage to the brain, and is a loss of memory of significant personal information, usually about traumatic or extremely stressful events. Usually this is seen as a gap or gaps in recall for aspects of someone's life history, but with severe acute trauma, such as during wartime, there can be a sudden acute onset of symptoms.[50]
Effects of trauma on memory
'Betrayal Trauma Theory' proposes that in cases of childhood abuse, dissociative amnesia is an adaptive response, and that “victims may need to remain unaware of the trauma not to reduce suffering but rather to promote survival.”[51] When stress interferes with memory, it is possible that some of the memory is kept by a system that records emotional experience, but there is no symbolic placement of it in time or space.[52] Traumatic memories are retrieved, at least at first, in the form of dissociated mental imprints of the affective and sensory elements of the traumatic experience. Clients have reported the slow emergence of a personal narrative that can be considered explicit (conscious) memory.
Psychiatrist Bessel van der Kolk[40] divided the effects of traumas on memory functions into four sets
- traumatic amnesia; this involves the loss of memories of traumatic experiences. The younger the subject and the longer the traumatic event is, the greater the chance of significant amnesia. He stated that subsequent retrieval of memories after traumatic amnesia is well documented in the literature, with documented examples following natural disasters and accidents, in combat soldiers, in victims of kidnapping, torture and concentration camp experiences, in victims of physical and sexual abuse, and in people who have committed murder.
- global memory impairment; this makes it difficult for subjects to construct an accurate account of their present and past history. "The combination of lack of autobiographical memory, continued dissociation and of meaning schemes that include victimization, helplessness and betrayal, is likely to make these individuals vulnerable to suggestion and to the construction of explanations for their trauma-related affects that may bear little relationship to the actual realities of their lives"
- dissociative processes; this refers to memories being stored as fragments and not as unitary wholes.
- traumatic memories’ sensorimotor organization. Not being able to integrate traumatic memories seems to be linked to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).[53]
According to van der Kolk, memories of highly significant events are usually accurate and stable over time; aspects of traumatic experiences appear to get stuck in the mind, unaltered by time passing or experiences that may follow. The imprints of traumatic experiences appear to be different from those of nontraumatic events, perhaps because of alterations in attentional focusing or the fact that extreme emotional arousal interferes with memory.[40] van der Kolk and Fisler's hypothesis is that under extreme stress, the memory categorization system based in the hippocampus fails, with these memories kept as emotional and sensory states. When these traces are remembered and put into a personal narrative, they are subject to being condensed, contaminated and embellished upon.
When there is inadequate recovery time between stressful situations, alterations may occur to the stress response system, some of which may be irreversible, and cause pathological responses, which may include memory loss, learning deficits and other maladaptive symptoms. In animal studies, high levels of cortisol can cause hippocampal damage, which may cause short-term memory deficits; in humans, MRI studies have shown reduced hippocampal volumes in combat veterans with PTSD, adults with posttraumatic symptoms and survivors of repeated childhood sexual or physical abuse. Trauma may also interfere with implicit memory, where periods of avoidance may be interrupted by intrusive emotional occurrences with no story to guide them. A difficult issue is whether those presumably abused accurately recall their experiences.[54]
Criticisms
The existence of repressed memory recovery has not been accepted by mainstream psychology,[55][56][57][58] nor unequivocally proven to exist, and some experts in the field of human memory feel that no credible scientific support exists for the notions of repressed/recovered memories.[59][60] One research report states that a distinction should be made between spontaneously recovered memories and memories recovered during suggestions in therapy.[61] A common criticism is that a recovered memory is tainted by, or a product of, the process of recovery or the suggestions used in that process.[citation needed]
Many critics believe that memories may be distorted and false. Psychologist Elizabeth Loftus questions the concept of repressed memories and their possibility of them being accurate. Loftus focuses on techniques that therapists use in order to help the patients recover their memory. Such techniques include age regression, guided visualization, trance writing, dream work, body work, and hypnosis. Loftus' research indicates that repressed memory faces problems, such as memory alteration. In one case a teenage boy was able to “conjure a memory of an event that never occurred.” According to Loftus, if a stable person could be influenced to remember an event that never occurred, an emotionally stressed person would be even more susceptible.[62]
Medico-legal issues
Serious issues arise when recovered but false memories result in public allegations; false complaints carry serious consequences for the accused. Many of those who make false claims sincerely believe the truth of what they report. A special type of false allegation, the false memory syndrome, arises typically within therapy, when people report the 'recovery' of childhood memories of previously unknown abuse. The influence of practitioners' beliefs and practices in the eliciting of false 'memories' and of false complaints has come under particular criticism.[63] Sometimes these memories are used as evidence in criminal prosecutions.
