Jump to content

Edit filter log

Details for log entry 32886247

11:33, 26 June 2022: Fitnesscave (talk | contribs) triggered filter 149, performing the action "edit" on Memory and retention in learning. Actions taken: Warn; Filter description: User adds link containing username (examine)

Changes made in edit



There are many other methods of improving our memory and retention, however these are the most well-known and credible methods.
There are many other methods of improving our memory and retention, however these are the most well-known and credible methods.
https://humanfitnesscave.blogspot.com/2022/06/if-you-want-to-be-intelligent-eat.html


== References ==
== References ==

Action parameters

VariableValue
Edit count of the user (user_editcount)
0
Name of the user account (user_name)
'Fitnesscave'
Age of the user account (user_age)
345937
Groups (including implicit) the user is in (user_groups)
[ 0 => '*', 1 => 'user' ]
Rights that the user has (user_rights)
[ 0 => 'createaccount', 1 => 'read', 2 => 'edit', 3 => 'createtalk', 4 => 'writeapi', 5 => 'viewmywatchlist', 6 => 'editmywatchlist', 7 => 'viewmyprivateinfo', 8 => 'editmyprivateinfo', 9 => 'editmyoptions', 10 => 'abusefilter-log-detail', 11 => 'urlshortener-create-url', 12 => 'centralauth-merge', 13 => 'abusefilter-view', 14 => 'abusefilter-log', 15 => 'vipsscaler-test', 16 => 'collectionsaveasuserpage', 17 => 'reupload-own', 18 => 'move-rootuserpages', 19 => 'createpage', 20 => 'minoredit', 21 => 'editmyusercss', 22 => 'editmyuserjson', 23 => 'editmyuserjs', 24 => 'purge', 25 => 'sendemail', 26 => 'applychangetags', 27 => 'spamblacklistlog', 28 => 'mwoauthmanagemygrants' ]
Whether the user is editing from mobile app (user_app)
false
Whether or not a user is editing through the mobile interface (user_mobile)
false
Page ID (page_id)
60621622
Page namespace (page_namespace)
0
Page title without namespace (page_title)
'Memory and retention in learning'
Full page title (page_prefixedtitle)
'Memory and retention in learning'
Edit protection level of the page (page_restrictions_edit)
[]
Last ten users to contribute to the page (page_recent_contributors)
[ 0 => 'AnomieBOT', 1 => 'Headbomb', 2 => 'Carlos Muñoz-Neira', 3 => 'Monkbot', 4 => 'InternetArchiveBot', 5 => 'Mccapra', 6 => '128.106.188.34', 7 => 'Citation bot', 8 => '112.198.45.250', 9 => 'Bender the Bot' ]
Page age in seconds (page_age)
99654615
Action (action)
'edit'
Edit summary/reason (summary)
'/* Methods of improving memory and retention */ '
Old content model (old_content_model)
'wikitext'
New content model (new_content_model)
'wikitext'
Old page wikitext, before the edit (old_wikitext)
'{{Hatnote|For other senses of this term, see [[Memory]] or [[Learning]]}} [[File:Modal model of memory.tif|thumb|Model of the Memory Process]][[Memory|Human memory]] is the process in which information and material is encoded, stored and retrieved in [[Brain|the brain]].<ref name="auto">{{Cite book|title=The Processing of Memories (PLE : Forgetting and Retention.|last=Spear, Norman E.|date=2014|publisher=Taylor and Francis|isbn=9781317743842|oclc=879947178}}</ref> Memory is a property of the [[central nervous system]], with three different classifications: short-term, long-term and sensory memory.<ref name=":0">{{Cite book|title=Human Memory : Third Edition.|last=Radvansky, Gabriel A.|date=2017|publisher=Taylor and Francis|isbn=9781134871735|oclc=979169944}}</ref> The three types of memory have specific, different functions but each are equally important for memory processes. Sensory information is transformed and encoded in a certain way in the brain, which forms a memory representation.<ref name="auto1">{{Cite book|title=The Structure of Long-term Memory: A Connectivity Model of Semantic Processing|last=Klimesch|first=Wolfgang|publisher=Psychology Press|year=2013}}</ref> This unique coding of information creates a memory.<ref name="auto1" /> Memory and retention are linked because any retained information is kept in human memory stores, therefore without human memory processes, retention of material would not be possible.<ref name="auto4" /> In addition, memory and the process of [[learning]] are also closely connected. Memory is a site of storage and enables the retrieval and encoding of information, which is essential for the process of learning.<ref name=":0" /> Learning is dependent on memory processes because previously stored knowledge functions as a framework in which newly learned information can be linked.<ref name="auto8" /> Information is retained in human memory stores in different ways, but it is primarily done so through active learning, repetition and recall.<ref name=":4">{{Cite book|title=Foundations of human memory.|last=Kahana, Michael Jacob.