Jump to content

Intellectual curiosity

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Zanthius (talk | contribs) at 20:13, 10 April 2017 (History). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Intellectual curiosity (also called epistemic curiosity) is curiosity about what objects are composed of, about how systems function, and about mathematical relationships. It can be differentiated from another type of curiosity that does not lead to acquisition of knowledge, such as curiosity about the intimate secrets of other people. It is a facet of openness to experience in the Five Factor Model used to describe human personalities[1] . It is similar to need for cognition and typical intellectual engagement.

History

In 1738, Hume differentiated between

plove of knowledge

and

passion derived from a quite different principle [that is] an insatisfiable desire for knowing the actions and circumstances of neighbours

(p. 453)[2]



Later, in 1954, Berlyne differentiated it into perceptual curiosity and epistemic curiosity. [3]

A psychometric scale to assess epistemic and perceptual curiosity was developed in 2004. [4]

Intellectual curiosity and intellectual development in children

Humans seem to be born with intellectual curiosity, but depending on how parents react to questions from their children, intellectual curiosity might be increased or decreased. Parents that always react negatively to questions asked by their children, are discouraging them from asking questions, and that is likely to make them less curious. On the other hand, parents that always react positively to questions asked by their children, are encouraging them to ask questions, and that is likely to make them more curious. There is a book about how to foster intellectual curiosity and a love for knowledge in children [5].

How parents/caregivers might increase or decrease intellectual curiosity in children.
How parents/caregivers might increase or decrease intellectual curiosity in children.

In relation to academic performance

It has been positively correlated with academic performance (0.20)[6], just like general intelligence (0.35) and conscientiousness (0.20).

Traits correlated with academic performance

Intellectual curiosity and scientific progress

Toby E. Huff has argued that the European civilization had a high level of intellectual curiosity during the scientific revolution [7]. He also argues that other civilizations have had a high level of intellectual curiosity in their most progressive stages.

The neurobiological basis of intellectual curiosity

The temporal lobe is involved in understanding. Intellectual curiosity might be regarded as the trait that motivates growth of understanding in the temporal lobe. Motivation is effectuated by the neurotransmitter dopamine.

Similarity to other concepts

Due to a high level of correlation (.78), it has been argued that need for cognition and typical intellectual engagement basically are measuring the same trait[8].

References

  1. ^ Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO personality Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.[page needed]
  2. ^ Selby-Bigge, David Hume. Ed. by L. A. (1987). A treatise of human nature (2. ed ed.). Oxford [u.a.]: Clarendon Pr. ISBN 978-0198245889. {{cite book}}: |edition= has extra text (help)
  3. ^ BERLYNE, D. E. (August 1954). "A THEORY OF HUMAN CURIOSITY". British Journal of Psychology. General Section. 45 (3): 180–191. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1954.tb01243.x.
  4. ^ Goff, Maynard; Ackerman, Phillip L. (1992). "Personality-intelligence relations: Assessment of typical intellectual engagement". Journal of Educational Psychology. 84 (4): 537–552. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.537.
  5. ^ Swanson, Kristen; Krause, Dianne; Selak, Bill; Casas, Jimmy; Hasty, Desmond; Juliani, A. J.; Gabriele, Anthony; Mogg, Steve; Dougherty, Kevin; Couros, George. Intellectual Curiosity In Our Schools. LeanPub.
  6. ^ von Stumm, Sophie; Hell, Benedikt; Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (November 2011). "The Hungry Mind". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 6 (6): 574–588. doi:10.1177/1745691611421204. {{cite journal}}: More than one of |pages= and |page= specified (help)
  7. ^ Huff, Toby E. (2010). Intellectual curiosity and the scientific revolution a global perspective (1. publ. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521170529.
  8. ^ Woo, Sang Eun; Harms, Peter D.; Kuncel, Nathan R. (October 2007). "Integrating personality and intelligence: Typical intellectual engagement and need for cognition". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (6): 1635–1639. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.022.