It is generally accepted that people sometimes are unable to recall traumatic experiences. The current version (DSM-IV) of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, published by the American Psychiatric Association, states that "Dissociative amnesia is characterized by an inability to recall important personal information, usually of a traumatic or stressful nature, that is too extensive to be explained by ordinary forgetfulness."[64] The term "recovered memory", however, is not listed in DSM-IV or used by any mainstream formal psychotherapy modality.[65]
Legal issues
Some criminal cases have been based on a witness's testimony of recovered repressed memories, often of alleged childhood sexual abuse. In some jurisdictions, the statute of limitations for child abuse cases has been extended to accommodate the phenomena of repressed memories as well as other factors. The repressed memory concept came into wider public awareness in the 1980s and 1990s followed by a reduction of public attention after a series of scandals, lawsuits, and license revocations.[66]
Alan Scheflin, a law professor, stated in 1999, "Both those who argue that repressed memories are always false and those who argue that repressed memories are always true [...] appear to be mistaken. Although the science is limited on this issue, the only three relevant studies conclude that repressed memories are no more and no less accurate than continuous memories....”[67]
A U.S. District Court accepted repressed memories as admissible evidence in a specific case.[68] Dalenberg argues that the evidence shows that recovered memory cases should be allowed to be prosecuted in court.[38]
The apparent willingness of courts to credit the recovered memories of complainants but not the absence of memories by defendants has been commented on: "It seems apparent that the courts need better guidelines around the issue of dissociative amnesia in both populations."[69]
In July, 2012, the Minnesota Supreme Court ruled that repressed memory is not acceptable as evidence in a legal action and is the equivalent of superstition, hearsay or "junk science."[70][71]
In 1995, the Ninth Circuit Court of Appeals, ruled in Franklin v. Duncan and Franklin v. Fox, Murray et al., (312 F3d. 423, see also 884 FSupp 1435, N.D. Calif.)[72] that repressed memory is not admissible as evidence in a legal action because of its unreliability, inconsistency, unscientific nature, tendency to be therapeutically induced evidence, and subject to influence by hearsay and suggestibility. The court overturned the conviction of a man accused of murdering a nine-year-old girl purely based upon the evidence of a 21-year-old repressed memory by a lone witness, who also held a complex personal grudge against the defendant.[18][73][74]
In a 1996 ruling, a U.S. District Court allowed repressed memories entered into evidence in court cases.[75] Jennifer Freyd writes that Ross Cheit's case of suddenly remembered sexual abuse is one of the most well-documented cases available for the public to see. Cheit prevailed in two lawsuits, located five additional victims and tape-recorded a confession.[12]
On 16 December 2005 the Irish Court of Criminal Appeal issued a certificate confirming a Miscarriage of Justice to a former nun Nora Wall whose 1999 conviction for child rape was partly based on Repressed Memory evidence. The judgement stated that: "There was no scientific evidence of any sort adduced to explain the phenomenon of ‘flashbacks’ and/or ‘retrieved memory’, nor was the applicant in any position to meet such a case in the absence of prior notification thereof." [76]
Recovered memory therapy
Recovered memory therapy is a range of psychotherapy methods based on recalling memories of abuse that had previously been forgotten by the patient.[77] The term recovered memory therapy is not listed in DSM-IV or used by mainstream formal psychotherapy modality.[65][78] Opponents of the therapy advance the explanation that therapy can create false memories through suggestion techniques; this has not been corroborated, though some research has shown supportive evidence.[79][80] Nevertheless, the evidence is questioned by some researchers.[65][81][82] It is possible for patients who retract their claims—after deciding their recovered memories are false—to suffer post-traumatic stress disorder due to the trauma of illusory memories.[83] According to a book written in 1995, the number of reported retractions is small when compared to the large number of actual child sexual abuse cases.[84]
Professional organizations
The Working Group on Investigation of Memories of Child Abuse of the American Psychological Association presented findings mirroring those of the other professional organizations (see External Links subpage for references to various statements made independently by these organisations). The Working Group made five key conclusions:
- Controversies regarding adult recollections should not be allowed to obscure the fact that child sexual abuse is a complex and pervasive problem in America that has historically gone unacknowledged;
- Most people who were sexually abused as children remember all or part of what happened to them;
- It is possible for memories of abuse that have been forgotten for a long time to be remembered;
- It is also possible to construct convincing pseudo-memories for events that never occurred; and
- There are gaps in our knowledge about the processes that lead to accurate and inaccurate recollections of childhood abuse.[85]
See also
References
- ^ a b Questions and Answers about Memories of Childhood Abuse
- ^ Pettus, Ashley. "cultural symptom? repressed memory". Harvard Magazine. Retrieved 4 March 2013.