|date=2014|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0199387649|oclc=884861998}}</ref> Information that is encoded and stored within memory stores can often be forgotten. There are multiple explanations for why this happens. These include: ineffective encoding of material, decay of information, interference, competition of newly learned material and retrieval failure.<ref name="auto7" /> There are multiple ways of improving the abilities of human memory and retention when engaging in learning. These depend on the nature of how the information was originally encoded into memory stores, and whether the stored material is regularly retrieved and recalled.<ref name="auto4">{{Cite book|title=Psychology: Themes and Variations|last=Weiten|first=Wayne|year=2013|isbn=9780176721275|oclc=1028226770}}</ref> Human memory has been studied throughout history, and there is extensive literature available to help understand its complexity. __TOC__ == Types of memory == === Long-term === [[File:LongTermMemory.jpg|thumb|left|Types of Long-term Memory]] [[Long-term memory]] is the site for which information such as facts, physical skills and abilities, procedures and semantic material are stored. Long-term memory is important for the retention of learned information, allowing for a genuine understanding and meaning of ideas and concepts.<ref name=":4" /> In comparison to short-term memory, the storage capacity of long-term memory can last for days, months, years or for an entire lifetime.<ref name=":4" /> Long-term memory has three components. [[Procedural memory]] is responsible for guiding how we perform certain tasks and providing the knowledge of how to do things, such as walking or talking.<ref name=":2" /> [[Semantic memory]] is responsible for providing general world knowledge through the information we have accumulated over our lives.<ref name=":2" /> [[Episodic memory]] is responsible for storing autobiographical events that we have personally experienced, which can be stated explicitly.<ref name=":2" /> === Short-term === [[Short-term memory]] is responsible for retaining and processing information very temporarily. It is the information that we are currently aware of thinking about.<ref name=":0" />{{Rp|13}} The storage capacity and duration of short-term memory is very limited; information can be lost easily with distraction.<ref name=":0" /> A famous paper written by psychologist George Miller in 1956 analyses this concept further. Miller wrote how short-term memory only has the ability to process or hold seven, plus or minus two items at a time, which then expires after roughly 30 seconds.<ref name=":0" /> This is due to short-term memory only having a certain number of "slots" in which to store information in.<ref name=":0" /> Despite the quick disappearance of information, short-term memory is an essential step for retaining information in long-term memory stores. Without it, information would not be able to be relayed into long-term memory. === Sensory === [[Sensory memory]] is thought of as a process of perception provided by the human senses.<ref name=":0" /> Sensory memory retains the information perfectly and accurately for a few hundred milliseconds.<ref name=":0" /> As the retention of information only lasts for such a short amount of time, this type of memory is often thought of as being a part of the process of perception instead of memory. [[Iconic memory]] and [[echoic memory]] are categories of sensory memory, and relay literal copies of sensory events to our memory. Iconic memory relays visual information and echoic memory relays auditory information.<ref name=":1">{{Cite book|title=Principles of Learning and Memory: Classic Edition|last=Crowder|first=Robert|publisher=New York: Taylor & Francis|year=2015}}</ref> Both types of memory have an unlimited capacity, but have a very short duration; iconic memory lasts for 50-500 milliseconds and echoic memory lasts for 8–10 seconds.<ref name=":1" /> == How information is learned and retained in memory == Processes of memory and learning are very interconnected and are often considered as the same. Psychologists however determine these as two distinct processes. [[Learning]] can be defined as the acquisition of knowledge gained through experience or studying, which can modify a behaviour.{{cn|date=May 2021}} [[Memory]] can be defined as the ability to remember previous experiences. Memory is essential for learning new information, as it functions as a site for storage and retrieval of learned knowledge. Two categories of long-term memory are used when engaging in learning. The first kind is procedural: how-to processes, and the second is declarative: specific information that can be recalled and reported.<ref name=":2">{{Cite book|title=Learning and Memory: The Brain in Action|url=https://archive.org/details/learningmemorybr00spre|url-access=registration|last=Sprenger|first=Marilee|publisher=Association for Supervision & Curriculum Development|year=1999}}</ref> There are many different ways of retaining information in memory. Acquiring and retaining new knowledge relies on the formation of associations created in memory stores.