- ^ a b "Amnesia". Encyclopedia Britannica online. Encyclopædia Britannica Inc. Retrieved 4 March 2013.
- ^ Albach, Francine (December-1996). "Memory recovery of childhood sexual abuse". Dissociation. 9 (4): 261–273. ISSN 0896-2863. hdl:1794/1774.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Pope H., et al. Psychol. Med., 37. 225 - 233 (2007).
- ^ Hinz, Paul (1920). Die Verfassung des Freistaates Preussen. J. Bensheimer. pp. 55–57.
- ^ Boag, Simon (2012). Freudian Repression. London: Karnac Books. ISBN 9781855757387.
- ^ Williams LM (1994). "Recall of childhood trauma: a prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse". J Consult Clin Psychol. 62 (6): 1167–76. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.62.6.1167. PMID 7860814.
- ^ a b Bowman, E.S. (1996). "Delayed memories of child abuse: Part I: An overview of research findings on forgetting, remembering, and corroborating trauma". Dissociation. 9: 221–231.; lay summary "Corroboration of child abuse memories". About.com. 2006-02-20. Retrieved 2008-01-06.
- ^ Chu, J (1999). "Memories of childhood abuse: dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration". American Journal of Psychiatry. 156 (5): 749–55. PMID 10327909.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Duggal, S. (1998). "Recovered memory of childhood sexual trauma: A documented case from a longitudinal study". Journal of Traumatic Stress. 11 (2): 301–321. doi:10.1023/A:1024403220769. PMID 9565917. Retrieved 2007-12-31.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ a b Freyd, Jennifer J. (1996). Betrayal Trauma - The Logic of Forgetting Childhood Abuse. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-06805-X.
- ^ Cheit, Ross E. (1998). "Consider This, Skeptics of Recovered Memory". Ethics & Behavior. 8 (2): 141–160. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0802_4. Retrieved 2007-12-25.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|month=
(help) - ^ a b Corwin, D (1997). "Videotaped Discovery of a Reportedly Unrecallable Memory of Child Sexual Abuse: Comparison with a Childhood Interview Videotaped 11 Years Before". Child Maltreatment. 2 (2): 91–112. doi:10.1177/1077559597002002001. Retrieved 2008-01-09.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b van der Kolk BA, Fisler R (1995). "Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: overview and exploratory study". J Trauma Stress. 8 (4): 505–25. PMID 8564271. Retrieved 2008-03-22.
- ^ Scheflin, A (1999-11-01). "Ground lost: The false memory/recovered memory therapy debate". Psychiatric Times. 16 (11).
- ^ Victims of the State: George Franklin www.victimsofthestate.org/CA/Franklin.htm
- ^ a b Holding, Reynolds (2011-06-23). "Repressed Memory Case Ruling / Appeals court refuses to restore murder conviction". The San Francisco Chronicle.
- ^ Harry N. MacLean, Once Upon a Time: A True Story of Memory, Murder and the Law. NY: Harper Collins, 1993.
- ^ McHugh, PR. Try to remember: Psychiatry's clash over meaning, memory and mind. Dana Press. pp. 45–6. ISBN 1-932594-39-6.