<ref name="auto7">{{Cite book|title=Memory|last=Whitehead|first=Anne|publisher=Routledge|year=2008}}</ref> Memory relies on the creation of associations, just as the creation of associations relies on memory when encoding and retaining new material.<ref name="auto8">{{Cite book|title=Human Learning and Memory|last=Lieberman|first=David A.|publisher=Cambridge University Press|year=2012|isbn=9781139206266|oclc=778277969}}</ref> The retention and retrieval of information in memory requires the information to be firmly embedded within a neural network; which can be done so through traditional methods of repetition and connecting new information with old information.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal|last=Smith|first=Elizabeth|date=1995|title=The Impairment of Retention|journal=Neurobiology of Learning and Memory|volume=63|via=Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals}}</ref> The process of repetition facilitates the process within the brain of solidifying connections.<ref name="auto9" /> When learning new information, the brain seeks to associate this material with previously stored knowledge through assimilation.<ref name="auto9">{{Cite book|title=Yellow Elephant: Improve your Memory and Learn More, Faster, Better|last=Ali|first=Tansel|publisher=Hardie Grant Books|year=2013|isbn=9781743580141|oclc=859748107}}</ref> When we learn something new, our brain creates new neural pathways. Therefore, repetition when engaging in learning is important for retaining this information in long-term memory stores.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Dykes|first=Barbara|date=2009|title=Repeat After Me: The Link Between Repetition and Memory|journal=Teacher: The National Education Magazine}}</ref> [[Chunking (psychology)|Chunking]] has also proved to be a useful strategy for retaining information.<ref name=":3">{{Citation|last=Richardson|first=John T.E.|chapter=Evolving Concepts of Working Memory|date=1996-07-11|pages=3–24|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=9780195100990|doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195100990.003.0001|title=Working Memory and Human Cognition}}</ref> Chunking is the process of grouping together individual items of similarity. By attaching a new name to these groups and remembering the name rather than each item, the amount of information remembered has shown to improve significantly.<ref name=":3" /> Through the separation of individual items, it becomes much easier to retain information, as our short-term memory can be so limiting. Overall, chunking enhances the ability of human memory to retain information.<ref name=":3" /> In addition, the [[Recall (memory)|recollection]] of learned information is essential to retaining such material in the long-term.<ref name="auto7"/> Recall refers to the re-accessing of previously learned information held in long-term memory stores. During this process, the brain relays a specific pattern of neural activity that echoes the original perception of that event.<ref name=":5" /> Regular recalling of stored information helps to improve memory retention. The more the material is recalled, the more it becomes engrained within our memory.<ref name="auto4"/> When we repeatedly think about knowledge we have learned, our brain strengthens the existing neural pathways which embeds this knowledge further within our long-term memory stores.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Jensen|first=R|date=1999|title=What happens to your Brain when learning|journal=Neurobiology of Learning and Memory|volume=69|via=Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals}}</ref> == Why learned information is forgotten == There are many explanations for why we forget learned information. A well-known explanation to explain why this happens is the ineffective encoding of material. This is when material appears to have been forgotten over time, however it is very likely that this material was never encoded into memory stores properly in the first place.<ref name="auto4"/> This is also referred to as pseudo-forgetting, and it is usually attributable to distractions, or a lack of attention when engaging in learning that lead to ineffective encoding.<ref name="auto4"/> Neural pathways and memory codes may still have been formed, however subsequent forgetting of learned information implies that it was ineffectively encoded within these pathways.<ref name="auto4" /> Another explanation for why we forget learned information is the decay of information. This concept determines the impermanence of memory storage as an explanation for forgetting.<ref name="auto"/> [[Decay theory]] posits that the process of forgetting is due to the inevitable fading of memory traces over time.<ref name="auto4"/> For this theory, the length of time that the information has been retained within memory stores is important. Essentially, the memories held in long-term stores start to fade as time passes, particularly if the memories haven't been re-visited.<ref name="auto" /> [[Interference theory]] provides another explanation for the forgetting of learned information. New memories interfere with old memories, and limits our ability to recall these over time.<ref name="auto8"/> There are two types of interference; retroactive and proactive.<ref name="auto4"/> [[Retroactive interference]] is when newly learned information impairs previously retained information, and [[proactive interference]] is when previously learned information interferes with newly retained information.<ref name="auto4"/> Essentially, interference theory posits that stored memories interfere and hamper one another, which is why we forget learned information. The competition between newly learned information and previously learned information can cause retained information to be forgotten. According to theories, the capacity of our memory stores are finite and can only retain a limited amount of information.<ref name="auto8"/> Therefore, the creation of newer memories can lead to the destruction or replacement of older memories due to competition of finite memory stores.<ref name="auto8"/> Retrieval failure provides another explanation for why we forget learned information. According to this theory, we forget information because it is inaccessible in long-term memory stores. Access to this information depends on retrieval cues, and the absence of these cues causes difficulties in recalling retained information.<ref name="auto1"/> Forgetting learned information occurs most often when the context and state are very different when encoding and retrieving. In these situations, there are no retrieval cues which can result in cue-dependent forgetting.<ref name="auto1" /> For example, many people do not remember much about their childhood. However, once returning to an old house or school; which provide retrieval cues, childhood memories usually begin to return.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Tulving|first=E|date=1974|title=Cue-dependent forgetting.|journal=American Scientist|volume=62|issue=1|pages=74|via=JSTOR|bibcode=1974AmSci..62...74T}}</ref> Retrieval failure and an absence in cues can be very influential for forgetting learned information. == Methods of improving memory and retention == There are several methods of improving our memory, and our ability to retain more information when engaging in learning. [[Chunking (psychology)|Chunking]] is a well-known method of improving memory and retention. In order to effectively chunk information together, connections and relations between the different items must be made. In combination, associating groups of items with things held in memory stores can make this more memorable, and can improve retention.<ref name="auto9"/> [[File:Five senses.jpg|thumb|Five Human Senses: Hearing, Sight, Taste, Smell and Touch]] Another method for improving memory and retention is imaginative and abstract thinking. Using imagination and thinking abstractly when learning new things are effective ways of improving memory and enabling a great amount of material to be effectively retained. Imagination creates stronger visuals and connections, which can lead to significant improvement in memory and retention.<ref name="auto9"/> The VAI memory principle: Visualisation, Association and Imagination, improves memory and retention when learning considerably.<ref name="auto9"/> This principle combines different methods of improving memory and retention to create one comprehensive method for engaging in successful learning. Other methods of maximising our ability to learn and retain new material is to use as many human senses as possible. These include: sight, touch, smell and hearing. All of these senses should be engaged when learning new information.<ref name="auto9"/> Research has shown that when engaging as many senses as possible at once, retention of information improves the most.<ref>{{Cite book|title=Yellow Elephant: Improve your Memory and Learn More, Faster, Better.|last=Ali|first=T|year=2013}}</ref> Deeper processing of the originally learned material results in more effective encoding and retrieval, due to [[semantic processing]] having taken place. Semantic processing occurs after we hear information and encode its meaning, allowing for deeper processing.<ref name=":2" /> Semantic encoding can therefore lead to greater levels of retention when learning new information. The avoidance of interfering stimuli such as music and technology when learning, can improve memory and retention significantly. These distractions interfere with the encoding of material in long-term memory stores.