- ^ Loftus, EF (2002). "Who Abused Jane Doe? The Hazards of the Single Case History Part 1". 26 (3). Skeptical Inquirer.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) Loftus, EF (2002). "Who Abused Jane Doe? The Hazards of the Single Case History Part 2". 26 (4). Skeptical Inquirer.{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Harry N. MacLean, "Repressed Memory Dealt a Serious Blow." http://www.harrymaclean.com/blog/tag/minnesota-supreme-court/
- ^ Freyd, J. (1994). "Betrayal trauma: traumatic amnesia as an adaptive response to childhood abuse" (PDF). Ethics & Behavior. 4 (4): 307–330. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0404_1. Retrieved 2008-01-13.
- ^ Freyd, Jennifer J. (2007). "Archiving Dissociation as a Precaution Against Dissociating Dissociation" (PDF). Journal of Trauma & Dissociation. 8 (3). Retrieved 2007-12-29.
- ^ Gonsalves, B; Paller, KA (2002). "Mistaken memories: remembering events that never happened". Neuroscientist. 8 (5): 391–5. doi:10.1177/107385802236964. PMID 12374423.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Williams LM (1994). "Recall of childhood trauma: a prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse" (PDF). J Consult Clin Psychol. 62 (6): 1167–76. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.62.6.1167. PMID 7860814.
- ^ Laney, C; Loftus, EF (2005). "Traumatic memories are not necessarily accurate memories". Can J Psychiatry. 50 (13): 823–8. PMID 16483115.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ See Loftus E (1997) Creating false memories Scientific American 227 no 3 for a popular account
- ^ Porter, S; Yuille, JC; Lehman, DR (1999). "The nature of real, implanted, and fabricated memories for emotional childhood events: implications for the recovered memory debate". Law Hum Behav. 23 (5): 517–37. doi:10.1023/A:1022344128649. PMID 10487147.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Crook, L; Dean, MC (1999). "Lost in a shopping mall--A breach of professional ethics". Ethics Behavior. 9 (1): 39–50. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0901_3. PMID 11657487.
- ^ Pope, K (1996). "Memory, abuse, and science: questioning claims about the false memory syndrome epidemic". Am Psychologist. 51 (9): 957–74. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.9.957. PMID 8819364.
- ^ Brandon, S; Boakes, J; Glaser, D; Green, R (1998). "Recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Implications for clinical practice". Br J Psychiatry. 172 (4): 296–307. doi:10.1192/bjp.172.4.296. PMID 9722329.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Questions and Answers about Memories of Childhood Abuse American Psychiatric Association
- ^ Sheflin and Brown state that a total of 25 studies on amnesia for child sexual abuse exist and that they demonstrate amnesia in their study subpopulations. Sheflin, AW (1996). "Repressed memory or dissociative amnesia: what the science says". J Psychiat Law. 24: 143–88. ISSN = 0093-1853 =.
{{cite journal}}
: Check|issn=
value (help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help). An editorial in the British Medical Journal however, prefaces mention of the Sheflin and Brown study with "on critical examination, the scientific evidence for repression crumbles.""Editorial — Recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse: The Royal College of Psychiatrists issues important precautions". British Medical Journal (BMJ). 316 (7130). 14 February 1998Template:Inconsistent citations{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|unused_data=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: postscript (link) - ^ Chu, JA (1999). "Memories of childhood abuse: Dissociation, amnesia and corroboration". Am J Psychiatry. 156 (5): 749–55. PMID 10327909.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Hammond DC; Brown DP.; Scheflin AW (1998). Memory, trauma treatment, and the law. New York: W.W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-70254-5.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Whitfield MD, CL (1995-06-03). Memory and Abuse - Remembering and Healing the Effects of Trauma. Deerfield Beach, FL: Health Communications Inc. ISBN 1-55874-320-0.