<ref name=":2" /> Matching retrieval cues when learning new information, to information that has originally been encoded in our brain, has also been shown to improve our memory and retention.<ref name="auto9"/> Retrieval cues can trigger stored memories, and are important for enhancing learning of new material. There are many other methods of improving our memory and retention, however these are the most well-known and credible methods. == References == {{Reflist}} {{Navboxes |title = Articles related to memory |list = {{Human intelligence topics}} {{Memory}} {{Nervous system physiology}} {{Mental processes}} }} {{Authority control}} [[Category:Neuropsychological assessment]] [[Category:Memory|Memory]] [[Category:Sources of knowledge]] [[Category:Mental processes]]'
New page wikitext, after the edit (new_wikitext)
'{{Hatnote|For other senses of this term, see [[Memory]] or [[Learning]]}} [[File:Modal model of memory.tif|thumb|Model of the Memory Process]][[Memory|Human memory]] is the process in which information and material is encoded, stored and retrieved in [[Brain|the brain]].<ref name="auto">{{Cite book|title=The Processing of Memories (PLE : Forgetting and Retention.|last=Spear, Norman E.|date=2014|publisher=Taylor and Francis|isbn=9781317743842|oclc=879947178}}</ref> Memory is a property of the [[central nervous system]], with three different classifications: short-term, long-term and sensory memory.<ref name=":0">{{Cite book|title=Human Memory : Third Edition.|last=Radvansky, Gabriel A.|date=2017|publisher=Taylor and Francis|isbn=9781134871735|oclc=979169944}}</ref> The three types of memory have specific, different functions but each are equally important for memory processes. Sensory information is transformed and encoded in a certain way in the brain, which forms a memory representation.<ref name="auto1">{{Cite book|title=The Structure of Long-term Memory: A Connectivity Model of Semantic Processing|last=Klimesch|first=Wolfgang|publisher=Psychology Press|year=2013}}</ref> This unique coding of information creates a memory.<ref name="auto1" /> Memory and retention are linked because any retained information is kept in human memory stores, therefore without human memory processes, retention of material would not be possible.<ref name="auto4" /> In addition, memory and the process of [[learning]] are also closely connected. Memory is a site of storage and enables the retrieval and encoding of information, which is essential for the process of learning.<ref name=":0" /> Learning is dependent on memory processes because previously stored knowledge functions as a framework in which newly learned information can be linked.<ref name="auto8" /> Information is retained in human memory stores in different ways, but it is primarily done so through active learning, repetition and recall.<ref name=":4">{{Cite book|title=Foundations of human memory.|last=Kahana, Michael Jacob.|date=2014|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0199387649|oclc=884861998}}</ref> Information that is encoded and stored within memory stores can often be forgotten. There are multiple explanations for why this happens. These include: ineffective encoding of material, decay of information, interference, competition of newly learned material and retrieval failure.<ref name="auto7" /> There are multiple ways of improving the abilities of human memory and retention when engaging in learning. These depend on the nature of how the information was originally encoded into memory stores, and whether the stored material is regularly retrieved and recalled.<ref name="auto4">{{Cite book|title=Psychology: Themes and Variations|last=Weiten|first=Wayne|year=2013|isbn=9780176721275|oclc=1028226770}}</ref> Human memory has been studied throughout history, and there is extensive literature available to help understand its complexity. __TOC__ == Types of memory == === Long-term === [[File:LongTermMemory.jpg|thumb|left|Types of Long-term Memory]] [[Long-term memory]] is the site for which information such as facts, physical skills and abilities, procedures and semantic material are stored. Long-term memory is important for the retention of learned information, allowing for a genuine understanding and meaning of ideas and concepts.<ref name=":4" /> In comparison to short-term memory, the storage capacity of long-term memory can last for days, months, years or for an entire lifetime.<ref name=":4" /> Long-term memory has three components. [[Procedural memory]] is responsible for guiding how we perform certain tasks and providing the knowledge of how to do things, such as walking or talking.<ref name=":2" /> [[Semantic memory]] is responsible for providing general world knowledge through the information we have accumulated over our lives.