- ^ a b Dalenberg, C (2006). "Recovered memory and the Daubert criteria: recovered memory as professionally tested, peer reviewed, and accepted in the relevant scientific community". Trauma Violence Abuse. 7 (4): 274–310. doi:10.1177/1524838006294572. PMID 17065548.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Kluft, RP (1995). "The confirmation and disconfirmation of memories of abuse in Dissociative Identity Disorder patients: A naturalistic study" (PDF). Dissociation. 8: 253–8.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|coauthors=
(help) - ^ a b c van der Kolk, BA; Fisler, R (1995). "Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: Overview and exploratory study" (PDF). J Traumatic Stress. 8: 505–25Template:Inconsistent citations
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: postscript (link) Cite error: The named reference "Van1995" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page). - ^ Cheit, RE (1998). "Consider this, skeptics of recovered memory". Ethics Behav. 8 (2): 141–60. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0802_4.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|month=
(help) - ^ Herman, JL (1997). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books. p. 290. ISBN 0-465-08730-2.
The ordinary response to atrocities is to banish them from consciousness.
{{cite book}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|coauthors=
(help) - ^ Stocks, JT (1998). "Recovered memory therapy: a dubious practice technique". Soc Work. 43 (5): 423–36. doi:10.1093/sw/43.5.423. PMID 9739631.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Kihlstrom, JF (1997). "Hypnosis, memory and amnesia". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 29 (1362): 3521727–32. doi:10.1098/rstb.1997.0155. PMC 1692104. PMID 9415925.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ "Scientific status of refreshing recollection by the use of hypnosis. Council on Scientific Affairs". JAMA: the Journal of the American Medical Association. 253 (13): 1918–23. 1985. doi:10.1001/jama.253.13.1918. PMID 3974082.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Zola, SM (1998). "Memory, amnesia, and the issue of recovered memory: neurobiological aspects". Clin Psychol Rev. 18 (8): 915–32. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00037-3. PMID 9885767.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Schacter, DL; Slotnick, SD (2004). "The cognitive neuroscience of memory distortion". Neuron. 44 (1): 149–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.017. PMID 15450167.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Josselyn, S. A., & Frankland, P. W. (2012). Infantile amnesia: A neurogenic hypothesis. Learning & Memory, 19(9), 423-433. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.021311.110
- ^ Loewenstein, R. J. (1991). Psychogenic amnesia and psychogenic fugue: A comprehensive review. (pp. 189-222). Arlington, VA, US: American Psychiatric Association, Arlington, VA.
- ^ Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
- ^ Freyd, J (1994). "Betrayal trauma: Traumatic amnesia as an adaptive response to childhood abuse". Ethics Behav. 4 (4): 307–29. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0404_1.
- ^ van der Kolk, Bessel (1994). "The body keeps the score: memory and the evolving psychobiology of posttraumatic stress" (PDF). Harvard Rev Psychiat. 1 (5): 253–65. doi:10.3109/10673229409017088Template:Inconsistent citations
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: postscript (link) - ^ Diagnostic symptoms of PTSD include reexperience such as flashbacks and nightmares, difficulty falling or staying asleep, feelings of panic or fear, depression, headache, and physiological symptoms including irregular heartbeat and diarrhoea.Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) The Royal College of Psychiatrists
- ^ Bremner, JD (2002). Does Stress Damage the Brain? Understanding Trauma-Related Disorders from a Neurological Perspective. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.
- ^ McNally, R.J. (2004). "The Science and Folklore of Traumatic Amnesia". Clinical Psychology Science and Practice. 11 (1): 29–33. doi:10.1093/clipsy/bph056.
- ^ McNally RJ (2007). "Dispelling confusion about traumatic dissociative amnesia". Mayo Clin. Proc. 82 (9): 1083–90. doi:10.4065/82.9.1083. PMID 17803876.
- ^ McNally RJ (2004). "Is traumatic amnesia nothing but psychiatric folklore?". Cogn Behav Ther. 33 (2): 97–101, discussion 102–4, 109–11. doi:10.1080/16506070410021683. PMID 15279316.
- ^ McNally RJ (2005). "Debunking myths about trauma and memory". Can J Psychiatry. 50 (13): 817–22. PMID 16483114.
- ^ Amicus Curiae brief in Taus v. Loftus (Superme Court of California 2006-02-21).
- ^ Revisiting the memory wars: repressed/recovered memory and dissociation; ABC TV, Catalyst, 22 Sept. 2011.