<ref name=":2" /> [[Episodic memory]] is responsible for storing autobiographical events that we have personally experienced, which can be stated explicitly.<ref name=":2" /> === Short-term === [[Short-term memory]] is responsible for retaining and processing information very temporarily. It is the information that we are currently aware of thinking about.<ref name=":0" />{{Rp|13}} The storage capacity and duration of short-term memory is very limited; information can be lost easily with distraction.<ref name=":0" /> A famous paper written by psychologist George Miller in 1956 analyses this concept further. Miller wrote how short-term memory only has the ability to process or hold seven, plus or minus two items at a time, which then expires after roughly 30 seconds.<ref name=":0" /> This is due to short-term memory only having a certain number of "slots" in which to store information in.<ref name=":0" /> Despite the quick disappearance of information, short-term memory is an essential step for retaining information in long-term memory stores. Without it, information would not be able to be relayed into long-term memory. === Sensory === [[Sensory memory]] is thought of as a process of perception provided by the human senses.<ref name=":0" /> Sensory memory retains the information perfectly and accurately for a few hundred milliseconds.<ref name=":0" /> As the retention of information only lasts for such a short amount of time, this type of memory is often thought of as being a part of the process of perception instead of memory. [[Iconic memory]] and [[echoic memory]] are categories of sensory memory, and relay literal copies of sensory events to our memory. Iconic memory relays visual information and echoic memory relays auditory information.<ref name=":1">{{Cite book|title=Principles of Learning and Memory: Classic Edition|last=Crowder|first=Robert|publisher=New York: Taylor & Francis|year=2015}}</ref> Both types of memory have an unlimited capacity, but have a very short duration; iconic memory lasts for 50-500 milliseconds and echoic memory lasts for 8–10 seconds.<ref name=":1" /> == How information is learned and retained in memory == Processes of memory and learning are very interconnected and are often considered as the same. Psychologists however determine these as two distinct processes. [[Learning]] can be defined as the acquisition of knowledge gained through experience or studying, which can modify a behaviour.{{cn|date=May 2021}} [[Memory]] can be defined as the ability to remember previous experiences. Memory is essential for learning new information, as it functions as a site for storage and retrieval of learned knowledge. Two categories of long-term memory are used when engaging in learning. The first kind is procedural: how-to processes, and the second is declarative: specific information that can be recalled and reported.<ref name=":2">{{Cite book|title=Learning and Memory: The Brain in Action|url=https://archive.org/details/learningmemorybr00spre|url-access=registration|last=Sprenger|first=Marilee|publisher=Association for Supervision & Curriculum Development|year=1999}}</ref> There are many different ways of retaining information in memory. Acquiring and retaining new knowledge relies on the formation of associations created in memory stores.<ref name="auto7">{{Cite book|title=Memory|last=Whitehead|first=Anne|publisher=Routledge|year=2008}}</ref> Memory relies on the creation of associations, just as the creation of associations relies on memory when encoding and retaining new material.<ref name="auto8">{{Cite book|title=Human Learning and Memory|last=Lieberman|first=David A.|publisher=Cambridge University Press|year=2012|isbn=9781139206266|oclc=778277969}}</ref> The retention and retrieval of information in memory requires the information to be firmly embedded within a neural network; which can be done so through traditional methods of repetition and connecting new information with old information.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal|last=Smith|first=Elizabeth|date=1995|title=The Impairment of Retention|journal=Neurobiology of Learning and Memory|volume=63|via=Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals}}</ref> The process of repetition facilitates the process within the brain of solidifying connections.<ref name="auto9" /> When learning new information, the brain seeks to associate this material with previously stored knowledge through assimilation.<ref name="auto9">{{Cite book|title=Yellow Elephant: Improve your Memory and Learn More, Faster, Better|last=Ali|first=Tansel|publisher=Hardie Grant Books|year=2013|isbn=9781743580141|oclc=859748107}}</ref> When we learn something new, our brain creates new neural pathways. Therefore, repetition when engaging in learning is important for retaining this information in long-term memory stores.