- ^ Geraerts, E. (2007). "The Reality of Recovered Memories: Corroborating Continuous and Discontinuous Memories of Childhood Sexual Abuse" (PDF). Psychological Science. 18 (7): 564–568. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01940.x. PMID 17614862.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Loftus, Elizabeth (May 1993). "The Reality of Repressed Memories". American Psychologist. 48 (1): 518–537.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: date and year (link) - ^ Boakes, J (1999). "False complaints of sexual assault: recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse". Med Sci Law. 39 (2): 112–20. PMID 10332158.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|author-name-separator=
(help); Unknown parameter|author-separator=
ignored (help) - ^ Section 300.12:DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, American Psychiatric Association
- ^ a b c Whitfield, Charles L. (2001). Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors. Haworth Press. p. 56. ISBN 0-7890-1901-9.
{{cite book}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) Cite error: The named reference "whitfield56" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page). - ^ Robbins Susan P., The Social and Cultural Context of Satanic Ritual Abuse Allegations, published in Institute for Psychological Therapies magazine, vol 10 1998.[1]
- ^ Scheflin, A (1999). "Ground lost: the false memory/recovered memory therapy debate". Psychiatric Times. 16Template:Inconsistent citations
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: postscript (link) - ^ "The Validity of Recovered Memory: Decision of a US District Court" Judge Edward F. Harrington, Presentation by Jim Hopper Ph.D. The legal documentation citation is: 923 Federal Supplement 286 (D. Mass. 1996), United States District Court - District of Massachusetts Ann Shahzade, plaintiff Civil Action No.: V. 92-12139-EFH George Gregory, Defendant. [2]
- ^ Porter S; et al. (2001). "Memory for murder. A psychological perspective on dissociative amnesia in legal contexts". Int J Law Psychiatry. 24 (1): 23–42. doi:10.1016/S0160-2527(00)00066-2. PMID 11346990Template:Inconsistent citations
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help)CS1 maint: postscript (link) - ^ "Minn. Supreme Court rejects repressed memory 'junk science' against priest; media yawns."http://www.themediareport.com/2012/07/31/minn-supreme-court-rejects-repressed-memory/
- ^ "Minnesota Supreme Court rejects church abuse case based on repressed memories."http://www.twincities.com/localnews/ci_21155266
- ^ Franklin v. Duncan Court Order http://scholar.google.com/scholar_case?case=3066228776991890480
- ^ Victims of the State: George Franklin http://www.victimsofthestate.org/CA/Franklin.htm
- ^ Los Angeles Times http://articles.latimes.com/keyword/george-thomas-sr-franklin.
{{cite news}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help) - ^ 923 Federal Supplement 286 (D. Mass. 1996); CIVIL ACTION NO.: 92-12139-EFH MEMORANDUM AND ORDER; May 8, 1996
- ^ "D.P.P.-v- Nora Wall [2005] IECCA 140 (16 December 2005)". Bailii.org. Retrieved 2012-11-10.
- ^ Lief, Harold I (1999). "Patients Versus Therapists: Legal Actions Over Recovered Memory Therapy". Psychiatric Times. XVI (11).
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Underwager, Ralph (1994). Return of the Furies: An Investigation into Recovered Memory Therapy. Open Court Pub Co. p. 360. ISBN 978-0-8126-9271-6.
{{cite book}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Loftus, EF (1995). "The formation of false memories" (PDF). Psychiatric Annals. 25: 720–725.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Pezdek, K (1999). "Planting false childhood memories: The role of event plausibility" (PDF). Child Development. 70 (4): 887–895. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00064.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Hammond, D. Corydon; Brown, Daniel P.; Scheflin, Alan W. (1998). Memory, trauma treatment, and the law. New York: W.W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-70254-5.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Brandon S, Boakes J, Glaser D, Green R (1998). "Recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Implications for clinical practice". Br J Psychiatry. 172 (4): 296–307. doi:10.1192/bjp.172.4.296. PMID 9722329.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Lambert, K (2007-10-01). "Brain Stains". Scientific American. Retrieved 2008-01-25.
{{cite web}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Whitfield M.D., Charles L. (1995). Memory and Abuse - Remembering and Healing the Effects of Trauma. Deerfield Beach, FL: Health Communications, Inc. p. 375. ISBN 1-55874-320-0.
- ^ Colangelo JJ (2007) Recovered memory debate revisited: practice implications for mental health counselors. (PRACTICE) Journal of Mental Health Counseling