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Dykes|first=Barbara|date=2009|title=Repeat After Me: The Link Between Repetition and Memory|journal=Teacher: The National Education Magazine}}</ref> [[Chunking (psychology)|Chunking]] has also proved to be a useful strategy for retaining information.<ref name=":3">{{Citation|last=Richardson|first=John T.E.|chapter=Evolving Concepts of Working Memory|date=1996-07-11|pages=3–24|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=9780195100990|doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195100990.003.0001|title=Working Memory and Human Cognition}}</ref> Chunking is the process of grouping together individual items of similarity. By attaching a new name to these groups and remembering the name rather than each item, the amount of information remembered has shown to improve significantly.<ref name=":3" /> Through the separation of individual items, it becomes much easier to retain information, as our short-term memory can be so limiting. Overall, chunking enhances the ability of human memory to retain information.<ref name=":3" /> In addition, the [[Recall (memory)|recollection]] of learned information is essential to retaining such material in the long-term.<ref name="auto7"/> Recall refers to the re-accessing of previously learned information held in long-term memory stores. During this process, the brain relays a specific pattern of neural activity that echoes the original perception of that event.<ref name=":5" /> Regular recalling of stored information helps to improve memory retention. The more the material is recalled, the more it becomes engrained within our memory.<ref name="auto4"/> When we repeatedly think about knowledge we have learned, our brain strengthens the existing neural pathways which embeds this knowledge further within our long-term memory stores.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Jensen|first=R|date=1999|title=What happens to your Brain when learning|journal=Neurobiology of Learning and Memory|volume=69|via=Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals}}</ref> == Why learned information is forgotten == There are many explanations for why we forget learned information. A well-known explanation to explain why this happens is the ineffective encoding of material. This is when material appears to have been forgotten over time, however it is very likely that this material was never encoded into memory stores properly in the first place.<ref name="auto4"/> This is also referred to as pseudo-forgetting, and it is usually attributable to distractions, or a lack of attention when engaging in learning that lead to ineffective encoding.<ref name="auto4"/> Neural pathways and memory codes may still have been formed, however subsequent forgetting of learned information implies that it was ineffectively encoded within these pathways.<ref name="auto4" /> Another explanation for why we forget learned information is the decay of information. This concept determines the impermanence of memory storage as an explanation for forgetting.<ref name="auto"/> [[Decay theory]] posits that the process of forgetting is due to the inevitable fading of memory traces over time.<ref name="auto4"/> For this theory, the length of time that the information has been retained within memory stores is important. Essentially, the memories held in long-term stores start to fade as time passes, particularly if the memories haven't been re-visited.<ref name="auto" /> [[Interference theory]] provides another explanation for the forgetting of learned information. New memories interfere with old memories, and limits our ability to recall these over time.<ref name="auto8"/> There are two types of interference; retroactive and proactive.<ref name="auto4"/> [[Retroactive interference]] is when newly learned information impairs previously retained information, and [[proactive interference]] is when previously learned information interferes with newly retained information.<ref name="auto4"/> Essentially, interference theory posits that stored memories interfere and hamper one another, which is why we forget learned information. The competition between newly learned information and previously learned information can cause retained information to be forgotten. According to theories, the capacity of our memory stores are finite and can only retain a limited amount of information.<ref name="auto8"/> Therefore, the creation of newer memories can lead to the destruction or replacement of older memories due to competition of finite memory stores.<ref name="auto8"/> Retrieval failure provides another explanation for why we forget learned information. According to this theory, we forget information because it is inaccessible in long-term memory stores. Access to this information depends on retrieval cues, and the absence of these cues causes difficulties in recalling retained information.<ref name="auto1"/> Forgetting learned information occurs most often when the context and state are very different when encoding and retrieving. In these situations, there are no retrieval cues which can result in cue-dependent forgetting.<ref name="auto1" /> For example, many people do not remember much about their childhood. However, once returning to an old house or school; which provide retrieval cues, childhood memories usually begin to return.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Tulving|first=E|date=1974|title=Cue-dependent forgetting.|journal=American Scientist|volume=62|issue=1|pages=74|via=JSTOR|bibcode=1974AmSci..62...74T}}</ref> Retrieval failure and an absence in cues can be very influential for forgetting learned information. == Methods of improving memory and retention == There are several methods of improving our memory, and our ability to retain more information when engaging in learning. [[Chunking (psychology)|Chunking]] is a well-known method of improving memory and retention. In order to effectively chunk information together, connections and relations between the different items must be made. In combination, associating groups of items with things held in memory stores can make this more memorable, and can improve retention.<ref name="auto9"/> [[File:Five senses.jpg|thumb|Five Human Senses: Hearing, Sight, Taste, Smell and Touch]] Another method for improving memory and retention is imaginative and abstract thinking. Using imagination and thinking abstractly when learning new things are effective ways of improving memory and enabling a great amount of material to be effectively retained. Imagination creates stronger visuals and connections, which can lead to significant improvement in memory and retention.<ref name="auto9"/> The VAI memory principle: Visualisation, Association and Imagination, improves memory and retention when learning considerably.<ref name="auto9"/> This principle combines different methods of improving memory and retention to create one comprehensive method for engaging in successful learning. Other methods of maximising our ability to learn and retain new material is to use as many human senses as possible. These include: sight, touch, smell and hearing. All of these senses should be engaged when learning new information.<ref name="auto9"/> Research has shown that when engaging as many senses as possible at once, retention of information improves the most.<ref>{{Cite book|title=Yellow Elephant: Improve your Memory and Learn More, Faster, Better.|last=Ali|first=T|year=2013}}</ref> Deeper processing of the originally learned material results in more effective encoding and retrieval, due to [[semantic processing]] having taken place. Semantic processing occurs after we hear information and encode its meaning, allowing for deeper processing.<ref name=":2" /> Semantic encoding can therefore lead to greater levels of retention when learning new information. The avoidance of interfering stimuli such as music and technology when learning, can improve memory and retention significantly. These distractions interfere with the encoding of material in long-term memory stores.<ref name=":2" /> Matching retrieval cues when learning new information, to information that has originally been encoded in our brain, has also been shown to improve our memory and retention.<ref name="auto9"/> Retrieval cues can trigger stored memories, and are important for enhancing learning of new material. There are many other methods of improving our memory and retention, however these are the most well-known and credible methods. https://humanfitnesscave.blogspot.com/2022/06/if-you-want-to-be-intelligent-eat.html == References == {{Reflist}} {{Navboxes |title = Articles related to memory |list = {{Human intelligence topics}} {{Memory}} {{Nervous system physiology}} {{Mental processes}} }} {{Authority control}} [[Category:Neuropsychological assessment]] [[Category:Memory|Memory]] [[Category:Sources of knowledge]] [[Category:Mental processes]]'
Unified diff of changes made by edit (edit_diff)
'@@ -54,4 +54,5 @@ There are many other methods of improving our memory and retention, however these are the most well-known and credible methods. +https://humanfitnesscave.blogspot.com/2022/06/if-you-want-to-be-intelligent-eat.html == References == '
New page size (new_size)
17645
Old page size (old_size)
17560
Size change in edit (edit_delta)
85
Lines added in edit (added_lines)
[ 0 => 'https://humanfitnesscave.blogspot.com/2022/06/if-you-want-to-be-intelligent-eat.html' ]
Lines removed in edit (removed_lines)
[]
Whether or not the change was made through a Tor exit node (tor_exit_node)
false
Unix timestamp of change (timestamp)
1656243134